Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 05 July 2023
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic Highlights in Psychology: Cognitive Bias View all 14 articles

Cognitive biases in first-episode psychosis with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

Vanessa Sanchez-Gistau,,,
Vanessa Sanchez-Gistau1,2,3,4*Angel Cabezas,,,Angel Cabezas1,2,3,4Nuria Manzanares,Nuria Manzanares1,4Montse Sole,,,Montse Sole1,2,3,4Lia Corral,,,Lia Corral1,2,3,4Elisabet Vilella,,,Elisabet Vilella1,2,3,4Alfonso Gutierrez-Zotes,,,Alfonso Gutierrez-Zotes1,2,3,4
  • 1Hospital Universitari Institut Pere Mata of Reus, Reus, Spain
  • 2Institut d’Investigació Sanitària Pere Virgili (IISPV- CERCA), Tarragona, Spain
  • 3Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), ISCIII, Madrid, Spain
  • 4Universitat Rovira i Virgili (URV), Reus, Spain

Introduction: Psychotic disorders such schizophrenia and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are neurodevelopmental disorders with social cognitive deficits. Specifically, biased interpretation of social information can result in interpersonal difficulties. Cognitive biases are prevalent in psychosis, but no previous study has investigated whether the type and severity of cognitive biases differ between subjects experiencing first-episode psychosis (FEP) with (FEP-ADHD+) and without ADHD (FEP-ADHD).

Methods: A total of 121 FEP outpatients at the Early Intervention Service of Reus were screened for childhood ADHD through the Diagnostic Interview for ADHD (DIVA). Cognitive biases were assessed by the Cognitive Biases Questionnaire for Psychosis (CBQp). CBQp scores of FEPs groups were compared with those of healthy controls (HCs) with an analysis of covariance. Spearman correlation analysis explored associations between CBQp scores and psychopathology.

Results: Thirty-one FEPs met the criteria for childhood ADHD and reported significantly more cognitive bias [median (interquartile range): 47 (38–56)] than FEP-ADHD [42 (37–48)] and HCs [38 (35.5–43)]. CBQp scores did not differ between FEP-ADHD-and HCs when adjusted for age and sex. After controlling for clinical differences, Intentionalising (F = 20.97; p < 0.001) and Emotional Reasoning biases (F = 4.17; p = 0.04) were more strongly associated with FEP-ADHD+ than FEP-ADHD. Cognitive biases were significantly correlated with positive psychotic symptoms in both groups but only with depressive symptoms in FEP-ADHD (r = 0.258; p = 0.03) and with poor functioning in FEP-ADHD+ (r = −0.504; p = 0.003).

Conclusion: Cognitive bias severity increased from HCs to FEP-ADHD-patients to FEP-ADHD+ patients. FEP-ADHD+ patients may be a particularly vulnerable group in which metacognitive targeted interventions are needed.

Introduction

The cognitive model of psychosis suggests that psychotic symptoms may arise because of biased information processing (Garety et al., 2007). In accordance with this model, people with sub threshold (Livet et al., 2020) and full psychotic symptoms (Freeman et al., 2001) are more prone to cognitive biases. Cognitive biases refer to automatic errors in both cognitive processing and content across specific situations (Beck, 1963). A substantial body of research has demonstrated that cognitive biases contribute to the processes of reasoning and metacognition (Garety et al., 2001; Freeman, 2007; Morrison et al., 2007; Bob et al., 2016). From this perspective, the development and maintenance of delusions may be due to the presence of dysfunctional patterns of thought that leads to incorrect judgments and abnormal interpretations or perceptions. The cognitive biases in psychosis that have been most extensively studied are jumping to conclusions (JTC) (Ross et al., 2015; Dudley et al., 2016; McLean et al., 2017) attributional biases (Langdon et al., 2010; Sanford and Woodward, 2017), and belief inflexibility (Moritz and Woodward, 2006). However, it is evident that subjects with psychotic disorder present varied cognitive biases (De Rossi and Georgiades, 2022) including Beck’s emotional biases (Beck, 1963). Thus, the emotional biases of catastrophising (C) and dichotomous thinking (DT) have also been involved in psychoses (Gawęda and Prochwicz, 2015). Peters E and colleagues developed the Cognitive Biases Questionnaire for Psychosis (CBQp) (Peters et al., 2014) to easily and comfortably assess cognitive biases in psychosis. The CBQp was based on the Blackburn Cognitive Styles Test (Blackburn et al., 1986) which was designed to assess frequent cognitive biases in depression. The cognitive biases included in the CBQp are jumping to conclusions (JTC), dichotomous thinking (DT), intentionalising (Int), emotional reasoning (ER), and catastrophizing (C).

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder affecting 3–7% of school-age children (Polanczyk et al., 2015). It is characterized by motor hyperactivity and impulsiveness and inattention or distractibility that produces functioning problems in the family and school environments’ and in the relationship with peers; frequently, these difficulties persist in adulthood (Barnett, 2016). In addition to impairments in cognitive function, deficits in social cognition and interpersonal difficulties are also important features of ADHD. Within social cognition, deficits in theory of mind and emotion recognition and processing are the domains that have been most investigated; however, findings have been ambiguous (Morellini et al., 2022). Therefore, research on cognitive distortions in ADHD remains scarce, with some studies focusing on attentional and attribution bias (Hartmann et al., 2020; Jenness et al., 2021).

In addition to the genetic overlap between some risk alleles (Hamshere et al., 2013), psychotic disorders and ADHD share some clinical manifestations. Males are overrepresented, both have a high comorbidity with substance abuse, and both manifest difficulties in emotional regulation and peer relationships. Deficits in cognition are central symptoms of neurodevelopmental disorders and have been associated with poor functional outcomes and poor response to treatment. A previous report by our group (Sanchez-Gistau et al., 2020) compared cognitive performance between patients in their first episode of psychosis (FEP) with and without childhood ADHD (c-ADHD) and healthy controls (HCs); we found a gradient in the severity of cognitive impairment, with FEP patients with ADHD (FEP-ADHD+ patients) being the most impaired. Compared to FEP-ADHD, FEP-ADHD+ were more frequently men, showed a worse antipsychotic response and had a higher risk of drug consumption. The present study builds on this previous study by aiming to determine whether the type and severity of different cognitive biases (measured with the CBQp) differ between FEP patients with and without c-ADHD relative to a control group. In addition, we aimed to investigate the relationship between cognitive bias and psychopathological symptoms in both FEP-ADHD+ and FEP-ADHD-patients.

Methods

Participants

We invited all consecutive outpatients referred to the Early Intervention Programme (EIP) at the University Hospital Institut Pere Mata of Reus, Spain, from January 2015 to July 2019 fulfilling the following inclusion criteria: age between 14 and 35 years; FEP, defined as the “onset of full psychotic symptoms within the last 12 months”; and less than 6 months of antipsychotic treatment. The exclusion criteria were as follows: psychosis induced by substances or other medical conditions, intellectual disability, severe head injury or a lack of fluency in Spanish. During the target period, 152 FEP subjects were referred to the EIP. Six subjects refused to participate and 15 did not fulfil the inclusion criteria. Of the 133 FEP subjects included in our previous study, 11 did not complete the CBQp. Therefore, the final sample consisted of 122 FEP subjects, 31 FEP-ADHD+ subjects and 91 FEP-ADHDsubjects. The sample of HCs (N = 26) was drawn from our previous validation study of the CBQp in the Spanish language (Corral et al., 2020).

Ethical approval was obtained by the Committee for Ethical Clinical and Pharmacological Investigation of the Pere Virgili Research Institute (IISPV). After a complete description of the study was given to the subjects, written informed consent was obtained.

Assessments

Clinical assessments

Clinical assessments were administered by two experienced psychiatrists of the team. Clinical variables related to psychosis, such as the duration of untreated psychosis, current pharmacological treatment, and frequency of drug use in the past 6 months, were assessed through a direct interview. The dose of each antipsychotic was converted to chlorpromazine (CPMZ) equivalents in mg/day (Gardner et al., 2010). We defined as drug users those individuals who used a specific drug “at least several times a week.” The severity of psychotic symptoms was assessed using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) (Kay et al., 1990) and the severity of affective symptoms by the Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia (CDSS) (Addington et al., 1993) and the Young Mania Rating scale (YMRS) (Young et al., 1978). Finally the level of functioning was assessed by the Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) (American Psychiatric Association, 1994).

The Spanish version of the Structured Clinical Interview for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV) for Axis I disorders (SCID-I) (First et al., 1997) confirmed the diagnosis of psychosis following DSM criteria. For descriptive purposes we grouped the diagnoses of schizophrenia, schizophreniform and schizoaffective disorders as “schizophrenia spectrum disorders”; manic and depressive episodes with psychotic symptoms as “affective psychoses” and brief psychotic disorders and psychosis not otherwise specified were categorized as “other psychosis.”

Assessment of ADHD

A child and adolescent psychiatrist blind to clinical assessments administered the Spanish version of the Diagnostic Interview for ADHD in Adults (DIVA) (Ramos-Quiroga et al., 2019). Additional information was provided by a parent or close relative. The DIVA is the gold-standard assessment for ADHD in adults assessing the severity of each of the 18 symptoms required to meet the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for ADHD in both childhood and adulthood. Those symptoms must cause impairment in at least two settings and must not be better explained by another psychiatric disorder. The diagnosis is considered definite when six or more criteria are met for each of the symptom domains of hyperactivity-impulsivity and/or attention deficit. We specifically asked about childhood-onset ADHD (c-ADHD) in order to avoid confusion with recent prodromal or current full psychotic symptoms. Adult-onset symptoms were therefore not considered for ADHD diagnoses. FEP subjects fulfilling the criteria for a definite diagnosis of c-ADHD were categorized as FEP-ADHD+ otherwise; they were categorized as FEP-ADHD.

Assessment of cognitive biases

The CBQp (Peters et al., 2014) has been recently translated and validated for the Spanish population by our group (Corral et al., 2020). It is a self-report questionnaire containing 30 scenarios, 15 involving the theme of Anomalous Perception (AP) and 15 involving the theme of Threatening Events (TE); for each vignette, the subject is asked to choose one of three options that best describe that situation. Six vignettes are included for each of the five cognitive biases: Intentionalising (Int), Catastrophising (C), Dichotomous thinking (DT), Jumping to conclusions (JTC) and Emotional reasoning (ER). The score of each cognitive bias subscale ranges from 5 to 18, and the CBQp total score ranges from 30 to 90 points.

Statistical analyses

Demographic data were compared among the FEP-ADHD+, FEP-ADHD-and HC groups using the chi-squared test with Yates’ correction or Fisher’s exact test for discrete variables and one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by post hoc Tukey pairwise comparisons. Differences in clinical variables between the two FEP groups were explored by the chi-squared test with Yates’ correction or Fisher’s exact test for discrete variables and Student’s t test or the Mann–Whitney U test for continuous variables.

First, differences in cognitive bias (total scores, theme scores and bias scores) between the three groups were analysed by between-subject univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by post hoc Tukey pairwise comparisons. Significant group differences (at p < 0.05) were controlled by age and sex by subsequent univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA).

Second, differences in cognitive bias scores between clinical groups were analysed while controlling for the effects of clinical variables on which FEP-ADHD+ and FEP-ADHD-groups differed at p < 0.10. Finally, Spearman correlation analyses were used to separately explore associations between the CBQp total score, TE and SA theme scores, cognitive bias scores and psychopathological symptoms in each clinical group.

All analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS for Windows, version 20.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY).

Results

Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics

Thirty-one FEP subjects (25.4%) fulfilled the criteria for c-ADHD: 35.4% as the inattentive subtype, 25.8% as the hyperactive–impulsive subtype, and 38.8% as the combined subtype. Ten out of 31 ADHD subjects (32.25%) had been previously diagnosed with c-ADHD in a child and adolescent mental health unit, but only three were taking treatment for ADHD: one was on methylphenidate, one on guanfacine and one on atomoxetine.

As it can be seen in Table 1, the three groups significantly differed in age and sex. Both clinical groups were significantly younger than the HC group, and males were overrepresented in the FEP-ADHD+ group (90.3%). The severity of clinical variables did not differ between the FEP groups, but FEP-ADHD+ subjects used tobacco and cannabis more frequently and were treated with a higher dose and a greater number of antipsychotics than FEP-ADHDsubjects.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Socio-demographic and clinical characteristics of the groups.

Differences in cognitive biases

As shown in Table 2, the ANOVA revealed group differences in all cognitive biases except for JTC. Post hoc pairwise comparisons showed that FEP-ADHD+ patients scored significantly higher than HCs on all scores. After adjusting for age and sex, these differences remained significant except for the C bias. FEP-ADHD-patients exhibited significantly higher scores than HCs on the CBQp total score, the TE theme and the ER bias; however, after adjusting for age and sex, these differences were no longer significant.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Comparison of cognitive biases between the groups.

The two clinical groups were further directly compared after adjusting for sociodemographic and clinical differences at a threshold of p < 0.10, that is, after adjusting for sex, years of education, tobacco and cannabis use and antipsychotic dose (See Table 3). Compared to FEP-ADHD-patients, FEP-ADHD+ patients scored significantly higher on the CBQp total score (F = 7.11; p = 0.009), TE (F = 4.10; p = 0.04) and AP (F = 8.94; p = 0.003) themes and Int (F = 20.97; p < 0.001) and RE (F = 4.17; p = 0.04) biases.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparison of CBQ between FEP ADHD+ and FEP-ADHD-.

Correlation between psychopathological symptoms and cognitive biases

In the correlation analyses (Table 4), the CBQp total score was significantly correlated with the PANSS positive symptoms subscale score (PANSS-P) in both clinical groups and was correlated with the PANSS general symptoms subscale score (PANSS-G) and CDSS score in only the FEP-ADHD group. In the FEP-ADHD+ group, the CBQp total score was also correlated with lower GAF scores. Regarding the TE and AP themes, TE was correlated with the PANSS-P scores in both clinical groups and with the PANSS-G and CDSS scores in the FEP-ADHD group. The AP theme score was associated with PANSS-P and PANSS-G scores in only the FEP-ADHD group.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Association of cognitive biases with psychopathological symptoms and daily functioning in the clinical groups.

Regarding cognitive biases, Int was positively correlated with the PANSS-P score in FEP-ADHD-patients and with worse GAF scores in FEP-ADHD+ patients. Positive correlations were found between the C and DT bias scores and the PANSS-P score in both clinical groups and between the CDSS score in the FEP-ADHD group. The DT bias score also correlated with the PANSS-G score in the FEP-ADHD group. JTC correlated with PANSS-P score in FEP-ADHD+ patients and with general PANSS-G scores, and CDSS score in FEP-ADHD-patients. Finally, the ER bias score was associated with positive PANSS-P and PANSS-G scores in only in the FEP-ADHD group.

Discussion

As far as we know, this is the first study to assess and compare the severity of cognitive bias (measured by the CBQp) between FEP-ADHD+ and FEP-ADHD subjects relative to HCs. Our results therefore must be considered preliminary and caution is required when interpreting the subsequent findings.

We found a gradient of cognitive bias severity from the FEP-ADHD+ group: [median (interquartile range) 47 (38–56)], to the FEP-ADHD group [42 (37–48)] to the HC group [38 (35.5–43)]. The FEP-ADHD+ group presented significantly higher scores not only on the total score but also in the CBQp themes of TE and AP than both the FEP-ADHD-and HC groups. Regarding specific cognitive biases, FEP-ADHD+ patients exhibited greater Int, DT, C and ER bias scores than HCs, while FEP-ADHD-patients presented only greater ER bias scores than HCs. The FEP-ADHD+ group therefore showed the most marked differences from HCs. However, the HC group used as a control group was not specifically recruited for the present study but was used in our previous study to validate the questionnaire in the Spanish population (Corral et al., 2020). Consequently, groups differed in terms of age and sex ratios. When the results were further adjusted for these differences, differences between FEP-ADHD-patients and HCs were no longer significant. In contrast, FEP-ADHD+ patients still significantly scored higher than HCs, except for the C bias score. Unexpectedly, the three groups did not significantly differ in the JTC bias score. JTC is the most investigated cognitive bias in psychosis, and when assessed by the probabilistic behavior task (the beads task) (Garety et al., 2005), JTC bias has been frequently observed in patients with established schizophrenia (Ross et al., 2015; Dudley et al., 2016; McLean et al., 2017) and FEP (Falcone et al., 2015) as well as in those at clinical risk of psychosis (Livet et al., 2020). However, when assessed by self-report questionnaires, mixed results have been reported (Bastiaens et al., 2013; Ahuir et al., 2021; Pena-Garijo et al., 2022; Pugliese et al., 2022). One possible explanation is that patients with psychosis often have a little awareness of their cognitive deficits and biases (Moritz et al., 2004). Therefore, a dissociation between the objective (assessed by task performance) and subjective measures (assessed by self-report) cannot be ruled out. In addition, Beck’s cognitive biases have an emotional component rather than a psychotic cognitive–perceptual component, which may explain why they are also present in the healthy population (Bastiaens et al., 2018). Unfortunately, depressive and anxiety symptoms were not evaluated in the HC group. Thus, it would have been useful in order to identify a potential relationship between the emotional component and the cognitive distortions in that group.

A novel finding of the present study is that FEP-ADHD+ patients showed more severe cognitive biases than FEP-ADHD-patients, even after controlling for clinical and sociodemographic differences. Apart from scoring higher on the two themes (AP and TE), it is particularly interesting that the FEP-ADHD+ group exhibited more DT and significantly more Int and ER than the FEP-ADHD group. Consistent with these findings, in their original validation report comparing subjects with psychosis with HCs and depressed subjects, Peters and colleagues (Peters et al., 2014) reported that the psychosis group scored higher than the depressed group on the Int and ER biases. Moreover, Int was the only bias where the depressed group and HCs did not score differently. The authors suggested that these two specific biases may represent a particular “paranoid thinking style” that distinguishes individuals with psychosis from other clinical populations. In accordance, we found that FEP-ADHD+ patients were more likely to exhibit the Int bias (F = 20.97; p < 0.001). The Int bias refers to the implicit and automatic inclination to interpret human actions as intentional and to think that negative actions toward oneself were committed on purpose (i.e., intentionally). In the ADHD literature, research on interpretation bias is very scarce. There is inconsistent evidence that hostile attribution bias (HAB) with ambiguous situations and ambiguous faces occurs more frequently in children and adults with ADHD than in HCs (King et al., 2009; Sibley et al., 2010; Schneidt et al., 2019). No previous study to date has addressed this issue in FEP patients with and without ADHD; thus, further investigation is needed to replicate or refute our results and disentangle whether FEP patients with c-ADHD exhibit greater cognitive bias than FEP patients without c-ADHD.

Notably, when exploring the relationship between cognitive biases and psychopathological symptoms, only weak correlations were found. Regarding specific biases, the severity of positive symptoms was associated with the C and DT biases in both clinical groups; however, the severity of positive symptoms was associated with the JTC bias in the FEP-ADHD+ group and with the Int and ER biases in the FEP-ADHD group. Nevertheless, positive correlations between cognitive biases and depressive symptoms and general symptoms were found only in FEP-ADHD-patients. Although neither clinical group differed in psychopathological symptoms or functioning, our study suggests a different pattern of biases related to positive, general and depressive symptoms in FEP-ADHD+ patients compared to FEP-ADHD-patients. Moreover, poor functioning was associated with the CBQp total score and the Int bias in only the FEP-ADHD+ group. Given the lack of previous studies, we can only speculate that the dominant cognitive biases in FEP-ADHD-patients may be related to depressive and anxious symptoms. On the other hand, the severity, type of cognitive biases and the lack of relationship with depressive and general symptoms in the FEP-ADHD+ group may reflect traditional psychotic thinking, which in turn might be associated with worse functioning. Moreover, children with ADHD present deficits in recognizing facial emotions and others’ emotional states in addition to deficits in emotional processing (Pishyareh et al., 2015). Thus, we speculate that greater cognitive biases in the FEP-ADHD+ group may interact with the social processing difficulties already present in ADHD to pose a higher risk of impaired functioning.

Our results extend previous findings suggesting that young adults with c-ADHD and FEP suffer additional impairments (Peralta et al., 2011; Rho et al., 2015; Sanchez-Gistau et al., 2020). Specifically, we report for the first time greater cognitive biases (in general) and more severe Int and ER biases (in particular) in FEP-ADHD+ patients. Together, these findings indicate that FEP-ADHD+ subjects may be a particularly vulnerable group and a high-priority target for interventions addressing both cognitive biases. Metacognitive training therapy (MCT) was developed by Moritz S and colleagues two decades ago to address problems related to cognitive biases and social cognition in psychosis (Moritz and Woodward, 2007). Previous research has indicated that MCT is an effective psychological intervention for people with schizophrenia (Moritz and Lysaker, 2018; Moritz et al., 2022). Specifically, in patients with recent-onset psychosis, MCT has demonstrated to be effective for improving psychotic symptoms, cognitive insight, and attributional style, as well as for reducing cognitive distortions (Ochoa et al., 2017; Ahuir et al., 2018). Our findings therefore may have clinical relevance for treatment recommendations. As reported, ADHD is a prevalent condition in FEP accompanied by prominent cognitive bias; thus, an adapted intervention for this subgroup aiming to reduce the most prevalent bias can be recommended. Our findings indicate the necessity of conducting metacognitive intervention studies specifically designed to assess the effectiveness of these particular interventions in this particular subgroup of patients.

Limitations and strengths

Some limitations must be taken into account when interpreting our finding. With regards the ADHD diagnoses, despite we used an structured interview for assessing ADHD symptoms recall bias regarding childhood onset symptoms cannot be entirely ruled out. We tried to avoid the possibility of overlapping ADHD symptoms with psychotic symptoms by restricting the diagnoses of ADHD to childhood-onset, that is, onset of symptoms before the age of 7 years according to the DSM-IV criteria. Moreover, the healthy control group HCs who participated in the previous validation study was not specifically assessed for ADHD and differed in terms of age and sex distribution. Second, despite controlling for clinical and socio-demographic differences, the scores of CBQp might have been influenced by other variables that were not adjusted for, such as variables related to stress and childhood trauma. It has also to be acknowledged that sample size limited our ability to conduct secondary analyses stratified by ADHD subtype or by psychotic diagnoses. Moreover, the low percentage of females prevented us to investigate sex differences in the studied variables and ADHD.

However, despite these limitations, we have included a real-world clinical practice sample in their early stages of the illness coming from a particular geographical area. Our relatively homogeneous sample, allows us to minimize the impact of the burden of a chronic disease and long-term antipsychotic treatment. Finally cross-sectional assessment did not allow us to infer a causal relationship between cognitive bias and ADHD in FEP patients.

In summary, we report a gradient of severity in CBQp scores among the three groups, with the FEP-ADHD+ group differing the most markedly from the FEP-ADHD-and HC groups. The severity of cognitive biases, however, did not differ between the FEP-ADHD-and HC groups after adjusting for age and sex. Importantly, the Int and ER biases were the most strongly associated with the FEP-ADHD+ group, but no bias was associated with the FEP-ADHD group.

Conclusion

Our present findings together with previous findings indicate that FEP-ADHD+ subjects represent a clinical subgroup with a worse potential prognosis than FEP-ADHD subjects. Further research on the relationships among cognitive biases, cognitive performance and environmental factors are needed to develop individualized pharmacological and psychological interventions, such as MCT, for FEP subpopulations. The relationship between ADHD and psychosis is still an important knowledge gap that requires further investigation.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon a reasonable request.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Committee for Ethical Clinical and Pharmacological Investigation of the Pere Virgili Research Institute (CEIM of IISPV). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

VS-G designed the current study. AC, MS, VS-G, and LC contributed to the acquisition of clinical data. NM was blinded to the results of the clinical assessments and performed the ADHD interviews. VS-G, AG-Z, and EV undertook the statistical analysis and all authors took part in the interpretation of the results. VS-G wrote the first draft of the article. All authors critically revised the first draft and provided their contributions and have approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the AGAUR Agència de Gestió d’Ajuts Universitaris i de Recerca (SGR2017_444).

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the participants and families who gave freely of their time to take part in the research. We would also like to thank Encarna Peñas for its assistance in participant recruitment.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Addington, D., Addington, J., and Maticka-Tyndale, E. (1993). Assessing depression in schizophrenia: the calgary depression scale. Br. J. Psychiatry Suppl. 22, 39–44.

Google Scholar

Ahuir, M., Cabezas, Á., Miñano, M. J., Algora, M. J., Estrada, F., Solé, M., et al. (2018). Improvement in cognitive biases after group psychoeducation and metacognitive training in recent-onset psychosis: a randomized crossover clinical trial. Psychiatry Res. 270, 720–723. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.10.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahuir, M., Crosas, J. M., Estrada, F., Zabala, W., Pérez-Muñoz, S., González-Fernández, A., et al. (2021). Cognitive biases are associated with clinical and functional variables in psychosis: a comparison across schizophrenia, early psychosis and healthy individuals. Revista de Psiquiatria y Salud Mental 14, 4–15. doi: 10.1016/j.rpsm.2020.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association . DSM-IV. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington: APA, (1994).

Google Scholar

Barnett, R. (2016). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet 387:737. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(16)00332-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastiaens, T., Claes, L., Smits, D., De Wachter, D., van der Gaag, M., and De Hert, M. (2013). The cognitive biases questionnaire for psychosis (CBQ-P) and the Davos assessment of cognitive biases (DACOBS): validation in a Flemish sample of psychotic patients and healthy controls. Schizophr. Res. 147, 310–314. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.04.037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastiaens, T., Claes, L., Smits, D., Vanwalleghem, D., and De Hert, M. (2018). Self-reported cognitive biases are equally present in patients diagnosed with psychotic versus nonpsychotic disorders. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 206, 122–129. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T. (1963). Thinking and depression I- idiosyncratic content and cognitive distortions. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 9, 324–333. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1963.01720160014002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blackburn, I. M., Jones, S., and Lewin, R. J. (1986). Cognitive style in depression. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 25, 241–251. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1986.tb00704.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bob, P., Pec, O., Mishara, A. L., Touskova, T., and Lysaker, P. H. (2016). Conscious brain, metacognition and schizophrenia. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 105, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corral, L., Labad, J., Ochoa, S., Cabezas, A., Muntané, G., Valero, J., et al. (2020). Cognitive biases questionnaire for psychosis (CBQp): Spanish validation and relationship with cognitive insight in psychotic patients. Front. Psych. 11:596625. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.596625Ç

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Rossi, G., and Georgiades, A. (2022). Thinking biases and their role in persecutory delusions: a systematic review. Early Interv. Psychiatry 16, 1278–1296. doi: 10.1111/eip.13292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dudley, R., Taylor, P., Wickham, S., and Hutton, P. (2016). Psychosis, delusions and the “jumping to conclusions” reasoning Bias: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Schizophr. Bull. 42, 652–665. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falcone, M. A., Murray, R. M., Wiffen, B. D. R., O’Connor, J. A., Russo, M., Kolliakou, A., et al. (2015). Jumping to conclusions, neuropsychological functioning, and delusional beliefs in first episode psychosis. Schizophr. Bull. 41, 411–418. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

First, M.B., Spitzer, R., and Gibbon, M. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press Inc;(1997). SCID-I. Entrevista clínica estructurada para los trastornos del eje I. del DSM-IV. Masson, Barcelona 1999

Google Scholar

Freeman, D. (2007). Suspicious minds: the psychology of persecutory delusions. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 27, 425–457. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, D., Garety, P. A., and Kuipers, E. (2001). Persecutory delusions: developing the understanding of belief maintenance and emotional distress. Psychol. Med. 31, 1293–1306. doi: 10.1017/s003329170100455x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, D. M., Murphy, A. L., O’Donnell, H., Centorrino, F., and Baldessarini, R. J. (2010). International consensus study of antipsychotic dosing. Am. J. Psychiatry. 167, 686–693. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09060802

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garety, P. A., Bebbington, P., Fowler, D., Freeman, D., and Kuipers, E. (2007). Implications for neurobiological research of cognitive models of psychosis: a theoretical paper. Psychol. Med. 37, 1377–1391. doi: 10.1017/S003329170700013X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garety, P. A., Freeman, D., Jolley, S., Dunn, G., Bebbington, P. E., Fowler, D. G., et al. (2005). Reasoning, emotions, and delusional conviction in psychosis. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 114, 373–384. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.3.373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garety, P. A., Kuipers, E., Fowler, D., Freeman, D., and Bebbington, P. E. (2001). A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychol. Med. 31, 189–195. doi: 10.1017/s0033291701003312

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gawęda, Ł., and Prochwicz, K. (2015). A comparison of cognitive biases between schizophrenia patients with delusions and healthy individuals with delusion-like experiences. Eur. Psychiatry 30, 943–949. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamshere, M. L., Stergiakouli, E., Langley, K., Martin, J., Holmans, P., Kent, L., et al. (2013). Shared polygenic contribution between childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and adult schizophrenia. Br. J. Psychiatry J. Ment. Sci. 203, 107–111. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.117432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartmann, D., Ueno, K., and Schwenck, C. (2020). Attributional and attentional bias in children with conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits: a case-control study. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 14, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/s13034-020-00315-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenness, J. L., Lambert, H. K., Bitrán, D., Blossom, J. B., Nook, E. C., Sasse, S. F., et al. (2021). Developmental variation in the associations of attention Bias to emotion with internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. Res. Child Adolesc. Psychopathol. 49, 711–726. doi: 10.1007/s10802-020-00751-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kay, S. R., Fiszbein, A., Vital-Herne, M., and Fuentes, L. S. (1990). The positive and negative syndrome scale--Spanish adaptation. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 178, 510–517. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199008000-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, S., Waschbusch, D. A., Pelham, W. E. J., Frankland, B. W., Andrade, B. F., Jacques, S., et al. (2009). Social information processing in elementary-school aged children with ADHD: medication effects and comparisons with typical children. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 37, 579–589. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9294-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langdon, R., Ward, P. B., and Coltheart, M. (2010). Reasoning anomalies associated with delusions in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 36, 321–330. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livet, A., Navarri, X., Potvin, S., and Conrod, P. (2020). Cognitive biases in individuals with psychotic-like experiences: a systematic review and a meta-analysis. Schizophr. Res. 222, 10–22. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2020.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLean, B. F., Mattiske, J. K., and Balzan, R. P. (2017). Association of the jumping to conclusions and evidence integration biases with delusions in psychosis: a detailed meta-analysis. Schizophr. Bull. 43, sbw056–sbw354. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morellini, L., Ceroni, M., Rossi, S., Zerboni, G., Rege-Colet, L., Biglia, E., et al. (2022). Social cognition in adult ADHD: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 13, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.940445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, S., Ferahli, S., and Naber, D. (2004). Memory and attention performance in psychiatric patients: lack of correspondence between clinician-rated and patient-rated functioning with neuropsychological test results. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 10, 623–633. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704104153

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, S., and Lysaker, P. H. (2018). Metacognition research in psychosis: uncovering and adjusting the prisms that distort subjective reality. Schizophr. Bull. 45, 17–18. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sby151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, S., Menon, M., Balzan, R., and Woodward, T. S. (2022). Metacognitive training for psychosis (MCT): past, present, and future. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 1–7, 1–7. doi: 10.1007/s00406-022-01394-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, S., and Woodward, T. S. (2006). A generalized bias against disconfirmatory evidence in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 142, 157–165. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.08.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, S., and Woodward, T. S. (2007). Metacognitive training in schizophrenia: from basic research to knowledge translation and intervention. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 20, 619–625. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3282f0b8ed

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, A. P., French, P., and Wells, A. (2007). Metacognitive beliefs across the continuum of psychosis: comparisons between patients with psychotic disorders, patients at ultra-high risk and non-patients. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 2241–2246. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochoa, S., López-Carrilero, R., Barrigón, M. L., Pousa, E., Barajas, A., Lorente-Rovira, E., et al. (2017). Randomized control trial to assess the efficacy of metacognitive training compared with a psycho-educational group in people with a recent-onset psychosis. Psychol. Med. 47, 1573–1584. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716003421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pena-Garijo, J., Palop-Grau, A., Masanet, M. J., Lacruz, M., Plaza, R., Hernández-Merino, A., et al. (2022). Self-reported cognitive biases in psychosis: validation of the Davos assessment of cognitive biases scale (DACOBS) in a Spanish sample of psychotic patients and healthy controls. J. Psychiatr. Res. 155, 526–533. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.09.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peralta, V., de Jalón, E. G., Campos, M. S., Zandio, M., Sanchez-Torres, A., and Cuesta, M. J. (2011). The meaning of childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity symptoms in patients with a first episode of schizophrenia-spectrum psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 126, 28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, E. R., Moritz, S., Schwannauer, M., Wiseman, Z., Greenwood, K. E., Scott, J., et al. (2014). Cognitive biases questionnaire for psychosis. Schizophr. Bull. 40, 300–313. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pishyareh, E., Tehrani-Doost, M., Mahmoodi-Gharaie, J., Khorrami, A., and Rahmdar, S. R. (2015). A comparative study of sustained attentional bias on emotional processing in ADHD children to pictures with eye-tracking. Iran. J. Child Neurol. 9, 64–70.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Polanczyk, G. V., Salum, G. A., Sugaya, L. S., Caye, A., and Rohde, L. A. (2015). Annual research review: a meta-analysis of the worldwide prevalence of mental disorders in children and adolescents. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 56, 345–365. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pugliese, V., Aloi, M., Maestri, D., de Filippis, R., Gaetano, R., Pelizza, L., et al. (2022). Validation of the Italian version of the Davos assessment of cognitive biases scale (DACOBS) in a sample of schizophrenia spectrum disorder patients and healthy controls. Riv. Psichiatr. 57, 127–133. doi: 10.1708/3814.37991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramos-Quiroga, J. A., Nasillo, V., Richarte, V., Corrales, M., Palma, F., Ibanez, P., et al. (2019). Criteria and concurrent validity of DIVA 2.0: a semi-structured diagnostic interview for adult ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 23, 1126–1135. doi: 10.1177/1087054716646451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rho, A., Traicu, A., Lepage, M., Iyer, S. N., Malla, A., and Joober, R. (2015). Clinical and functional implications of a history of childhood ADHD in first-episode psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 165, 128–133. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.03.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, R. M., McKay, R., Coltheart, M., and Langdon, R. (2015). Jumping to conclusions about the beads task? A meta-analysis of delusional ideation and data-gathering. Schizophr. Bull. 41, 1183–1191. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanchez-Gistau, V., Manzanares, N., Cabezas, A., Sole, M., Algora, M., and Vilella, E. (2020). Clinical and cognitive correlates of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in first-episode psychosis: a controlled study. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 36, 90–99. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2020.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanford, N., and Woodward, T. S. (2017). Symptom-related attributional biases in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 22, 263–279. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2017.1314957

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneidt, A., Jusyte, A., and Schönenberg, M. (2019). Interpretation of ambiguous facial affect in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 269, 657–666. doi: 10.1007/s00406-018-0879-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sibley, M. H., Evans, S. W., and Serpell, Z. N. (2010). Social cognition and interpersonal impairment in Young adolescents with ADHD. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 32, 193–202. doi: 10.1007/s10862-009-9152-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, R. C., Biggs, J. T., Ziegler, V. E., and Meyer, D. A. (1978). A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity. Br. J. Psychiatry 133, 429–435. doi: 10.1192/bjp.133.5.429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive bias, CBQp, ADHD, first episode psychosis (FEP), schizophrenia, social cognition

Citation: Sanchez-Gistau V, Cabezas A, Manzanares N, Sole M, Corral L, Vilella E and Gutierrez-Zotes A (2023) Cognitive biases in first-episode psychosis with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Front. Psychol. 14:1127535. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1127535

Received: 19 December 2022; Accepted: 26 May 2023;
Published: 05 July 2023.

Edited by:

Rashmi Gupta, Indian Institute of Technology Bombay, India

Reviewed by:

Giulia Maria Giordano, University of Campania Luigi Vanvitelli, Italy
Francisco Reyes-Madrigal, Manuel Velasco Suárez National Institute of Neurology and Neurosurgery, Mexico

Copyright © 2023 Sanchez-Gistau, Cabezas, Manzanares, Sole, Corral, Vilella and Gutierrez-Zotes. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Vanessa Sanchez-Gistau, sanchezv@peremata.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.