Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Psychol., 02 February 2023
Sec. Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Physical and Psychological Proximity in Humans: From the body to the mind and vice-versa View all 6 articles

Editorial: Physical and psychological proximity in humans: From the body to the mind and vice-versa

  • 1Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology and Health Studies, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 2Independent Max Planck Research Group for Social Neuroscience, Max Planck Institute of Psychiatry, Munich, Germany
  • 3Department of System Neuroscience, National Institute for Physiological Sciences (NIPS), Okazaki, Japan
  • 4Precision Psychiatry and Social Physiology Laboratory (PPSP), CHU Sainte-Justine Research Center, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 5Department of Psychiatry, University of Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 6Department of Psychology, Sapienza University, Rome, Italy
  • 7IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy
  • 8Fondazione Istituto Italiano Di Tecnologia (IIT), Sapienza University of Rome and Center for Life Nano- & Neuroscience, Rome, Italy

There is now a large consensus that mental processes are embodied phenomena (e.g., Clark, 1999; Barsalou, 2008; Adams, 2010; Shapiro, 2014; Da Rold, 2018; Zwaan, 2021; Ale et al., 2022). In the field of Embodied Cognition, the relationship between bodily, cognitive, and emotional states has been under intense investigation (e.g., Wilson, 2002; Shapiro, 2010; Costantini et al., 2011; Winkielman et al., 2015; Baumeister et al., 2017; Fini et al., 2017, 2021a). In the last years, specific attention has been given to interoceptive signals i.e., the signals coming from the inner parts of the body, such as heartbeats, breaths, gastrointestinal signals, and other internal sensations (e.g., Tsakiris et al., 2011; Pezzulo et al., 2018; Porciello et al., 2018; Iodice et al., 2019; Tschantz et al., 2022). These signals are important to assess internal emotional states (Critchley and Garfinkel, 2017), defining self-boundaries (Monti et al., 2021, 2022), adaptively regulating our physical needs (Pezzulo et al., 2015) and contributing also to the expression and maintenance of psychological disorders (Paulus and Stein, 2010). Although the understanding of the impact of bodily signals on psychological functioning has significantly enriched our knowledge in the field, additional research has yet to be conveyed to explore the intimate link between interoceptive signals and mental life. With the current research theme, we collected a number of diverse contributions, illustrating how internal bodily signals inform our mental processes: ranging from functions pertaining the bodily self, word processing, and stress management, to (interlinked) complex interpersonal dynamics. An integration of such diverse research strands, we believe, will be critical for the understanding of complex psychological and social processes, lying at the heart of what makes us humans (e.g., decisions, emotions, interpersonal coordination). By recognizing and detecting signals of different nature—bodily, psychological and interpersonal—this research field adopts a holistic approach to the study of human behavior.

One of the most popular experimental paradigms that allows the study of the contribution from both interoceptive and exteroceptive processing (e.g., vision, hearing) to self-awareness is the rubber hand illusion (RHI). By stroking a fake, rubber hand while synchronously stroking a participant's hidden hand, an illusion that the rubber hand belongs to oneself is induced and participants report that the location of their real hand is closer to the rubber hand than it really is (Botvinick and Cohen, 1998).

On that account, Della Longa et al. demonstrated that the RHI equally emerges with pleasant and painful stimulations. Their results confirm the importance of interoceptive signals (affective touch and painful stimulations compared to neutral touch) in promoting self-other distinction and bodily ownership.

The role of interoceptive signals in human cognitive functioning does not end with self-awareness. Instead, it also taps into other cognitive mental processes, such as language acquisition and conceptual processing. Paoletti et al. showed that toddlers engaged in a lexical decision task with abstract and concrete concepts presented an increase in temperature of the nasal tip, possibly indicating an increased parasympathetic dominance during the processing of abstract concepts. As the Words as Social Tools (WAT) Theory (Borghi et al., 2017, 2018, 2019) claims that abstract concepts are socially grounded, they might trigger a socio-linguistic component which might be more likely expressed by the predominance of the parasympathetic over the sympathetic system. Thus, the bodily temperature pattern, which signals a specific autonomic nervous system activation, seems to be associated with higher-level cognitive functions such as linguistic processing.

Internal bodily states are also sensitive to stressful contextual conditions. Therefore, various bodily signals are often used as parameters to probe the physiological status of the organism immersed in different environments. In their article, Peters et al., redefined different types of stress by specifying their internal bodily correlates. They identified “good stress”, “tolerable stress”, and “toxic stress”. In “good stress” humans adopt the most adequate response to face the stressful stimulus, in order to reduce the uncertainty or environmental “entropy” [for the notion of Free Energy Principle see Friston et al. (2006) and Friston (2010)] and it is usually associated with cerebral insulin suppression, brain-induced ketogenesis, and brain-induced heart rate acceleration. The “tolerable stress”, consisting of the habituation to stressful situations by flattening/broadening our own goals, leads to a reduction of arousal and hormonal responses. Finally, in the case of “toxic stress”, people manifest a permanent brain energy-consuming arousal state during the day and the lack of brain energy-saving deep sleep at night which contribute to the overall increase in total brain energy consumption. In particular, the authors suggested how some physiological conditions caused by a maladaptation to chronic stress might be at the origin of metabolic dysregulation, such as Type 2 diabetes mellitus and obesity. Thus, internal bodily signals appear to also reflect how humans interact with the environment.

The environment can be perceived as more or less stressful and aversive across time and context. One prominent example is social proximity before and during the COVID-19 situation. Before the pandemic, being physically closer to friends and family was often considered a source of pleasure and reassurance, whereas during peaks of infection, it was a reason for anxiety and distress. Scerrati et al., provided evidence for the flexibility of categorization processing depending on social proximity evaluation (see also Fini et al., 2021b). In detail, they showed, through an affective priming paradigm, that those who evaluated social proximity as positive processed positive words preceded by images of social proximity faster, while the contrary applied to individuals evaluating social proximity as negative. Although interoceptive signals were not directly measured, these results highlight how an internal emotional state affects the perception of socially relevant information.

Until now we underlined the entanglement among internal bodily signals, bodily awareness, cognitive processes, human-environment interaction, and the processing of socially relevant information. Ultimately, it is worth mentioning the impact of interoceptive signals on human-human interactions. Indeed, the autonomic nervous system may play a crucial role also in regulating individuals' ability to coordinate and adapt to others' behavior (Thayer et al., 2009) during interpersonal interactions. In these contexts, where two or more individuals coordinate their actions, thoughts, or words, and continuously predict, monitor, and adapt to each other's behavior (e.g., Vesper et al., 2010; Hale et al., 2020; Moreau et al., 2020, 2022; Boukarras et al., 2021, 2022; Boukarras et al.; Era et al., 2022; Sacheli et al., 2022), the study of collective psychophysiology has gained attention — especially from a second-person perspective (cf. Schilbach et al., 2013; Bolis et al., 2023). Research on this phenomenon, conceptualized as the relationship between pairs or groups of individuals' physiological dynamics, has shown how interacting with others results in interpersonal synchrony in cardiac (Danyluck and Page-Gould, 2019), electrodermal (Zeevi et al., 2022) and respiratory (Müller and Lindenberger, 2011) signals. More importantly, interpersonal attunement (e.g., coordination, synchrony, or similarity) appears to be associated with the quality of the interactions (Palumbo et al., 2017; Bolis et al., 2021; Czeszumski et al., 2022). In this regard, Boukarras et al., shed light on the importance of capturing interpersonal synergies and dynamics, also indexed by the relationship between two or more individuals' internal bodily signals, when investigating working environments (i.e. Organizational Neuroscience).

Taken together, the above-mentioned studies point to the importance of exploring internal bodily processing in tight interrelation with the subjective and intersubjective dimensions of human behavior, cognitive functioning, and social interaction, across scales and contexts, ranging from the simplest processes of self-awareness to the most complex social structures. A challenging empirical research that remains largely unexplored concerns the investigation of collective psychophysiology during so-called high-level psychological functions, e.g., internalizing and processing complex abstractions, especially within real-time social interactions (cf. Bolis and Schilbach, 2020; Borghi and Fernyhough, 2023; Fini et al., 2023).

Author contributions

CF and VE conceived and wrote the manuscript. DB and QM critically revised the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the authors who participated in the Research Topic.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Adams, F. (2010). Embodied cognition. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 9, 619–628. doi: 10.1007/s11097-010-9175-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ale, M., Sturdee, M., and Rubegni, E. (2022). A systematic survey on embodied cognition: 11 years of research in child-computer interaction. Int. J. Child-Comput. 33, 100478. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcci.2022.100478

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 617–645. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, J. C., Foroni, F., Conrad, M., Rumiati, R. I., and Winkielman, P. (2017). Embodiment and emotional memory in first vs. second language. Front. Psychol. 8, 394. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolis, D., Dumas, G., and Schilbach, L. (2023). Interpersonal attunement in social interactions: from collective psychophysiology to inter-personalized psychiatry and beyond. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 378,20210365. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolis, D., Lahnakoski, J. M., Seidel, D., Tamm, J., and Schilbach, L. (2021). Interpersonal similarity of autistic traits predicts friendship quality. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 16, 222–231. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolis, D., and Schilbach, L. (2020). “I interact therefore I am”: the self as a historical product of dialectical attunement. Topoi. 39, 521–534. doi: 10.1007/s11245-018-9574-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghi, A. M., Barca, L., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Pezzulo, G., and Tummolini, L. (2019). Words as social tools: language, sociality and inner grounding in abstract concepts. Phys. Life Rev. 29, 120–153. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2018.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghi, A. M., Barca, L., Binkofski, F., and Tummolini, L. (2018). Varieties of abstract concepts: development, use and representation in the brain. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 373, 20170121. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghi, A. M., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Cimatti, F., Scorolli, C., and Tummolini, L. (2017). The challenge of abstract concepts. Psychol. Bull. 143, 263. doi: 10.1037/bul0000089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghi, A. M., and Fernyhough, C. (2023). Concepts, abstractness and inner speech. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 378, 20210371. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Botvinick, M., and Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands “feel” touch that eyes see. Nature. 391, 756–756. doi: 10.1038/35784

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boukarras, S., Era, V., Aglioti, S. M., and Candidi, M. (2021). Competence-based social status and implicit preference modulate the ability to coordinate during a joint grasping task. Sci. Rep. 11, 1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-84280-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boukarras, S., Özkan, D. G., Era, V., Moreau, Q., Tieri, G., and Candidi, M. (2022). Midfrontal theta transcranial alternating current stimulation facilitates motor coordination in dyadic human–avatar interactions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 34, 897–915. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, A. (1999). An embodied cognitive science?. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 345–351. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01361-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costantini, M., Ambrosini, E., Scorolli, C., and Borghi, A. M. (2011). When objects are close to me: affordances in the peripersonal space. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 302–308. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0054-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Critchley, H. D., and Garfinkel, S. N. (2017). Interoception and emotion. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 17, 7–14. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.04.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czeszumski, A., Liang, S. H. Y., Dikker, S., König, P., Lee, C. P., Koole, S. L., et al. (2022). Cooperative behavior evokes interbrain synchrony in the prefrontal and temporoparietal cortex: a systematic review and meta-analysis of fNIRS Hyperscanning Studies. Eneuro 9(2). doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0268-21.2022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Da Rold, F. (2018). Defining embodied cognition: The problem of situatedness. New Ideas Psychol. 51, 9–14. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2018.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danyluck, C., and Page-Gould, E. (2019). Social and physiological context can affect the meaning of physiological synchrony. Sci. Rep. 9, 1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-44667-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Era, V., Candidi, M., Pezzetta, R., Pulcini, C., D'Antonio, S., Zabberoni, S., et al. (2022). The dopaminergic system supports flexible and rewarding dyadic motor interactive behaviour in Parkinson's disease. Soc. Cogn. Affect Neurosci. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsac040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fini, C., Bardi, L., Bolis, D., Fusaro, M., Lisi, M. P., Michalland, A. H., et al. (2023). The social roots of self development: from a bodily to an intellectual interpersonal dialogue. Psychol. Res. 1–13. doi: 10.1007/s00426-022-01785-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fini, C., Bardi, L., Epifanio, A., Committeri, G., Moors, A., and Brass, M. (2017). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of the inferior frontal cortex affects the “social scaling” of extrapersonal space depending on perspective-taking ability. Exp. Brain Res. 235, 673–679. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4817-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fini, C., Era, V., Da Rold, F., Candidi, M., and Borghi, A. M. (2021a). Abstract concepts in interaction: the need of others when guessing abstract concepts smooths dyadic motor interactions. R. Soc. Open Sci. 8, 201205. doi: 10.1098/rsos.201205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fini, C., Tummolini, L., and Borghi, A. M. (2021b). Contextual modulation of preferred social distance during the Covid-19 pandemic. Sci. Rep. 11, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-02905-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nature Rev. Neurosci. 11, 127–138. doi: 10.1038/nrn2787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K., Kilner, J., and Harrison, L. (2006). A free energy principle for the brain. J. Physiol. Paris. 100, 70–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hale, J., Ward, J. A., Buccheri, F., Oliver, D., and Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2020). Are you on my wavelength? Interpersonal coordination in dyadic conversations. J. Nonverbal Behav. 44, 63–83. doi: 10.1007/s10919-019-00320-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iodice, P., Porciello, G., Bufalari, I., Barca, L., and Pezzulo, G. (2019). An interoceptive illusion of effort induced by false heart-rate feedback. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 116, 13897–13902. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1821032116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monti, A., Porciello, G., Panasiti, M. S., and Aglioti, S. M. (2021). The inside of me: interoceptive constraints on the concept of self in neuroscience and clinical psychology. Psychol. Res. 1–10. doi: 10.1007/s00426-021-01477-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monti, A., Porciello, G., Panasiti, M. S., and Aglioti, S. M. (2022). Gut markers of bodily self-consciousness in men. Iscience. 25, 105061. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2022.105061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreau, Q., Candidi, M., Era, V., Tieri, G., and Aglioti, S. M. (2020). Midline frontal and occipito-temporal activity during error monitoring in dyadic motor interactions. Cortex. 127, 131–149. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreau, Q., Tieri, G., Era, V., Aglioti, S. M., and Candidi, M. (2022). The performance monitoring system is attuned to others' actions during dyadic motor interactions. Cerebral Cortex. 33, 222–234. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, V., and Lindenberger, U. (2011). Cardiac and respiratory patterns synchronize between persons during choir singing. PLoS ONE. 6, e24893. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palumbo, R. V., Marraccini, M. E., Weyandt, L. L., Wilder-Smith, O., McGee, H. A., Liu, S., et al. (2017). Interpersonal autonomic physiology: a systematic review of the literature. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 21, 99–141. doi: 10.1177/1088868316628405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, M. P., and Stein, M. B. (2010). Interoception in anxiety and depression. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 451–463. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0258-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pezzulo, G., Iodice, P., Barca, L., Chausse, P., Monceau, S., and Mermillod, M. (2018). Increased heart rate after exercise facilitates the processing of fearful but not disgusted faces. Sci. Rep. 8, 1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18761-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pezzulo, G., Rigoli, F., and Friston, K. (2015). Active inference, homeostatic regulation and adaptive behavioural control. Prog. Neurobiol. 134, 17–35. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porciello, G., Monti, A., and Aglioti, S. M. (2018). The Gastric Network: how the stomach and the brain work together at rest. Elife. 7, e37009. doi: 10.7554/eLife.37009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sacheli, L. M., Musco, M. A., Zazzera, E., Banfi, G., and Paulesu, E. (2022). How shared goals shape action monitoring. Cerebral Cortex. 32, 4934–4951. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G., Schlicht, T., et al. (2013). Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behav. Brain Sci. 36, 393–414.

Google Scholar

Shapiro, L. (2010). Embodied Cognition. England: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780203850664

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, L. A. (2014). The Routledge Handbook of Embodied Cognition. doi: 10.4324/9781315775845

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., Hansen, A. L., Saus-Rose, E., and Johnsen, B. H. (2009). Heart rate variability, prefrontal neural function, and cognitive performance: the neurovisceral integration perspective on self-regulation, adaptation, and health. Ann. Behav. Med. 37, 141–153. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9101-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsakiris, M., Tajadura-Jiménez, A., and Costantini, M. (2011). Just a heartbeat away from one's body: interoceptive sensitivity predicts malleability of body-representations. Proc. Biol. Sci. 278, 2470.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Tschantz, A., Barca, L., Maisto, D., Buckley, C. L., Seth, A. K., and Pezzulo, G. (2022). Simulating homeostatic, allostatic and goal-directed forms of interoceptive control using active inference. Biol. Psychol. 169, 108266. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2022.108266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vesper, C., Butterfill, S., Knoblich, G., and Sebanz, N. (2010). A minimal architecture for joint action. Neural Net. 23, 998–1003. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2010.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 9, 625–636 doi: 10.3758/BF03196322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winkielman, P., Niedenthal, P., Wielgosz, J., Eelen, J., and Kavanagh, L. C. (2015). Embodiment of Cognition and Emotion. doi: 10.1037/14341-004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeevi, L., Kellmann, E. L., Boiman, G., Hart, Y., and Atzil, S. (2022). Bio-behavioral synchrony is a potential mechanism for mate selection in humans. Sci. Rep. 12, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-08582-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zwaan, R. A. (2021). Two challenges to “embodied cognition” research and how to overcome them. J. Cognit. 4, 1. doi: 10.5334/joc.151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: internal bodily signals, self-consciousness, human-human interaction, stress responses, interpersonal proximity

Citation: Fini C, Bolis D, Moreau Q and Era V (2023) Editorial: Physical and psychological proximity in humans: From the body to the mind and vice-versa. Front. Psychol. 14:1113851. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1113851

Received: 01 December 2022; Accepted: 20 January 2023;
Published: 02 February 2023.

Edited by:

Agata Gasiorowska, University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Poland

Reviewed by:

Aleksandra Szymkow, University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Poland

Copyright © 2023 Fini, Bolis, Moreau and Era. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chiara Fini, yes chiarafini@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.