Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Psychol., 14 June 2023
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings

A rule-based theoretical account of social stories to address the double empathy problem

  • 1Centre for Applied Autism Research (CAAR), University of Bath, Bath, United Kingdom
  • 2Department for Inclusion & Access to Learning, University of Malta, Msida, Malta

Social Stories™ (SS) is one of the most popular and researched interventions for autistic children. To date, research that focuses on outcomes has been prioritized over the investigation of the psychological mechanisms that inform the intervention. In this article we consider theoretical accounts proposed thus far which could underpin SS. We argue that mechanisms that are based on social deficit theories lack validity, and propose a rule-based theoretical account to inform a strengths-based approach toward conceptualizing the mechanisms that underpin SS. We apply this account to the ‘double-empathy problem’ to propose that SS can be adapted to involve all parties in the development and delivery of SS support by adopting a rule-based perspective. We use the example of systemizing (the drive to analyze and explore systems in terms of ‘if-and-then’ rules), which is proposed to be a relative autistic strength, as a form of rule-based thinking that can provide a theoretical account of SS and a framework to address the double-empathy problem.

Introduction

Social Stories™ (SS) (Gray and Garand, 1993; Gray, 1998) is one of the most frequently used story-based interventions (see Pane et al., 2015) by parents of autistic children1 (Green et al., 2006; Hess et al., 2008) and by practitioners who support autistic individuals (Smith and Gillon, 2004; Hsieh et al., 2018). Kokina and Kern’s (2010) analysis of SS literature indicates that SS research prior to 2010 focused on utilizing SS to support autistic individuals in a variety of tasks such as the reduction of inappropriate behaviors, improvement in social behaviors, supporting the acquisition of academic and functional skills, and assisting in novel events/transitions. The concern with reducing behaviors, and the labeling of autistic behaviors as ‘inappropriate’ by others (typically non-autistic others), could be indicative of a deficit outlook, as opposed to the neurodiversity-affirming perspective which is adopted in this article.

More recently, SS have also been used to support autistic children’s understanding in various contexts such as abduction prevention skills (e.g., Kurt and Kutlu, 2019), giving compliments (e.g., Almutlaq and Martella, 2018) turn-taking skills (e.g., Malmberg et al., 2015), greeting skills (e.g., Kagohara et al., 2013), and menstrual care (e.g., Klett and Turan, 2012). SS have also been used to support autistic children’s episodic memory (e.g., Hutchins and Prelock, 2018) and to increase task engagement (e.g., Cihak et al., 2012), amongst other purposes.

The SS intervention is described as a highly acceptable intervention by practitioners in the field of autism (Styles, 2011). Attitudes toward the intervention are also reported to be positive (Dodd et al., 2008; Acar et al., 2017; Camilleri et al., 2022). The low cost of the intervention and its availability are possible factors that contribute to the positive attitudes toward the intervention amongst practitioners and parents. Whilst SS outcomes research is extensive, a great degree of variability in terms of outcomes is reported in the SS literature (Camilleri et al., 2021). The literature also highlights a lack of a clear theoretical underpinning or rationale for SS (Reynhout and Carter, 2006; Camilleri et al., 2021). However, it is not uncommon for psychological studies to focus on measuring outcomes, especially since this would inform the very pertinent question of whether an intervention is effective (Windgassen et al., 2016).

The recent increase in interest in the use of digital technology as a means to develop and deliver SS (e.g., Constantin et al., 2013, 2017; Hanrahan et al., 2020; Smith et al., 2020, 2021; Camilleri et al., 2021; Saeed and Safi, 2020; Safi et al., 2022) has been accompanied by a further interest in understanding the intervention’s mechanisms and theoretical underpinnings (e.g., Riga et al., 2021). The investigation of mechanisms underpinning SS can contribute to two very important aspects of psychological interventions or supports. First, it advances our understanding of the key psychological processes, or mechanisms (i.e., the processes involved in, or responsible for, an action to occur), that affect positive outcomes of SS. Second, it also helps to locate issues and questions within existing theories and conceptualization of the SS support tool. In turn, the investigation of a possible theoretical rationale, or mechanism, may assist in the evaluation, optimization, and development of further approaches or support tools (Bawazir and Jones, 2017).

Thus, to continue to build on the limited, but developing, discussion on the underpinning mechanisms that inform the SS intervention, in the first part of this article we consider the theoretical accounts for SS which have been proposed thus far. Subsequently, in the second part, we propose a ‘rule-based’ theoretical rationale for SS. This rule-based rationale is informed by a neurodiversity paradigm that defines autism as “a natural identity with strengths and weaknesses” (Gillespie-Lynch et al., 2017, p. 3). We also employ the ‘systemizing’ theory (Baron-Cohen and Lombardo, 2017) as an example of rule-based thinking to inform our discussion.

Autism is clinically defined in terms of difficulties in social communication and interaction and patterns of restricted and repetitive behaviors, interests and activities (American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5 Task Force, 2013; World Health Organization, 2004). However, researchers and self-advocates have called for more strengths-based approaches to our understanding of autism (see Urbanowicz et al., 2019), incorporating strengths such as the ability to hyperfocus, attention to detail, good semantic memory, and deliberative analytical thinking (e.g., Brosnan et al., 2016, 2017; Russell et al., 2019; Urbanowicz et al., 2019). Such strengths are considered “context-dependent,” and are not necessarily fixed traits (Russell et al., 2019). Nevertheless, this strengths-based lens/framework is contrasted with social deficit-driven epistemology, which is argued to stem from the medicalization of the autistic individual (Kapp, 2019).

In this article we utilize a neurodiversity paradigm (Singer, 2016) which views autism as a natural and valuable form of human diversity (Chapman, 2020). This neurodiversity paradigm, as described by Dwyer (2022), challenges the view that communication difficulties, or disability, exist within the autistic person alone. Rather, disability is the result of the interaction between the characteristics of an individual and the environment around that person. Similarly, in this article, we utilize what Milton refers to as the double empathy problem (Milton, 2012) to challenge social deficit-driven ontologies, and propose that SS can contribute toward addressing the double-empathy problem by involving all parties in the SS process by adopting a rule-based perspective, which could be considered an autistic strength.

Social Stories™

Social Stories™ (SS) were introduced 30 years ago by Gray and Garand (1993), and are short narratives that follow specific guidelines to objectively describe a person, skill, event, concept, or social situation (Gray, 1998; Timmins, 2016). They are personalized and detailed accounts written specifically to provide a narrative of a situation (refer to Box 1 for an example of a SS). SS sometimes also can include visual aids which accompany the narratives, such as line drawings, photographs or cartoons. These aids are aimed toward illustrating or highlighting the key points of the story.

BOX 1. Example of a social story.

The social story in Box 1 has been taken from the Stories Online for Autism (SOFA, sofa-app.org) digital application.

Social Stories™ aim to facilitate the accurate transfer of information between an author (i.e., the person developing the story) and the individual that the story is written for; also known as the audience (Gray, 2010). Each story that is developed is tailored to the individual’s abilities, attention span, and learning style and consists of three parts: introduction, body, and conclusion (Howley and Arnold, 2005). The stories are written in first- and/or third-person perspectives and have a positive and patient tone. Stories should inform and answer ‘where,’ ‘when,’ ‘who,’ ‘what,’ ‘how,’ and ‘why’ questions, and consist of more descriptive sentences than coaching sentences. Descriptive sentences are sentences that describe the facts relating to the situation clearly and objectively, whilst coaching sentences are sentences that describe or suggest responses or actions. Social stories can also include perspective sentences and affirmative sentences. Perspective sentences describe the internal states, feelings, and beliefs of other people, whilst affirmative sentences tend to highlight or stress an important point. The social stories should then be developed and refined in a way that should help to maximize the transfer of information (Gray, 2010).

Over time the guidelines for the development of SS, as defined by Gray and Garand (1993) have been updated. Gray (2022) states that these changes can be seen more as revisions and reorganizations which resulted from research and experience of using SS. The latest revision of these guidelines (Social Stories 10.3) was introduced by Gray in 2021 with 10 criteria for the development of SS. These guidelines emphasize the need for stories to (1) humbly, yet accurately, share social information with the audience, (2) be informed by in-depth understanding of the audience, (3) have one title, a three-part structure (introduction, body and conclusion), and descriptive and coaching sentences, (4) be tailored to the abilities, attention span, learning style and, whenever possible, talents and interests of the audience, (5) have a patient and supportive ‘voice,’ and (6) answer relevant ‘who,’ ‘what,’ ‘when,’ ‘where,’ why and ‘how’ questions. They also specify for stories to (7) celebrate and praise, and (8) consist of three times as many descriptive sentences as coaching sentences. Criteria 9 and 10 highlight the iterative process of refining and improving the story in order to ensure that Social Humility and the 10 criteria that guide social story development are consistent with its introduction and review over time (Gray, 2021). Finally, the 10 criteria must be adhered to for a story to be considered a Social Story™ (Wright et al., 2016).

Gray and Garand (1993) and Gray (2022) states that these guidelines are based on a number of overarching principles. The first, according to Gray (1998, p. 168) is the need to “abandon all assumptions.” Gray (1998) argues that what makes it difficult for autistic and non-autistic people to understand each other and interact is that mutual social understandings are based on assumptions that could be erroneous or inaccurate. Thus, Gray’s second principle states that there must be a recognition that social impairment in autism is a result of erroneous assumptions made on both sides of the social equation. Accordingly, whilst acknowledging possible inaccuracies and imperfections in all assumptions related to social interactions, Gray (2018) states in her third principle that each interaction, and each person’s idiosyncratic perspective, is equally valid and deserving of respect. The most recent version of the guidelines includes more focus on what Gray (2022) describes as ‘social humility’. This concept is meant to encourage the acknowledgement of authors’ inherently fallible assumptions related to social interaction. Thus, social humility aims to decrease the chance of the authors’ erroneous understanding of individuals identified as autistic, through embracing the perspective of the autistic individual.

Original theoretical rationale for social stories: social-deficit perspectives

Gray (1998) had proposed that the rationale for SS is based on what at the time represented the growing understanding of social cognition in autism. She argues that Theory of Mind (ToM, Baron-Cohen et al., 1985; Leslie, 1987; Happé and Frith, 1995), as well as Weak Central Coherence (WCC, Frith, 1989; Happé, 2000; Happé and Frith, 2006), are important areas of research to SS. ToM is defined as the capacity for prediction and interpretation of behavior by using representations of hidden causally efficacious mental states (Westra and Carruthers, 2018); that is, the ability to attribute beliefs, emotions, intentions, and goals to ourselves and others. Central Coherence is defined as the cognitive tendency to process information comprehensively, globally, and in context (Noens and Van Berckelaer-Onnes, 2008). Adequate central coherence aids individuals in making sense of the ‘whole picture’ and seeing structure and meaning. Baron-Cohen et al. (1985) describe how autistic individuals could have difficulties in ToM, whilst Happé and Frith (2006) describe how autistic individuals could present with weak central coherence (i.e., with a preference for processing local detail over global processing).

Using ToM and WCC theories as a foundation, Gray (1998) argues that SS provide an autistic individual with access to a social “secret-code” (p.169) and thus compensate for that individual’s poor mind-reading, or mentalizing (ToM), abilities. Gray (1998) states that SS also “raise awareness of yet another secret” (Gray, 1998, p. 169) by integrating information at different levels, and thus provide a more meaningful and contextual (central coherence) understanding of everyday activities. Tassini et al. (2021) suggest that the drive for local processing, and the ensuing tendency to focus on a specific part of a situation, may lead to difficulties in understanding the bigger picture.

Thus, through the WCC lens, SS could be seen as providing explicit information that contributes toward a more meaningful understanding of everyday activities. However, if there is a bias, or strength, toward local processing (see Koldewyn et al., 2013; Scher Lisa and Shyman, 2019), it is unclear how SS can help audiences with overcoming that bias and understand the context from a more global perspective. Thus, whilst it is possible that SS could be used to highlight global aspects of a situation (such as the context), there is no empirical evidence exploring a link between WCC and SS and such a rationale has been criticized for having “no real theoretical engagement” (Bawazir and Jones, 2017, p. 533).

Gray’s first and second versions (10 and 10.1, Gray, 2015) of her criteria specifically advise on the inclusion of perspective sentences that are aimed to make explicit the perspective of others. The impact of perspective sentences in SS, specifically on improving the audience’s adaptive behaviors, was investigated by Okada et al. (2008) who found the inclusion of perspective sentences had little to no impact on improving target behaviors. Thus, the use or inclusion of perspective sentences is not explicitly detailed in Gray’s 10.2 (Gray, 2018) and 10.3 (Gray, 2021) guidelines. Rather, the 10.3 guidelines (Gray, 2021) emphasize further the need for the authors to recognize the uniqueness of every human experience and perspective. Perspective-taking is alluded to, through the term ‘social humility,’ and aims to ensure that the authors, rather than the audience, make a concerted effort to see to potential difficulties related to misunderstandings in perception. Thus, there seems to have been an increase in emphasis on the concept of mutual misunderstanding. This is reflected in the evolution of the SS criteria. Initially, Gray and Garand (1993) and Gray (2015) urged authors to “improve their understanding of the audience” (Criteria 10.1). In the latest criteria, the term “Social Humility” is included specifically in the first criterion (10.3. Gray, 2021). Thus, whilst the notion of acknowledging mutual misunderstandings of both the author and audience of the SS has always informed Gray’s (1998) philosophy, in a later version of her criteria (10.3) the term “Social Humility” takes center stage.

The heightened focus and explicit use of the term “Social Humility” in SS criteria is also suggestive of the shift in the ontological account of autism literature; from a view of a social communication deficit underpinned by poor ToM (medical model) to a social relational (see Thomas, 2004) view of a shared misunderstanding of social communication (between autistic and non-autistic communication partners). Specifically, the (medical model) view that an autistic person’s inability to read the subtext of a social situation is the central deficit in autism has been challenged by Milton (2012, 2014, 2017), who argues that ToM (also termed cognitive empathy; see Mazza et al., 2014), is bi-directional. Milton states that whilst autistic people may present with a particular way of experiencing the world, this alone does not explain the breakdown in reciprocity and mutual understanding between autistic and non-autistic individuals. Milton (2012) proposes the ‘double empathy problem’, whereby poor mutual understanding is not due to autistic cognition alone (see Crompton et al., 2020). Rather, it is resulting from difficulties with reading each other’s minds. That is, autistic and non-autistic individuals who are interacting both have difficulties ‘reading’ their autistic or non-autistic counterparts. Milton (2017) refers to this phenomenon as a mismatch of salience. Hence, it is argued that “the problem is just as much one for the non-autistic person as for the person with autism” (Chown, 2014, p. 1672). This view, that interaction takes two, was investigated further by Brewer et al. (2016), Edey et al. (2016), and also by Heasman and Gillespie (2018), who indicated that mental states and expressions of autistic people were poorly recognized by non-autistic individuals.

Social learning and constructivist explanations

Bawazir and Jones (2017) attempt to explain the mechanisms behind SS by using Bandura’s (1971) social learning theory, in terms of learning experiences that involve observation, modeling, reinforcement, and the consequent cognitions that emerge from these learning experiences. Bawazir and Jones (2017) argue that the development and delivery of SS activate the components necessary for observational learning to occur (i.e., maintaining attention, retaining information, motoric reproduction, and motivation). Thus, they maintain that the changes in the responses of autistic individuals, as a result of using SS, could be explained by using social learning theory.

More recently, Riga et al. (2021) have suggested that social stories rely on Social Constructivism theory. Social Constructivism is a theory proposed by Vygotsky (1968) which suggests that knowledge is socially constructed through interaction with others and the use of socio-cultural tools. From a Social Constructivist perspective, a SS could be considered a socio-cultural tool that provides opportunities for creating shared meaning. Riga et al. (2021) argue that SS create opportunities for meaningful interaction between the author and the audience, and thus help in constructing shared understanding.

Social constructionism and social learning theory emphasize two different aspects of learning. The former refers to the construction of knowledge through collaboration whilst the latter the acquisition of new behaviors through modeling and imitation. Interestingly, both theories advocate for the importance of interactive and social elements in the learning or transmission of knowledge. Thus, in utilizing such theoretical frameworks, both Bawazir and Jones (2017) and Riga et al. (2021) allude to the importance of active engagement between the author and the audience of the SS intervention.

Further proposed theoretical explanations

Rowe (1999) utilizes the Piagetian notion of schemas (or schemata) to explain the SS mechanism. Schemas are mental units of understanding. They are representations of experiences that can be organized into complex relationships with one another, which then contribute toward a person’s view of the world. According to Rowe (1999), SS can help the audience to scaffold a schema which they have not developed yet. Or it can help to make an implicit schema explicit. Thus, by sharing factual information about an event or experience (i.e., by scaffolding a schema), a SS can help increase an autistic person’s understanding of a situation.

As SS have been used to retell an event that has occurred in the autistic child’s life, and also to link an individual’s experience to their future planning, the role of episodic memory has been highlighted by Hutchins and Prelock (2018). The authors suggest that SS can support a rich personal recall of an event to ensure a greater impact of the intervention.

Reynhout and Carter (2011) provide the only review of the theoretical rationales underpinning SS to date. Besides proposing ToM and WCC (also referred to as ‘strong specific coherence’) theories, Reynhout and Carter (2011) also propose executive functioning, stimulus over-selectivity (also referred to as ‘strong stimulus selectivity’), visual learning style, and language comprehension as perceptual and cognitive characteristics that may shed light on potential mechanisms underpinning SS. Specifically, they suggest that SS may support executive functions (which are mental control processes such as inhibition, working memory, cognitive flexibility, and planning) in autistic individuals and as a result, support planning and organization. Reynhout and Carter also suggest that autistic individuals may have a tendency for “strong stimulus selectivity.” This is the tendency to attend to one specific aspect of a situation or stimulus rather than seeing the “whole field” (Reynhout and Carter, 2011, p. 373). Thus, according to the authors, SS can make aspects of the situation which are necessary for a complete understanding of the situation more explicit.

A visual learning style (or preference) in autism has also been proposed by Reynhout and Carter (2011) to explain how visually cued instruction and written language in SS could serve as instructional supports. Furthermore, the explicit direction given, in terms of ‘who, what, when, where and why,’ may also contribute toward compensation for difficulties in language comprehension by making important information more prominent.

Finally, Reynhout and Carter (2011) discuss a behavioral explanation which is informed by antecedents and consequences. This behavioral explanation proposes that SS develop “loose contingency contracts” (p. 375). The authors argue that SS make antecedents and consequences of behaviors explicit. In other words, SS could be explained in terms of rules and patterns, where SS provide clear links between behaviors and their subsequent outcomes: i.e., if you do this activity, then you will get this outcome. Reynhout and Carter (2011) also draw on Demiri’s (2004) hypothesis on how SS rely on the use of rule-governed behavior to learn to respond appropriately in a given situation without having previous experience of dealing with similar contingencies in the past. From this perspective, SS highlight the appropriate antecedent events that signal that a particular behavior is necessary. The effectiveness of such contingencies approach is explored by Bradley and Noell (2021) who investigated the effectiveness of an intervention program designed to promote the acquisition and generalization of rule-governed social skills in autistic children. They argue that “rule-governed behavior allows individuals to behave according to contingencies that may not be explicitly stated or that have never been contacted directly” (Bradley and Noell, 2021, p. 2). Social stories, from this perspective, also indicate how the audience is expected to respond whilst also specifying the consequences or outcomes that will occur as a result of the audience’s response. This, according to Reynhout and Carter (2011) “would seem to be an attractively parsimonious explanation” (p. 376).

Novel theoretical rationale for social stories: rule-based perspectives

Taken together, there are two elements to the proposed theoretical accounts for SS thus far which have been highlighted repeatedly: (1) make implicit social information explicit, and (2) actively involve both communication partners. To inform further the SS mechanism discussion, strengths-based perspectives should be seriously considered. A strengths-based approach to understanding autism emphasizes strengths, or preference, in explicit deliberative analytical thinking. Thus, a theoretical account of SS that embraces collaborative and explicit perspectives as well as emphasizes rule-based thinking may be useful within the SS mechanisms debate, and also for addressing the double empathy problem.

A strength in rule-governed processing has been proposed to characterize autism. Theoretical accounts of deliberative analytical thinking strengths in autism, such as the Dual Process Theory of Autism (Brosnan et al., 2016, 2017; Lewton et al., 2019; Ashwin and Brosnan, 2020; Brosnan and Ashwin, 2022a,b), may also be a potential framework for understanding the mechanisms underpinning social stories. The Dual Process Theory of Autism proposes an autistic preference for slower, explicit, deliberative, analytical processing (compared with a non-autistic preference for rapid, implicit, intuitive processing). Deliberative analytical processing incorporates the mental manipulation of cause-effect relationships (Crespi, 2021). A specific form of processing cause-effect relationships has been termed ‘systemizing’, which is the drive to construct rule-based systems that function in an ‘if-and-then’ manner. When systemizing, the input is ‘IF,’ the operation is ‘AND,’ and the output becomes ‘THEN’ (Baron-Cohen and Lombardo, 2017). Systemizing is therefore a specific example of a rule-based perspective that may be useful for understanding the mechanisms underpinning SS.

What is systemizing?

Systemizing is a drive to (1) analyze the variables in a system, (2) derive the underlying rules that govern the behavior of a system, and (3) construct systems that allow for prediction to occur (Baron-Cohen et al., 2003, 2009; Baron-Cohen, 2021). Here, a system is defined as something that takes inputs, which can then be operated in variable ways, to deliver different outputs in a rule-governed way. Baron-Cohen et al. (2003) propose at least six kinds of systemizing domains – Technical, Natural, Abstract, Social, Organizable, and Motoric – which share this same underlying if-and-then process of pattern-seeking.

Baron-Cohen and Lombardo (2017) propose a ‘systemizing mechanism’ that undertakes the three key cognitive processes of input (IF), operation (AND), output (THEN). They also speculate on the neural basis of systemizing. Rather than one discrete neural ‘module,’ they propose a number of neurological processes which could explain the higher-than-average rule-seeking drive of autistic individuals. These are: a heightened initial low-level processing of sensory/perceptual input, a heavily biased attentional process that promotes a detail-oriented focus, and a motivational component that impacts an individual’s drive to understand systems, involving the brain’s reward system through dopaminergic neurons.

Systemizing is common amongst all humans (Wakabayashi et al., 2007; Kidron et al., 2018; Naor-Ziv et al., 2021; Van Der Zee and Derksen, 2021). Evidence suggests that autistic individuals have higher levels of systemizing than non-autistic individuals (Baron-Cohen, 2006). An intense search for structure is termed ‘hyper-systemizing’ (Baron-Cohen et al., 2009; Van Der Zee and Derksen, 2017). Hyper-systemizing also refers to a tendency to be change-resistant whilst also presenting with a cognitive style that is apt at law-based pattern recognition (Baron-Cohen and Lombardo, 2017). The hyper-systemizing process can be informed by greater attention to detail (Baron-Cohen et al., 2009; Brosnan et al., 2012). Rutherford and Subiaul’s (2016) study, which focuses on the motivational urge for systemizing, indicated that autistic children showed a stronger explanatory drive than non-autistic children. The authors use the term “explanatory drive” to explain an individual’s desire to explain ambiguity and to explain how systems work.

The evidence for hyper–systemizing being characteristic in autistic individuals is increasing, as established in Van Der Zee and Derksen’s (2021) narrative literature search, which provides a comprehensive overview of the state of research on systemizing. The authors also suggest that measuring an individual’s systemizing abilities could be useful in distinguishing between autistic individuals and non-autistic individuals (also see Baron-Cohen et al., 2003; Wheelwright et al., 2006; Auyeung et al., 2009).

Discussion

Systemizing and social stories

Many everyday experiences can be described in terms of input-operation-output systems that can be lawfully explained. The examples in Box 2 are potentially causal patterns that can be inferred in terms of a rule or law. Each of these examples pertains to a different class of systems; the first is the natural system, the second can be considered a social system, whilst the third is a numeric system. In all three, cases, when all three elements line up, the system repeats itself and a causal pattern is observed (Baron-Cohen and Lombardo, 2017).

BOX 2. Example of if-and-then systems.

Such If-And-Then patterns can be found in examples of SS. This can be seen in the SS presented in Box 3. In this SS, the goal is to share information about a novel event in preparation for when new clothes will be bought. The SS in Box 3 consists of two causal patterns; one building on the other. Another example of causal patterns in SS is in Box 4, which consists of eight causal patterns. This story focuses on supporting understanding within a social domain by sharing information on ‘greeting someone.’ Neither story is written using strict if-and-then vocabulary. Rather, they are stories that meet Gray’s SS criteria in that they are positively written, have a patient tone, are guiding and not directing, and are balanced in terms of having an introduction, body and conclusion. The analysis described in Box 4, which was carried out post hoc, highlights the causal patterns (in input-operation-output format) in the story. It evidences a number of “qualifiers” such as “if,” “may,” “try,” “more,” and “or.” Such words contribute to a more authentic description of the theme in question, and are used to communicate qualities of the causal patterns; such as the frequency of the pattern, as well as different iterations of the pattern. Therefore, the analyses in Boxes 3, 4 highlight how SS may be utilizing causal patterns, to engage more effectively the audience’s exploratory drive.

BOX 3. Example of a social story, focusing on sharing information about novel events, analyzed in terms of if-and-then patterns.

The social story in Box 3 has been taken from The New Social Story Book, by C. Gray, p. 35. Copyright 2010 Carol Gray.

BOX 4. An example of a social story, focusing on sharing information about a greeting, analyzed in terms of if-and-then patterns.

The social story in Box 4 has been taken from The New Social Story Book, by C. Gray, p. 83. Copyright 2010 Carol Gray.

Thus, by following Gray’s Criteria, particularly in relation to (1) ensuring that SS answer ‘where,’ ‘when,’ ‘who,’ ‘what,’ ‘how’ and ‘why’ questions, and (2) focusing on describing over directing through more descriptive than coaching sentences (Gray, 2010, 2015), SS can offer more opportunities for highlighting the if-and-then (input-operation-output) framework described by Baron-Cohen and Lombardo (2017) whilst emphasizing knowledge and mental processes. As a result, it can be argued that SS can (1) offer opportunities for more efficient systemizing by providing the necessary information for the if-and-then process to ensue, and (2) offer opportunities for systemizing through observation (i.e., by observing an operation unfolding in the story, as illustrated in Box 2).

Recent SS research suggests that SS can be useful for increasing understanding (as Gray originally suggested) whilst also reducing anxiety in autistic children (Smith et al., 2020, 2021). This increase in understanding is consistent with the notion of how rule-based strengths of autistic individuals (e.g., systemizing) can be underpinning the SS mechanism, as SS are a means to communicate and identify rules and causal patterns. Increased understanding may therefore serve to also increase a sense of certainty in predicting future events as described by the SS. Autism is associated with elevated levels of intolerance of uncertainty, which mediates between autistic traits and anxiety (Boulter et al., 2014; Vasa et al., 2018; Jenkinson et al., 2020). It has recently been suggested that social situations are difficult for autistic individuals precisely because of their inherently uncertain nature (rather than their social nature per se, Berkay and Jenkins, 2022).

Thus, SS can help to predict patterns and reduce uncertainty. However, due to their dynamic, complex, and uncertain nature, some social communicative acts (such as detecting the ironic intent of a speaker or negotiating eye contact) may not be predictable. In fact, there is no evidence (see Kokina and Kern, 2010; Camilleri et al., 2021) that suggests that SSs could be used successfully in contexts which are so dynamic. Similarly, Baron-Cohen (2002, p. 248) also states that “systemizing is of almost no use when it comes to predicting moment-by-moment changes in a person’s behavior.” Nevertheless, SSs could still be potentially used to target social communicative goals which are less dynamic. They could also be used to target aspects (or elements) of these processes which could be more predictable. Furthermore, it is unlikely that one story alone can explain the complexity of a social communicative act. As highlighted in Gray’s criteria (see Gray, 2021), ideally a number of complementary social stories should be used (Criterion 10: Mix and Match to Build Concepts) to help develop further the understanding of complex and dynamic concepts.

Hence, it is possible that systemizing is a mechanism underpinning SS, as the SS encourages authors to explicitly share information that is informed by the audience’s propensity for systemizing. This may, in turn, increase certainty in the autistic individual with respect to the content of the SS which reduces their level of anxiety (as an elevated intolerance of uncertainty relates to greater anxiety). In this way, SS can be seen as a mechanism of support for autism that builds upon autistic strengths in systemizing, rather than addressing weaknesses in ToM (or cognitive empathy). If the author constructs the SS with the rule-based communication preferences of their audience in mind, a systemizing account of SS embraces Gray’s principle of ‘social humility’ to address Milton’s ‘alignment of salience’ that characterizes the double empathy problem.

Social stories and the double empathy problem

The original definition of SS given by Gray and Garand (1993) highlights the process of sharing information between audiences and authors. By recognizing that in using a SS as a tool to communicate accurate information, there is also the recognition that there are two people involved in the exchange. Gray (1998) argues that it is important for authors to acknowledge that the perspectives of non-autistic people and the perspectives of autistic people could be different. The social stories that are developed, usually by parents or practitioners, are developed by first gathering information that helps improve the authors’ understanding of the audience in relation to the goal to be reached. This first informs the author who is trying to understand the current situation from the audience’s perspective (i.e., trying to walk in the autistic person’s shoes). Gray (2021) recommends that authors engage with the audience to gather their thoughts, feelings, and perspectives, and subsequently use this knowledge to inform the stories. This process, as described by Gray (2021), can be the process with which the double empathy problem, as described by Milton, is addressed. In this manner, the story development process helps create a platform from which the audience’s (i.e., the autistic person) perspective is better understood. This joint effort can be completed through verbal or nonverbal interchanges, and should focus on using various strategies for engagement and the sharing of information between non-autistic and autistic individuals. Structuring this joint effort in an explicit, logical and systematic way can help to mediate between the non-autistic and the autistic individual’s communication preferences, and in doing so potentially reducing what Milton (2012, 2017) refers to as a mismatch of salience between the autistic and non-autistic individual.

SS are typically written by non-autistic authors, for autistic audiences. If systemizing tends to be higher in autistic individuals compared to non-autistic individuals, it may be that non-autistic authors need to adjust more than the autistic audience to present SS in an if-and-then format. This would enable SS to support relative autistic strengths in systemizing. If-and-then patterns can enable autistic individuals, as well as non-autistic individuals, to analyze objects or events in terms of small units and enable focus on one detail (i.e., the input) at a time. Furthermore, the SS provides clear information on how the input is transformed, operationalized, or manipulated (i.e., the operation). Finally, the SS will also highlight the outcome of the operation on the input (i.e., the output). In this manner, a SS could be seen to create a meaningful and accurate if-and-then pattern through which both the author and the audience may infer and operationalize a rule about the world around them. In this way, the SS process (i.e., the development and delivery of the SS) could be harnessing the autistic individual’s strong explanatory drive (Rutherford and Subiaul, 2016), the autistic individual’s preference for structured patterns (Strathearn et al., 2018), and the autistic individual’s capacity for systemizing (Van Der Zee and Derksen, 2021).

Future research

We recommend that future SS research emphasizes investigating two aspects highlighted in this article: (1) if-and-then patterns in social stories, and (2) social humility in the development (or co-development) of social stories. SS research can include analysis of stories in terms of if-and-then patterns or systems to ascertain if social stories that highlight such patterns are more likely to result in positive outcomes when compared to social stories which do not highlight such patterns.

As highlighted in this article, the function of systemizing is to identify laws, rules and/or regularities that govern a system in order to understand how that system works and to predict what it will do (Baron-Cohen and Lombardo, 2017). However, accurate prediction is not always possible, especially because some everyday situations (particularly social situations) contain high levels of uncertainty. However, SS (as described in Box 4) can comprise causal hypotheses, in the form of classic logic (i.e., that each proposition has a truth value of either “true” or “false,” but not both; see Pijnacker et al., 2009) whilst also hypothesizing scenarios with “extra information” (i.e., information that could invalidate or challenge the original causal relationship). Future research could focus on investigating if SS with multiple causal patterns, compared to SS with little to no causal patterns, can support further understanding of complex and dynamic everyday social situations and if they can help mitigate the exceptions encountered in real-world social situations.

The extent to which perceived uncertainty is a mediating factor addressed by SS can also be explored. Many aspects of social communication and interaction do not have lawful regularities that are amenable to identifying a single input to operate on, and are inherently uncertain (Berkay and Jenkins, 2022). SS may reduce uncertainty in a situation, however, Baron-Cohen (2002) states how systemizing is not effective in anticipating the fluctuations in someone’s “moment-to-moment” (p. 248) behavior. Speculatively, this may relate to SS being more effective for preparation for novel and predictable events, than for facilitating communication (e.g., Hutchins and Prelock, 2013). Golan and Baron-Cohen (2006) found that whilst emotion recognition could be developed through systemizing, there was limited broader generalization beyond what was explicitly learned. Thus, it may be that SS effectiveness is related to lawfulness of the situation, and may have limited generalization. This may be a way for informing when SS are, and are not, likely to be effective.

Furthermore, if systemizing is a drive as well as a cognitive process (Baron-Cohen and Lombardo, 2017), and a strength in rule-based systemizing is underpinning the SS support tool, individuals who are apt systemizers should benefit more from utilizing SS. Thus, measures such as the Adolescent Systemizing Spectrum Quotient (Auyeung et al., 2012) could be utilized to investigate if high systemizing scores can predict more positive SS research outcomes. Other measures for the identification of rule-based strengths, such as Pacini and Epstein’s (1999) Rational Experiential Inventory (as used in Dual Process Theories, see Brosnan et al., 2016) could also be utilized to investigate the relationship between possible aptitudes for rule-based deliberative analytical thinking styles and SS effectiveness. Future research can explore if systemizing relates to effectiveness in some categories of SS relative to other categories of SS (e.g., support learning a new skill vs. assisting in novel events/transitions) or if deliberative analytical thinking generally relates to SS effectiveness, rather than systemizing specifically.

The importance of social humility for the development of SS should also be investigated further. Importantly, the author may have to adapt to the style of the audience. This paper argues that SS can reconcile the diverse style of communication of autistic and non-autistic individuals and thus mediate the double-empathy problem. Thus, it is recommended for research to investigate if SS which have been mutually developed or co-constructed, to various degrees, together by autistic and non-autistic authors are more effective than stories that are developed solely by non-autistic authors. Autistic individuals self-delivering SS that they have self-developed (for example using the co-developed SOFA-app.org) also represents a potentially fascinating avenue of future neuroaffirmative research.

Conclusion

This article argues that the investigation of outcomes of SS has taken priority over the investigation of the psychological processes underpinning the intervention. However, the identification of these mechanisms should be given more importance as it could locate more effectively issues and questions related to the effectiveness of the support tool, whilst contributing further to the issue of variability in outcomes research. In light of this, the article sheds light on some of the strengths and limitations of the theoretical mechanisms that have been proposed so far, and argues against those that are based on social-deficit ontologies of autism, as they are lacking a practical, philosophical and empirical foundation.

This article takes a neuroaffirmative approach toward the use of the SS support tool and SS research. This approach challenges the use of SS as a means to “reduce inappropriate behaviors.” The labeling of autistic behaviors as inappropriate by others (typically non-autistic others) that require reduction can be seen as stemming from a deficit model which implies that disability is a result of within-person factors which can be changed. In this article, we argue that disability is a result of the interaction of the person with the environment. From this perspective, SS could be used to reduce behaviors that autistic individuals themselves would like to reduce or support autistic individuals to interact with their environment. Consequently, we suggest that SS be used as Carol Gray originally intended, as a means to share accurate information effectively between author and audience.

This article reviews the growing evidence of the tendency for autistic individuals to prioritize rule-based thinking as a way of better understanding the world. This contributes to a strengths-based account of SS. Importantly, this article does not propose changing how SS are written, nor moving away from Gray’s 10 criteria. Rather, it propositions that (1) rule-based communication preferences of the audience are kept in mind when developing stories, and that (2) causal if-and-then patterns are used to analyze further the SS.

Finally, whilst the underlying mechanisms of SS are still speculative, and also, not necessarily mutually exclusive (i.e., more than one process may inform SS); at this stage, we propose that SS can encourage both the author and the audience to jointly analyze experience and events in terms of if-and-then patterns. Thus, a SS can provide the opportunity to address the double-empathy problem by highlighting a common if-and-then ruled-based structure to develop a shared understanding. Thus, the SS tool can serve as a support between non-autistic and autistic people. This is an efficient and parsimonious account of SS, which warrants further investigation.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

All procedures performed in this study were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee.

Author contributions

All authors contributed to the conceptualization of the study. LJC wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^The way autism is, or should be, described is lacking consensus (Botha et al., 2021). However, autistic adults (Taboas et al., 2023) and autistic advocates (Sinclair, 2013) prefer identify-first language (i.e., autistic person/individual), over person-first language (i.e., person/individual with autism). Thus, in this paper identity-first language is used.

References

Acar, C., Tekin-Iftar, E., and Yikmis, A. (2017). Effects of mother-delivered social stories and video modeling in teaching social skills to children with autism Spectrum disorders. J. Spec. Educ. 50, 215–226. doi: 10.1177/0022466916649164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Almutlaq, H., and Martella, R. (2018). Teaching elementary-aged students with autism Spectrum disorder to give compliments using a social story delivered through an iPad application. Int. J. Special Educ. 33, 482–492. Available at: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ1185588

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5 Task Force (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5™. 5th Edn American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.

Google Scholar

Ashwin, C., and Brosnan, M. (2020). “The dual process theory of autism” in Thinking, reasoning, and decision making in autism. eds. K. Morsanyi and R. Byrne (New York: Routledge), 13–38.

Google Scholar

Auyeung, B., Allison, C., Wheelwright, S., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2012). Brief report: development of the adolescent empathy and systemizing quotients. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 2225–2235. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1454-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Auyeung, B., Wheelwright, S., Allison, C., Atkinson, M., Samarawickrema, N., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). The Children’s empathy quotient and systemizing quotient: sex differences in typical development and in autism Spectrum conditions. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 39, 1509–1521. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0772-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1971). Social learning theory. Morristown: Prentice-Hall.

Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 248–254. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01904-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). The hyper-systemizing, assortative mating theory of autism. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 30, 865–872. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.01.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S. (2021). The pattern seekers: How autism drives human invention. New York: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Ashwin, C., Tavassoli, T., and Chakrabarti, B. (2009). Talent in autism: hyper-systemizing, hyper-attention to detail and sensory hypersensitivity. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 364, 1377–1383. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., and Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 21, 37–46. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., and Lombardo, M. (2017). Autism and talent: the cognitive and neural basis of systemizing. Autism Spectr. Disord. 19, 345–353. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2017.19.4/sbaroncohen

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan, N., and Wheelwright, S. (2003). The systemizing quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high–functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 358, 361–374. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bawazir, R., and Jones, P. (2017). A theoretical framework on using social stories with the creative arts for individuals on the autistic Spectrum. Int. J. Med. Health Sci. 11, 533–541. doi: 10.5281/zenodo.1132555

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkay, D., and Jenkins, A. C. (2022). A role for uncertainty in the neural distinction between social and nonsocial thought. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 18, 491–502. doi: 10.1177/17456916221112077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Botha, M., Hanlon, J., and Williams, G. L. (2021). Does language matter? Identity-first versus person-first language use in autism research: a response to Vivanti. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 53, 870–878. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04858-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boulter, C., Freeston, M., South, M., and Rodgers, J. (2014). Intolerance of uncertainty as a framework for understanding anxiety in children and adolescents with autism Spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 1391–1402. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-2001-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, R. L., and Noell, G. H. (2021). Rule-governed behavior: teaching social skills via rule-following to children with autism. Dev. Neurorehabil. 25, 433–443. doi: 10.1080/17518423.2021.2018735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewer, R., Biotti, F., Catmur, C., Press, C., Happé, F., Cook, R., et al. (2016). Can Neurotypical individuals read autistic facial expressions? Atypical production of emotional facial expressions in autism Spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 9, 262–271. doi: 10.1002/aur.1508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brosnan, M., and Ashwin, C. (2022a). Differences in art appreciation in autism: a measure of reduced intuitive processing. J. Autism Dev. Disord. doi: 10.1007/s10803-022-05733-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brosnan, M., and Ashwin, C. (2022b). Thinking, fast and slow on the autism spectrum. Autism :136236132211324. doi: 10.1177/13623613221132437

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brosnan, M., Ashwin, C., and Lewton, M. (2017). Brief report: intuitive and reflective reasoning in autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 47, 2595–2601. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3131-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brosnan, M. J., Gwilliam, L. R., and Walker, I. (2012). Brief report: the relationship between visual acuity, the embedded figures test and systemizing in autism Spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 2491–2497. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1505-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brosnan, M., Lewton, M., and Ashwin, C. (2016). Reasoning on the autism spectrum: a dual process theory account. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 46, 2115–2125. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2742-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camilleri, L. J., Maras, K., and Brosnan, M. (2021). Autism Spectrum disorder and social story research: a scoping study of published, peer-reviewed literature reviews. Rev. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 9, 21–38. doi: 10.1007/s40489-020-00235-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camilleri, L. J., Maras, K., and Brosnan, M. (2022). The impact of using digitally-mediated social stories on the perceived competence and attitudes of parents and practitioners supporting children with autism. PLoS One 17:e0262598. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0262598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, R. (2020). “Defining neurodiversity for research and practice” in Neurodiversity studies: a new critical paradigm. eds. H. B. Roqvist, N. Chown, and A. Stenning (London: Routledge), 218–220.

Google Scholar

Chown, N. (2014). More on the ontological status of autism and double empathy. Disability Soc. 29, 1672–1676. doi: 10.1080/09687599.2014.949625

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cihak, D. F., Kildare, L. K., Smith, C. C., McMahon, D. D., and Quinn-Brown, L. (2012). Using video social stories™ to increase task engagement for middle school students with autism spectrum disorders. Behav. Modif. 36, 399–425. doi: 10.1177/0145445512442683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Constantin, A., Johnson, H., Smith, E., Lengyel, D., and Brosnan, M. (2017). Designing computer-based rewards with and for children with autism spectrum disorder and/or intellectual disability. Comput. Hum. Behav. 75, 404–414. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.05.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Constantin, A., Pain, H., and Waller, A. (2013). Informing the design of an authoring tool for developing social stories. In Human-Computer Interaction–INTERACT 2013: 14th IFIP TC 13 International Conference, Cape Town, South Africa, September 2–6, 2013, Proceedings, Part II 14546–553). Berlin Heidelberg.

Google Scholar

Crespi, B. (2021). Pattern unifies autism. Front. Psych. 12:59. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.621659

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crompton, C. J., Ropar, D., Evans-Williams, C. V., Flynn, E. G., and Fletcher-Watson, S. (2020). Autistic peer-to-peer information transfer is highly effective. Autism 24, 1704–1712. doi: 10.1177/1362361320919286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demiri, V. (2004). Teaching social skills to children with autism using social stories: An empirical study. PhD diss, Hofstra University, New York.

Google Scholar

Dodd, S., Hupp, S. D. A., Jewell, J. D., and Krohn, E. (2008). Using parents and siblings during a social story intervention for two children diagnosed with PDD-NOS. J. Dev. Phys. Disabil. 20, 217–229. doi: 10.1007/s10882-007-9090-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dwyer, P. (2022). The neurodiversity approach(es): what are they and what do they mean for researchers? Hum. Dev. 66, 73–92. doi: 10.1159/000523723

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edey, R., Cook, J., Brewer, R., Johnson, M. H., Bird, G., and Press, C. (2016). Interaction takes two: typical adults exhibit mind-blindness towards those with autism spectrum disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 125, 879–885. doi: 10.1037/abn0000199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, U. (1989).Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.

Google Scholar

Gillespie-Lynch, K., Kapp, S. K., Brooks, P. J., Pickens, J., and Schwartzman, B. (2017). Whose expertise is it? Evidence for autistic adults as critical autism experts. Front. Psychol. 8:438. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golan, O., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Systemizing empathy: teaching adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism to recognize complex emotions using interactive multimedia. Dev. Psychopathol. 18, 591–617. doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, C. A. (1998). “Social stories and comic strip conversations with students with Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism” in Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism?. Current issues in autism. eds. E. Schopler, G. B. Mesibov, and L. J. Kunce (Boston: Springer), 167–198.

Google Scholar

Gray, C. (2010). The new social story book. Arlington: Future Horizons.

Google Scholar

Gray, C. (2015). Comparison of social stories™ 10.0–10.2 criteria. Available at: https://carolgraysocialstories.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/09/Social-Stories-10.0-10.2-Comparison-Chart.pdf (Accessed February 27, 2022)

Google Scholar

Gray, C. (2018). Social stories™ 10.2. Available at: https://carolgraysocialstories.com/wp-content/uploads/2018/12/Social-Stories-10.2-Criteria.pdf (Accessed February 27, 2022)

Google Scholar

Gray, C. (2021). Social stories™ 10.3. Available at: https://carolgraysocialstories.com/wp-content/uploads/2021/10/Social-Stories-10.3-Criteria.pdf (Accessed February 27, 2022)

Google Scholar

Gray, C. (2022). Origins of the social story™ philosophy – Carol Gray – social stories. Carol Gray – Social stories. Available at: https://carolgraysocialstories.com/social-stories/origins-of-the-social-story-philosophy/ (Accessed February 27, 2022)

Google Scholar

Gray, C. A., and Garand, J. D. (1993). Social stories: improving responses of students with autism with accurate social information. Focus Autistic Behav. 8, 1–10. doi: 10.1177/108835769300800101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, V. A., Pituch, K. A., Itchon, J., Choi, A., O’Reilly, M., and Sigafoos, J. (2006). Internet survey of treatments used by parents of children with autism. Res. Dev. Disabil. 27, 70–84. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2004.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanrahan, R., Smith, E., Johnson, H., Constantin, A., and Brosnan, M. (2020). A pilot randomised control trial of digitally-mediated social stories for children on the autism spectrum. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 50, 4243–4257. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04490-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Happé, F. (2000). “Parts and wholes, meaning and minds: central coherence and its relation to theory of mind” in Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience. eds. S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, and D. J. Cohen (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 203–221.

Google Scholar

Happé, F., and Frith, U. (1995). “Theory of mind in autism” in Learning and cognition in autism (Boston, MA: Springer), 177–197.

Google Scholar

Happé, F., and Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 5–25. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heasman, B., and Gillespie, A. (2018). Perspective-taking is two-sided: misunderstandings between people with Asperger’s syndrome and their family members. Autism 22, 740–750. doi: 10.1177/1362361317708287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, K. L., Morrier, M. J., Heflin, L. J., and Ivey, M. L. (2008). Autism treatment survey: services received by children with autism spectrum disorders in public school classrooms. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 961–971. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0470-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howley, M., and Arnold, E. (2005). Revealing the hidden social code: Social stories for people with autistic spectrum disorders. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

Google Scholar

Hsieh, M.-Y., Lynch, G., and Madison, C. (2018). Intervention techniques used with autism spectrum disorder by speech-language pathologists in the United States and Taiwan: a descriptive analysis of practice in clinical settings. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 27, 1091–1104. doi: 10.1044/2018_AJSLP-17-0039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutchins, T. L., and Prelock, P. A. (2013). Parents’ perceptions of their children’s social behavior: the social validity of social StoriesTM and comic strip conversations. J. Posit. Behav. Interv. 15, 156–168. doi: 10.1177/1098300712457418

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutchins, T. L., and Prelock, P. A. (2018). Using story-based interventions to improve episodic memory in autism Spectrum disorder. Semin. Speech Lang. 39, 125–143. doi: 10.1055/s-0038-1628365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, R., Milne, E., and Thompson, A. (2020). The relationship between intolerance of uncertainty and anxiety in autism: a systematic literature review and meta-analysis. Autism 24, 1933–1944. doi: 10.1177/1362361320932437

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kagohara, D. M., Achmadi, D., van der Meer, L., Lancioni, G. E., O’Reilly, M. F., Lang, R., et al. (2013). Teaching two students with Asperger syndrome to greet adults using social stories™ and video modeling. J. Dev. Phys. Disabil. 25, 241–251. doi: 10.1007/s10882-012-9300-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kapp, S. (2019). How social deficit models exacerbate the medical model: autism as case in point. Autism Policy Pract. 2, 3–28. Available at: https://openaccessautism.org/index.php/app/article/view/16

Google Scholar

Kidron, R., Kaganovskiy, L., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2018). Empathizing-systemizing cognitive styles: effects of sex and academic degree. PLoS One 13:e0194515. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0194515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klett, L. S., and Turan, Y. (2012). Generalized effects of social stories with task analysis for teaching menstrual care to three young girls with autism. Sex. Disabil. 30, 319–336. doi: 10.1007/s11195-011-9244-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokina, A., and Kern, L. (2010). Social story™ interventions for students with autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 40, 812–826. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0931-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koldewyn, K., Jiang, Y. V., Weigelt, S., and Kanwisher, N. (2013). Global/local processing in autism: not a disability, but a disinclination. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 2329–2340. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1777-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurt, O., and Kutlu, M. (2019). Effectiveness of social stories in teaching abduction-prevention skills to children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 49, 3807–3818. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04096-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: the origins of "theory of mind.". Psychol. Rev. 94, 412–426. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.94.4.412

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewton, M., Ashwin, C., and Brosnan, M. (2019). Syllogistic reasoning reveals reduced bias in people with higher autistic-like traits from the general population. Autism 23, 1311–1321. doi: 10.1177/1362361318808779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malmberg, D. B., Charlop, M. H., and Gershfeld, S. J. (2015). A two experiment treatment comparison study: teaching social skills to children with autism Spectrum disorder. J. Dev. Phys. Disabil. 27, 375–392. doi: 10.1007/s10882-015-9420-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazza, M., Pino, M., Mariano, M., Tempesta, D., Ferrara, M., De Berardis, D., et al. (2014). Affective and cognitive empathy in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:791. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00791

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milton, D. (2012). On the ontological status of autism: the ‘double empathy problem’. Disabil. Soc. 27, 883–887. doi: 10.1080/09687599.2012.710008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milton, D. (2014). Autistic expertise: a critical reflection on the production of knowledge in autism studies. Autism 18, 794–802. doi: 10.1177/1362361314525281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milton, D. (2017). A mismatch of salience: Explorations of the nature of autism from theory to practice. West Sussex: Pavilion Press.

Google Scholar

Naor-Ziv, R., Bernaz Lavi, A., and Glicksohn, J. (2021). Empathizing-systemizing: from style to intention. Personal. Individ. Differ. 179:110922. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.110922

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noens, I. L. J., and Van Berckelaer-Onnes, I. A. (2008). The central coherence account of autism revisited: evidence from the ComFor study. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2, 209–222. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2007.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okada, S., Ohtake, Y., and Yanagihara, M. (2008). Effects of perspective sentences in social stories™ on improving the adaptive behaviors of students with autism Spectrum disorders and related disabilities. Educ. Train. Dev. Disabil. 43, 46–60. Available at: https://okayama.elsevierpure.com/en/publications/effects-of-perspective-sentences-in-social-stories-on-improving-t

Google Scholar

Pacini, R., and Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of rational and experiential information processing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76, 972–987. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pane, H. M., Sidener, T. M., Vladescu, J. C., and Nirgudkar, A. (2015). Evaluating function-based social stories™ with children with autism. Behav. Modif. 39, 912–931. doi: 10.1177/0145445515603708

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pijnacker, J., Geurts, B., van Lambalgen, M., Kan, C. C., Buitelaar, J. K., and Hagoort, P. (2009). Defeasible reasoning in high-functioning adults with autism: evidence for impaired exception-handling. Neuropsychologia 47, 644–651. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynhout, G., and Carter, M. (2006). Social stories™ for children with disabilities. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 445–469. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0086-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynhout, G., and Carter, M. (2011). Social stories™: a possible theoretical rationale. Eur. J. Spec. Needs Educ. 26, 367–378. doi: 10.1080/08856257.2011.595172

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riga, A., Ioannidi, V., and Papayiannis, N. (2021). Social stories and digital literacy practices for inclusive education. Eur. J. Special Educ. Res. 7. 119–141. doi: 10.46827/ejse.v7i2.3773

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, C. (1999). The Stanley Segal award: do social stories benefit children with autism in mainstream primary schools? Br. J. Special Educ. 26, 12–14. doi: 10.1111/1467-8527.t01-1-00094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, G., Kapp, S. K., Elliott, D., Elphick, C., Gwernan-Jones, R., and Owens, C. (2019). Mapping the autistic advantage from the accounts of adults diagnosed with autism: a qualitative study. Autism Adulthood 1, 124–133. doi: 10.1089/aut.2018.0035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutherford, M., and Subiaul, F. (2016). Children with autism spectrum disorder have an exceptional explanatory drive. Autism 20, 744–753. doi: 10.1177/1362361315605973

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Safi, M. F., Alnuaimi, M., and Sartawi, A. (2022). Using digital social stories to improve social skills in children with autism: a pilot experimental single-subject study. Advances in Autism. 8, 243–251. doi: 10.1108/aia-02-2021-0013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scher Lisa, J., and Shyman, E. (2019). Challenging weak central coherence: a brief exploration of neurological evidence from visual processing and linguistic studies in autism Spectrum disorder. Annal. Behav. Neurosci., 2, 136–143. doi: 10.18314/abne.v2i1.1606

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinclair, J. (2013). Why I dislike “person first” language. Autonomy 1, 2–3. Available at: https://blogs.exeter.ac.uk/exploringdiagnosis/files/2017/03/Sinclair-Why-I-Dislike-First-Person-Language.pdf

Google Scholar

Singer, J. (2016). Neurodiversity: The birth of an idea. Kindle Edn. Amazon Digital Services, LLC: Judy Singer S. C. V. Tassini, M. C. Melo, O. F. A. Bueno, and C. B. de Mello (2022) Weak central coherence in adults with ASD: Evidence from eye-tracking and thematic content analysis of social scenes. Appl. Neuropsychol. Adult. 1–12.

Google Scholar

Smith, E., Constantin, A., Johnson, H., and Brosnan, M. (2021). Digitally-mediated social stories support children on the autism Spectrum adapting to a change in a ‘real-World’ Context. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 51, 514–526. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04558-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, D. L., and Gillon, G. T. (2004). Autistic spectrum disorder: caseload characteristics, and interventions implemented by speech-language therapists. Kairaranga 5, 46–54. doi: 10.54322/kairaranga.v5i2.271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, E., Toms, P., Constantin, A., Johnson, H., Harding, E., and Brosnan, M. (2020). Piloting a digitally-mediated social story intervention for autistic children led by teachers within naturalistic school settings. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 75:101533. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2020.101533

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strathearn, L., Kim, S., Bastian, D. A., Jung, J., Iyengar, U., Martinez, S., et al. (2018). Visual systemizing preference in children with autism: a randomized controlled trial of intranasal oxytocin. Dev. Psychopathol. 30, 511–521. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417001018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Styles, A. (2011). Social stories: does the research evidence support the popularity? Educ. Psychol. Pract. 27, 415–436. doi: 10.1080/02667363.2011.624312

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taboas, A., Doepke, K., and Zimmerman, C. (2023). Preferences for identity-first versus person-first language in a US sample of autism stakeholders. Autism 27, 565–570. doi: 10.1177/13623613221130845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tassini, S., Cardoso Melo, M., Mello, C., and Bueno, O. (2021). Weak central coherence in adults with ASD: Evidence from eye-tracking and thematic content analysis of social scenes.

Google Scholar

Thomas, C. (2004). Rescuing a social relational understanding of disability. Scand. J. Disabil. Res. 6, 22–36. doi: 10.1080/15017410409512637

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Timmins, S. (2016). Successful social StoriesTM for young children with autism: Growing up with social StoriesTM Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

Google Scholar

Urbanowicz, A., Nicolaidis, C., Houting, J. D., Shore, S. M., Gaudion, K., Girdler, S., et al. (2019). An expert discussion on strengths-based approaches in autism. Autism Adulthood 1, 82–89. doi: 10.1089/aut.2019.29002.aju

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Der Zee, E., and Derksen, J. (2017). Identifying autism through empathizing and systemizing abilities. Soc. Sci. 6:124. doi: 10.3390/socsci6040124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Der Zee, E., and Derksen, J. J. L. (2021). The power of systemizing in autism. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 52, 321–331. doi: 10.1007/s10578-020-01014-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vasa, R. A., Kreiser, N. L., Keefer, A., Singh, V., and Mostofsky, S. H. (2018). Relationships between autism spectrum disorder and intolerance of uncertainty. Autism Res. 11, 636–644. doi: 10.1002/aur.1916

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L.S. (1968). Thought and language (newly revised, translated, and edited by Alex Kozulin). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Wakabayashi, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Uchiyama, T., Yoshida, Y., Kuroda, M., and Wheelwright, S. (2007). Empathizing and systemizing in adults with and without autism Spectrum conditions: cross-cultural stability. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 1823–1832. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0316-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westra, E., and Carruthers, P. (2018). Theory of mind. Encyclopedia Evol. Psychol. Sci., 1–7. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2376-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheelwright, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Goldenfeld, N., Delaney, J., Fine, D., Smith, R., et al. (2006). Predicting autism Spectrum quotient (AQ) from the systemizing quotient-revised (SQ-R) and empathy quotient (EQ). Brain Res. 1079, 47–56. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Windgassen, S., Goldsmith, K., Moss-Morris, R., and Chalder, T. (2016). Establishing how psychological therapies work: the importance of mediation analysis. J. Ment. Health 25, 93–99. doi: 10.3109/09638237.2015.1124400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2004). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems. Alphabetical index (Vol. 3). World Health Organization.

Google Scholar

Wright, B., Marshall, D., Adamson, J., Ainsworth, H., Ali, S., Allgar, V., et al. (2016). Social stories™ to alleviate challenging behaviour and social difficulties exhibited by children with autism spectrum disorder in mainstream schools: design of a manualised training toolkit and feasibility study for a cluster randomised controlled trial w. Health Technol. Assess. 20, 1–258. doi: 10.3310/hta20060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism, Social Stories™, theoretical account, psychological mechanism, rule-based, systemizing, double empathy problem

Citation: Camilleri LJ, Maras K and Brosnan M (2023) A rule-based theoretical account of social stories to address the double empathy problem. Front. Psychol. 14:1085355. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1085355

Received: 31 October 2022; Accepted: 22 May 2023;
Published: 14 June 2023.

Edited by:

Mary Elizabeth Stewart, Heriot-Watt University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Metehan Kutlu, YüzüncüYıl University, Türkiye
Alina Simona Rusu, Babeș-Bolyai University, Romania

Copyright © 2023 Camilleri, Maras and Brosnan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Louis John Camilleri, lc2295@bath.ac.uk; louis.j.camilleri@um.edu.mt

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.