Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 09 June 2023
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Clinical Application of Psychiatric Assessment and Treatment in Psychosomatic Diseases View all 11 articles

Neural basis of the attention bias during addiction stroop task in methamphetamine-dependent patients with and without a history of psychosis: an ERP study

\r\nGengdi Huang,&#x;Gengdi Huang1,2Chuanliang Han&#x;Chuanliang Han3Jihui Yang&#x;Jihui Yang2Caihong YeCaihong Ye2Iqbal JavedIqbal Javed2Fen LiuFen Liu2Zhi KongZhi Kong2Ying LiYing Li2Yingmei ZhuYingmei Zhu2Guangyong YiGuangyong Yi2Chuanjia JuChuanjia Ju2Xiaojian Jia
Xiaojian Jia2*Mei Yang
Mei Yang2*
  • 1State Key Laboratory of Chemical Oncogenomics, Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Chemical Genomics, Peking University Shenzhen Graduate School, Shenzhen, China
  • 2Department of Addiction Medicine, Shenzhen Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders, Shenzhen Kangning Hospital, Shenzhen Mental Health Center, the School of Mental Health of Southern University of Science and Technology, Shenzhen University, Anhui Medical University, and Jining Medical College, Shenzhen, China
  • 3Shenzhen Key Laboratory of Neuropsychiatric Modulation and Collaborative Innovation Center for Brain Science, Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Brain Connectome and Behavior, CAS Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Brain Cognition and Brain Disease Institute, Shenzhen Institute of Advanced Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Shenzhen-Hong Kong Institute of Brain Science, Shenzhen Fundamental Research Institutions, Shenzhen, China

Background: Attentional bias plays an important role in sustaining various types of drug addiction. No prior studies examined methamphetamine (MA)-associated psychosis (MAP) relationships between ERP time course and performance on an addiction Stroop task in MA abusers. The aim of the present study was to determine whether MA abusers with (MAP+) or without (MAP-) psychosis exhibit alterations of the ERP during the addiction Stroop task.

Methods: Thirty-one healthy controls (CTRL), 14 MAP-, and 24 MAP+ participants were recruited and completed the addiction Stroop task during EEG recording using 32 electrodes. Group variations were compared on measures of behavioral task performance and event-related potentials (ERP) of performance monitoring (N200, P300, N450). The Barratt impulsiveness scores were analyzed to investigate correlations with ERP changes.

Results: MA-related word stimulus elicited a more negative N200 amplitude over left-anterior electrodes in MAP- abusers; furthermore, a positive association between the N200 amplitude and Barratt attentional scores and non-planning scores was observed, while no such differences were found in MAP+ abusers. There were no significant differences in reaction time (RT) and error rate between each group.

Conclusion: This is the first study to examine psychosis relationships between ERP time course and performance on an addiction Stroop task in MA abusers with or without psychosis. These findings support the association between attentional bias measured by the MA addiction Stroop task and N200 component as well as indicate the possibility of using this cognitive task in combination with ERP technology to detect psychosis factors among abstinent MA abusers.

Introduction

Methamphetamine is a psychomotor stimulant with high liability for abuse, and MA abuse has become a public health concern across the globe. MA abuse has been of particular concern for a number of reasons including its association with the psychotic and cognitive symptoms that are similar to those observed in schizophrenia (Hsieh et al., 2014). Methamphetamine-associated psychosis is commonly referred to psychiatric services, displaying signs of positive symptoms such as delusions, paranoia and persecutory ideation and hallucinations (Sommers et al., 2006; Yang et al., 2020). Research has found that MAP is present in up to half of those with chronic methamphetamine dependence, leading to a heavier burden on drug treatment services (Grant, 2012). Further detection is needed to understand the underlying mechanism between MA abusers with (MAP+) or without (MAP-) psychosis.

The involvement of cognitive control modifications in addictive behaviors is evident through the presence of attentional bias, impaired decision-making, deficient response inhibition, and compulsive maladaptive behaviors (Goldstein and Volkow, 2011; Zilverstand et al., 2018). Furthermore, the cognitive processing of addiction-related stimuli is a key factor in substance cue reactivity; thus, it is essential to consider when exploring the neural basis of cognitive processing of exposures to substance cues on drug-seeking, craving, and relapse. The Stroop task (MacLeod, 1991) necessitates cognitive control as successful execution of the task demands individuals to react to one aspect of a stimulus while disregarding another contradictory element. More specifically, participants are directed to identify the ink color of a sequence of color words, consequently inhibiting the instinctive inclination to read the semantic meaning of the words. Previous research has demonstrated the association of color-naming Stroop performance in substance abuse, and MA-dependent subjects have made more errors and responded more slowly than controls in the color-naming Stroop task (Nestor et al., 2011). A related task is the addiction Stroop task (Cox et al., 2006), an analogous task of the classic color-naming Stroop task, where matched neutral and addiction-related words are used. Previous research using an addiction Stroop task has determined that the interference effects, such as an extended reaction time and increased error rate, are indicative of an attentional bias toward substance-related cues (Cox et al., 2006). Attentional bias in drug-word Stroop tests is thought to be a factor in the maintenance of drug-taking behavior and has been shown to be a reliable predictor of relapse elapse (Cox et al., 2002; Waters et al., 2003; Poireau et al., 2022). Attention bias assessed using the MA addiction Stroop task in MA abusers showed impairment in terms of a higher error rate of MA-related words relative to the CTRL participants (Chen et al., 2020). However, there is a scarcity of understanding regarding attention bias impairment in MAP+ abusers.

Event-related potentials (ERPs), high temporal resolution measures of human brain processing, have revealed the temporal sequence of the sub-processes involved in the Stroop interference and conflict resolution (Zhao et al., 2020). Previous research has suggested that a fronto-central negative-polarity effect in the 200 to 350 latency range, often referred to as the N200 (Folstein and Van Petten, 2008), can be elicited by conflict in a Stroop task (Boenke et al., 2009), and later, an N450-latency effect can also be observed (Larson et al., 2014). However, no difference was found in P300 latency or amplitude between the congruent and incongruent stimuli (Rosenfeld and Skogsberg, 2006). Electroencephalography (EEG) has been proposed as a neurophysiological biomarker to delineate psychotic disorders, expanding our understanding of the underlying neural mechanisms (Cao et al., 2022; Han et al., 2022a,b; Wang Q. et al., 2022). There has been limited research investigating patterns of the EEG that characterize MA abusers to detect electrophysiological abnormalities of their cortical networks and their associations with behavioral factors, including reduced working memory performance (Newton et al., 2003). A power spectrum analysis revealed an apparent EEG slowing in MA abusers (Newton et al., 2004). An ERP study of MA addiction Stroop task showed that attentional ERP components such as P300 were reduced with decreased craving within the first 3 abstinent months, and increased P300 amplitudes elicited by MA-related words were observed over left-anterior electrode sites (Haifeng et al., 2015). In addition, disruptions to resting EEG microstates were observed in MA abusers, leading to alterations in the microstate topographies over time, and these variations were associated with attention bias and a history of drug use (Chen et al., 2020). Recent research suggests that the left frontal electrode plays a distinct role in MAP. During resting eyes closed, MAP+ showed a higher delta/alpha frequency activity globally, while during resting eyes open, MAP+ displayed a higher delta/alpha frequency activity in all electrodes except the left frontal, when compared to the CTRL. Additionally, during the cognitive task, MAP+ exhibited a higher delta/alpha frequency activity in all electrodes except the left frontal (Howells et al., 2018). An EEG delta/alpha frequency activity assessment can help to identify the neurophysiological mechanisms associated with MAP disorder. However, the electrophysiological effects of MAP+ abusers remain largely unexplored, despite its growing prevalence.

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) has become an essential tool in the study of mental illness, which helps clinicians and researchers better understand the patterns of brain activity and structures that are associated with different disorders, such as depression (Wang J. et al., 2022), Alzheimer's disease (Gao et al., 2022a), schizophrenia (Gao et al., 2022b). Few imaging studies, however, have characterized brain dysfunction associated with MAP (Yang et al., 2021; Jia et al., 2022) nor investigated EEG differences in brain dysfunction of MAP. Therefore, neurological dysfunctions related to cognitive performance and psychosis in MA abusers need to be elucidated. The present study aimed to investigate the neurological functions using EEG measurement during addiction Stroop task in MA abusers with or without psychosis compared with age-matched normal participants.

Materials and methods

Participants

All participants were recruited from Shenzhen Kangning Hospital and local communities to take part in a set of neuropsychological tests, a psychiatric interview (see Table 1), and electrophysiological recordings. Participants were enrolled into three subgroups: 24 patients with MA-associated psychosis (MAP+, 5 female), 14 MA users without psychosis (MAP-, 2 female), and 31 healthy controls (CTRL, 5 female) with matched age, gender, and education. All participants were required to have normal or corrected-to-normal vision, normal hearing, be aged between 18 and 59 years, and belong to the Chinese Han ethnicity. The MAP+ met a lifetime diagnosis of MA-associated psychosis, while the course of symptoms could be longer than 6 months. The inclusion criteria for the MAP group were 2-fold: (1) Patients had to meet the diagnostic criteria for MA dependence, and they had to exhibit at least three instances of hallucinations and/or delusions; (2) patients were required to have abstained from MA use for a minimum of 15 days to ensure that any observed effects were not due to acute drug use or withdrawal. The MAP- received a diagnosis of MA dependence or abuse, without current or past psychotic symptoms. Subjects were excluded if they had any severe neurological disease, including head trauma, cardiovascular disease, and physical illness. Those with other psychiatric disorders in the DSM-IV axis I, or abuse of other substances, except for tobacco, coffee, and alcohol drinking without alcoholism, were also excluded. Ethical approval for this study was issued by the Research Ethics Committee of Shenzhen Kangning Hospital (2019-k003-01), and written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki was obtained from all the participants after receiving a full explanation of the study. As a token of appreciation for their time spent on the study, each participant was given 200 yuan after completion of the study.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The demographic data of abstinent methamphetamine users with and without psychosis and healthy control subjects.

Measures

MA-use patients (including the MAP- and the MAP+) completed the UCLA Natural History Interview (NHI) to provide detailed drug use information, and the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) (Patton et al., 1995) was used to measure impulsiveness, which consists of 30 items for three domains of impulsivity (attention, motor, and non-planning).

Addiction stroop task

The addiction Stroop task was constructed and performed on E-Prime 2.0 software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.). Reaction times and responses for the participant's key presses were recorded. Four stickers were placed on the Q, R, U, and P keys on the keyboard, each representing one of the four colors the participant used to select a response in the Stroop task (red, yellow, blue, and green, respectively). Each word remained on the screen for 1,500 ms or was ended by the reaction button, and each of the 16 words [8 MA-related words and 8 matched control words, which were used in previous research (Haifeng et al., 2015)] were shown in the four colors consequently creating 64 trials for each block, 4 blocks in total. These 256 trials were randomized across participants, and the same category of the words was set not to appear three times consecutively. Before the first block, a practice block would be performed to avoid unfamiliarity with task operations. The fixation cross and the following word were presented on a black background 75 cm away from the eyes. All evaluations were performed following the standardized instructions by trained researchers.

EEG acquisition and processing

While participants performed the Stroop task, EEG data were recorded from 32 Ag/AgCl scalp electrodes (BrainCap, GmbH, Germany) according to the international 10–20 system. The placement of the recording reference was at Cz, while the ground was positioned at approximately AFz. The impedances were kept below 10 kΩ with the sampling frequency at 1,000 Hz.

Utilizing MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA, United States) and the EEGLAB toolbox (Delorme and Makeig, 2004), we processed the continuous EEG data with in-house scripts. An offline digital band-pass filter (0.1–30 Hz) was applied. Epochs were extracted from −200 to 1,000 ms relative to the onset of the word stimulus and baseline corrected using the prestimulus interval (−200 to 0 ms). Independent component analysis (ICA) was used to correct eye movement, muscle artifacts, and heartbeat artifacts. All EEG epochs were processed for artifact detection by visual inspection and EEGLAB, and detection of obvious eye blinks and epochs with amplitude values exceeding ±100 mV at any electrode were rejected and later re-referenced to the average reference (Tafuro et al., 2019; Overbye et al., 2021). To guarantee the quality of data, patients with >20% of bad epochs for each condition and/or five bad channels were removed from the analysis, and one MAP- participant with more than 20% of bad epochs was excluded.

ERPs

ERP analyses mainly focused on the components of N200, P300, and N450. Time course of the average of left-anterior frontal channels was used to obtain the amplitude and latency of N200, P300, and N450. The time windows for evaluating ERP peaks were determined by inspecting the grand-averaged waveforms; the time windows are as follows: 200 to 300 ms for N200; 250 to 450 ms for P300; and 400 to 650 ms for N450. The mean amplitude and latency (FDR corrected) of these components in MA-related word trials were measured on left-anterior electrodes (F3, F7, FC5 electrodes), and a 50% fractional area technique (Kiesel et al., 2008) was applied to measure the latency of components.

Statistics

For demographic and clinical characteristics, the groups were compared with Student's t-test or one-way ANOVAs (analysis of variance) followed by Tukey's post-hoc test, and a chi-square test was conducted for categorical variable comparisons (Table 1). For the Stroop effect, error rate and mean reaction time (RT) were analyzed using a 3 × 2 mixed-design ANOVA with the groups (MAP- vs. MAP+ vs. HCs) as a between-subjects factor and stimulus type (MA word vs. neutral word) as a within-subject factor. In addition, only MA word trials were considered in the EEG data analysis; a one-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) followed by Tukey's post-hoc test was used to analyze the amplitude and latency of the groups in outcome measures, with the groups as the between-subject factor. The entire statistical analysis was conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics version 26 (IBM Corp., Armonk, N.Y., USA).

Results

Participant characteristics and behavior

The outcomes (mean values) of all dependent variables (CTRL, n =31; MAP-, n = 13; MAP+, n = 24) are presented in Table 1. There was no significant difference in the mean age and education between groups. MAP+ and MAP- show no difference in methamphetamine use variables and Barratt impulsiveness scores.

The descriptive behavioral data for both groups, including mean RTs (ms) and error rate (%) for each condition, are shown in Figure 1. The two-way ANOVAs were conducted on the mean RTs and the error rates. For the analysis of the RTs, all incorrect trials were excluded. Figure 1A illustrates the procedure of the MA addiction Stroop task. A 3 × 2 ANOVA on the RTs showed no significant main effect for group or condition (MA-related words and neutral words), respectively (Figure 1B, group: F (2, 128) = 1.270, p = 0.284; condition: F (1, 128) = 0.008, p = 0.930), and the group × condition interaction was also not significant (Figure 1B, F (2, 128) = 0.007, p = 0.993). There were no significant differences between the groups in the error rate of each condition (Figure 1C, group: F (2, 128) = 0.311, p = 0.733; condition: F (1, 128) = 0.008, p = 0.930; group × condition: F (2, 128) = 0.031, p = 0.970).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Illustration of the experimental procedure and behavioral performance. (A) Sequence of trial events in an MA-related words trial. (B) RT for correct trial across conditions. (C) The error rate committed on each group and each trial type.

ERPs

The N200, P300, N450 peaks were observed only in MA-related word condition of each group. The ERP waveforms and topographical distributions of the N200, P300, and N450 components for the analyzed trial types are shown in Figures 2A, E. A significant interaction emerged for N200 amplitude between the groups (Figure 2B, F2.65 = 3, p = 0.028); subsequently, the post-hoc analysis showed that compared with the CTRL group, the MAP- group showed a more negative N200 amplitude in the MA-related word condition on left-anterior electrodes (p = 0.009), while no significant effects emerged in the mean amplitude of P300 (Figure 2C, F (2.64) = 1.668, p = 0.196) and N450 (Figure 2D, F (2.64) = 0.455, p = 0.636). With regard to latency, none of the changes in the latency of N200, P300, and N450 were statistically significant (please refer to Supplementary Figures 1AC, N200: F (2.65) = 1.023, p = 0.365; P300: F (2.65) = 0.177, p = 0.838; N450: F (2.65) = 0.039, p = 0.962, respectively).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Averaged ERP amplitudes and topographical maps. (A) Grand mean averages (uV) of ERP waveforms to the MA-related words in the CTRL, MAP-, MAP+ groups across left-anterior electrode sites. (B–D) Mean N200, P300, and N450 amplitudes (uV) in each group, averaged across left-anterior electrode sites. Error bars represent SEMs. (E) Topographical maps of N200, P30, and N450 across different groups.

Correlations with Barratt impulsiveness scores

To further examine the relationship between behavior and electrophysiological signature, we correlated the Barratt total score and sub-domain scores separately with the difference in amplitudes of the N200 on MA-related word trials. Pearson's correlation test was used to assess the correlation between variables (Figures 3AD). A positive relationship of N200 amplitude with attentional impulsivity score (p = 0.026, r = 0.614) and non-planning impulsivity score (p = 0.015, r = 0.656) was observed in MAP- abusers, while this positive correlation was not found in Barratt total score (p = 0.080, r = 0.502) and motor impulsivity score (p = 0.788, r = 0.083).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Scatterplot of the relation between N200 amplitude and Barratt impulsiveness scores. (A) Barratt total scores. (B) Barratt motor scores. (C) Barratt attentional scores. (D) Barratt non-planning scores.

Discussion

This is an initial study to examine the relationships between ERP time course and performance on an addiction Stroop task in MA abusers with or without psychosis history. The current study investigated whether MA abusers with or without a psychosis history are characterized by deficits in ERPs during the addiction Stroop task. We found that MA-related words stimulus elicited a more negative N200 amplitude over left-anterior electrodes in MAP- abusers; furthermore, a positive association between N200 amplitude and Barratt attentional scores and non-planning scores was observed, while no such differences were found in MAP+ abusers.

Attentional bias toward addiction-related cues can impede executive functions that are pivotal in sustaining abstinence from drugs. To measure this attention bias, addiction Stroop tasks, which are similar to the classical Stroop tasks but comprised of both drug-related and neutral words, are used when assessing research participants who use drugs. Despite the lack of any noteworthy disparity in behavioral performance data between groups, high-temporal-resolution ERP technology is more adept at detecting the subtle distinctions in attentional processes, as more negative amplitudes over anterior electrode electrodes elicited by MA-related words that were observed among MAP- abusers and not MAP+ abusers can be attributed to the attentional bias of MAP abusers for MA-related cues. Previous studies (Potvin et al., 2018; Guerin et al., 2019) have shown that those with methamphetamine use disorder have cognitive deficits in many areas compared with controls, with inhibitory control, assessed through the color-word Stroop task, being particularly impaired. Minor addiction Stroop effect can be attributed to certain factors. First, the duration of withdrawal time may affect the Stroop effect, as MA abusers in our study were abstinent for a long time ranging from 2 to 68 months. In a recent study, it was observed that individuals who had recently abstained from MA abuse displayed a higher level of Stroop RT interference in comparison with both the control group and those who had been abstinent from MA abuse for a longer period of time. Conversely, no significant difference was observed between the long-term abstinent MA-abusing individuals and the control group (Salo et al., 2009). Second, RTs and error rate are not sensitive indicators of attentional bias from MA abusers in the Stroop task. Previous studies had showed no difference in RTs and error rate, however, there was evidence of intraindividual variability (IIV) and excessively long RTs (tau) in MA abusers who were abstinent for 2 to 60 months (Fassbender et al., 2015). Finally, quiet investigations of the addiction Stroop task did not detect any behavioral performance between drug abusers and participants, with quick response and high accuracy (Fehr et al., 2006; Haifeng et al., 2015; Chen et al., 2021), which might refer to the ceiling effect of this task, so a task of low difficulty would not be able to distinguish between drug users and the general population in terms of behavior.

ERPs, a highly informative and dynamic method of tracking brain activity with a high temporal resolution, are characterized by a series of positive and negative components. N200, P300, N450, and conflict slow potential (SP) latency and amplitude on the variable Stroop test were usually measured to differ between conditions (Ergen et al., 2014; Sahinoglu and Dogan, 2016; Fang et al., 2022). According to a prior investigation, smoking-associated images elicited a relatively negative response at frontal and central electrode locations during the 200–250 ms timeframe, as well as left frontal negativity between 400 and 500 ms at the F7 site, as compared to neutral images in the context of the addiction Stroop test (Fehr et al., 2007). Although we found relative negativity at left-anterior electrode sites between 200 and 300 ms, N450 did not reach a significant level. The functional implications of N450 have been extensively studied in order to differentiate between conflict resolution and response selection processes. This has been achieved by analyzing the ERP data obtained from various versions of the Stroop test. The results indicate that N450 exhibits greater negativity in response to incongruent trials as compared to congruent trials (Chuderski et al., 2016; Guo et al., 2018). Further inquiry into the part that N450 plays in addiction is needed in the future. Additionally, an ERP study of the addiction Stroop task revealed that abstinence from MA resulted in a decrease in left-anterior P300 to MA-related words at 3 and 6 months, and this decrease was associated with a decrease in craving (Haifeng et al., 2015). Despite the lack of P300 alteration in MA abusers in the present study, it is reasonable to assume that the period of abstinence is a crucial factor in MA-related words related to P300. It is apparent that further EEG investigations on the addiction Stroop task necessitate validation in subsequent research.

Correlations between ERPs and measured results provided useful information and may further multiply the results of the present research. Pearson's correlation analysis between the Barratt scores and the N200 amplitude was conducted to determine the relationship between subjective traits and N200 amplitude elicited by MA-related cues. A positive association between N200 amplitude and Barratt attentional scores and non-planning scores was observed in MAP- abusers. It may be concluded that those people who scored high in the Barratt attentional score and non-planning score showed a higher N200 amplitude in MA-related word condition.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Research Ethics Committee of Shenzhen Kangning Hospital. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

MY and XJ were instrumental in the conception and design of the work. FL and ZK were involved in the acquisition of the data. YL, YZ, and GY were involved in the investigation and formal analysis. GH, CH, and JY were responsible for analyzing and interpreting the data and as well as writing the article. In the course of a rigorous review process, GH, IJ, and CJ collaborated to scrutinize the article and have committed to being answerable for all elements of the project. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by grants from the Science and Technology Planning Project of Shenzhen Municipality (KCXFZ20211020164543007 and 20210617155253001), the Guangdong Basic and Applied Basic Research Foundation (2021A1515012141), the Shenzhen Fund for Guangdong Provincial High-level Clinical Key Specialties (SZGSP013), the STI2030-Major Projects (2022ZD0207100, HGD), and the China Postdoctoral Science Foundation (2022M723299).

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all the participants for their contribution.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1173711/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Figure 1. Averaged ERP latencies. (A–C) Mean N200, P300, and N450 amplitudes (uV) in each group, averaged across left-anterior electrode sites, respectively. Error bars represent SEMs.

References

Boenke, L. T., Ohl, F. W., Nikolaev, A. R., and Lachmann, T. (2009). Leeuwen Cv. Different time courses of Stroop and Garner effects in perception—An event-related potentials study. Neuroimage 45, 1272–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, Y., Han, C., Peng, X., Su, Z., Liu, G., Xie, Y., et al. (2022). Correlation between resting theta power and cognitive performance in patients with Schizophrenia. Front. Human Neurosci. 16, 853994. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.853994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, T., Su, H., Wang, L., Li, X., Wu, Q., Zhong, N., et al. (2021). Modulation of methamphetamine-related attention bias by intermittent theta-burst stimulation on left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Front. Cell Dev. Biol. 9, 667476. doi: 10.3389/fcell.2021.667476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, T., Su, H., Zhong, N., Tan, H., Li, X., Meng, Y., et al. (2020). Disrupted brain network dynamics and cognitive functions in methamphetamine use disorder: insights from EEG microstates. BMC Psychiatry 20, 334. doi: 10.1186/s12888-020-02743-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chuderski A. Senderecka M. Kałamała P. Kroczek B. Ociepka M. E. R. P. correlates of the conflict level in the multi-response Stroop task. Brain Res. (2016) 1650. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.08.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, W. M., Fadardi, J. S., and Pothos, E. M. (2006). The addiction-stroop test: Theoretical considerations and procedural recommendations. Psychol Bull. 132, 443–76. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, W. M., Hogan, L. M., Kristian, M. R., and Race, J. H. (2002). Alcohol attentional bias as a predictor of alcohol abusers' treatment outcome. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 68, 237–243. doi: 10.1016/S0376-8716(02)00219-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme A. Makeig S. E. E. G. L. A. B. an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. (2004) 134, 9. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

Google Scholar

Ergen, M., Saban, S., Kirmizi-Alsan, E., Uslu, A., Keskin-Ergen, Y., Demiralp, T., et al. (2014). Time-frequency analysis of the event-related potentials associated with the Stroop test. Int J Psychophysiol Org. Psychophysiol. 94, 463–72. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.08.177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fang, Z., Lynn, E., Huc, M., Fogel, S., Knott, V. J., Jaworska, N., et al. (2022). Simultaneous EEG + fMRI study of brain activity during an emotional Stroop task in individuals in remission from depression. Cortex. 155, 237–50. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fassbender, C., Lesh, T. A., Ursu, S., and Salo, R. (2015). Reaction time variability and related brain activity in methamphetamine psychosis. Biol. Psychiatry. 77, 465–74. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.07.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fehr, T., Wiedenmann, P., and Herrmann, M. (2006). Nicotine Stroop and addiction memory—An ERP study. Int J Psychophysiol. 62, 224–32. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fehr, T., Wiedenmann, P., and Herrmann, M. (2007). Differences in ERP topographies during color matching of smoking-related and neutral pictures in smokers and non-smokers. Int J Psychophysiol. 65, 284–93. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, J. R., and Van Petten, C. (2008). Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. Psychophysiology 45, 152–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00602.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Y., Tong, X., Hu, J., Huang, H., Guo, T., Wang, G., et al. (2022a). Decreased resting-state neural signal in the left angular gyrus as a potential neuroimaging biomarker of schizophrenia: an amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation and support vector machine analysis. Front. Psychiatry. 13, 949512. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.949512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Y., Zhao, X., Huang, J., Wang, S., Chen, X., Li, M., et al. (2022b). Abnormal regional homogeneity in right caudate as a potential neuroimaging biomarker for mild cognitive impairment: a resting-state fMRI study and support vector machine analysis. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14, 979183. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.979183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldstein, R. Z., and Volkow, N. D. (2011). Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 652–69. doi: 10.1038/nrn3119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, K. M. (2012). LeVan TD, Wells SM, Li M, Stoltenberg SF, Gendelman HE, et al. Methamphetamine-associated psychosis. J. Neuroimmune. Pharmacol. 7, 113–39. doi: 10.1007/s11481-011-9288-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guerin, A. A., Bonomo, Y., Lawrence, A. J., Baune, B. T., Nestler, E. J., Rossell, S. L., et al. (2019). Cognition and related neural findings on methamphetamine use disorder: insights and treatment implications from Schizophrenia research. Front. Psychiatry 10, 880. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00880

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, Z., Wu, X., Liu, J., Yao, L., and Hu, B. (2018). Altered electroencephalography functional connectivity in depression during the emotional face-word Stroop task. J. Neural. Eng. 15, 056014. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/aacdbb

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haifeng, J., Wenxu, Z., Hong, C., Chuanwei, L., Jiang, D., Haiming, S., et al. (2015). P300 event-related potential in abstinent methamphetamine-dependent patients. Physiol. Behav. 149, 142–8. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, C., Guo, M., Ke, X., Zeng, L., Li, M., Haihambo, N., et al. (2022a). Oscillatory biomarkers of autism: evidence from the innate visual fear evoking paradigm. Cogni. Neurodynamics 3, 6. doi: 10.1007/s11571-022-09839-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, C., Wang, T., Wu, Y., Li, H., Wang, E., Zhao, X., et al. (2022b). Compensatory mechanism of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder recovery in resting state alpha rhythms. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 16, 883065. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2022.883065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howells, F. M., Temmingh, H. S., Hsieh, J. H., van Dijen, A. V., Baldwin, D. S., Stein, D. J., et al. (2018). Electroencephalographic delta/alpha frequency activity differentiates psychotic disorders: a study of schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and methamphetamine-induced psychotic disorder. Transl. Psychiatry. 8, 75. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0105-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsieh, J. H., Stein, D. J., and Howells, F. M. (2014). The neurobiology of methamphetamine induced psychosis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 537. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jia, X., Wang, J., Jiang, W., Kong, Z., Deng, H., Lai, W., et al. (2022). Common gray matter loss in the frontal cortex in patients with methamphetamine-associated psychosis and schizophrenia. Neuroimage Clin. 36, 103259. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2022.103259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiesel, A., Miller, J., Jolicoeur, P., and Brisson, B. (2008). Measurement of ERP latency differences: a comparison of single-participant and jackknife-based scoring methods. Psychophysiology 45, 250–74. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00618.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larson, M. J., Clayson, P. E., and Clawson, A. (2014). Making sense of all the conflict: a theoretical review and critique of conflict-related ERPs. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 93, 283–97. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLeod C. M. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol Bull. (1991) 109, 163–203. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nestor, L. J., Ghahremani, D. G., Monterosso, J., and London, E. D. (2011). Prefrontal hypoactivation during cognitive control in early abstinent methamphetamine-dependent subjects. Psychiatry Res. 194, 287–95. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newton, T. F., Cook, I. A., Kalechstein, A. D., Duran, S., Monroy, F., Ling, W., et al. (2003). Quantitative EEG abnormalities in recently abstinent methamphetamine dependent individuals. Clin. Neurophysiol. 114, 410–5. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00409-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newton, T. F., Kalechstein, A. D., Hardy, D. J., Cook, I. A., Nestor, L., Ling, W., et al. (2004). Association between quantitative EEG and neurocognition in methamphetamine-dependent volunteers. Clinical Neurophysiology. Int. J. Fed. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 194–8. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00314-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Overbye, K., Walhovd, K. B., Fjell, A. M., Tamnes, C. K., and Huster, R. J. (2021). Electrophysiological and behavioral indices of cognitive conflict processing across adolescence. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 48, 100929. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100929

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patton J. H. Stanford M. S. Barratt E. S. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. (1995) 51, 768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6&lt

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poireau M. Milpied T. Maillard A. Delmaire C. Volle E. Bellivier F. Biomarkers of relapse in cocaine use disorder: a narrative review. Brain Sci. (2022) 12, 13. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12081013.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Potvin, S., Pelletier, J., Grot, S., Hebert, C., Barr, A. M., Lecomte, T., et al. (2018). Cognitive deficits in individuals with methamphetamine use disorder: a meta-analysis. Addict. Behav. 80, 154–60. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenfeld, J. P., and Skogsberg, K. R. (2006). P300-based Stroop study with low probability and target Stroop oddballs: the evidence still favors the response selection hypothesis. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 60, 240–50. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sahinoglu, B., and Dogan, G. (2016). Event-related potentials and the stroop effect. Eurasian J. Med. 48, 53–7. doi: 10.5152/eurasianjmed.2016.16012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salo, R., Nordahl, T. E., Galloway, G. P., Moore, C. D., Waters, C., Leamon, M. H., et al. (2009). Drug abstinence and cognitive control in methamphetamine-dependent individuals. J. Subst. Abuse Treat. 37, 292–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2009.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sommers, I., Baskin, D., and Baskin-Sommers, A. (2006). Methamphetamine use among young adults: health and social consequences. Addict. Behav. 31, 1469–76. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tafuro, A., Ambrosini, E., Puccioni, O., and Vallesi, A. (2019). Brain oscillations in cognitive control: a cross-sectional study with a spatial stroop task. Neuropsychologia 133, 107190. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang J. Zhao X. Bi Y. Jiang S. Sun Y. Lang J. Executive function elevated by long term high-intensity physical activity the regulation role of beta-band activity in human frontal region. Cogn. Neurodynamics. (2022) 3, 5. doi: 10.1007/s11571-022-09905-z.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Q., Gao, Y., Zhang, Y., Wang, X., Li, X., Lin, H., et al. (2022). Decreased degree centrality values as a potential neuroimaging biomarker for migraine: a resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging study and support vector machine analysis. Front. Neurol. 13, 1105592. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.1105592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waters, A. J., Shiffman, S., Sayette, M. A., Paty, J. A., Gwaltney, C. J., Balabanis, M. H., et al. (2003). Attentional bias predicts outcome in smoking cessation. Health Psychol. 22, 378–87. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.22.4.378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, M., Jia, X., Zhou, H., Ren, P., Deng, H., Kong, Z., et al. (2021). Brain dysfunction of methamphetamine-associated psychosis in resting state: approaching schizophrenia and critical role of right superior temporal deficit. Addict. Biol. 26, e13044. doi: 10.1111/adb.13044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, M., Yang, C., Liu, T., and London, E. D. (2020). Methamphetamine-associated psychosis: links to drug use characteristics and similarity to primary psychosis. Int. J. Psychiatry Clin. Pract. 24, 31–7. doi: 10.1080/13651501.2019.1676451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, X., Li, H., Wang, E., Luo, X., Han, C., Cao, Q., et al. (2020). Neural Correlates of working memory deficits in different adult outcomes of ADHD: an event-related potential study. Front. Psychiatry. 11, 348. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zilverstand, A., Huang, A. S., Alia-Klein, N., and Goldstein, R. Z. (2018). Neuroimaging impaired response inhibition and salience attribution in human drug addiction: a systematic review. Neuron 98, 886–903. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: methamphetamine, methamphetamine-associated psychosis, addiction stroop task, attention bias, EEG

Citation: Huang G, Han C, Yang J, Ye C, Javed I, Liu F, Kong Z, Li Y, Zhu Y, Yi G, Ju C, Jia X and Yang M (2023) Neural basis of the attention bias during addiction stroop task in methamphetamine-dependent patients with and without a history of psychosis: an ERP study. Front. Psychol. 14:1173711. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1173711

Received: 25 February 2023; Accepted: 16 May 2023;
Published: 09 June 2023.

Edited by:

Yujun Gao, Wuhan University, China

Reviewed by:

Lin Liu, Peking University, China
Jin-Bo Sun, Xidian University, China
Jie Gong, Xidian University, China
Nghi C. D. Truong, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, United States

Copyright © 2023 Huang, Han, Yang, Ye, Javed, Liu, Kong, Li, Zhu, Yi, Ju, Jia and Yang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mei Yang, ym8342@163.com; Xiaojian Jia, jiaxiaojian@outlook.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.