Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 23 May 2023
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings

“Attitudes to voices”: a survey exploring the factors influencing clinicians’ intention to assess distressing voices and attitudes towards working with young people who hear voices

Aikaterini Rammou,
Aikaterini Rammou1,2*Clio Berry,Clio Berry2,3David Fowler,David Fowler1,2Mark Hayward,Mark Hayward1,2
  • 1School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom
  • 2Research & Development Department, Sussex Partnership NHS Foundation Trust, Hove, United Kingdom
  • 3Brighton and Sussex Medical School, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom

Introduction: Due to the general psychopathological vulnerability of young people who hear distressing voices, research has stressed the importance for clinicians to assess this experience in youth. Nonetheless, the limited literature on the topic comes from studies with clinicians in adult health services and it primarily reports that clinicians do not feel confident in systematically assessing voice-hearing and doubt the appropriateness of doing so. We applied the Theory of Planned Behavior and identified clinicians’ job attitudes, perceived behavioral control, and perceived subjective norms as putative predictors of their intent to assess voice-hearing in youth.

Method: Nine hundred and ninety-six clinicians from adult mental health services, 467 from Child and Adolescent Mental Health (CAMHS) and Early Intervention in Psychosis (EIP) services and 318 primary care clinicians across the UK completed an online survey. The survey gathered data on attitudes toward working with people who hear voices, stigmatizing beliefs, and self-perceived confidence in voice-related practices (screening for, discussing and providing psychoeducation material about voice-hearing). Responses from youth mental health clinicians were compared with professionals working in adult mental health and primary care settings. This study also aimed to identify what youth mental health clinicians believe about assessing distressing voices in adolescents and how beliefs predict assessment intention.

Results: Compared to other clinicians, EIP clinicians reported the most positive job attitudes toward working with young voice-hearers, the highest self-efficacy in voice-hearing practices, and similar levels of stigma. Job attitudes, perceived behavioral control and subjective norms explained a large part of the influences on clinician’s intention to assess voice-hearing across all service groups. In both CAMHS and EIP services, specific beliefs relating to the usefulness of assessing voice-hearing, and perceived social pressure from specialist mental health professionals regarding assessment practices predicted clinician intention.

Discussion: Clinicians’ intention to assess distressing voices in young people was moderately high, with attitudes, subjective norms and perceived behavioral control explaining a large part of its variance. Specifically in youth mental health services, promoting a working culture that encourages opening and engaging in discussions about voice-hearing between clinicians, and with young people, and introducing supportive assessment and psychoeducation material about voice-hearing could encourage conversations about voices.

1. Introduction

Young people can experience shame and stigma in relation to the onset of voice-hearing experiences (Yang et al., 2015) and they may be reluctant to disclose this experience to others (Bogen-Johnston et al., 2019; Parry et al., 2020). This can result in them first asking for help with other difficulties such as peer relationships, anxiety and depression (Boydell et al., 2013; Stowkowy et al., 2013; Falkenberg et al., 2015), and rarely volunteering information on hearing voices, unless being asked directly and sensitively in a normalizing environment (Mertin and Hartwig, 2004; Kelleher et al., 2014). Moreover, young people are vulnerable to feeling that their own understanding and explanations for their voice-hearing experiences are dismissed due to the power of clinicians’ opinions and biomedical explanations (Bampton, 2012). Therefore, professionals need to have the capacity to be vigilant for and prepared to ask about hearing voices and other unusual experiences during routine assessments (Stowkowy et al., 2013), in a sensitive and normalizing way using simple and non-medical language (Sikich, 2013).

Once voice-hearing is disclosed, patients have reported a desire to discuss their experiences with mental health clinicians (Coffey and Hewitt, 2008; Bogen-Johnston et al., 2019; Griffiths et al., 2019) and receive support in managing their voice-hearing (Baker et al., 1997). Such conversations about these experiences could help patients explore their voices and potentially reduce their negative impact (Romme et al., 2009; Watkins et al., 2020). Regarding young people, a cautious-but-curious investigation of the psychopathological and psychosocial context of their voice-hearing experience is recommended (Maijer et al., 2019). This is vital, as evidence suggests that young people who report such experiences could be a target group for early intervention to improve their functional outcomes, given that psychotic-like experiences might be an early marker of later, ongoing mental distress (Lindgren et al., 2019; Carey et al., 2021). Gaining accurate and detailed information about voice-hearing may therefore facilitate clinicians in devising a helpful support plan (England, 2007). However, it appears unlikely that systematic and effective assessment of voice-hearing experiences among young people is happening consistently in routine clinical practice as clinicians appear to lack confidence in doing so (Coffey et al., 2004; Harrison et al., 2008; White et al., 2019), and can doubt the value and appropriateness of discussing voice-hearing experiences (Coffey et al., 2004; Harrison et al., 2008; White et al., 2019).

The Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB; Ajzen, 2005) is a useful model for explaining clinician behaviors (Eke et al., 2012; Levy et al., 2016; Lecomte et al., 2018). TPB proposes three main drivers of intention to perform an action: attitudes -comprising the imagined outcome of the action and how much that outcome is valued; subjective norms -comprising perceptions of what others would usually do and what they would approve or disapprove of doing, and how important that is to the person planning an action; and perceived behavioral control -comprising internal (e.g., self-efficacy for the action) and external facilitators and barriers (e.g., other duties that may need to be performed at a given time). These TPB drivers predict intention to perform the behavior, which in turn predicts actual behavior (Armitage and Conner, 2001). Thus, the TPB can explain influences on clinicians’ intention to assess voice-hearing experiences once disclosed by patients.

The attitude component of TPB can incorporate stigmatizing attitudes about voice-hearers and beliefs about the legitimacy or value of discussing voice-hearing experiences. Evidence suggests that clinicians’ attitudes are likely to be quite negative. Clinicians reportedly often struggle to discuss voice-hearing with patients, especially regarding voice content and meaning, and can feel skeptical about the value or appropriateness of such conversations (Coffey et al., 2004; Harrison et al., 2008; White et al., 2019). Mental health clinicians can hold the belief that talking about voices might do harm or cause further distress (Coffey and Hewitt, 2008; McMullan et al., 2018), which reduces the likelihood of them engaging in such conversations. More broadly, While clinicians report feeling empathic toward voice-hearers (Kramarz et al., 2020), they can feel powerless and helpless in reducing voice-related distress (McMullan et al., 2018).

The norm component of TPB could refer to clinical practice culture, which may discourage detailed discussions about voice-hearing with patients. For instance, White et al. (2019) revealed that recently qualified mental health nurses could not identify examples of colleagues having discussions with patients about their voices. This could set a workplace culture that discourages discussion about voices with patients during experiential clinical learning (Cleary et al., 2011; Wright et al., 2011).

The final component of TPB, perceived behavioral control, relates to clinicians’ perceived confidence in their ability to perform an action. Evidence suggests that clinicians report a lack of subjective understanding of voice-hearing experience and lack self-efficacy in asking detailed questions about voices (Kramarz et al., 2020). Perceived behavioral control also relates to the degree that a clinician has control over the action regarding situational factors such as time constrains (McCluskey and Vries, 2020). Moreover, lack of subjective understanding, perceived clinical risks related to commanding voices and what they might ask hearers to do (e.g., self-harm), and the diversity of voice-hearing experiences appear to be associated with professionals’ reported lack of clinical confidence (Kramarz et al., 2020).

Lastly, TPB allows for the inclusion of background factors that could influence the three main drivers of behavioral intention. These could include personal, social and informational variables that have been found to affect clinicians’ intention to discuss voices in past research (Ajzen, 2005). Evidence suggests that the quality of professional experience, (e.g., contact that disconfirms negative stereotypes or includes a common goal), is more important than the duration of work (Lauber et al., 2006; Dabby et al., 2015) in reducing stigmatizing views of mental illness (Couture and Penn, 2003; Jorm et al., 2012). Moreover, both more professional contact and having a personal relationship with someone with lived experience has been associated with less stigmatizing views of people with mental health problems in primary care clinicians and psychiatrists (Caplan et al., 2016; Sandhu et al., 2019). In terms of training in voice-hearing, general mental health professionals report little training in assessing voice-hearing (Kramarz et al., 2020; McCluskey and Vries, 2020), and even in specialist Early Intervention in Psychosis services (EIP) the training has been described as variable (Bogen-Johnston et al., 2020).

There is additionally a need to consider how assessment intention may vary for different professional groups. Although clinicians within specialist psychosis services reportedly lack self-efficacy in, and have concerns regarding, discussing voice-hearing experiences with young patients (Bogen-Johnston et al., 2020), primary care clinicians report lack of confidence in interviewing (Brunero et al., 2018) and anxiety in supporting people with mental health difficulties more generally (Roberts et al., 2013), and the least confidence in identifying and managing psychotic experiences, such as voice-hearing, in youth, compared to other mental health problems (Kehoe et al., 2020). Moreover, medical professionals have been found to hold more stigmatizing attitudes toward people with psychosis compared to mental health professionals (Hori et al., 2011; Mittal et al., 2014; Smith et al., 2017).

1.1. The current study

It is clear that more needs to be learnt about the responses of clinicians when a patient speaks about their distressing voice-hearing experiences (e.g., McCluskey & de Vries, 2020). The TPB offers a framework for exploring how clinician characteristics and experiences can predict intention to discuss voices. For the purposes of this study, “assessing voice-hearing” was the primary behavior of interest, referring to a detailed exploration of the experience including questions about its phenomenology (e.g., frequency, duration, content), the meaning and beliefs assigned to the voices by the patients, and the impact on their emotions and functioning. Understanding the influences on clinician intention can support the development, evaluation, and implementation of targeted training approaches.

With respect to the different service groups, this study aimed to explore differences in clinicians’ perceived self-efficacy in voice-hearing practice, stigma, and attitudes toward working with young people between Child and Adolescent Mental health services (CAMHS), EIP, and primary care clinicians. Adult mental health service clinicians were also sampled as a reference group, and both they, and a randomly selected half of primary care clinicians answered the questionnaire with reference to adult voice-hearers, for comparative purposes (Aim 1). This study also investigated the influence of TPB components as predictors of intention to assess distressing voice-hearing within different service groups. Based on findings about correlates to clinicians’ intention to discuss voices, the putative influence of relevant background factors on intention to assess voice-hearing was also taken into consideration. These included the dispositional factors of stigmatizing beliefs and general job attitudes toward working with patients who hear voices, and informational factors of professional and personal voice-hearing experience, perceived self-efficacy to voice-hearing practice, and past training in working with voice-hearers (see Figure 1) (Aim 2). Finally, this study aimed to identify, specifically for clinicians working with young people who hear voices, the most influential specific behavioral, normative and control beliefs on clinicians’ intention to assess distressing voices (Aim 3).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Diagrammatic representation of potential predictors of clinicians’ intention to assess distressing voice-hearing following disclosure.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Ethical statement

The study was sponsored by the University of Sussex, UK and received ethical approval from the Health Research Authority (Reference: 048 HAY/IRAS ID: 257355). Participants gave (electronic) informed consent for their participation before completing the self-report questionnaires online.

2.2. Design

This study was a between and within-group cross-sectional exploratory study using a battery of self-report questionnaires.

2.3. Participants and procedure

Clinicians were invited to complete an online survey by the research department of their National Health Service (NHS) Trust or the local Clinical Research Network and distribution of advertisement material. Data were collected via the Qualtrics online survey platform. Participants were informed that after completing and submitting the consent page of the survey that their consent was to be assumed and any data entered after that point would be recorded. Participation was anonymous and voluntary.

An a-priori sample size calculation for was conducted using G*Power software (Faul et al., 2009) for a multiple hierarchical linear regression model with 11 predictors of intention to assess distressing voices, indicating a minimum sample size of 262 for each service group, for an effect size of f 2 = 0.10, a = 0.05 and power of 0.95. Participants had to be clinicians working in an NHS service; Child and Adolescent Mental Health Services (CAMHS), Early Intervention in Psychosis (EIP) Services and/or adult mental health services, or in primary care services. Nine hundred and ninety-six clinicians from adult mental health services, 467 from CAMHS and EIP, working in 27 NHS mental health Trusts took part in the study. In primary care services, 158 clinicians completed the survey asking about adult patients and 160 the survey asking about young patients (12–18 years of age), working in 32 Integrated Care Boards across the UK.

To allow for comparison between predictors of intention to assess distressing voice-hearing depending on the age of patients that clinicians typically work with within their respective services, CAMHS and EIP clinicians completed a survey with reference to patients who were 12–18 years of age, whereas Adult mental health service clinicians were asked about patients aged 19 years and over. Primary care clinicians were randomized so that half of them completed the survey with reference to patients 12–18 years of age and the remaining half about patients 19 years or over. Participant demographic and professional background characteristics appear in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample characteristics for all service groups (N = 1751).

2.4. Measures

2.4.1. Demographic and professional background.

Demographic and professional background data relating to profession, years of experience in mental health services in general, and in young people’s services more specifically were captured. Clinicians were also asked to indicate the total number of voice-hearers they had worked with, frequency of contact with patients with distressing voices, personal experience with voice-hearing, level, and type of training to support patients who hear voices, and their perceived need and willingness to receive further training on supporting and assessing distressed voice-hearers.

2.4.2. Voice-hearing practice self-efficacy.

A self-efficacy questionnaire was constructed following recommendations by Bandura (2006), with three items aiming to measure clinicians’ self-perceived capability for three different tasks: (1) ask a patient if they hear voices; (2) discuss voice-hearing experiences with a patient who hears distressing voices; and (3) provide useful information to a patient who hears distressing voices. The items were rated on a 100-point scale, from 0 = “Cannot do” to 100 = “Highly certain can do.”

2.4.3. Theory of planned behavior (TPB).

2.4.3.1. Direct measures.

A TPB measure was constructed to capture clinicians’ intention to assess distressing voice-hearing following patient disclosure, following author guidelines (Fishbein and Ajzen, 2010) and additional researcher recommendations (Francis et al., 2004). A definition of the term “assessing” was provided; “asking a service user a series of open-ended questions to get detailed information about their experience”. The measure included items relating to the direct predictors of intention to assess voice-hearing, namely attitudes (n = 7 items, e.g., “When a young person discloses hearing distressing voices to me, assessing their voice-hearing experiences would be…,” with a response scale from “Very harmful” to “Very beneficial”), subjective norms (n = 5; e.g., “When a young person discloses hearing distressing voices to me, most people who are important to me professionally would approve of my assessing their voice-hearing experiences” with a response scale from “Very strongly disagree” to “Very strongly agree”), and perceived behavioral control (n = 6; e.g., “When a young person discloses hearing distressing voices to me, I am confident that I will assess their voice-hearing experiences”; with a response scale from “Very strongly disagree” to “Very strongly agree”). Three items captured behavioral intention (e.g., “When a young person discloses hearing voices to me, I intend to assess their voice-hearing experiences from now on”; with a response scale from “Very strongly disagree” to “Very strongly agree”).

Responses were recorded on a 7-point Likert scale, with higher scores representing more positive attitudes, approving subjective norms, and higher perceived behavior control.

The mean of item scores was calculated to provide an overall construct score. The TPB subscales for all predictor factors showed good internal consistency in all service groups, αs > 0.79. Specifically, for the attitudes subscale, the internal consistency estimates were α = 0.82 for the adult mental health, α = 0.82 for youth mental health (EIP and CAMHS), α = 0.90 for adult primary care and α = 0.87 for primary care for young people. The internal consistency estimates for the subjective norms subscale were α = 0.81 for the adult mental health, α = 0.79 for youth mental health (EIP and CAMHS), α = 0.90 for adult primary care and α = 0.85 for primary care for young people. The internal consistency estimates for the perceived behavioral control scale were α = 0.85 for the adult mental health, α = 0.85 for youth mental health (EIP and CAMHS), α = 0.89 for adult primary care and α = 0.85 for primary care for young people. For the intention subscale, the internal consistency was α = 0.93 for the adult mental health, α = 0.94 for youth mental health (EIP and CAMHS), α = 0.96 for adult primary care and α = 0.98 for primary care for young people.

2.4.3.2. Indirect measures.

The TPB questionnaire for the EIP and CAMHS clinicians additionally included indirect, belief-based predictors in the form of 30 specific belief items associated with forming attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control regarding assessing voice-hearing and the outcome evaluation for each belief (Ajzen, 1991; Supplementary Table S1). Indirect measures were calculated by multiplying the individual belief with its corresponding outcome evaluation and then summing the products for each TPB predictor construct. Higher scores indicate that a clinician is in favor of, experiences social pressure to, and feels in control of assessing voice-hearing. Further details on the development and scoring of the indirect measures can be found in the Supplementary Method Information S1 and Supplementary Table S2. The final TPB questionnaire for the CAMHS and EIP clinicians had 76 items, 53 of which related to the indirect TPB measures.

2.4.4. Attitudes relating to working with people with distressing voices.

To measure clinicians’ attitudes relating to working with people with distressing voices, a 35-item modified version (McLeod et al., 2002) of the Alcohol and Alcohol Problems Perception Questionnaire (AAPPQ; Cartwright, 1980) was used. The original AAPPQ had been designed to capture therapists’ attitudes toward working with patients who abuse alcohol, and it has been modified (McLeod et al., 2002; Berry and Greenwood, 2016) to capture attitudes of mental health professionals to working with people with psychosis. For the purpose of this study, the questionnaire items were amended to refer specifically to voice-hearing. Items were scored on a 7-point Likert scale, with higher scores reflecting more positive attitudes. The following mean subscale scores were calculated, all of which had acceptable internal consistency: role security (before item removal) α = 0.87 for adult mental health, α = 0.89 for youth mental health, α = 0.89 for adult primary care, α = 0.91 for young people primary care; role security after two items were removed (“I feel I have the right to ask a client for any information that is relevant to their problems with distressing voice-hearing” and “I feel I have the right to ask clients questions about their distressing voice-hearing when necessary”) α = 0.92 for adult mental health, α = 0.90 for youth mental health, α = 0.90 for adult primary care, α = 0.91 for young people primary care after two items being removed); therapeutic commitment (before item removal) α = 0.87 for adult mental health, α = 0.90 for youth mental health, α = 0.83 for adult primary care, α = 0.86 for young people primary care; therapeutic commitment after one item was removed (“I wish there was more respect to staff who work with service users who hear distressing voices”) α = 0.88 for adult mental health, α = 0.91 for youth mental health, α = 0.85 for adult primary care, α = 0.87 for young people primary care; and empathy (before item removal) α = 0.69 for adult mental health, α = 0.72 for youth mental health, α = 0.69 for adult primary care, α = 0.66 for young people primary care; and empathy after one item was removed (“I find it difficult to have empathy for service users’ experience of hearing distressing voices”) α = 0.75 for adult mental health, α = 0.77 for youth mental health, α = 0.71 for adult primary care, α = 0.66 for young people primary care.

2.4.5. Stigma toward voice-hearing.

The Attribution Questionnaire-9 [AQ-9; adapted from Corrigan et al., (2014)] was used to capture stigma toward people who experience voice-hearing. The questionnaire presents a vignette portraying a male patient with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. As voice-hearing can be present in different diagnoses (Larøi, 2012; Maijer et al., 2017), the diagnosis was removed and to minimize bias related to patient gender, the language was amended to be gender-neutral. Alongside the vignette, nine questions were used to assess responsibility (blame, pity, danger, and help) and dangerousness (danger, fear, avoidance, coercion, and institutionalization). Each response is scored on a 9-point Likert scale, from 1 (Not at all) to 9 (Very much). Clinician stigma was estimated as the total score of all items, with higher scores representing more stigmatizing attitudes. Corrigan et al. (2014) found AQ-9 internal consistency and test–retest reliability for mental health clinicians to be 0.71 and 0.87, respectively. The internal consistency estimates in this study were α = 0.69 for adult mental health, α = 0.57 for youth mental health, α = 0.68 for adult primary care and α = 0.55 for young people primary care. To improve the internal consistency of AQ-9, two items were removed from the total score estimation (“I would feel pity for Sam,” “How likely is it that you would help Sam?”), resulting in Cronbach’s alpha of α = 0.75 for adult mental health, α = 0.70 for youth mental health, α = 0.75 for adult primary care and α = 0.69 for young people primary care.

2.5. Data analysis

Participants’ responses to the survey were exported to SPSS Version 25 (IBM Corp., 2017). Welch’s t-tests and Pearson’s chi-square tests were used to investigate whether data missingness was related to any demographic or background variables either within each service group or in the whole sample. A Bonferroni-corrected value of p accounted for multiple comparisons (p = 0.005).

To explore differences in clinicians’ perceived self-efficacy in voice-hearing practice, stigma, and attitudes toward working with patients who hear voices (aim 1), two one-way MANOVAs were used to identify any differences in voice-hearing practice self-efficacy (first model), and in attitudes toward working with people who hear voices and stigma (second model) between clinicians working in CAMHS, EIP, Adult Mental health and Primary Care services. Post hoc tests with Bonferroni corrections were used in line with recommendations from Field (2017). ANOVAs were conducted to investigate the effect of service group on each outcome variable, using Browne-Forsythe F robust test with a Bonferroni corrected value of p = 0.007, and Hedges’ g effect size corrected for unequal sample sizes (Hedges and Olkin, 1985). Due to univariate normality tests showing distributional issues, Pillai’s trace test was selected (Field, 2017).

To explore the influence of TPB direct measures and other background factors as predictors to assess distressing voice hearing following patient disclosure within different service groups (aim 2), a hierarchical multiple regression analysis was performed. The multiple regression model was conducted separately for the adult mental health, CAMHS, EIP and primary care service groups and for the overall sample. The hierarchy of entry for predictors was as follows: first direct TPB measures of attitude, subjective norms and perceived behavioral control were entered; secondly, all remaining explanatory variables (m-AAPPQ role security, therapeutic commitment, empathy, and total AQ-9 stigma scores) were added (Figure 1); finally, dummy variables for personal experience with voice-hearing (Yes/No), specific training in working with voice-hearers (Formal training vs. No formal training but considerable clinical experience vs. No training and/or minimal experience) and professional experience of working with voice-hearers (having worked with more vs. less than 10 voice-hearers) were added. When testing the model with the total sample, dummy variables representing service group (EIP, CAMHS, primary care groups versus adult mental health clinicians being the reference category) were added in the first block to investigate whether the type of service significantly contributes to intention to assess distressing voices in patients. Effect sizes for individual predictors were calculated using Cohen’s f 2 (Cohen, 1988). To identify specific indirect TPB behavioral, normative, control beliefs that exerted the greatest influence on clinicians’ intention to assess distressing voices, the sample was dichotomized based on no/low versus. moderate/high intention (Francis et al., 2004). Two binary logistic regression models were run separately within the CAMHS and the EIP clinicians. The models were build based on the principle of parsimony, including only predictors that improved the model (Field, 2017).

Data assumptions underlying the MANOVAs (aim 1), hierarchical linear (aim 2) and logistic regression models (aim 3) were tested (Field, 2017) within each and in the overall sample. All main analyses were conducted with and without potential outlier cases. Differences in results following the removal of outliers are reported where they occurred.

To mitigate any multivariate normality deviations, confidence intervals and significance values estimation for both type of regressions were based on the Bootstrapped results (BCa 95%CI and N = 2000 samples). For Aim 2, robust regressions were also run as a sensitivity analysis to ensure regression coefficients were not biased due to any homoscedasticity issues. The missing values analysis for the variables used in all groups indicated that the highest rate of missing cases was for the stigma AQ-9 scale in all service groups, ranging from 8.9% (N = 14) in primary care clinicians who responded the survey for adult patients to 34.1% (N = 73) in CAMHS clinicians (see Supplementary Analysis Information S1, Supplementary Tables S3, S4 for further details). Missing values analysis for the additional 30 indirect TPB belief items completed by the EIP clinicians revealed differences between completers and non-completers in all control belief items (ps <. 005). Completers of the items were older and had more experience working in mental health services than non-completers (ps = or < 0.005). There were no significant differences between completers and non-completers of indirect TPB belief items in CAMHS clinicians (ps > 0.005). Pairwise deletion of cases was selected, using all available cases in each analysis. Descriptive statistics of the study variables are summarized in Supplementary Tables S5, S6.

3. Results

3.1. Aim 1: service group differences in voice-hearing practice self-efficacy, stigma, and attitudes toward working with people who hear voices

Using Pillai’s trace, there was a significant effect of type of service on clinicians’ voice-hearing practice self-efficacy, V = 0.30, F (12,4,908) = 45.18, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.10 (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. ANOVAs results for the voice-hearing practice self-efficacy scales, attitudes toward working with patients who hear voices (m-AAPPQ subscales) and stigma (AQ-9) by clinicians’ type of service.

Post hoc Games Howell tests showed that adult mental health clinicians did not significantly differ from CAMHS clinicians in self-efficacy scores in asking a patient if they hear voices and in discussing voice-hearing with a patient distressed by voices (p = 0.801 and p = 0.128 respectively). However, both adult and CAMHS clinicians had higher scores than primary care clinicians, irrespective of the target patient age group (ps < 0.001), with Hedge’s g ranging from 0.68 to 1.27. There were no differences among primary care clinicians based on the target patient age group being adult or young people (p = 0.919 and p = 0.979 respectively). Self-efficacy to provide useful information to patients with distressing voices showed similar differences between the five service groups, although CAMHS and adult mental health services clinicians seemed to be different, with adult mental health clinicians having higher scores from CAMHS clinicians (p = 0.047, g = 0.20), although this result was not robust to Bonferroni correction. Again, the scores of the primary care clinicians did not differ significantly according to target patient age (p = 0.866). EIP clinicians had significantly higher scores in all self-efficacy items compared to all other groups (p < 0.001), with effect sizes ranging from g = 0.31 to 2.17.

Pillai’s trace also indicated a significant effect of service type on clinician stigma, therapeutic commitment, role security and empathy scores, V = 0.30, F (12, 4,194) = 38.92, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.10 (Table 2).

Post hoc Games Howell tests showed that adult mental health, CAMHS, and EIP service groups did not differ significantly from each other in stigma (ps > 0.05) but reported significantly reduced stigma compared to primary care clinicians (p < 0.001, g = 0.61–0.81). Primary care practitioner stigma scores did not differ with reference to adult versus young person target patients (p = 0.999). Therapeutic commitment scores were significantly higher for EIP clinicians compared to all other service groups (ps < 0.001, g = 0.52–2.04), and higher for adult mental health compared to CAMHS clinicians (p = 0.022, g = 0. 28). Primary care clinicians scored lower on therapeutic commitment compared to all mental health clinicians (p < 0.001, g = 1.17–2.06) but did not differ depending on target patient age (p = 0.767). EIP clinicians reported greater role security scores compared to all other clinicians (ps < 0.001, g = 0.57–1.94). There was no difference in role security between CAMHS and adult clinicians (p = 0.130). The primary care service groups scored lower compared to all mental health clinicians (p < 0.001, g = 0.88–1.94), and this score did not differ depending on target patient age (p = 0.418). EIP clinicians reported greater empathy compared to all other service groups (adult mental health and primary care clinicians, p < 0.001, g = 0.033 and g = 0.80–0.95; CAMHS, p = 0.003, g = 0.40). Adult mental health clinicians reported significantly greater empathy than primary care clinicians (ps < 0.001, g = 0.41–0.53) but not compared to CAMHS clinicians (p = 0.931). Primary care clinicians scored lower on empathy compared to all mental health clinicians (p < 0.001, g = 0.41–0.95), however, when the target patient age was 12–18 years, there was no difference in reported empathy among primary care compared to CAMHS clinicians (p = 0.032).

3.2. Aim 2: Predictors of intention To assess distressing voice-hearing across different service groups

For adult mental health clinicians, intention to assess distressing voice-hearing was significantly predicted by more positive TPB attitudes toward doing so, f 2 = 0.02, and subjective norms, f 2 = 0.03, greater perceived behavioral control, f 2 = 0.03, greater therapeutic commitment, f 2 = 0.004, and reduced empathy, f 2 = 0.01. The final model was a significant fit to the data. The predictors explained 52% of the intention to assess voice-hearing, with TPB measures, namely attitudes, subjective norms and perceived behavioral control, accounting for 98.1% of that (Table 3). In CAMHS clinicians, more positive TPB subjective norms, f 2 = 0.01, lower therapeutic commitment, f 2 = 0.03, greater role security, f 2 = 0.02, and greater empathy, f 2 = 0.02, were significant predictors of intention. The final regression model significantly fitted the data. The model explained 60% of the variance in intention, of which subjective norms explained 91.7% (Table 4). In EIP clinicians (Table 5), both TPB attitudes, f 2 = 0.01, and subjective norms, f 2 = 0.02, predicted intention to assess voices, but presence of either self-reported formal training and/or considerable experience working with voice-hearers, f 2 = 0.02, had a negative relationship with intention to assess voice-hearing. Again, the final model significantly predicted clinicians’ intention, with most of the variance in clinicians’ intention explained by the TPB measures.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Linear model of predictors of TPB intention to assess distressing voice-hearing after disclosure of the experience in adult mental health clinicians (N = 966).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Linear model of predictors of TPB intention to assess distressing voice-hearing after disclosure of the experience in CAMHS clinicians (N = 214).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Linear model of predictors of TPB intention to assess distressing voice-hearing after disclosure of the experience in EIP clinicians (N = 253).

When potential outlier cases (N = 19 for adult mental health, N = 1 for CAMHS and N = 6 for EIP) were excluded, the bootstrapped regression showed similar results for the adult mental health group, whereas in the CAMHS group, perceived behavioral control became a significant predictor of intention with B(SE) = 0.28 (0.10), β = 0.30, t = 2.87, p = 0.03, BCa95% [0.02, 0.49] and in the EIP group, the TPB subjective norms no longer significantly predicted intention with B(SE) = 0.10 (0.06), β = 0.10, t = 1.62, p = 0.05, BCa95% [0, 0.36].

For the primary care clinicians, irrespective of target patient age, the significant predictors of intention to assess distressing voice-hearing were TPB attitudes, f 2 = 0.03 for adult and f 2 = 0.06 for young patients, subjective norms, f 2 = 0.03 for adult and f 2 = 0.01 for young patients, and perceived behavioral control, f 2 = 0.02 for adult and f 2 = 0.03 for young patients. The final model explained 71% of the variance in intention to assess voice-hearing in adult patients, with TPB variables accounting for 95.8, and 77% of the variance in this intention in 12–18-year-olds, with TPB variables accounting for 98.7% (Table 6, 7).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Linear model of predictors of TPB intention to assess distressing voice-hearing after disclosure of the experience in Primary care clinicians who completed the adult patient version of the survey (N = 158).

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Linear model of predictors of TPB intention to assess distressing voice-hearing after disclosure of the experience in Primary care clinicians who completed the young people version of the survey (N = 160).

When potential outlier cases (N = 2 and N = 1 for the primary care clinicians adult patients and young people target patients, respectively) were excluded, TPB perceived behavioral control was no longer a significant predictor of intention to assess voice-hearing in adult patients, although it remained at trend level; B(SE) = 0.37 (0.12), β =0.32, t = 2.97, p = 0.05, BCa95% [0.01, 0.56]. Stigma (AQ-9) became marginally a significant predictor of intention to assess voice-hearing in young people, with stigma inversely linked to intention, B(SE) = − 0.03 (0.01), β = −0.10. t = −2.15, p = 0.04, BCa95% [−0.05, −0.001].

When including all service groups (N = 1751), type of service, f-s2 < 0.02, more positive attitudes, f 2 = 0.03, greater subjective norms, f 2 = 0.03, greater perceived behavioral control, f 2 = 0.02, and greater empathy, f 2 = 0.002, significantly predicted intention to assess distressing voice-hearing (Table 8). Compared to adult mental health services, EIP and CAMHS clinicians had greater intention to assess voice-hearing, f 2 < 0.02. The final model provided significant fit to the data overall, with predictors explaining 60% of the intention to assess voice-hearing. Most of the variance in the model seemed to be explained by the TPB measures (52%).

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Linear model of predictors of TPB intention to assess distressing voice-hearing after disclosure of the experience for all participants (N = 1751).

3.3. Aim 3: the effect of TPB beliefs-based measures on intention to assess distressing voice-hearing in young people.

Clinicians were split into no or low intention versus moderate or high intention to assess distressing voice-hearing based on their mean TPB intention score, with scores ranging from 1 to 5 indicating no to low intention and 6 to 7 moderate to high intention. Based on the principle of parsimony (Field, 2017), five weighted beliefs, two behavioral, two normative and one control belief for CAMHS, and four behavioral beliefs and one normative belief for EIP clinicians, were retained in the final logistic regression models. The overall model accuracy of predicting clinicians’ intention group based on their belief scores was at 86.4% (78.9% for the null model) for CAMHS and 91.8% (84% for the null model) for EIP clinicians.

For EIP and CAMHS clinicians, the behavioral belief that assessing voice-hearing would help with constructing a detailed formulation of the young person’s presentation significantly increased the likelihood of having a moderate/high intention to assess voice-hearing. In CAMHS, a one-point increase in this belief increased the odds of having high/medium intention to assess group by 32% (Table 9) and in EIP by 84%. Similarly, the normative belief that specialist mental health professionals think that they should assess distressing voice-hearing after disclosure was associated with clinicians having moderate/high intention to assess voices in both service groups. In CAMHS, a one-point increase in this normative belief increased the likelihood of clinicians belonging in the moderate/high group by 20% (p = 0.002) (Table 9) and for EIP clinicians by 39% (p < 0.001) (Table 10). Specifically, in CAMHS, the control belief that having voice-hearing assessment tools are available in their day-to-day clinical practice was positively associated with higher likelihood of clinicians reporting moderate/high intention to assess voice-hearing in young people (p = 0.036) (Table 9). However, no significant associations were found between intention and beliefs about whether assessing voice-hearing would lead to mistakenly labeling the young person with a mental health disorder such as psychosis or whether the clinician believes the young person thinks they should assess their voice-hearing experiences, ps > 0.05. Among EIP clinicians, those who believed less intensely that assessing voice-hearing would lead to over-focusing on voices and incomplete exploration of other critical areas of a young person’s presentation were more likely to belong in the moderate/high intention group (p = 0.001) (Table 10).

TABLE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Summary of binary logistic regression examining the effect of indirect TPB weighted beliefs on TPB intention for CAMHS clinicians.

TABLE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Table 10. Summary of binary logistic regression examining the effect of indirect TPB weighted beliefs on TPB intention for EIP clinicians.

When the analysis was repeated for EIP clinicians without potential outlier cases (N = 32), an additional behavioral belief was also found to contribute significantly to the model (the belief that assessing voice-hearing would help evaluate the impact of voices on the young person’s functioning, OR = 2.25, 95% CI [0.47–10.86], Wald statistic = 1.02, p = 0.004). Re-running the analysis for CAMHS clinicians without potential outlier cases (N = 12) resulted in only one predictor belief contributing significantly to the resulting model (normative belief about specialist mental health professionals approving of their assessing voice-hearing; OR = 1.28, 95% CI [1.13–1.46], Wald statistic = 14.28, p < 0.001, Nagelkerke R2 = 0.62, Model χ2(5) = 62.41, p < 0.001). Welch’s t-test results exploring the differences in all 30 weighted beliefs for no/low versus moderate/high intention groups are presented in Supplementary Tables S7, S8.

4. Discussion

This study used a TPB framework to explore the factors that influenced the intention of clinicians to assess the distressing voice-hearing experiences of patients. More specifically, interest was focused upon the intention of clinicians working with young people who were experiencing voices. Comparisons were made between clinicians working within EIP, CAMHS and primary care. A broader comparison group was sampled from clinicians working with adults in secondary and primary care services.

EIP clinicians reported more positive attitudes (therapeutic commitment, role security, empathy) toward working with young voice-hearers, higher self-efficacy in voice-hearing practices compared to all other service groups, and similar levels of stigma toward voice-hearing youth compared to other mental health clinicians. In contrast, primary care clinicians reported the opposite results, irrespective of the patient age group. The present study supported the utility of the TPB as a framework for understanding potential influences on clinician’s intention to assess distressing voice-hearing following patient disclosure across service type and patient age groups. Although the addition of background factors, such as job attitudes toward working with voice-hearers, was found to contribute significantly to clinicians’ intention in some service groups, the importance of their contribution was negligible. Specific beliefs relating to the usefulness of assessing voice-hearing and to the social pressure coming from the approval/disapproval of other specialist mental health professionals regarding assessing voice-hearing in 12-18-year-old patients were linked with clinician intention to do so in both CAMHS and EIP clinicians.

Examination of the first aim showed that all service groups reported at least moderate levels of self-efficacy in asking patients if they hear voices and discussing voice-hearing, regardless of the patient age group. The lowest scores in self-efficacy across service groups were about providing useful voice-hearing information. Primary care clinicians had the lowest scores for both adult and young patients in self-efficacy for all voice-hearing practices. Although asking about the presence of voice-hearing or discussing the experience may have become part of mental health clinical practice in recent years (British Psychological Society, 2014), providing information potentially requires clinicians’ active engagement with the experience and access to information that could be helpful for patients. Additionally, mental health clinicians’ moderate self-reported confidence in asking about or discussing voice-hearing does not necessarily mean that they consider such conversations to be appropriate nor that they actually engage in them (Coffey et al., 2004; Coffey and Hewitt, 2008; Harrison et al., 2008; White et al., 2019). However, if clinicians’ confidence translates into asking about the presence of voice-hearing, it could be especially beneficial for the early detection of such experiences in young people who might be skeptical in disclosing them, unless they are asked directly (Mertin and Hartwig, 2004; Kelleher et al., 2014).

All secondary mental health service groups (EIP, CAMHS, adult mental health) reported similar levels of stigma in comparison to each other but these levels were lower compared to primary care clinicians, with a moderate to large effect, irrespective of the patient age group. Previous literature has shown that primary care clinicians tend to report more negative attitudes toward people with psychotic experiences compared to mental health clinicians (Hori et al., 2011; Mittal et al., 2014; Smith et al., 2017). In the present study, almost half of the primary care clinicians had no formal training in supporting voice-hearers and no or limited clinical experience with this patient group. Thus, higher levels of stigmatizing attitudes could possibly be due to having less work experience (Caplan et al., 2016; Al Saif et al., 2019) or due to having fewer positive experiences with this patient group, rather than contact more broadly. Considering that lack of training in supporting this group can be linked to lower levels of confidence in discussing voice-hearing experiences with patients (Kramarz et al., 2020), this could lead to less opportunities for positive contact experiences and building therapeutic rapport that could disconfirm negative stereotypes and reduce stigmatizing views (Couture and Penn, 2003; Jorm et al., 2012). Furthermore, most primary care clinicians in this study (about 81%) did not have personal or familial experience of hearing voices compared to about 68% in mental health professionals, which could be an additional factor for displaying higher levels of stigmatizing attitudes (Sandhu et al., 2019; Oliveira et al., 2020).

Findings on job attitudes toward working with voice-hearers showed differences among service groups, with EIP clinicians reporting the greatest therapeutic commitment, role security and empathy. Concerning working with young voice-hearers, differences between CAMHS and EIP had moderate to large effect sizes for therapeutic commitment and role security and small for empathy. Higher levels of motivation and satisfaction, feeling more adequate in their role, feeling legitimate when engaging in their clinical tasks with this patient group and relating to a greater extent with patients’ experiences, could be intuitively expected for EIP clinicians as they have more training and/or experience working with patients with voice-hearing and other psychotic experiences. All mental health clinicians had higher role security, therapeutic commitment and empathy compared to primary care clinicians, with a moderate to large effect, except for CAMHS who did not differ in empathy from primary care clinicians. The lower positive attitudes in primary care clinicians could be partly attributed to the lack of positive reinforcement when consulting with patients with mental health difficulties that could leave them with low levels of satisfaction in the care they provide (Zolnierek and Clingerman, 2012) and create doubts about their professional credibility (Harrison and Zohhadi, 2005; Brunero et al., 2018). Differences in self-reported empathy between mental health and primary care clinicians could be explained by the increased social contact of the former group with voice-hearers, which could have increased feelings of empathy and allowed for personal connections (Pettigrew and Tropp, 2006; Agrawal et al., 2016; Maranzan, 2016).

Exploring the predictors of clinicians’ intention to assess distressing voice-hearing following disclosure by patients indicated that TPB employed a well-fit model. The three TPB predictors (attitudes, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control) accounted for more than half of the variance in intention that was higher than the 39% of variance explained typically by TPB (Armitage and Conner, 2001). Mean scores indicated that, overall, clinicians reported high intention to assess voices. This finding is comparable to other studies who found mental health clinicians’ intention to discuss the meaning and content of voices moderately high (MacLeod, 2011) and that the majority attended to the content of hallucinations, despite ambivalence in attitudes toward the value of doing so (Aschebrock et al., 2003). Overall, more positive attitudes, more approving subjective norms and greater perceived behavioral control significantly predicted intention to assess distressing voice-hearing. For adult mental health clinicians and primary care clinicians, all three TPB measures were significant predictors of intention to assess voices. By comparison, in relation to young patients, attitudes and subjective norms in EIP and only subjective norms in CAMHS seemed to significantly explain part of the variance in intention.

Focusing on young people 12–18 years of age, this study found that specific beliefs might be linked to CAMHS and EIP clinicians’ intention to assess voice-hearing. These beliefs concerned: 1) that assessing voices would help with constructing a detailed formulation of the young person’s presentation; and 2) that other specialist mental health professionals (e.g., psychiatrists, psychologists) would approve and think clinicians should assess distressing voice-hearing after disclosure of the experience. Specifically, in CAMHS, having voice-hearing assessment tools (e.g., questionnaires) available was positively related with moderate to high intention to assess voice-hearing in young people. In contrast to EIP, where tools might be more easily accessible in routine clinical practice, CAMHS might not have readily available tools that would support exploration of voice-hearing and related experiences.

Previous research has demonstrated that subjective norms are strong predictors of clinicians intention (Perkins et al., 2007; Kelly et al., 2012), highlighting the increased importance that managers and colleagues can play in influencing clinician’s behaviors. Specifically in mental health studies employing TPB to explain clinicians’ intention of using evidence-based practices (e.g., cognitive behavioral therapy for psychosis), social norms and individual attitudes have been strong predictors of intention, with social norms determining whether evidence-based practice will be delivered (Burgess et al., 2017; Lecomte et al., 2018). Research on influences of psychotherapists’ current clinical practice has also emphasized the importance of other clinicians or role models and informal discussions with colleagues as key determinants of their current practice and treatment decisions (Cook et al., 2009). While in this study clinicians’ average ratings on subjective norms seemed to be slightly to moderately approving, a discouraging service culture toward discussing distressing voices could be due to several reasons including lack of confidence (e.g., Coffey and Hewitt, 2008) and practical issues (e.g., lack of staff) that might lead to prioritizing task completion rather than engaging with patients (McMullan et al., 2018; White et al., 2019; McCluskey and Vries, 2020). Specific to young people, having a working culture that deters clinicians from in-depth discussions on voice-hearing might have to do with the experience per se; voice-hearing in young people may not be commonly linked with severe mental illness and could potentially be considered as part of normal development (Maijer et al., 2019) thus dismissed.

Perceived behavioral control did not significantly predict intention to assess voices in young people, although it seemed to predict clinicians’ intention overall. According to a meta-analysis (Notani, 1998), perceived behavioral control is often a poor predictor of intention when the target behavior is relatively unfamiliar to the participants, as one might need an adequate level of actual experiences in order to truly appreciate the barriers involved in achieving the target behavior. Since assessment of distressing voices in young people might be an unfamiliar behavior for clinicians, their perceptions of control may be based on unrealistic assumptions.

4.1. Strengths and limitations

This is the first study to employ TPB to understand the influences on clinician’s intention to discuss distressing voice-hearing in young people and one of the few studies to explore staff views on that subject. Previous studies have focused mostly on mental health acute wards and smaller samples, usually of nurses (Coffey and Hewitt, 2008; McMullan et al., 2018). The study had a relatively large sample size when compared to other studies and had sufficient statistical power. Moreover, it included a range of clinicians from both primary care and secondary mental health services, from multiple regions, generating a representative UK sample of staff from healthcare.

The study did, however, have several limitations. Although asking CAMHS clinicians to answer questions about patients aged 12–18 would refer to a commonly treated age group, it is possible that EIP clinician exposure to this age group is very limited. According to the 2019–2010 National Clinical Audit of Psychosis (Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2020), patients under 18 years of age only constituted 1.8% of the caseload for UK EIP services. This might have influenced clinicians’ responses, and the reliability and validity of findings given the sample size for this service group. Rigidity of professional boundaries could be another factor to consider when interpreting clinicians’ responses. Some clinicians might not have viewed assessing voice-hearing as be part of their professional role (e.g., health care assistants, students). This is supported by our finding that 15.7% of the overall sample reported that they do not conduct patient mental health assessments as part of their current role. However, the definition of “assessment” given in the TPB questionnaire reflected an in-depth detailed conversation about distressing voice-hearing experience, and did not refer to a formal psychiatric assessment, which most clinical staff should be able to engage with as part of their role. Finally, the modified AAPPQ (McLeod et al., 2002) and the AQ-9 (Corrigan et al., 2014), were adapted to capture clinicians’ attitudes toward working with patients who hear distressing voices, and the TPB and voice-hearing practice self-efficacy questionnaires were developed for this study. Although the internal consistency of all measures was examined and deemed acceptable, further psychometric testing is needed to ensure the reliability of those measures in the context of clinicians working with voice-hearers.

4.2. Future directions

Future studies should aim to examine voice-hearing practices, rather than focusing on behavioral intention. Despite evidence that intention is a moderate predictor of clinician self-reported behavior (Godin et al., 2000; Eccles et al., 2006), other factors might mediate the relationship between behavioral intention and implementation. Perkins et al. (2007) indicated that even in cases where clinician intention is high to perform a goal-directed behavior, there might be other obstacles encountered en route to behavioral performance (e.g., habits and automatic processes, behavioral skills and cues). Additionally, studies have found different TPB components to predict behaviors depending on clinicians’ professional group membership and their specific norms (Perkins et al., 2007; Hrisos et al., 2009; Kortteisto et al., 2010). Thus, research on guiding the implementation of changes regarding clinicians’ behavior toward patients with distressing voices might be worth focusing on specific service groups. Considering the important role of subjective norms in predicting clinicians’ behavior, research on interventions aiming to increase intention to discuss distressing voices could improve understanding on the most effective forms of social influence within health services.

4.3. Implications

Considering the modest clinician confidence in providing useful information to patients with distressing voice-hearing, offering more knowledge on this experience to clinicians might increase their confidence to facilitate discussions. As a lack of material support and resources has been identified as one of the key barriers to the translation of knowledge into healthcare practice (Cochrane et al., 2007), having access to material and resources (e.g., psychoeducation leaflets, questionnaires) could support clinicians to engage in conversations about voice-hearing. Regarding young people, any information should be developmentally appropriate, and clinicians’ responsiveness should be tailored to their developmental stage to enhance engagement with this patient group (Jones et al., 2017).

The promotion of in-depth conversations between clinicians and patients about distressing voice-hearing may also benefit from changes in the work environment. Rather than intervening to alter clinicians’ job attitudes toward working with voice-hearers or specific attitudes on assessment, training professionals who set the example or are highly appreciated within a service could be beneficial. This could refer to specialist or senior mental health professionals who facilitate supervision and are responsible for team training activities. They could stimulate conversations about voice-hearing within clinical teams and promote a view that talking about voices is approved by peers and could be beneficial to patients (Coffey and Hewitt, 2008). Clinician training about continuing a discussion about voice-hearing could focus upon using an open and non-judgmental approach (Romme et al., 2009; Griffiths et al., 2019), and employing curiosity about the young person’s beliefs about their experience considering their cultural or personal frames of reference (Coughlan et al., 2022). The responses of other people to the young person in relation to voice-hearing and the young person’s perceptions of these responses should also be explored as they could influence the distress and the nature of the voices (Parry et al., 2020). Subsequent training of selected clinicians within a service could focus upon the delivery of brief and targeted therapy for voices, such as Coping Strategy Enhancement (Hayward et al., 2018) which has been evaluated as an acceptable and helpful way for young people to initiate a therapeutic conversation about their distressing voices (Hayward et al., 2022).

Acknowledging that some clinicians might find it difficult to explore the nature of young people’s voice-hearing experiences (Byrne et al., 2020), the use of structured tools in young people’s services might reduce uncertainty among clinicians and facilitate conversations (Bogen-Johnston et al., 2020). Examples of assessment measures that could stimulate and guide conversations about voice hearing experiences in young people are the Auditory Vocal Hallucination Rating Scale (Jenner and van de Willige, 2002; Bartels-Velthuis et al., 2012), Psychotic Symptom Rating Scales for Auditory Hallucinations (Haddock et al., 1999) and Hamilton Program for Schizophrenic Voices Questionnaire (Van Lieshout and Goldberg, 2007) which have all been used in youth samples (Bartels-Velthuis et al., 2016; Hayward et al., 2022; Rammou et al., 2022).

4.4. Conclusion

EIP clinicians had the lowest stigma, most positive job attitudes and highest self-efficacy in voice-hearing practices with young people, while the responses of primary care clinicians demonstrated the opposite. Clinicians’ intention to assess distressing voices in both young and adult patients after disclosure was moderately high, with the TPB variables of attitudes, subjective norms and perceived behavioral control explaining a large part of its variance. Intention toward the facilitation of in-depth discussions about voice hearing in young people were influenced by the practices of specialist mental health colleagues and by beliefs about the contribution that could be made to client formulations. Promoting a work culture that encourages discussions about voice-hearing between colleagues and with patients, as well as introducing supportive material about voices (e.g., questionnaires, psychoeducation leaflets), might have a positive impact upon encouraging discussion about voices, especially in CAMHS.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because of ethical reasons. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to the corresponding author, ar353@sussex.ac.uk.

Ethics statement

The study was sponsored by the University of Sussex, UK and received ethical approval from the Health Research Authority (Reference: 048 HAY/ IRAS ID: 257355). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

AR, CB, MH, and DF made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the study. AR was responsible for the data collection and organization. Statistical analysis was led by AR and CB and interpretation was led by AR. The manuscript was drafted by AR, with significant contributions from CB, MH, and DF. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Economic and Social Research Council (PhD studentship award number ES/J500173/1).

Acknowledgments

We thank all clinicians who participated in the study for their time and all participating NHS Trusts for their support to study recruitment.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1167869/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

TPB: Theory of Planned Behavior, CAMHS: Child and Adolescent Mental Health Services, EIP: Early Intervention in Psychosis Services, NHS: National Health Service,

References

Agrawal, S., Capponi, P., López, J., Kidd, S., Ringsted, C., Wiljer, D., et al. (2016). From surviving to advising: a novel course pairing mental health and addictions service users as advisors to senior psychiatry residents. Acad. Psychiatry 40, 475–480. doi: 10.1007/s40596-016-0533-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 50, 179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ajzen, I. (2005). Attitudes. Personality Behav. (2. ed.). Open Univ. Press.

Google Scholar

Al Saif, F., Al Shakhoori, H., Nooh, S., and Jahrami, H. (2019). Association between attitudes of stigma toward mental illness and attitudes toward adoption of evidence-based practice within health care providers in Bahrain. PLoS One 14:e0225738. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0225738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armitage, C. J., and Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: a meta-analytic review. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 471–499. doi: 10.1348/014466601164939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aschebrock, Y., Gavey, N., McCreanor, T., and Tippett, L. (2003). Is the content of delusions and hallucinations important? Australas. Psychiatry 11, 306–311. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1665.2003.00560.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, P., Coleman, R., Murray, I., and Thomas, P. (1997). From victims to allies. Nurs Times 93, 40–42.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bampton, S. (2012). Young peoples’ experiences of hearing voices: a phenomenological study of self and sense-making in first-episode psychosis -OpenGrey. Available at: http://www.opengrey.eu/item/display/10068/1004406 (Accessed January 30, 2017)

Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (2006). “Guide for constructing self-efficacy scales” in Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents. ed. T. U. F. Pajares (Greenwich: Information Age Publishing), 307–337.

Google Scholar

Bartels-Velthuis, A. A., van de Willige, G., Jenner, J. A., and Wiersma, D. (2012). Consistency and reliability of the auditory vocal hallucination rating scale (AVHRS). Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 21, 305–310. doi: 10.1017/S2045796012000108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartels-Velthuis, A. A., Wigman, J. T. W., Jenner, J. A., Bruggeman, R., and van Os, J. (2016). Course of auditory vocal hallucinations in childhood: 11-year follow-up study. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 134, 6–15. doi: 10.1111/acps.12571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, C., and Greenwood, K. (2016). The relevance of professionals’ attachment style, expectations and job attitudes for therapeutic relationships with young people who experience psychosis. Eur. Psychiatry 34, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogen-Johnston, L., de Visser, R., Strauss, C., Berry, K., and Hayward, M. (2019). “That little doorway where I could suddenly start shouting out”: barriers and enablers to the disclosure of distressing voices. J. Health Psychol. 24, 1307–1317. doi: 10.1177/1359105317745965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogen-Johnston, L., deVisser, R., Strauss, C., and Hayward, M. (2020). A qualitative study exploring how practitioners within early intervention in psychosis services engage with service users’ experiences of voice hearing? J. Psychiatr. Ment. Health Nurs. 27, 607–615. doi: 10.1111/jpm.12612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boydell, K. M., Volpe, T., Gladstone, B. M., Stasiulis, E., and Addington, J. (2013). Youth at ultra high risk for psychosis: using the revised network episode model to examine pathways to mental health care. Early Interv. Psychiatry 7, 170–186. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-7893.2012.00350.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

British Psychological Society (2014). Understanding psychosis and schizophrenia: Why people sometimes hear voices, believe things that others find strange, or appear out of touch with reality, and what can help. Leicester, United Kingdom.

Google Scholar

Brunero, S., Ramjan, L. M., Salamonson, Y., and Nicholls, D. (2018). Generalist health professional’s interactions with consumers who have a mental illness in nonmental health settings: a systematic review of the qualitative research. Int. J. Ment. Health Nurs. 27, 1634–1649. doi: 10.1111/inm.12472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgess, A. M., Chang, J., Nakamura, B. J., Izmirian, S., and Okamura, K. H. (2017). Evidence-based practice implementation within a theory of planned behavior framework. J. Behav. Health Serv. Res. 44, 647–665. doi: 10.1007/s11414-016-9523-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, R. E., Reeves, S., Bird, J. C., Jones, W., Shiers, D., Morrison, A. P., et al. (2020). Clinicians’ views of treatment types for first episode psychosis delivered in a randomised controlled trial (MAPS). EClinicalMedicine 24:100421. doi: 10.1016/j.eclinm.2020.100421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caplan, S., Little, T. V., and Garces-King, J. (2016). Stigma about mental illness among multidisciplinary health care providers in the Dominican Republic. Int. Perspect. Psychol. Res, Prac, Consult. 5, 192–206. doi: 10.1037/ipp0000057

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey, E., Gillan, D., Healy, C., Dooley, N., Campbell, D., McGrane, J., et al. (2021). Early adult mental health, functional and neuropsychological outcomes of young people who have reported psychotic experiences: a 10-year longitudinal study. Psychol. Med. 51, 1861–1869. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720000616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cartwright, A. K. J. (1980). The attitudes of helping agents toward the alcoholic client: the influence of experience, support, training, and self-esteem. Addiction 75, 413–431. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1980.tb01406.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cleary, M., Horsfall, J., Mannix, J., O’Hara-Aarons, M., and Jackson, D. (2011). Valuing teamwork: insights from newly-registered nurses working in specialist mental health services. Int. J. Ment. Health Nurs. 20, 454–459. doi: 10.1111/j.1447-0349.2011.00752.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cochrane, L. J., Olson, C. A., Murray, S., Dupuis, M., Tooman, T., and Hayes, S. (2007). Gaps between knowing and doing: understanding and assessing the barriers to optimal health care. J. Contin. Educ. Health Prof. 27, 94–102. doi: 10.1002/chp.106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coffey, M., and Hewitt, J. (2008). ‘You don’t talk about the voices’: voice hearers and community mental health nurses talk about responding to voice hearing experiences. J. Clin. Nurs. 17, 1591–1600. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2702.2007.02185.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coffey, M., Higgon, J., and And Kinnear, J. (2004). Therapy as well as the tablets: an exploratory study of service users’ views of community mental health nurses’ (CMHNs) responses to hearing voices. J. Psychiatr. Ment. Health Nurs. 11, 435–444. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2850.2004.00740.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the Behavioural science (2nd). Academic Press. United States

Google Scholar

Cook, J. M., Schnurr, P. P., Biyanova, T., and Coyne, J. C. (2009). Apples don’t fall far from the tree: influences on psychotherapists’ adoption and sustained use of new therapies. Psychiatr. Serv. 60, 671–676. doi: 10.1176/ps.2009.60.5.671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corrigan, P. W., Powell, K. J., and Michaels, P. J. (2014). Brief battery for measurement of stigmatizing versus affirming attitudes about mental illness. Psychiatry Res. 215, 466–470. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coughlan, H., Humphries, N., Clarke, M. C., Healy, C., and Cannon, M. (2022). Psychotic-like experiences? Trajectories and typologies of hallucinations and delusions from early adolescence to early adulthood in a population-based sample of Irish youth. Ir. J. Psychol. Med. 39, 207–222. doi: 10.1017/ipm.2021.31

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Couture, S. M., and Penn, D. L. (2003). Interpersonal contact and the stigma of mental illness: a review of the literature. J. Ment. Health 12, 291–305. doi: 10.1080/09638231000118276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dabby, L., Tranulis, C., and Kirmayer, L. J. (2015). Explicit and implicit attitudes of Canadian psychiatrists toward people with mental illness. Can. J. Psychiatr. 60, 451–459. doi: 10.1177/070674371506001006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccles, M. P., Hrisos, S., Francis, J., Kaner, E. F., Dickinson, H. O., Beyer, F., et al. (2006). Do self-reported intentions predict clinicians’ behaviour: a systematic review. Implement. Sci. 1, 1–10. doi: 10.1186/1748-5908-1-28

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eke, G., Holttum, S., and Hayward, M. (2012). Testing a model of research intention among U.K. clinical psychologists: a logistic regression analysis. J. Clin. Psychol. 68, 263–278. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20860

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

England, M. (2007). Accuracy of nurses’ perceptions of voice hearing and psychiatric symptoms. J. Adv. Nurs. 58, 130–139. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2006.04162.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falkenberg, I., Valmaggia, L., Byrnes, M., Frascarelli, M., Jones, C., Rocchetti, M., et al. (2015). Why are help-seeking subjects at ultra-high risk for psychosis help-seeking? Psychiatry Res. 228, 808–815. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.05.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., and Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, A. (2017). Discovering statistics using {IBM} {SPSS} statistics. 5th edition Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.

Google Scholar

Fishbein, M., and Ajzen, I. (2010). Predicting and changing behavior: The reasoned action approach. Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Francis, J., Eccles, M., Johnston, M., and Walker, A. (2004). Constructing questionnaires based on the theory of planned behaviour: a manual for health services researchers. Available at: http://openaccess.city.ac.uk/1735/1/TPB%2520Manual%2520FINAL%2520May2004.pdf (Accessed May 30, 2017).

Google Scholar

Godin, G., Naccache, H., Morel, S., and Ébacher, M. F. (2000). Determinants of nurses’ adherence to universal precautions for venipunctures. Am. J. Infect. Control 28, 359–364. doi: 10.1067/mic.2000.107594

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, R., Mansell, W., Edge, D., and Tai, S. (2019). Sources of distress in first-episode psychosis: a systematic review and qualitative Metasynthesis. Qual. Health Res. 29, 107–123. doi: 10.1177/1049732318790544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haddock, G., McCarron, J., Tarrier, N., and Faragher, E. B. (1999). Scales to measure dimensions of hallucinations and delusions: the psychotic symptom rating scales (PSYRATS). Psychol. Med. 29, 879–889. doi: 10.1017/S0033291799008661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, A., and Zohhadi, S. (2005). Professional influences on the provision of mental health care for older people within a general hospital ward. J. Psychiatr. Ment. Health Nurs. 12, 472–480. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2850.2005.00868.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, J., Newell, R., and Small, N. (2008). Do nurses’ responses cause more distress than the presence of visions and voices? Ment. Health Pract. 11, 17–19. doi: 10.7748/mhp2008.02.11.5.17.c6339

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayward, M., Edgecumbe, R., Jones, A.-M., Berry, C., and Strauss, C. (2018). Brief coping strategy enhancement for distressing voices: an evaluation in routine clinical practice. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 46, 226–237. doi: 10.1017/S1352465817000388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayward, M., Frost, H., Naito, A., and Jones, A.-M. (2022). Coping strategy enhancement for the treatment of distressing voices in young people: a service evaluation within routine clinical practice. Clin. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 27, 1209–1220. doi: 10.1177/13591045211061803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedges, L., and Olkin, I. (1985). “Statistical methods in Meta-analysis ”, Academic Press. United States.

Google Scholar

Hori, H., Richards, M., Kawamoto, Y., and Kunugi, H. (2011). Attitudes toward schizophrenia in the general population, psychiatric staff, physicians, and psychiatrists: a web-based survey in Japan. Psychiatry Res. 186, 183–189. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.08.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hrisos, S., Eccles, M. P., Francis, J. J., Dickinson, H. O., Kaner, E. F., Beyer, F., et al. (2009). Are there valid proxy measures of clinical behaviour? A systematic review. Implement. Sci. 4:37. doi: 10.1186/1748-5908-4-37

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

IBM Corp. (2017). IBM SPSS statistics for windows. Academic Press. United States

Google Scholar

Jenner, J. A., and van de Willige, G. (2002). The auditory vocal hallucination rating scale (AVHRS). University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen.

Google Scholar

Jones, S., Hassett, A., and Sclare, I. (2017). Experiences of engaging with mental health services in 16-to 18-year-olds: an interpretative phenomenological analysis. SAGE Open 7:215824401771911. doi: 10.1177/2158244017719113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jorm, A. F., Reavley, N. J., and Ross, A. M. (2012). Belief in the dangerousness of people with mental disorders: a review. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 46, 1029–1045. doi: 10.1177/0004867412442406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kehoe, M., Winton-Brown, T., Lee, S., Hopkins, L., and Pedwell, G. (2020). General practitioners’ management of young people with mental health conditions in Australia. Early Interv. Psychiatry 14, 124–129. doi: 10.1111/eip.12892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelleher, I., Devlin, N., Wigman, J. T. W., Kehoe, A., Murtagh, A., Fitzpatrick, C., et al. (2014). Psychotic experiences in a mental health clinic sample: implications for suicidality, multimorbidity and functioning. Psychol. Med. 44, 1615–1624. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, P. J., Deane, F. P., and Lovett, M. J. (2012). Using the theory of planned behavior to examine residential substance abuse workers intention to use evidence-based practices. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 26, 661–664. doi: 10.1037/a0027887

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kortteisto, T., Kaila, M., Komulainen, J., Mäntyranta, T., and Rissanen, P. (2010). Healthcare professionals’ intentions to use clinical guidelines: a survey using the theory of planned behaviour. Implement. Sci. 5:51. doi: 10.1186/1748-5908-5-51

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramarz, E., Lyles, S., Fisher, H. L., and Riches, S. (2020). Staff experience of delivering clinical care on acute psychiatric wards for service users who hear voices: a qualitative study. Psychosis 13, 58–64. doi: 10.1080/17522439.2020.1781234

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larøi, F. (2012). How do auditory verbal hallucinations in patients differ from those in non-patients? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:25. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lauber, C., Nordt, C., Braunschweig, C., and Rössler, W. (2006). Do mental health professionals stigmatize their patients? Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 113, 51–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00718.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lecomte, T., Samson, C., Naeem, F., Schachte, L., and Farhall, J. (2018). Implementing cognitive behavioral therapy for psychosis: an international survey of clinicians’ attitudes and obstacles. Psychiatr. Rehabil. J. 41, 141–148. doi: 10.1037/prj0000292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, M. A., Holttum, S., Dooley, J., and Ononaiye, M. (2016). Predictors of IAPT psychological well-being practitioners’ intention to use CBT self-help materials routinely in their clinical practice. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 9:e11. doi: 10.1017/S1754470X16000076

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindgren, M., Jonninen, M., Jokela, M., and Therman, S. (2019). Adolescent psychosis risk symptoms predicting persistent psychiatric service use: A 7 year follow-up study. Eur Psychiatry 55, 102–108. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2018.10.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLeod, D. (2011). Investigating issues influencing the decision to discuss the content and meaning of voices with people who hear voices. Available at: http://www.opengrey.eu/item/display/10068/985445 (Accessed January 30, 2017).

Google Scholar

Maijer, K., Hayward, M., Fernyhough, C., Calkins, M. E., Debbané, M., Jardri, R., et al. (2019). Hallucinations in children and adolescents: an updated review and practical recommendations for clinicians. Schizophr. Bull. 45, S5–S23. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sby119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maijer, K., Palmen, S. J. M. C., and Sommer, I. E. C. (2017). Children seeking help for auditory verbal hallucinations; who are they? Schizophr. Res. 183, 31–35. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.10.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maranzan, K. A. (2016). Interprofessional education in mental health: an opportunity to reduce mental illness stigma. J. Interprof. Care 30, 370–377. doi: 10.3109/13561820.2016.1146878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCluskey, A., and Vries, J. (2020). Help, I can’t help: a qualitative study of psychiatric nurse’s perspectives on their care for people who hear voices. J. Psychiatr. Ment. Health Nurs. 28, 138–148. doi: 10.1111/jpm.12642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLeod, H. J., Deane, F. P., and Hogbin, B. (2002). Changing staff attitudes and empathy for working with people with psychosis. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 30, 459–470. doi: 10.1017/S1352465802004071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McMullan, E., Gupta, A., and Collins, S. C. (2018). Experiences of mental health nursing staff working with voice hearers in an acute setting: an interpretive phenomenological approach. J. Psychiatr. Ment. Health Nurs. 25, 157–166. doi: 10.1111/jpm.12448

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mertin, P., and Hartwig, S. (2004). Auditory hallucinations in nonpsychotic children: diagnostic considerations. Child Adolesc. Mental Health 9, 9–14. doi: 10.1046/j.1475-357X.2003.00070.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mittal, D., Corrigan, P., Sherman, M. D., Chekuri, L., Han, X., Reaves, C., et al. (2014). Healthcare providers’ attitudes toward persons with schizophrenia. Psychiatr. Rehabil. J. 37, 297–303. doi: 10.1037/prj0000095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Notani, A. S. (1998). Moderators of perceived behavioral control’s predictiveness in the theory of planned behavior: a meta-analysis. J. Consum. Psychol. 7, 247–271. doi: 10.1207/s15327663jcp0703_02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliveira, A. M., Machado, D., Fonseca, J. B., Palha, F., Silva Moreira, P., Sousa, N., et al. (2020). Stigmatizing attitudes toward patients with psychiatric disorders among medical students and professionals. Front. Psych. 11:326. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parry, S., Loren, E., and Varese, F. (2020). Young People’s narratives of hearing voices: systemic influences and conceptual challenges. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 28, 715–726. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2532

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perkins, M. B., Jensen, P. S., Jaccard, J., Gollwitzer, P., Oettingen, G., Pappadopulos, E., et al. (2007). Applying theory-driven approaches to understanding and modifying clinicians’ behavior: what do we know? Psychiatr. Serv. 58, 342–348. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.58.3.342

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pettigrew, T. F., and Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 90, 751–783. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rammou, A., Berry, C., Fowler, D., and Hayward, M. (2022). Distress factors of voice-hearing in young people and social relating: exploring a cognitive-interpersonal voice-hearing model. Psychol. Psychother. Theory Res. Pract. 95, 939–957. doi: 10.1111/papt.12411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, J. H., Crosland, A., and Fulton, J. (2013). “I think this is maybe our Achilles heel⋯” exploring GPs’ responses to young people presenting with emotional distress in general practice: a qualitative study. BMJ Open 3:e002927. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-002927

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romme, M. A. J., Escher, S., Dillon, J., Corstens, D., and Morris, M. (2009). Living with voices: 50 stories of recovery. PCCS Books in association with Birmingham City University.United Kingdom

Google Scholar

Royal College of Psychiatrists (2020). National Clinical Audit of psychosis –National Report for the early intervention in psychosis audit 2019/2020. London: Healthcare Quality Improvement Partnership.

Google Scholar

Sandhu, H. S., Arora, A., Brasch, J., and Streiner, D. L. (2019). Mental health stigma: explicit and implicit attitudes of Canadian undergraduate students, medical school students, and psychiatrists. Can. J. Psychiatr. 64, 209–217. doi: 10.1177/0706743718792193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sikich, L. (2013). Diagnosis and evaluation of hallucinations and other psychotic symptoms in children and adolescents. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 22, 655–673. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2013.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. D., Mittal, D., Chekuri, L., Han, X., and Sullivan, G. (2017). A comparison of provider attitudes toward serious mental illness across different health care disciplines. Stigma Health 2, 327–337. doi: 10.1037/sah0000064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stowkowy, J., Colijn, M. A., and Addington, J. (2013). Pathways to care for those at clinical high risk of developing psychosis. Early Interv. Psychiatry 7, 80–83. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-7893.2012.00368.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Lieshout, R. J., and Goldberg, J. O. (2007). Quantifying self-reports of auditory verbal hallucinations in persons with psychosis. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science / Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement 39, 73–77. doi: 10.1037/cjbs2007006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watkins, S., Gupta, A., and Sanderson, C. (2020). The experience of talking about hearing voices with family, friends, and others. Psychosis 12, 1–10. doi: 10.1080/17522439.2019.1690561

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, M. R., Stein-Parbury, J., Orr, F., Dawson, A., Stein-Parbury, J., Orr, F., et al. (2019). Working with consumers who hear voices: the experience of early career nurses in mental health services in Australia. Int. J. Ment. Health Nurs. 28, 605–615. doi: 10.1111/inm.12566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, D., Lavoie-Tremblay, M., Drevniok, U., Racine, H., and Savignac, H. (2011). Relational dimensions of a positive integration experience for new mental health nurses. Arch. Psychiatr. Nurs. 25, 164–173. doi: 10.1016/j.apnu.2010.07.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, L. H., Link, B. G., Ben-David, S., Gill, K. E., Girgis, R. R., Brucato, G., et al. (2015). Stigma related to labels and symptoms in individuals at clinical high-risk for psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 168, 9–15. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zolnierek, C. D., and Clingerman, E. M. (2012). A medical-surgical Nurse’s perceptions of caring for a person with severe mental illness. J. Am. Psychiatr. Nurses Assoc. 18, 226–235. doi: 10.1177/1078390312446223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: clinician attitudes, early intervention in mental health, psychotic experiences, voice-hearing, youth mental health, theory of planned behavior, auditory verbal hallucinations

Citation: Rammou A, Berry C, Fowler D and Hayward M (2023) “Attitudes to voices”: a survey exploring the factors influencing clinicians’ intention to assess distressing voices and attitudes towards working with young people who hear voices. Front. Psychol. 14:1167869. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1167869

Received: 17 February 2023; Accepted: 28 April 2023;
Published: 23 May 2023.

Edited by:

Eleni Petkari, International University of La Rioja, Spain

Reviewed by:

Fuyin Yang, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, China
Sarah Parry, Lancaster University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2023 Rammou, Berry, Fowler and Hayward. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Aikaterini Rammou, a.rammou@sussex.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.