- 1Department of Psychology, Loyola University Chicago, Chicago, IL, United States
- 2Department of Applied Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, United States
- 3Departmenst of Psychology, University of California, Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, CA, United States
There is growing interest in stories as potentially powerful tools for science learning. In this mini-review article, we discuss theory and evidence indicating that, especially for young children, listening to and sharing stories with adult caregivers at home can make scientific ideas and inquiry practices meaningful and accessible. We review recent research offering evidence that stories presented in books can advance children’s science learning. Nonetheless, most of this work focuses on middle-class European-American U. S. children and involves narrative story books. Given the national imperative to increase Latine1 representation in STEM education and career pursuits in the U. S., we argue that it is vital that we broaden the definition of stories to include oral narrative storytelling and other conversational routines that Latine families engage in at home. Cultural communities with firmly rooted oral traditions, such as those from Latin American heritage, rely frequently on oral storytelling rather than book reading to convey world and community knowledge to young children. Therefore, we advocate for a strengths-based approach that considers Latine families’ everyday practices around science and storytelling on their own terms instead of contrasting them with European-American middle-class practices. We offer support for the view that for young children in Latine communities, culturally relevant oral practices, including personal narrative storytelling, can engender significant opportunities for family science learning at home.
1. Introduction
In this mini-review article, we focus on the ways stories can advance children’s science learning opportunities at home. Stories are culturally determined ways of communicating lived or imagined experiences (Bruner, 1996). Whereas most research and educational practice regarding stories centers on fostering language and literacy skills, there is growing interest in and evidence for stories supporting informal and formal science education (e.g., Brophy et al., 2008; Avraamidou and Osborne, 2009; Dahlstrom, 2014; Wilson-Lopez and Gregory, 2015; Cunningham, 2018). As we discuss, theory and evidence indicates that especially for young children, sharing stories can be a powerful vehicle for informal science learning in families. Paralleling the language and literacy work, most of the research on stories for science learning centers on story book reading. From our perspective, focusing on stories in books is not sufficient to realize the potential of stories to offer accessible and equitable science learning opportunities for young children. We argue it is necessary to broaden the focus on stories to include oral storytelling and other conversational routines that families engage in at home. This is especially important when we consider that among cultural communities with firmly rooted oral traditions, including those from Latin American heritage, oral storytelling rather than book reading may be a more common everyday practice for conveying knowledge to young children (Billings, 2009; Sánchez, 2009; Reese, 2012; Melzi et al., 2019). Unfortunately, the overreliance on print communication in formal and informal learning contexts often excludes such culturally relevant oral-based practices that can support children’s science learning. What is needed is an emphasis on ciencia en relatos - science in stories - that includes understanding and leveraging oral practices that are cultural resources for supporting Latine children’s science learning at home.
The motivation for this review is three-fold. First is the national imperative to broaden participation in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM). The U.S. Census Bureau reports that the Latine population reached 64.1 million in 2020 and is estimated to increase to 111 million by 2060, or nearly 28% of all Americans (Vespa et al., 2020). As the U.S. Latine population is increasing at a rapid pace, so too is the percentage of Latine students attending and graduating from college (Hussar et al., 2020). However, whereas 56% of the bachelor’s degrees in science fields go to White Non-Latine students, only 13.5% are awarded to Latine students (National Science Foundation, 2019). To broaden participation in STEM education of groups underrepresented in STEM, we need to identify and promote strategies that respond to and value the experiences and funds of knowledge that students bring from their homes and communities (González et al., 2013; Bricker and Bell, 2014; Hernández et al., 2016). In particular, we know little about the socio-cultural and familial experiences of Latine children that can contribute to their early science skills and learning.
Second, research has established that early informal learning experiences in homes, museums, and libraries can foster lasting interest and knowledge of STEM (e.g., National Research Council [NRC], 2009, 2012; Sobel and Jipson, 2016; National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine [NAS], 2018). For instance, parents’ support of children’s engagement with science activities predicts children’s developing attitudes and later participation in science (Alexander et al., 2012). Further, parents’ elaborative talk about science topics is related to children’s engagement during hands-on activities, their later learning and memory of science-related experiences, and their interest in science (Tenenbaum et al., 2005; Benjamin et al., 2010; Jant et al., 2014; Callanan et al., 2017). Science practices such as asking questions, observing, explaining, and making predictions are both strengths of young children’s everyday curiosity (Callanan and Oakes, 1992) and building blocks for more advanced STEM thinking (NGSS Lead States, 2013). Essentially, STEM learning opportunities involving family interactions at home and in other informal educational settings can open doors to future STEM educational and career paths. Nonetheless, such benefits may not be realized without uncovering and building on the experiences and practices of culturally and linguistically diverse families.
Third, although there is growing attention to science learning in Latine populations, too often this work takes a deficit approach by comparing Latine children with white, middle-class children and focusing on “gaps” in knowledge or practices (e.g., fewer books, less book reading). Instead, we advocate for work that can contribute to the strengths-based literature (e.g., Gutiérrez and Rogoff, 2003; Bang et al., 2012; Solis and Callanan, 2016, 2018), considering Latine families’ everyday practices around science and stories on their own terms. For Latine families in the U.S., sharing oral stories is pervasive in everyday routines, and firmly rooted in Latin American oral traditions (McDowell et al., 1993; Delgado-Gaitan, 1994; Sánchez, 2009). Moreover, consistent with sociocultural perspectives on development (e.g., Vygotsky, 1978; Rogoff, 1990), the social-linguistic milieu of shared reading, storytelling, and other conversational routines can provide a setting for children’s learning. This perspective drives a focus on culturally relevant oral practices, including personal storytelling, and efforts to understand how these social interactions can engender authentic and meaningful opportunities for Latine families’ science learning at home.
Given the applied significance of our topic, the review that follows illustrates the ways that stories in books and those told orally by families at home can provide rich opportunities for science learning, and underscores the implications for broadening STEM participation among Latine children.
2. Stories for science learning
Notwithstanding the research and educational practices centering on stories for promoting language and literacy skills (e.g., Reese, 1995; Sénéchal, 2015; Wasik et al., 2016; Flack et al., 2018), there is a growing need to identify whether and how stories can support other academic skills, especially science learning. As with the work on language and literacy, the current research on stories for science learning focuses mostly on books, and involves white, middle-class U.S. children. However, we must build on Bruner’s (1991) idea that oral storytelling is a natural form of human understanding that perhaps is more engaging for children and adults than scientific prose. Doing so encourages serious consideration of everyday home-based language practices of Latine families for whom stories in books may not be a primary way of conveying knowledge. Importantly, stories in books and told orally can convey science information that might not be available through direct experience and can boost children’s engagement with challenging science-related ideas (Kelemen et al., 2014; Browning and Hohenstein, 2015; Evans et al., 2016; Cho and Plummer, 2018). Stories also can be especially potent for making scientific ideas and inquiry practices meaningful and accessible (Graesser et al., 1980; Avraamidou and Osborne, 2009; Frykman, 2009; Klassen, 2010). By helping children connect with and see the importance of science problems, and how general and abstract science concepts can be applied to situations that are relevant to them, stories can motivate interest in and learning of science (Cordova and Lepper, 1996; Willingham, 2009; Murmann and Avraamidou, 2016) Moreover, stories can provide a springboard for elaborative discussions of science topics, involving cognitively challenging utterances about science and ideas, and scaffolding engagement in practices of science by caregivers and children (Haden, 2010; Solis and Callanan, 2018; Plummer and Cho, 2020; Shirefley et al., 2020). Embedding science information in stories can make representations of science-related knowledge and experiences stronger, more concrete, and meaningful (Haden et al., 2016; Marcus et al., 2023, in review). In these and other myriad ways, shared book reading and oral storytelling offer powerful mechanisms for children’s science learning at home.
2.1. Science in books
Most of the work on science learning through book reading has involved empirical studies in which researchers read story books to children to teach biological (e.g., Tare et al., 2010; Ganea et al., 2011, 2014; Waxman et al., 2014; Walker et al., 2015; Strouse and Ganea, 2021) and physical science information (Venkadasalam and Ganea, 2018; Ganea et al., 2021). For example, Kelemen et al. (2014) found gains in 5- to 8-year-olds’ understanding of natural selection after reading a storybook that conveyed the concept in narrative form. This was reflected not only in more accurate answers to test questions, but also more logical and coherent explanations applied to novel species, as well as children’s retention of their increased understanding over 3 months. Other work shows that despite concerns that fictional story books could interfere with children’s learning of science content (Ganea et al., 2014; Walker et al., 2015), fantastical content in story books might not hinder, and may even improve young children’s participation and engagement with science-related ideas (Hopkins and Lillard, 2021; Hopkins and Weisberg, 2021; Richert and Schlesinger, 2022). Some scholars have proposed that narrative story books may be a more engaging and productive way to communicate science topics and scientific processes to learners than typical scientific expository texts (Kurth et al., 2002; Avraamidou and Osborne, 2009; Glaser et al., 2009; Dahlstrom, 2014).
Caregivers report that they primarily share narrative story books at home (Price et al., 2009; Robertson and Reese, 2017), although children may not have strong preferences for one or the other book type (Kotaman and Tekin, 2017). Theory and research in early education emphasizes offering young children a “balanced diet” of narrative, expository and other types of texts to support learning (Teale, 2003; Pentimonti et al., 2010; Robertson and Reese, 2017). Nonetheless, the use of expository texts with young children to relay factual information is increasing in educational settings (Saracho, 2017; Bergman Deitcher et al., 2019). Moreover, research shows that caregivers use more cognitively demanding questions, emphasize new vocabulary, and their children talk more, during shared reading interactions involving expository as compared to narrative books (e.g., Pellegrini et al., 1990; Price et al., 2009; Zucker et al., 2010). Some researchers and educators suggest that expository texts may be especially supportive of lasting learning, enabling the transfer of science information conveyed in books to present and future learning opportunities (Ganea et al., 2008, 2011; Richert and Smith, 2011; Kotaman and Tekin, 2017).
Although direct comparisons of science learning from narrative and expository texts are rare, some studies favor one or the other genre, whereas other studies indicate comparable or complementary science learning from both types of books (Torr and Clugston, 1999; Gonzalez et al., 2010; Pollard-Durodola et al., 2015; Nevo and Vaknin-Nusbaum, 2018). To illustrate the mixed results, consider that in Browning and Hohenstein (2015), 5- to 7-year-olds who were introduced to evolution using a narrative text expressed deeper understanding than did those introduced to the same ideas through expository text. In contrast, Walker et al.’s (2015) preschool-aged participants were more likely to generalize causal biological information from picture books to real world situations when they had learned the information from a realistic compared to a fantasy story context. For the 4 to 5-year-olds in Venkadasalam and Ganea (2018), genre did not predict science learning, so long as the books were similarly engaging and provided accurate information. Likewise, Aydin et al. (2021) tested 3- to 5-year-old children’s learning of factual information about animals based on hearing both a storybook that contained anthropomorphism and a book that was non-narrative and did not include fantastical elements. Preschoolers in this study learned new facts about animals from both types of books. However, when the information in the narrative and expository books conflicted, older preschoolers tended to report information from the expository text; younger preschoolers showed no prioritization of information learned from one or the other book type.
2.2. Science stories and hands-on learning
A primary way that young children engage in science learning is through direct experience interacting with objects and the natural world (e.g., Piaget, 1970; Marin and Bang, 2018), and this is reflected in many early science educational opportunities for children in and out of school. When stories are combined with hands-on activities, stories can provide mechanisms for learning beyond what children might gain from hands-on engagement alone. To illustrate, several early childhood curricula pair book reading and hands-on STEM activities. Some involved specially crafted STEM-focused story books that provide visual depictions of math or engineering ideas, present problems for children to explore, and feature models for math or engineering investigations (Casey et al., 2004; Cunningham, 2018; Svarovsky et al., 2018). Engineering is Elementary curriculum units (www.eie.org; Cunningham and Lachapelle, 2014) begin with story books set in countries around the world in which the elementary-school protagonists solve problems with the help of adult engineers. There is evidence that these programs are effective, and in some cases, girls and children from groups underrepresented in STEM show particularly high learning gains (Cunningham, 2018; Svarovsky et al., 2018).
There are also an increasing number of researchers and educators seeking to understand the ways that stories in books, oral narratives, or picture-based narrative formats can advance informal STEM learning opportunities for children at home, and in libraries and museums (Pattison et al., 2020). In several studies, combining book reading or oral narratives with hands-on STEM activities in informal settings supported children’s increased interest and knowledge of STEM (e.g., Luke et al., 2010; Evans et al., 2016; Murmann and Avraamidou, 2016; Pattison et al., 2017, 2018; Tzou et al., 2019; Plummer and Cho, 2020; Letourneau et al., 2022). As another example, in Callanan et al. (2021), some families engaged with a hands-on story-based museum exhibit that conveyed a non-verbal narrative about the life and death of a mammoth. These families, in turn, talked more about science in related exhibits containing fossilized mammoth bones, than those who did not use the story-based exhibit. In other work, oral stories told by STEM experts fostered family STEM learning conversations during hands-on museum and library programs (Siegel, 2019; Zimmerman et al., 2018; Solis et al., 2023, in preparation). Notably, although these latter studies connecting stories and hands-on activities have primarily focused on white, middle-class families, they do support a move to transcend book reading to understand the ways that oral stories can provide science learning opportunities for children.
3. Science in stories: Implications for Latine children
As this brief review indicates, science books can be used to support children’s science learning. But there is still much to learn about the ways caregivers and children engage in science talk while reading science-related narrative and expository texts. Extratextual talk that goes beyond the printed word is likely important for science learning, just as it has been linked to development of specific oral language and early literacy skills (Haden et al., 1996; Fletcher and Reese, 2005; Hindman et al., 2008; Mol et al., 2008; Zucker et al., 2013). However, it is also the case that the few available studies with Latine families suggest that there may be distinctive patterns of associations between parental language during book sharing and child language outcomes with these families (e.g., Caspe, 2009; Escobar et al., 2017; Schick et al., 2017; Melzi et al., 2019). In these studies, Latine parents generally use less extratextual talk and fewer questions while sharing books with their children.
We need to address the serious gaps in current knowledge about the ways that caregivers from culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds, and particularly Latine communities, may engage with science as they read books with their children. Nevertheless, this step is not enough if we want to capitalize on Latine family practices as points of leverage to support children’s understanding of and interest in science. By broadening our consideration of science in stories to capture oral storytelling and other conversational routines, it is possible to gain purchase on the ways stories are cultural resources for Latine children’s science learning at home.
While we acknowledge the diversity of Latine families as a result of their immediate and broader ecologies (e.g., country of origin, immigration histories, rural vs. urban upbringing, languages spoken), we also believe that Latine families share a set of core values and lived experiences, among these the widespread preference for oral practices. Ethnographic work in U.S. Latine communities, for example, shows that adult family members frequently use oral stories to impart lessons about life, provide education related to moral and social issues, and transmit cultural beliefs, values, and attitudes to their children (Delgado-Gaitan, 1994; Delgado-Gaitán, 2004; Espinoza-Herald, 2007; Cortez, 2008; Sánchez et al., 2010; Solis, 2017). Family reminiscing (i.e., conversations about shared past events), traditional stories marked by dichos (i.e., popular sayings), as well as consejos (advice), refranes (proverbs), and adivinanzas (riddles) are forms of oral discourse that Latine families use to support children’s learning (Melzi et al., 2019). Work in Latin American communities outside of the U.S. documents a similar preference in families of young children. For instance, in Melzi and Caspe (2005) Spanish-speaking urban Peruvian families reported inventing and telling oral stories to their preschoolers more frequently than did English-speaking urban U.S. European-American families, who preferred book sharing. These everyday oral practices are formative, with research showing that oral sharing of stories with preschoolers predicts children’s school readiness, including oral language and early literacy skills (Reese, 1995; Melzi et al., 2022) and cognitive abilities (Fivush et al., 2006). Yet, all too often, oral practices of culturally and linguistically diverse families are overlooked.
A focus on STEM-related oral practices among Latine families is supported by a growing body of research. Consistent with other work involving families of diverse educational and economic backgrounds (Bang and Medin, 2010; Solis and Callanan, 2016, 2021; Calabrese Barton and Tan, 2020; Huitzilopochtli et al., 2021), Latine families often engage in conversations about nature, and especially animals, plants, weather, and astronomy (Pérez-Granados and Callanan, 1997; Kelemen et al., 2005; Callanan et al., 2019; Shirefley et al., 2020; Castañeda et al., 2022). Adivinanzas (riddles) are used to entertain children, but they can rely on nature and other science-related topics thereby fostering children’s knowledge and engaging them in science practices (e.g., analysis, explanation, interpretation). For example, in the following adivinanza (from Arreguín-Anderson and Ruiz-Escalante, 2018) the idea that plants have basic needs is conveyed through a simple riddle:
Similarly, dichos (sayings) are told in families’ homes to transmit wisdom and moral education. Some of these dichos are inspired by nature and people’s interactions with nature. Thus, they provide opportunities for adults to explain the nature analogies to children, and in doing so expand their knowledge about life and science (Arreguín-Anderson and Ruiz-Escalante, 2018).
Looking ahead, we must advance current understanding of stories as cultural resources for Latine families’ science learning at home. Doing so will not be easy because it requires moving away from a focus on book reading as a primary source of stories, as well as developing clearer understandings of how stories connect with hands-on activities in children’s lives. Those of us who study stories for science learning need not to repeat mistakes of research concerning shared book reading and early literacy skills that sought to change Latine caregivers’ behaviors, in turn, failing to produce the desired outcomes (see Melzi et al., 2019, for review). Efforts to support children’s science learning are more likely to be successful when they build upon families’ practices rather than seeking to replace them (cf. Melzi et al., 2022). However, insights into science in stories might still be limited without a corresponding expansion - even a “desettling” (Bang et al., 2012) - of definitions of what counts as science (see Huitzilopochtli et al., 2021; Pattison et al., 2022, for similar arguments). If we take a strengths-based approach that values the ways that science is manifested in Latine families’ stories, it should be possible to uncover the science in stories that are part of these children’s everyday, home-based language practices. Doing so will enable us to leverage their unique experiences to support Latine children’s science learning, and ultimately, broaden Latine children’s participation in STEM.
Author contributions
All authors developed the structure and content of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This research was supported by the National Science Foundation (NSF) under a collaborative grant DRL #2055382 (PI: GM), # 2055345 (PI: CH), and # 2055426 (PI: MC).
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Footnotes
1. ^We use Latine to refer to individuals whose cultural background originated in Spanish-speaking Latin America.The authors prefer to adopt the Spanish language gender inclusive ending “e” commonly used in Spanish-speaking countries, instead of the English term Latinx.
References
Alexander, J. M., Johnson, K. E., and Kelley, K. (2012). Longitudinal analysis of the relations between opportunities to learn about science and the development of interests related to science. Sci. Educ. 96, 763–786. doi: 10.1002/sce.21018
Arreguín-Anderson, M. G., and Ruiz-Escalante, J. A. (2018). Adivinanzas and dichos: preparing prospective educators to teach science by incorporating culturally responsive tools. J. Latinos Educ. 17, 84–91. doi: 10.1080/15348431.2016.1257427
Avraamidou, L., and Osborne, J. (2009). The role of narrative in communicating science. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 31, 1683–1707. doi: 10.1080/09500690802380695
Aydin, E., Ilgaz, H., and Allen, J. W. (2021). Preschoolers’ learning of information from fantastical narrative versus expository books. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 209:105170. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105170
Bang, M., and Medin, D. (2010). Cultural processes in science education: supporting the navigation of multiple epistemologies. Sci. Educ. 94, 1008–1026. doi: 10.1002/sce.20392
Bang, M., Warren, B., Rosebery, A. S., and Medin, D. (2012). Desettling expectations in science education. Hum. Dev. 55, 302–318. doi: 10.1159/000345322
Benjamin, N., Haden, C. A., and Wilkerson, E. (2010). Enhancing building, conversation, and learning through caregiver–child interactions in a children’s museum. Dev. Psychol. 46, 502–515. doi: 10.1037/a0017822
Bergman Deitcher, D., Johnson, H., and Aram, D. (2019). Does book genre matter? Boys’ and girls’ word learning from narrative and informational books in the preschool years. J. Res. Read. 42, 193–211. doi: 10.1111/1467-9817.12266
Billings, E. S. (2009). El alfabetismo y las familias latinas: a critical perspective on the literacy values and practices of Latino families with young children. J. Latinos Educ. 8, 252–269. doi: 10.1080/15348430902973385
Bricker, L. A., and Bell, P. (2014). “What comes to mind when you think of science? The perfumery!”: documenting science-related cultural learning pathways across contexts and timescales. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 51, 260–285. doi: 10.1002/tea.21134
Brophy, S., Klein, S., Portsmore, M., and Rogers, C. (2008). Advancing engineering education in P-12 classrooms. J. Eng. Educ. 97, 369–387. doi: 10.1002/j.2168-9830.2008.tb00985.x
Browning, E., and Hohenstein, J. (2015). The use of narrative to promote primary school children’s understanding of evolution. Education 43, 530–547. doi: 10.1080/03004279.2013.837943
Calabrese Barton, A., and Tan, E. (2020). Beyond equity as inclusion: a framework of “rightful presence” for guiding justice-oriented studies in teaching and learning. Educ. Res. 49, 433–440. doi: 10.3102/0013189X20927363
Callanan, M. A., Castañeda, C. L., Luce, M. R., and Martin, J. L. (2017). Family science talk in museums: predicting children’s engagement from variations in talk and activity. Child Dev. 88, 1492–1504. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12886
Callanan, M. A., Castañeda, C. L., Solis, G., Luce, M. R., Diep, M., McHugh, S. R., et al. (2021). “He fell in and that’s how he became a fossil!”: engagement with a storytelling exhibit predicts families’ explanatory science talk during a museum visit. Front. Psychol. 12:689649. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.689649
Callanan, M. A., and Oakes, L. M. (1992). Preschoolers’ questions and parents’ explanations: causal thinking in everyday activity. Cogn. Dev. 7, 213–233. doi: 10.1016/0885-2014(92)90012-G
Callanan, M., Shirefley, T., Castañeda, C. L., and Jipson, J. (2019). Young children’s ideas about astronomy. JAESE 6, 45–58. doi: 10.19030/jaese.v6i2.10339
Caspe, M. (2009). Low-income Latino mothers’ booksharing styles and children’s emergent literacy development. Early Child. Res. Q. 24, 306–324. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.03.006
Castañeda, C. L., Callanan, M. A., Shirefley, T. A., and Jipson, J. L. (2022). Early strengths in science: young children’s conversations about nature in Latine families. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 83:101453. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2022.101453
Casey, B., Kersh, J. E., and Young, J. M. (2004). Storytelling sagas: An effective medium for teaching early childhood mathematics. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 19, 167–172. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.01.011
Cho, K., and Plummer, J. (2018). Using science storybooks to support preschool-age children’s development of evidence-based explanations during museum-based programming. Poster presented at the NARST annual international conference, Atlanta, GA.
Cordova, D. I., and Lepper, M. R. (1996). Intrinsic motivation and the process of learning: beneficial effects of contextualization, personalization, and choice. J. Educ. Psychol. 88, 715–730. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.88.4.715
Cortez, M. (2008). Trying to fit in a different world. Int. J. Early Child. 40, 97–100. doi: 10.1007/BF03168366
Cunningham, C. M. (2018). Engineering in elementary STEM education: Curriculum design, instruction, learning, and assessment. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.
Cunningham, C. M., and Lachapelle, C. P. (2014). “Designing engineering experiences to engage all students” in Engineering in pre-college settings: Synthesizing research, policy, and practices. eds. S. Purzer, J. Strobel, and M. E. Cardella (Lafayette, IN: Purdue University Press), 117–142.
Dahlstrom, M. F. (2014). Using narratives and storytelling to communicate science with nonexpert audiences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 111, 13614–13620. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1320645111
Delgado-Gaitan, C. (1994). “Spanish-speaking families’ involvement in schools” in School, family and community interaction: A view from the firing lines. eds. C. L. Fagnano and B. Z. Weber (San Francisco: Westview Press), 85–98.
Delgado-Gaitán, C. (2004). Involving Latino families in schools: Raising student achievement through home-school partnerships, Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.
Escobar, K., Melzi, G., and Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2017). Mother and child narrative elaborations during booksharing in low-income Mexican-American dyads. Infant Child Dev. 26, e1–e18. doi: 10.1002/icd.2029
Espinoza-Herald, M. (2007). Stepping beyond sí se puede: Dichos as a cultural resource in mother daughter interaction in a Latino family. Anthropol. Educ. Q. 38, 260–277. doi: 10.1525/aeq.2007.38.3.260
Evans, E. M., Weiss, M., Lane, J. L., and Palmquist, S. (2016). “The spiral model: integrating research and exhibit development to foster conceptual change” in Cognitive development in museum settings: Relating research and practice. eds. D. M. Sobel and J. L. Jipson (New York: Psychology Press), 36–64.
Fivush, R., Haden, C. A., and Reese, E. (2006). Elaborating on elaborations: role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development. Child Dev. 77, 1568–1588. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00960.x
Flack, Z. M., Field, A. P., and Horst, J. S. (2018). The effects of shared storybook reading on word learning: a meta-analysis. Dev. Psychol. 54, 1334–1346. doi: 10.1037/dev0000512
Fletcher, K. L., and Reese, E. (2005). Picture book reading with young children: a conceptual framework. Dev. Rev. 25, 64–103. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2004.08.009
Frykman, S. (2009). Stories to tell? Narrative tools in museum education texts. Educ. Res. 51, 299–319. doi: 10.1080/00131880903156898
Ganea, P. A., Canfield, C. F., Simons-Ghafari, K., and Chou, T. (2014). Do cavies talk? The effect of anthropomorphic picture books on children’s knowledge about animals. Front. Psychol. 5:283. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00283
Ganea, P. A., Larsen, N. E., and Venkadasalam, V. P. (2021). The role of alternative theories and anomalous evidence in children’s scientific belief revision. Child Dev. 92, 1137–1153. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13481
Ganea, P. A., Ma, L., and DeLoache, J. S. (2011). Young children’s learning and transfer of biological information from picture books to real animals. Child Dev. 82, 1421–1433. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01612.x
Ganea, P. A., Pickard, M. B., and DeLoache, J. S. (2008). Transfer between picture books and the real world by very young children. J. Cogn. Dev. 9, 46–66. doi: 10.1080/15248370701836592
Glaser, M., Garsoffky, B., and Schwan, S. (2009). Narrative-based learning: possible benefits and problems. Communications 34, 429–447. doi: 10.1515/COMM.2009.026
González, N., Moll, L. C., and Amanti, C. (Eds.). (2013). Funds of knowledge: Theorizing practices in households, communities, and classrooms. New York, NY: Routledge.
Gonzalez, J. E., Pollard-Durodola, S., Simmons, D. C., Taylor, A. B., Davis, M. J., Kim, M., et al. (2010). Developing low-income preschoolers’ social studies and science vocabulary knowledge through content-focused shared book reading. J. Res. Educ. Effect. 4, 25–52. doi: 10.1080/19345747.2010.487927
Graesser, A. C., Hauft-Smith, K., Cohen, A. D., and Pyles, L. D. (1980). Advanced outlines, familiarity, and text genre on retention of prose. J. Exp. Educ. 48, 281–290. doi: 10.1080/00220973.1980.11011745
Gutiérrez, K. D., and Rogoff, B. (2003). Cultural ways of learning: individual traits or repertoires of practice. Educ. Res. 32, 19–25. doi: 10.3102/0013189X032005019
Haden, C. A. (2010). Talking about science in museums. Child Dev. Perspect. 4, 62–67. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00119.x
Haden, C. A., Cohen, T., Uttal, D., and Marcus, M. (2016). “Building learning: narrating and transferring experiences in a children’s museum” in Cognitive development in museum settings: Relating research and practice. eds. D. Sobel and J. Jipson (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 84–103.
Haden, C. A., Reese, E., and Fivush, R. (1996). Mothers’ extratextual comments during storybook reading: stylistic differences over time and across texts. Discourse Process. 21, 135–169. doi: 10.1080/01638539609544953
Hernández, D., Rana, S., Alemdar, M., Rao, A., and Usselman, M. (2016). Latino parents’ educational values and STEM beliefs. J. Multicult. Educ. 10, 354–367. doi: 10.1108/JME-12-2015-0042
Hindman, A. H., Connor, C. M., Jewkes, A. M., and Morrison, F. J. (2008). Untangling the effects of shared book reading: multiple factors and their associations with preschool literacy outcomes. Early Child. Res. Q. 23, 330–350. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.01.005
Hopkins, E. J., and Lillard, A. S. (2021). The magic school Bus dilemma: how fantasy affects children’s learning from stories. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 210:105212. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105212
Hopkins, E. J., and Weisberg, D. S. (2021). Investigating the effectiveness of fantasy stories for teaching scientific principles. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 203:105047. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105047
Huitzilopochtli, S., Foxworthy Gonzalez, J., Moschkovich, J., McHugh, S., and Callanan, M. (2021). “Noticing multilingual and non-dominant students’ strengths for learning mathematics and science” in Multilingual education yearbook 2021. eds. A. A. Essien and A. Msimanga (Cham: Springer), 155–172.
Hussar, B., Zhang, J., Hein, S., Wang, K., Roberts, A., Cui, J., Smith, M., Bullock Mann, F., Barmer, A., and Dilig, R. (2020). The condition of education 2020 (NCES 2020–144). U.S. Department of Education. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics.
Jant, E. A., Haden, C. A., Uttal, D. H., and Babcock, E. (2014). Conversation and object manipulation influence children’s learning in a museum. Child Dev. 85, 1771–2105. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12252
Kelemen, D., Callanan, M. A., Casler, K., and Pérez-Granados, D. R. (2005). Why things happen: teleological explanation in parent-child conversations. Dev. Psychol. 41, 251–264. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.1.251
Kelemen, D., Emmons, N. A., Seston Schillaci, R., and Ganea, P. A. (2014). Young children can be taught basic natural selection using a picture-storybook intervention. Psychol. Sci. 25, 893–902. doi: 10.1177%2F0956797613516009
Klassen, S. (2010). The relation of story structure to a model of conceptual change in science learning. Sci. & Educ. 19, 305–317. doi: 10.1007/s11191-009-9212-8
Kotaman, H., and Tekin, A. K. (2017). Informational and fictional books: young children’s book preferences and teachers’ perspectives. Early Child Dev. Care 187, 600–614. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2016.1236092
Kurth, L. A., Kidd, R., Gardner, R., and Smith, E. L. (2002). Student use of narrative and paradigmatic forms of talk in elementary science conversations. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 39, 793–818. doi: 10.1002/tea.10046
Letourneau, S. M., Bennett, D. T., Liu, C., Argudo, Y., Peppler, K., Keune, A., et al. (2022). Observing empathy in informal engineering activities with girls ages 7–14. J. Pre-Coll. Eng. Educ. Res. 11, 120–128. doi: 10.7771/2157-9288.1354
Luke, J. J., Ancelet, J. E., and ueiredo, C. (2010). Leap into science: Year 3 evaluation report. Institute for Learning Innovation. Available at: http://www.informalscience.org/leap-science-year-3-evaluation-report (Accessed January 9, 2020).
Marin, A., and Bang, M. (2018). “Look it, this is how you know:” Family forest walks as a context for knowledge-building about the natural world Cognition and Instruction, 36, 89–118. doi: 10.1080/07370008.2018.1429443
McDowell, J. H., Herrera-Sobek, M., and Cortina, R. J. (1993). “Hispanic oral tradition: form and content” in Handbook of Hispanic cultures in the United States: Literature and art. eds. N. Kanellos and C. Esteva-Fabregat (Houston, TX: Arte Público Press), 218–247.
Melzi, G., and Caspe, M. (2005). Variations in maternal narrative styles during book reading interactions. Narrat. Inq. 15, 101–125. doi: 10.1075/ni.15.1.06mel
Melzi, G., Schick, A., and Scarola, L. (2019). “Literacy interventions that promote home-to-school links for ethnoculturally diverse families of young children” in Ethnocultural diversity and the home-to-school link. eds. C. M. McWayne, F. Doucet, and S. M. Sheridan (Cham, Switzerland: Springer), 123–143.
Melzi, G., Schick, A. R., and Wuest, C. (2022). Stories beyond books: teacher storytelling supports children’s literacy skills. Early Educ. Dev. 34, 485–505. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2021.2024749
Mol, S. E., Bus, A. G., de Jong, M. T., and Smeets, D. J. H. (2008). Added value of dialogic parent-child book readings: a meta-analysis. Early Educ. Dev. 19, 7–26. doi: 10.1080/10409280701838603
Murmann, M., and Avraamidou, L. (2016). The use of fictional stories in science exhibits: the emperor who only believed his own eyes. Curator 59, 239–261. doi: 10.1111/cura.12165
National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine [NAS] (2018). How people learn II: Learners, contexts, and cultures. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
National Research Council [NRC] (2009). “Learning science in informal environments: people, places, and pursuits” in Committee on learning science in informal environments. eds. P. Bell, B. Lewenstein, A. W. Shouse, and M. A. Feder (Washington, DC: The National Academies Press)
National Research Council [NRC] (2012). “A framework for K-12 science education: Practices, crosscutting concepts, and core ideas” in Committee on a conceptual framework for new K-12 science education standards. Board on science education, division of behavioral and social sciences and education (Washington, DC: The National Academies Press)
National Science Foundation (2019). National Center for Science and Engineering Statistics. Women, minorities, and persons with disabilities in science and engineering: 2019. Special Report NSF 19–304. Alexandria, VA.
Nevo, E., and Vaknin-Nusbaum, V. (2018). Enhancing language and print-concept skills by using interactive storybook reading in kindergarten. J. Early Child. Lit. 18, 545–569. doi: 10.1177/1468798417694482
NGSS Lead States (2013). Next generation science standards: For states, by states. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
Pattison, S., Ramos Montañez, S., Svarovsky, G., and Tominey, S. (2022). Engineering for equity: exploring the intersection of engineering education, family learning, early childhood, and equity. Available at: https://blog.terc.edu/engineering-for-equity (Accessed January 10, 2022).
Pattison, S., Svarovsky, G, Gontan, I., Corrie, P. G., Benne, M., Weiss, S., et al. (2017). Head start on engineering: teachers, informal STEM educators, and learning researchers collaborating to engage low-income families with engineering. Connected Science Learning, 4. Available at: http://csl.nsta.org/2017/10/head-start-engineering (Accessed January 9, 2020).
Pattison, S., Svarovsky, G., Ramos-Montañez, S., and Kirkland, P. (2020). Storybook STEM: Children’s literature as a tool for supporting informal STEM learning, Cambridge, MA: TERC. www.terc.edu/storybookstem/ (Accessed October 10, 2022).
Pattison, S., Weiss, S., Ramos-Montañez, S., Gontan, I., Svarovsky, G., Corrie, P. G., et al. (2018). “Engineering in early childhood: describing family-level interest development systems” in Paper presented at the National Association for research in science teaching annual conference (Atlanta, GA).
Pellegrini, A. D., Perlmutter, J. C., Galda, L., and Brody, G. H. (1990). Joint reading between black head start children and their mothers. Child Dev. 61, 443–453. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02791.x
Pentimonti, J. M., Zucker, T. A., Justice, L. M., and Kaderavek, J. N. (2010). Informational text use in preschool classroom read-alouds. Read. Teach. 63, 656–665. doi: 10.1598/RT.63.8.4
Pérez-Granados, D. R., and Callanan, M. A. (1997). Conversations with mothers and siblings: young childrens’ semantic and conceptual development. Dev. Psychol. 33, 120–134. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.120
Piaget, J. (1970). Science of education and the psychology of the child, Trans. ed. D. Coltman London, England: Longman.
Plummer, J. D., and Cho, K. (2020). “Integrating narrative into the design of preschool science programs” in The Interdisciplinarity of the learning sciences, the 14th international conference of the learning sciences. eds. M. Gresalfi and I. Horn (Nashville, TN), 1585–1589.
Pollard-Durodola, S. D., Gonzalez, J. E., Simmons, D. C., and Simmons, L. E. (2015). Accelerating language skills and content knowledge through shared book reading, Baltimore, MD: Brooks.
Price, L. H., Van Kleeck, A., and Huberty, C. J. (2009). Talk during book sharing between parents and preschool children: a comparison between storybook and expository book conditions. Read. Res. Q. 44, 171–194. doi: 10.1598/RRQ.44.2.4
Reese, E. (1995). Predicting children’s literacy from mother-child conversations. Cogn. Dev. 10, 381–405. doi: 10.1016/0885-2014(95)90003-9
Reese, L. (2012). Storytelling in Mexican homes: connections between oral and literacy practices. Biling. Res. J. 35, 277–293. doi: 10.1080/15235882.2012.734006
Richert, R. A., and Schlesinger, M. A. (2022). Relations between fantasy and transfer of learning from storybooks. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 222:105474. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105474
Richert, R. A., and Smith, E. I. (2011). Preschoolers’ quarantining of fantasy stories. Child Dev. 82, 1106–1119. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01603.x
Robertson, S. J. L., and Reese, E. (2017). The very hungry caterpillar turned into a butterfly: Children’s and parents’ enjoyment of different book genres. J. Early Child. Lit. 17, 3–25. doi: 10.1177/1468798415598354
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Sánchez, C. (2009). Learning about students’ culture and language through family stories elicited by dichos. Early Childhood Educ. J. 37, 161–169. doi: 10.1007/s10643-009-0331-2
Sánchez, C., Plata, V., Grosso, L., and Leird, B. (2010). Encouraging Spanish-speaking families’ involvement through dichos. J. Latinos Educ. 9, 239–248. doi: 10.1080/15348431003761216
Saracho, O. N. (2017). Parents’ shared storybook reading–learning to read. Early Child Dev. Care 187, 554–567. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2016.1261514
Schick, A. R., Melzi, G., and Obregón, J. (2017). The bidirectional nature of narrative scaffolding: Latino caregivers’ elaboration while creating stories from a picture book. First Lang. 37, 301–316. doi: 10.1177/0142723716689692
Sénéchal, M. (2015). “Young children’s home literacy experiences” in The Oxford handbook of reading. eds. A. Pollatsek and R. Treiman (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 397–414.
Shirefley, T., Castañeda, C., Rodriguez-Gutierrez, J., Callanan, M., and Jipson, J. (2020). Science conversations during family book reading with girls and boys in two cultural communities. J. Cogn. Dev. 21, 551–572. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2020.1797750
Siegel, D. (Chair). (2019). Weaving stories into STEM learning. Poster symposium at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Toronto, CA.
Sobel, D., and Jipson, J. (2016). Cognitive development in museum settings: Relating research and practice. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Solis, G. (2017). Mexican and European heritage families’ elaborations during two storytelling activities. (Publication no. 10688313). Doctoral dissertation. University of California, Santa Cruz. ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global.
Solis, G., and Callanan, M. (2016). Evidence against deficit accounts: conversations about science in Mexican heritage families living in the United States. Mind Cult. Act. 23, 212–224. doi: 10.1080/10749039.2016.1196493
Solis, G., and Callanan, M. (2018). “Avoiding deficit assumptions in research on diverse families’ conversations about science” in Family engagement and STEM (science, technology, engineering and math), family school community partnership monograph series, Charlotte. eds. M. Caspe, T. Woods, and J. L. Kennedy (NC: Information Age Publishing)
Solis, G., and Callanan, M. (2021). Collaborative inquiry or teacher talk?: parent guidance during a science-related activity in Mexican-heritage families from two schooling groups. J. Cogn. Dev. 22, 448–466. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2021.1901710
Strouse, G. A., and Ganea, P. A. (2021). The effect of object similarity and alignment of examples on children’s learning and transfer from picture books. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 203:105041. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105041
Svarovsky, G. N., Wagner, C., and Cardella, M. E. (2018). Exploring moments of agency for girls during an engineering activity. Int. J. Educ. Math. Sci. Technol. 6, 302–319. doi: 10.18404/ijemst.428200
Tare, M., Chiong, C., Ganea, P., and DeLoache, J. (2010). Less is more: how manipulative features affect children’s learning from picture books. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 31, 395–400. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2010.06.005
Teale, W. H. (2003). Reading aloud to young children as a class-room instructional activity: insights from research and practice. In A. Kleeckvan, S. A. Stahl, and E. B. Bauer (Eds.), On reading books to children: Parents and teachers (pp. 109–133). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tenenbaum, H. R., Snow, C. E., Roach, K. A., and Kurland, B. (2005). Talking and reading science: longitudinal data on sex differences in mother–child conversations in low-income families. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 26, 1–19. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2004.10.004
Torr, J., and Clugston, L. (1999). A comparison between informational and narrative picture books as a context for reasoning between caregivers and 4-year-old children. Early Child Dev. Care 159, 25–41. doi: 10.1080/0300443991590104
Tzou, C., Meixi, S. E., Bell, P., LaBonte, D., Starks, E., and Bang, M. (2019). Storywork in STEM-art: making, materiality and robotics within everyday acts of indigenous presence and resurgence. Cogn. Instr. 37, 306–326. doi: 10.1080/07370008.2019.1624547
Venkadasalam, V. P., and Ganea, P. A. (2018). Do objects of different weight fall at the same time? Updating naive beliefs about free-falling objects from fictional and informational books in young children. J. Cogn. Dev. 19, 165–181. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2018.1436058
Vespa, J., Medina, L., and Armstrong, D. M., (2020). Demographic turning points for the United States: population projections for 2020 to 2060. Current population reports, P25-1144, U.S. Census Bureau, Washington, DC.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Walker, C. M., Gopnik, A., and Ganea, P. A. (2015). Learning to learn from stories: Children’s developing sensitivity to the causal structure of fictional worlds. Child Dev. 86, 310–318. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12287
Wasik, B. A., Hindman, A. H., and Snell, E. K. (2016). Book reading and vocabulary development: a systematic review. Early Child. Res. Q. 37, 39–57. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.04.003
Waxman, S. R., Herrmann, P., Woodring, J., and Medin, D. L. (2014). Humans (really) are animals: picture-book reading influences 5-year-old urban children’s construal of the relation between humans and non-human animals. Front. Psychol. 5:172. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00172
Willingham, D. T. (2009). Why don’t students like school? A cognitive scientist answers questions about how the mind works and what it means for the classroom. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Wilson-Lopez, A., and Gregory, S. (2015). Integrating literacy and engineering instruction for young learners. Read. Teach. 69, 25–33. doi: 10.1002/trtr.1351
Zimmerman, H. T., Carr, M., and Toro, S. A. (Chairs) (2018). Narrative in science and engineering inquiry activities: research investigating families learning in libraries and museums. Symposium presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New York City, NY.
Zucker, T. A., Cabell, S. Q., Justice, L. M., Pentimonti, J. M., and Kaderavek, J. N. (2013). The role of frequent, interactive prekindergarten shared reading in the longitudinal development of language and literacy skills. Dev. Psychol. 49, 1425–1439. doi: 10.1037/a0030347
Keywords: storytelling, informal science learning, parent–child conversations, book reading, home learning, strengths-based
Citation: Haden CA, Melzi G and Callanan MA (2023) Science in stories: Implications for Latine children’s science learning through home-based language practices. Front. Psychol. 14:1096833. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1096833
Edited by:
Bradley Morris, Kent State University, United StatesReviewed by:
Jennifer M. Clegg, Texas State University, United StatesAndres Bustamante, University of California, Irvine, United States
Copyright © 2023 Haden, Melzi and Callanan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Catherine A. Haden, ✉ Y2hhZGVuQGx1Yy5lZHU=