Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 27 February 2023
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement
This article is part of the Research Topic Behavior and Self-Similarity between Nano and Human Scales: From T-pattern and T-string Analysis (TPA) with THEME to T-Societies View all 12 articles

New insights into the behavioral structure of Pikler educators: An application of T-pattern detection and analysis with THEME

  • 1Department of Developmental Psychology and Education, University of the Basque Country UPV/EHU, Bilbao, Spain
  • 2Department of Psychobiology and Methodology of Health Sciences, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Cerdanyola del Vallès, Spain
  • 3Faculty of Psychology, Institute of Neurosciences, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain

Introduction: The recent generalization of early childhood schooling has given rise to a need for the development of rigorous, specific training programs aimed at early education professionals (0–3 years of age). This work emanates from the unique nature of Pikler-Lóczy education as a reference for early childhood education centers, and its expertise in providing quality care in an everyday classroom situation. The aim of the study is to use T-pattern detection and analysis, within the framework of observational methodology, to identify relevant aspects of the choreography followed by Pikler educators during breakfast, and thus provide substantial material with a view to creating a training plan.

Methods: Two expert educators in Pikler-Lóczy education were directly and systematically observed in their own classrooms, following a nomothetic, follow-up and multidimensional design. The observation instrument underwent a molarization process so that the detected elements would be more significant and understandable for novice educators.

Results: Using THEME, the invariant aspects of the educators’ behavior were analyzed. The significance level for the critical interval was the THEME default (α = 0.005). The results show: on the one hand, changes expected in T-patterns in accordance with the observation instrument molarity; and on the other hand, stability in terms of the comparison between the two educators.

Discussion: These results help us to identify the complex structure of the breakfast choreography, and how educators establish interaction with children. In addition to specific issues pertaining to Pikler-Lóczy education, the conclusions highlight the advantages of using T-pattern analysis within the framework of observational methodology, in order to dissect a mealtime routine in its natural context, and explain components of early childhood education intervention that satisfy basic childhood needs. This constitutes a starting point from which to generate instructive material for the training of professionals working in early education.

1. Introduction

Over the last few decades, early childhood schooling (0–3 years of age) has grown exponentially to become a generalized reality (European Commission, 2019). This has given rise to an increase in studies centered on the assessment of educational programs and interventions in nursery schools (Kaufman et al., 2015; Escolano-Pérez, 2020). A number of these studies warn of poor quality in such centers, especially in terms of educator-child interaction (Turnbull et al., 2009; Durlak, 2010; Hallam et al., 2016). This is largely due to the fact that this educational phase lacks its own identity (Herrán et al., 2014) for two reasons: on the one hand it has inherited educational traditions from successive cycles, introducing content that does not fit with the specific characteristics of early childhood (Sarmento and Ruiz, 2011); and, on the other hand the job is carried out in an intuitive, affectionate, maternal way, neglecting the professional dimension of daily tasks (Herrán, 2013; Falk, 2018a). Considering the proven effect of early experiences on human psychological development (Bick and Nelson, 2017), more and more authors are calling for a system rooted in the natural circumstances of infant daily life (Duncan and Magnuson, 2013; McLachlan et al., 2018), that helps to fulfill their early developmental needs (Csibra and Gergely, 2009; Bonawitz et al., 2011; Hallam et al., 2016; Tomasello, 2016; Salcuni et al., 2017). However, there are few rigorous training programs aimed at educators and early education professionals that provide a specific guide as to how to carry out everyday activities in the infant classroom, as well as how to assess progress in the performance of these activities.

Previous works have justified the interest and unique nature of the Pikler-Lóczy education (PikLEd) system as a reference for early education centers covering the 0–3 age group (Tardos, 2007; McCall et al., 2010; Herrán, 2013, 2018; Herrán et al., 2014; Salamon and Harrison, 2015). The PikLEd is an institutional childcare proposal exclusive to early childhood. It is based on over 70 years of practical and professional experience in the Lóczy Institute of Budapest (Pikler, 1968, 1971; David and Appell, 1986, 2010; Falk, 2002), which began as a foster home (1946–2011) but later became a nursery school (from 2006 to the present day). Assuming that an early experience of competence is crucial for the future shaping of the psyche (Bruner, 1972, 1973; Wallon, 1980; Pikler, 2018), this model revolves around two fundamental axes of everyday infant life: daily care and autonomous infant activity. It entails a set of original action protocols, known as choreographies, that transform everyday activities (for example, breakfast) into an educational field (Belza et al., 2019, 2020). The authors who have gone deeper into analyzing the practice of PikLEd have highlighted its relevance as a responsive care system (Hevesi, 2018; Pikler, 2018; Falk, 2018b) that is truly paedocentric (Falk, 2008; David and Appell, 2010; Herrán, 2013; Tardos and David, 2018; Vincze, 2018), and which facilitates infant socialization without violence (Kàllò, 2014, 2016; Pikler, 2018).

Over the last few years, a number of studies have provided new knowledge about PikLEd through the application of observational methodology (OM; Belasko et al., 2019, 2022; Belza et al., 2019, 2020; Sagastui et al., 2020, 2022). OM allows us to assess spontaneous behavior in its everyday context (Anguera, 1979, 2003; Portell et al., 2015). This methodological option offers valuable procedural resources for the study of daily life in the classroom and the adult-child relationships that are established therein (Anguera, 2001a). It also provides the objectivity and rigor essential for educational assessment, whilst at the same time affording the necessary flexibility for capturing the many, often complex processes of development and early learning in real life environments (Anguera, 2001b; Herrán, 2008; Early Head Start National Resource Center, 2013; Escolano-Pérez et al., 2017; Escolano-Pérez, 2020). OM offers guidelines for justifying and analyzing the quality of the records obtained using study dimensions and segmentation criteria of the spontaneous behavioral continuum in its natural context. Thus, OM involves the systematization of initial descriptive records – first dataset – through an observation instrument in order to obtain an equivalent code matrix – second dataset – that is built on the first (still qualitative data, where columns are dimensions/sub-dimensions, and rows are the successive units), which will be analyzed through specific quantitative techniques for categorical data. This way of connecting qualitative information and quantitative data has led to OM being considered as mixed-method in itself (Anguera and Hernández-Mendo, 2016; Anguera et al., 2017, 2018a; Onwuegbuzie and Johnson, 2021; Hitchcock and Onwuegbuzie, 2022).

Beneath the scope of OM, observational instruments were legitimized and applied, in order to capture details about the behavior of the expert PikLEd educator that facilitate the detection of two dimensional, instrumental and relational patterns in the daily performance of each everyday activity: breakfast, dressing and undressing, free play, etc (Belasko et al., 2019, 2022; Belza et al., 2019, 2020; Sagastui et al., 2020, 2022). This research shows that the habitual behavior of the expert PikLEd educator ensures the stability of the environment and the consistency of educational behaviors, these being understood as precise and adjusted ways of responding to the development of infant autonomy. Within the research line begun by Herrán (2013, 2014, 2016) and Herrán et al. (2014), important progress has been made in characterizing the action protocols or choreographies followed by these professionals in each educational field, thanks to the use of procedures designed to capture essential details of the sequence of educators’ instrumental actions and their connection with relational behaviors deployed with the child.

One of the everyday activities transformed into an educational field for PikLEd is breakfast. The systematic observation of this activity has revealed the sequence of basic actions that the educators repeat, as many times as necessary, so that the child has breakfast in a way that suits them, meaning that the child eats comfortably, calmly and quietly, in accordance with the hunger they feel at the time and their own perception of being full. These sequences are the detours (Wallon, 1985) they carry out in order to provide each child, at the table, with the utensils and food laid out on the countertop (Belza et al., 2019). The reiterative circularity of this instrumental sequence, together with the specific, unique and meaningful management of distances and postures (proxemic behavior), has shown that the expert PikLEd educator promotes the appearance of shared intentionality (Tomasello et al., 2005) in the development of mealtime routines (Belza et al., 2019). It is this shared intentionality that forms the basis of early cultural, instrumental and social learning (Tomasello, 2016). Furthermore, enlightening results have been obtained through analyzing the differentiated use of the utensils specific to the activity, along the lines of object pragmatics (Rodriguez and Moro, 1999) which views objects in terms of their social use and functions in everyday life, functions which children gradually appropriate thanks to adult-instigated mediation. Significant relationships were detected between various breakfast utensils with differing complexity and each child protagonist, showing that the educators adapt, and provide utensils depending on the level of autonomy and interest of each child (Belza et al., 2020).

The evidence provided by the aforementioned studies about the breakfast activity was made possible thanks to the diachronic analysis of observational data (Anguera et al., 2021); more specifically, lag sequential analysis (Bakeman and Gottman, 1989; Bakeman and Quera, 2011; Quera, 2018) and polar coordinate analysis (Sackett, 1980; Anguera, 1997). Lag sequential analysis detects regularities in behavior, making patterns of behavior visible, whereas polar coordinate analysis enables the vectorization of behavior, creating a complete map of interrelationships between behaviors. These techniques enabled the characterization of significant parts of the choreography, but not the choreography as a whole. A third very important technique for diachronic analysis in OM is based on the algorithm developed by Magnusson (1996, 2000) and used in THEME software, which makes the description and detection of complex real-time patterns possible. Its basic concept is the T-pattern, a statistical, hierarchical, self-similar (pseudo-fractal) pattern, recurring with significant translation symmetry since detected in human, animal, and brain network behavior (Casarrubea et al., 2015, 2018; Magnusson, 2020). T-pattern detection and analysis (TPA) with THEME has led to new insight into the structure of behavior. This third technique used for diachronic analysis in OM may be the best option for a complete characterization of the choreography. Until now, the use of TPA within the framework of OM, in the field of education, has made it possible: to identify patterns of academic persistency in pre-school age-groups (Santoyo et al., 2020), to examine the development of cognitive skills in infancy (Escolano-Pérez et al., 2019; Escolano-Pérez, 2020), to evaluate teaching-learning strategies in motor skills (Castañer et al., 2009), to assess the interaction and communication models of physical education teachers (García-Fariña et al., 2018; Camerino et al., 2019; Valero-Valenzuela et al., 2020), and to design and assess training programs focused on the professionals’ change in behavior (Portell et al., 2019; Sene-Mir et al., 2020).

Therefore, through the use of TPA within the framework of OM, it is possible to facilitate the design and evaluation of formative interventions of PikLEd aimed at novice educators; and this application would allow us to compensate for an important deficiency in the generalization of the PikLEd model. The Lóczy Institute in Budapest – now the Emmi Pikler nursery school – rigorously prepares its educators in situ, with periodical observations of their actions with the children which are then discussed by the pedagogic team as a whole (Kelemen, 2016). However, in other centers there is no training material for novice educators associated to evaluation guidelines, that would allow professionals to track the acquisition process of essential elements of the choreography (what to do) and non-verbal communication (how to do) inherent to PikLEd. The general aim of this research is to provide new insight into the choreography followed by the expert PikLEd educator, which is considered seminal for the creation of an innovative training plan that includes T-patterns as elements of a self-modeling strategy (Dowrick, 1999, 2012). Based on the main dimensions that have been established to structure the breakfast activity (Belza et al., 2019), the specific aims are: (1) to adapt the instrument Giving Breakfast in the Emmi Pikler Nursery School, GBEPNS (Belza, 2020) which increases the molarity of its instrumental and proxemic dimensions, in such a way as to make it easier for an educator who is not an expert in PikLEd to understand the training material created from its elements; (2) to identify invariant aspects that structure the expert educator’s behavior by analyzing the T-patterns from both an intra-individual perspective – between patterns obtained from the same expert educator when modifying the molarity of the observation system –, and an inter-individual perspective – between the behavior patterns of different expert PikLEd educators.

2. Materials and methods

This work forms part of a line of research into PikLEd developed under the scope of OM. In accordance with the design classification established in OM (Anguera et al., 2001), the study design as a whole is nomothetic, follow-up and multidimensional (N/F/M design), since the observation units are greater than one; there are two educators, to be exact. However, it is worth qualifying that, for each educator, the design used is ideographic given that the behavior of each one was observed and recorded as an independent unit, with an intra-session and inter-session follow-up over a 3 month period; and is multidimensional since it includes different criteria in the observation instruments used to record and code behavior (I/F/M design). This study applied systematic, direct, non-participative observation.

2.1. Participants

Eligibility criteria were established for the educators so as to increase the probability of the behavioral records obtained being representative of the PikLEd model. These criteria are: (1) having done their professional training in the Lóczy foster home itself, under the direction of the pediatrician Emmi Pikler; and (2) having actively taken part in the continuous training programs organized by the institution. Furthermore, applying the logic of multiple case study repetition (Yin, 2014), this study seeks to integrate most of the school educators involved in the breakfast activity.

There are three classrooms in the Emmi Pikler nursery school in Budapest, with a maximum of 12 children and three educators in each one. Applying the aforementioned criteria, two educators were selected (henceforth, Educator-1 and Educator-2) who habitually manage breakfast in their groups. Various studies within the OM framework show that, despite working with a reduced number of participants, the intensive monitoring of their everyday activity makes it possible to collect a large quantity of data and extract insightful conclusions about the studied reality (Rodríguez-Medina et al., 2018; Belza, 2020; Lapresa et al., 2020; Sagastui et al., 2020; Arias-Pujol et al., 2022).

In terms of the groups, it is worth highlighting that this school endeavors to maintain the same three educators in each group in order to establish a close relationship with the children as well as to enable a continuous monitoring of the children. For this reason, the placing of children into groups is not done in accordance with the age criterion, but depending on the number of available places in each classroom and the characteristics of the children themselves. To be specific, Educator-1’s group was made up of four boys and eight girls between the ages of 26 and 47 months at the start of the observations. Educator-2’s group was made up of five girls and five boys aged between 20 and 36 months. As breakfast is an option offered to all the children, participation varied from session to session.

It was agreed with the school directors to film one session per week in each classroom first thing in the morning, from 8.30 to 9.30, over a 3 month period. As recordings are frequently made in the classrooms, both the educators and the children are familiar with the presence of a camera. This is undoubtedly a relevant aspect to consider in relation to reactivity (Behar and Riba, 1993).

A total of 28 sessions were filmed over 3 months, 11 in Educator-1’s classroom and 17 in that of Educator-2. The acceptance criteria for the sessions was established in line with the following aspects: (1) Observability of the educators and their activity: the camera follows them continuously from the moment the first child arrives to have breakfast until the last breakfast is cleared away; (2) Inter-session consistency (or fulfillment of formal minimums of situation and space homogeneity) – all the sessions correspond to the same two classrooms in which the activity takes place, mainly in the dining room area or in the adjoining lobby or bathroom; (3) Intra-session consistency (or temporal completeness of the criteria that define the session) – all the sessions must temporally cover the aim, which is to serve and complete breakfast within the established time slot. The following exclusion criteria were formulated: (1) Change of classroom for organizational or maintenance reasons; (2) Insufficient recording time to analyze the complete breakfast of at least one child. Despite 25 sessions being recorded, the application of this last criterion excluded three of Educator-2ʼs sessions from the analysis. Finally, the sample of analyzed breakfasts was made up of 10 sessions in Educator-1’s classroom and 12 sessions in that of Educator-2. Each session lasted between 10 and 40 min, depending on how many children had breakfast and what time each child began.

The research project was assessed and approved by the Committee for Ethics in Research and Teaching from the University of two of the authors (INA0139), after obtaining the informed consent of the two educators and gaining the permission of the authorities responsible for the Emmi Pikler nursery school in Budapest, in accordance with Law 41/2002 of patient autonomy. The data has been treated in line with the regulation included in RD1720/2007 of development of the Organic Law 15/1999 of Personal Data Protection and in Law 14/2007 of Biomedical Research.

2.2. Instruments

2.2.1. Observation instrument

The observation instrument used is an adaptation of the field format Giving Breakfast in the Emmi Pikler Nursery School, GBEPNS (Belza, 2015, 2020; Belza et al., 2019, 2020), an ad hoc creation (Anguera et al., 2007) to observe the habitual behavior of the expert PikLEd educators during breakfast. This instrument is made up of six dimensions grouped into two macro-dimensions: instrumental, associated with the material conditions of the specific activity of breakfast; and relational, appertaining to the interaction and communication that the educators establish with the children at different levels (paralinguistic, proxemic, kinesic, and verbal). Each dimension is displayed in a feature list type catalog of behaviors (Anguera et al., 2018b).

GBEPNS was developed for a very molecular level of description, which allows us to collect the smallest details of the educators’ behavior during breakfast, but which may hinder the subsequent explanation and interpretation of this behavior when it is dissected. In accordance with the aims of this study, the initial field format underwent a molarization process through which the catalogs of behaviors, spaces and objects (utensils in this case) were redefined and reordered, with a view to making the detected elements more significant and understandable for novice educators. In order to carry out this adaptation of GBEPNS, in its instrumental and proxemic dimensions (henceforth GBEPNS-IP), a dynamic process of initial formulation of tentative groups was followed, subsequently modified in line with an empirical-inductive strategy, and once again, consistent with a deductive strategy. A group of PikLEd and OM experts intervened in this process. Similar aspects were grouped together in relation to their educational significance and their transferability to other classrooms, maintaining the specifics in terms of behavior related to children. The result of this process of adaptation of the GBEPNS-IP instrument is referred to as GBEPNS-IPv2. Table 1 shows the structure of GBEPNS-IPv2 and its correspondence with GBEPNS-IP.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Dimensions, sub-dimensions, and catalogs of the behaviors/spaces/utensils of the GBEPNS-IPv2 instrument and their concordance with the catalogs of the GBEPNS-IP instrument.

It can be noticed that each dimension is broken down into three sub-dimensions so as to structure the elements and guarantee mutual exclusiveness. The instrumental dimension includes the spaces in which the activity takes place, the educator’s actions (what she does) and which utensils she uses. Proxemic behavior refers to how the educator uses the space, and covers two facets: the static character that refers to the choice of place and posture within a space; and the dynamic character that involves the set of movements inherent to the activity itself. In aiming to capture the interactive dynamic that the educator establishes with the children in her care, the static facet is divided into two criteria: that of solitary static which refers to posture or position; and that of static in relation to the child present with whom she can make contact or stay very close to without moving. Once the catalogs of behaviors, spaces and utensils for each sub-dimension had been compiled, a system of alphanumerical codes was created to enable the coding of each of these elements.

2.2.2. Recording and analysis software

The free software program HOISAN, version 1.6.31 (Hernández-Mendo et al., 2012, 2014) was used to record and code all the sessions based on the system established in the field format. This program enables the databases to be exported to MS-EXCEL. Thus, the record files can easily be transformed into the syntactic structure of other programs for data analysis. In this case, we used the program THEME 6 Edu (Magnusson, 2000) for data management and analysis, which is freely available for download.2

2.3. Procedure

From the observation instruments GBEPNS-IP and GBEPNS-IPv2 we obtained records of the 22 sessions. The basic unit in field format record is the configuration or co-occurrence of codes from different dimensions. The configuration is formed by as many codes – at most – as there are number of sub-dimensions. When a behavioral aspect changes, the next configuration begins, thus enabling an exhaustive record of behavioral flow. In this way, the maximum systematization of the record is achieved, which produces large code matrices. At the same time, the duration that includes information about both the position of the code within the sequence and its occurrence, was used as a primary record parameter (Anguera et al., 2001; Bakeman and Quera, 2011). This enables the detection and analysis of T-patterns (TPA).

The HOISAN computer program allows us to obtain and store a large quantity of data and subject them to quality control. The files with the record made in HOISAN were exported to MS-EXCEL to be prepared in accordance with the instrument GBEPNS-IPv2. In this way two new code matrices were obtained, one for each educator, which were transformed into the syntactic structure compatible with the THEME program. This program was used to analyze the data of Educator-1, obtained with GBEPNS-IP and with GBEPNS-IPv2; and those of Educator-2, obtained with GBEPNS-IPv2.

2.4. Data quality check

Both qualitative and quantitative quality control procedures were carried out on the data. Consensual concordance (Anguera, 1990) took place in the last stage of construction of the observation instrument. This procedure, that seeks to achieve convergence between two observers, is increasingly present in OM (Arana et al., 2016). To this end, two of the authors of this study jointly carried out record trials with 15% of the sample, to decide which code to assign to each of the observed behaviors. The definitions of the behaviors in the catalog were therefore shaped and nuanced, thus strengthening the observation instrument.

In terms of quantitative quality control of the data, Krippendorff (2013) alpha coefficient was calculated, both intra-observer and inter-observer, via the HOISAN program. Ten per cent of the sample was recorded twice at different times for comparison, and once more by an external observer. In both cases the coefficient value was satisfactory since the degree of agreement between the two observers was 0.89, whilst it was 0.92 between the two records at different times.

2.5. Data analysis

According to Magnusson (2016), the THEME software considers data as a series of points that represent the occurrence of events during a period of observation (in TPA the event refers to the configuration or chain linking of codes from each sub-dimension of the field format in each of the event-types). The number of appearances of an event-type, divided by the duration of the observation period, produces the mean probability of this event-type occurring in a given unit of time, which is later used as the reference probability for the detection algorithms. When THEME detects an occurrence of a followed by B within a critical interval, it generates a simple T-pattern (AB). The critical interval is a key concept in TPA, and it refers a particular relationship between pairs of events in a time series—for an extended explanation of this concept see Magnusson (2000) and Casarrubea et al. (2015, 2018). The occurrences of simple T-patterns become events that subsequently constitute focal event-types in the next detection level. This process is repeated upwards level by level, in search of critical interval relationships with the patterns detected in previous levels, in order to generate ever more complex T-patterns.

This study independently analyzed the three aforementioned databases – in each one, the included sessions were concatenated into one multi-sample base – using the same search and selection criteria parameters to compare the obtained T-patterns (Amatria et al., 2017; Pic et al., 2021; Pic and Jonsson, 2021). More specifically, the search parameters applied were (Magnusson, 2017): (1) an occurrence frequency greater than 3; (2) a significance level of 0.005 (0.5% probability that the critical interval was randomly produced); (3) a minimum pattern occurrence in 50% of the sample. TPA detection was validated by simulation, through data randomization with shuffled and rotated randomized versions of the data sets. We used the standard number of randomization-searches (5 runs per type). The only accepted patterns were those for which the concordance probability between the randomized data and the real data was zero.

Given that the aim of this study was to analyze individual patterns, it is necessary to operationalize the selection conditions to make them comparable. Since the aim is that the patterns help to characterize the structure of everyday educational behavior as a whole, three selection criteria were applied to the patterns detected by THEME: (1) maximum length (patterns that include the greatest quantity of event-type or configurations); (2) maximum level of hierarchical structuring; (3) greatest session time coverage.

3. Results

In relation to the behavior of Educator-1, 4,716 configurations or data points were analyzed, whilst 2,319 configurations were analyzed in the case of Educator-2. The number of configurations was maintained both with GBEPNS-IP, and with GBEPNS-IPv2. In line with the second specific aim, we present the results below, in two sub-sections that show the intra-individual and inter-individual comparisons between patterns.

3.1. Changes in the structure of Educator-1ʼs behavior with the GBEPNS-IP and GBEPNS-IPv2 instrument

Starting from the code matrix based on GBEPNS-IP and applying the parameters and criteria defined in the data analysis section, the obtained pattern, among the 133 detected, is shown in Figure 1. It can be seen that in this T-pattern there is a certain structuring of Educator-1ʼs behavior, made up of two sub-sets of simpler patterns: on the one hand, the pattern shows the hidden structure concerning the management of breakfast in the different sessions (takes an item from the cupboard, serves table 1, serves table 2, returns to the counter); and on the other hand, a highly recurrent pattern that reflects accompaniment when there is no action.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. T-pattern of Educator-1ʼs behavior obtained with GBEPNS-IP.

Figure 2 shows the pattern resulting from the application of the selection criteria for the 849 T-patterns detected with the same search parameters, when using GBEPNS-IPv2. The level of behavioral structuring in this case is visibly greater, above all in reference to the instrumental actions; and at the same time, it integrates the simple pattern no action, again demonstrating the high recurrence of this event. Furthermore, this complex T-pattern reveals the appearance of new educational behaviors, such as closing the activity with the gets child to use the napkin action.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. T-pattern of Educator-1ʼs behavior obtained with GBEPNS-IPv2.

3.2. Stability in the behavioral structure between Educator-1 and Educator-2 with the GBEPNS-IPv2 instrument

Using the same selection criteria as in the previous analysis, we obtained the pattern shown in Figure 3 of the 201 T-patterns detected in the behavior of Educator-2. In this pattern there are three event-type sub-sets, which, although differing slightly in how they are structured in relation to Educator-1ʼs T-pattern, they practically replicate its composition: chain linking of instrumental actions (serves, puts, moves, takes/leaves) that alternate movements (moves nearer, moves away) between the counter and the table; again, there are repetitions of the central sub-set of accompaniment when there is no action, and finally, the use of the napkin.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. T-pattern Educator-2ʼs behavior obtained with GBEPNS-IPv2.

4. Discussion

This study focused on two dimensions: one of instrumental behavior, and the other of the expert PikLEd educator’s non-verbal behavior during the breakfast activity. The relevance of both these dimensions, instrumental and proxemic, has been revealed in previous studies as backbones of behavior flow on two levels – physical/instrumental and human/relational (Belza et al., 2019; Belza, 2020). Furthermore, due to their high degree of perceptibility and relationship with reality, these dimensions offer better training opportunities for novice educators than others perceived in the GBEPNS instrument. It was expected that the detection of T-patterns would identify greater levels of structuring in everyday behavior compared to other diachronic analysis techniques, and thus provide an account of the choreography as a whole. Below we discuss the results obtained relating to Educator-1ʼs behavior, in order to compare the identifying and explanatory capacity of the two observation instruments, GBEPNS-IP and GBEPNS-IPv2. Subsequently, we explain the T-patterns of Educator-1 and Educator-2, obtained with GBEPNS-IPv2, in order to clarify the potential behavioral homogeneity between the two.

Firstly, we can see that the variability of event-types drops to almost half when GBEPNS-IPv2 is applied to the analysis of Educator-1ʼs behavior, compared with the use of GBEPNS-IP. This fact adds more consistency to the record and reduces the dismembering of the behavior by residual aspects, without prejudicing significant information, given that the versatility of the events compared to the total is still high. The amount of different event-types, interpreted as behavior variability, shows that the PikLEd expert constantly adjusts to each moment, circumstance and child (Kàllò, 2016; Hevesi, 2018). If one of the defining aspects of early education is the educators’ capacity to adapt their practice to the children’s basic needs (Durlak, 2010; Escolano-Pérez, 2020), then PikLEd prioritizes the establishment of a close, continued relationship between the educator and the children in her/his care. In this way, everyday interactions can be furnished with the particular and individualized traits that come from mutual knowledge and which are so necessary at early ages (Falk, 2008). Despite this variability, the obtained results relating to Educator-1 support the use of the GBEPNS-IPv2 instrument which, combined with TPA, has allowed us to discover a more complex everyday behavioral structure for the expert PikLEd educator. It also provides new aspects of the choreography during breakfast whilst integrating the findings of previous studies (Belza et al., 2019, 2020) – the basic sequences that instrumentally order this mealtime routine and its cultural-socializing character. Therefore, the results sustain the idea that THEME is effective in obtaining clarifying patterns of this intervention in its natural context – a nursery school classroom.

Secondly, the aim was to check whether applying the most molar GBEPNS-IPv2 instrument and the same analysis strategy with THEME would lead to Educator-1ʼs results being replicated in Educator-2. Before going on to compare the patterns obtained from both, we want to highlight differences in the two breakfast situations. Since the temporality of the sessions was marked by the children’s attendance, together with the desire and rhythm in the breakfast itself, the duration of the sessions was disparate. This means, for example, that Educator-2ʼs sample, although including a greater number of sessions than that of Educator-1, produced a lower quantity of data due to fewer children having breakfast and the sessions being shorter. However, this fact does not appear to impede the detection of a similar structure in the behavior of both educators. The two T-patterns obtained are made up of event-type symmetries and have similar characteristics: (1) Chain linking of instrumental actions for breakfast management, (2) Recurrent cycles of accompaniment without action, (3) Finalization using a napkin. These results provide evidence about the stability of everyday intervention at breakfast time in this school, in two senses: On the one hand, they add evidence about the detours (Belza et al., 2019) made by the educators from the counter where the utensils – that they move, put down or serve, moving closer to the children at the table – are kept, until they return to the counter. Furthermore, the T-patterns obtained illustrate that it is the proxemic dimension that characterizes this instrumental sequence; guaranteeing coherence between the physical and human elements that make up the early educational breakfast environment, and showing the relevance of the intentional use of the space when it comes to establishing educator-child relationships. The expert PikLEd educator’s choice of posture and distance is neither accidental nor improvised, but is a response to the level of intervention/interaction that she needs to establish for the joint action (Tomasello et al., 2005; Sagastui et al., 2022). On the other hand, that both educators should display such similar behavior is due to the fact that, since its beginnings as a foster home, the main aim of the Lóczy Institute has been to provide the emotional security necessary for the healthy psychological development of institutionalized infancy (Pikler, 1971). Such important acquisitions in the earliest years cannot be integrated into the personality being structured if the child does not carry out her/his experiences within the framework of an interpersonal system of stable, continuous relationships (Falk, 2008). To this end, the aspects that make up each care were detailed and examined, in order to organize them in the form of a choreography, and thus ensure the continuity and homogeneity in the daily tasks of the educators who interact with the children (Herrán, 2013).

On an educational level, two other components of this choreography stand out. The accompaniment without action pattern is where the educator sits on her chair, and from that position accompanies both the breakfast of the child sitting at the table next to, or opposite her, along with the rest of those present, including those who are already in the play area. This sub-set of event-types is a recurring loop that is inserted into the choreography. They are attention intervals dependent on the aspiring action of the children, which allows the educator to attend and respond to the requirements of each moment in an appropriate way, even preempting them, thus displaying responsive care (Hevesi, 2018; Pikler, 2018). Her presence increases as the session progresses; something which is logical given that most of the children have been served, are finishing or are already playing. Even so, the fact that this pattern is repeated throughout the session characterizes the delimited space known as “her place” – i.e., her chair, situated near to the breakfast table, between this and the counter – as a transitional space between one action and the next, or between one interaction and the next. This shows another way of organizing and regulating the activity through the use of the space. Finally, it is worth highlighting the appearance of a previously unknown action – the culmination of the breakfast, consisting in the use and placement of the napkin, which closes the activity. In fact, in the case of Educator-2 this action takes place in another sub-area of the dining room – the handrail or corridor that connects to the play area. Therefore, the napkin, which denotes a certain degree of acceptance of social norms or conventions by the child (Belza et al., 2020), marks another important transition: from the breakfast activity to the one that immediately follows, which is play. Once the basic, affective needs have been covered – dietary, emotional and adaptable to nursery school – the children can then unleash their abilities associated with autonomous activity and free play. According to Tardos (2014), attending school involves facing certain daily challenges on the children’s part. In this sense, breakfast constitutes an activity that supports the transition from home to school; each child receives a welcome and a personalized breakfast so that they feel expected and comfortable (Tardos, 2014; Belza, 2015). These transitions are fundamental in infant daily life in order to ensure security and welfare. The expert PikLEd educator prepares them in such a way as to create a solid framework in which children can integrate references that help them settle, find their place and build structure (Falk, 2008).

In short, what can be deduced from this work, in addition to the specific matters relating to PikLEd, are the advantages of using TPA within the OM framework to dissect an everyday activity in its natural context, and explain the components of early education intervention that satisfy basic infant needs. The results reveal a complex structure of the everyday behavior of these educators, which provides insightful information about the breakfast choreography, and how the expert PikLEd educators establish interaction with the children. This represents a starting point from which to generate instructive material for training early education professionals. In this sense, since the detected patterns integrate and structure various dimensions of instrumental and non-verbal behavior, they allow us not only to assess the result of such training, but also to create feedback during the process itself (Santoyo et al., 2020; Sene-Mir et al., 2020). Therefore, this work makes a significant contribution in terms of addressing the gap that exists in specific training programs for professionals working with the 0–3 age group; a gap that has been highlighted by various authors (Herrán et al., 2014; Escolano-Pérez, 2020).

As previously mentioned, one of the limitations of this study was the sample composition. Since the emphasis was on carrying out a detailed study of everyday interactions, the number of sessions was somewhat limited, in addition to its heterogeneity for enabling the analysis of the relationships and integration of the different dimensions of the educators’ behavior with each child in particular. To be exact, it would be interesting to be able to integrate the role of kinesic behavior into these patterns, so that instruction in responsive communicative abilities in early education could be added to the training plan.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Committee for Ethics in Research and Teaching from the University of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Acknowledgments

HB a postdoctoral researcher, contracted by the University of the Basque Country, UPV/EHU, gratefully acknowledges the funding received for a postdoctoral research stay at the Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, via grants for Spanish university system retraining for 2021–2023. Margarita Salas modality, financed by the Ministerio de Universidades and Next Generation EU. HB, MP, and MA gratefully acknowledge the support of a Spanish government sub-project: Integration ways between qualitative and quantitative data, multiple case development, and synthesis review as main axis for an innovative future in physical activity and sports research [PGC2018-098742-B-C31] (2019–2021) (Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades, Programa Estatal de Generación de Conocimiento y Fortalecimiento Científico y Tecnológico del Sistema I + D + i), that is part of the coordinated project New approach to research in physical activity and sport from a mixed methods perspective (NARPAS_MM) [SPGC201800X098742CV0]. Also, they gratefully acknowledge the support given by the Generalitat de Catalunya (call SGR-Cat to support the scientific activity of research groups in Catalonia) to their RESEARCH GROUP AND INNOVATION IN DESIGNS (GRID). Technology and multimedia and digital application to observational designs [2021 SGR 00718] (2022-2024). Finally, second and fourth authors gratefully acknowledge the support of a Spanish government project Integración entre datos observacionales y datos provenientes de sensores externos: Evolución del software LINCE PLUS y desarrollo de la aplicación móvil para la optimización del deporte y la actividad física beneficiosa para la salud [EXP_74847] (2023). Ministerio de Cultura y Deporte, Consejo Superior de Deporte and European Union.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

References

Amatria, M., Lapresa, D., Arana, J., Anguera, M. T., and Jonsson, G. K. (2017). Detection and selection of behavioral patterns using theme: a concrete example in grassroots soccer. Sports 5:20. doi: 10.3390/sports5010020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T. (1979). Observational typology. Qual. Quant. 13, 449–484.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T. (1990). “Metodología observacional [observational methodology]” in Metodología de la investigación en ciencias del comportamiento. eds. J. Arnau, M. T. Anguera, and J. Gómez (Murcia: Universidad de Murcia), 123–236.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T. (1997). “From prospective patterns in behavior to joint analysis with a retrospective perspective” in Colloque sur invitation Méthodologie d’analyse des interactions sociales (Paris: Sorbonne Université).

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T. (2001a). “Hacia una evaluación de la actividad cotidiana y su contexto: ¿presente o futuro Para la metodología? [towards an evaluation of everyday activity and its context: present or future for methodology? Lecture of admission to the Royal Europan Academy of doctors in 1999]” in Estrategias de Evaluación y Medición del Comportamiento en Psicología. eds. A. B. Ramírez and A. A. Ferrer (México: Instituto Tecnológico de Sonora y Universidad Autónoma de Yucatán), 11–86.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T. (2001b). Cómo apresar las competencias del bebé mediante una aplicación de la metodología observacional [How to capture the baby’s skills through an application of observational methodology]. Contextos Educativos 10, 13–34. doi: 10.18172/con.484

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T. (2003). “Observational methods (general)” in Encyclopedia of psychological assessment. ed. R. Fernández-Ballesteros, Vol. 2 (London: Sage), 632–637.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Blanco-Villaseñor, A., and Losada, J. L. (2001). Diseños Observacionales, cuestión clave en el proceso de la metodología observacional. [Observational designs, a key question in the observational methodology process]. Metodol. C. Comport. 3, 135–160.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Blanco-Villaseñor, A., Losada, J. L., and Portell, M. (2018a). Pautas Para elaborar trabajos que utilizan la metodología observacional [Guidelines for designing and conducting a study that applies observational methodology]. Annu. Psicol. 48, 9–17. doi: 10.1016/j.anpsic.2018.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Blanco-Villaseñor, A., Losada, J. L., Sánchez-Algarra, P., and Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2018b). Revisiting the difference between mixed methods and multimethods: is it all in the name? Qual. Quant. 52, 2757–2770. doi: 10.1007/s11135-018-0700-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Camerino, O., Castañer, M., Sánchez-Algarra, P., and Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2017). The specificity of observational studies in physical activity and sports sciences: moving forward in mixed methods research and proposals for achieving quantitative and qualitative symmetry. Front. Psychol. 8:2196. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2016). Avances en estudios observacionales en Ciencias del Deporte desde los mixed methods [Advances in mixed methods observational studies in sports sciences]. Cuadernos de Psicología del Deporte, 16, 17–30.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Magnusson, M. S., and Jonsson, G. K. (2007). Instrumentos no estándar [Non-standard instruments]. Avances en Medición 5, 63–82.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Portell, M., Hernández-Mendo, A., Sánchez-Algarra, P., and Jonsson, G. K. (2021). “Diachronic analysis of qualitative data” in The Routledge reviewer’s guide to mixed methods analysis. eds. A. J. Onwuegbuzie and R. B. Johnson (London: Routledge), 125–138. doi: 10.4324/9780203729434-12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arana, J., Lapresa, D., Anguera, M. T., and Garzón, B. (2016). Ad hoc procedure for optimizing agreement between observational records. An. de Psicol. 32, 589–595. doi: 10.6018/analesps.32.2.213551

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arias-Pujol, E., Martorell-Mestres, M., Blanch-Miralbell, J., Bertran-Bachs, N., and Anguera, M. T. (2022). Implementation and evaluation in low intensity intervention programs from the CONNECT perspective of mixed methods: application in a case of an autistic child. Front. Psychol. 13:998658. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.998658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakeman, R., and Gottman, J. M. (1989). Observación de la interacción: introducción al análisis secuencial [Observation of interaction: Introduction to sequential analysis]. Madrid: Ediciones Morata.

Google Scholar

Bakeman, R., and Quera, V. (2011). Sequential analysis and observational methods for the behavioral sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, doi: 10.1017/CBO9781139017343

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behar, J., and Riba, C. (1993). “Sesgos en metodología observacional [Biases in observational methodology]” in Metodología observacional en la investigación psicológica. ed. M. T. Anguera, Vol. 2 (Barcelona: P.P.U), 9–148.

Google Scholar

Belasko, M., Herrán, E., and Anguera, M. T. (2019). Dressing toddlers at the Emmi Pikler nursery school in Budapest: educator instrumental behavioral pattern. Eur. Early Child. Educ. Res. J. 27, 872–887. doi: 10.1080/1350293X.2019.1678928

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belasko, M., Herrán, E., and Anguera, M. T. (2022). Observation dressing in a collaborative or trusting environment: the right to participate in one’s own care at the Emmi Pikler nursery School in Budapest. Early Years, 1–16. doi: 10.1080/09575146.2021.2016652

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belza, H. (2015). “Comenzar el día en la escuela infantil: Observación de la educadora Pikler-Lóczy dando de desayunar [starting day at nursery school: systematic observation of the educator Pikler-Lóczy during breakfast]” in Nuevos retos en la investigación psicodidáctica. eds. E. Bernaras and M. López (Bilbao: UPV/EHU, Argitalpen Zerbitzua = Servicio Editorial), 98–113.

Google Scholar

Belza, H. (2020). El arte de dar de desayunar. Observación sistemática de la conducta de la educadora de la Escuela Infantil Emmi Pikler. Bilbao (SP): Universidad del País Vasco/Euskal Herriko Unibertsitatea. Available online at: http://hdl.handle.net/10810/50518

Google Scholar

Belza, H., Herrán, E., and Anguera, M. T. (2019). Early childhood education and cultural learning: systematic observation of the behaviour of a caregiver at the Emmi Pikler nursery school during breakfast. Infancia y Aprendiz. 42, 128–178. doi: 10.1080/02103702.2018.1553268

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belza, H., Herrán, E., and Anguera, M. T. (2020). Early childhood, breakfast, and related tools: analysis of adults’ function as mediators. Eur. J. of Psychol. Educ. 35, 495–527. doi: 10.1007/s10212-019-00438-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bick, J., and Nelson, C. A. (2017). Early experience and brain development. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 8:e1387. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonawitz, E., Shafto, P., Gweon, H., Goodman, N. D., Spelke, E., and Schulz, E. (2011). The double-edged sword of pedagogy: instruction limits spontaneous exploration and discovery. Cognition 120, 322–330. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruner, J. (1972). Nature and uses of immaturity. Am. Psychol. 27, 687–708. doi: 10.1037/h0033144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruner, J. (1973). Organization of early skilled action. Child Dev. 44, 1–96. doi: 10.2307/1127671

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camerino, O., Valero-Valenzuela, A., Prat, Q., Manzano, D., and Castañer, M. (2019). Optimizing education: a mixed methods approach oriented to teaching personal and social responsibility (TPSR). Front. Psychol. 10, 14–39. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casarrubea, M., Jonsson, G. K., Faulisi, F., Sorbera, F., Di Giovanni, G., Benigno, A., et al. (2015). T-pattern analysis for the study of temporal structure of animal and human behavior: a comprehensive review. J. Neurosci. Methods 239, 34–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.09.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casarrubea, M., Magnusson, M. S., Anguera, M. T., Jonsson, G. K., Castañer, M., Santangelo, A., et al. (2018). T-pattern detection and analysis for the discovery of hidden features of behaviour. J. Neurosci. Methods 310, 24–32. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2018.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castañer, M., Torrents, C., Anguera, M. T., Dinušová, M., and Jonsson, G. K. (2009). Identifying and analyzing motor skill responses in body movement and dance. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 857–867. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.3.857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Csibra, G., and Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 148–153. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

David, M., and Appell, G. (1986). La educación del niño de 0 a 3 años: Experiencia del Instituto Lóczy [The education of the child from 0 to 3 years: Experience of Lóczy institute]. Madrid: Narcea.

Google Scholar

David, M., and Appell, G. (2010). Lóczy. Una insólita atención personal [Lóczy: An extraordinary personal attention]. Barcelona: Octaedro.

Google Scholar

Dowrick, P. W. (1999). A review of self-modeling and related interventions. Appl. Prev. Psychol. 8, 23–39. doi: 10.1016/S0962-1849(99)80009-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dowrick, P. W. (2012). Self-modeling: expanding the theories of learning. Psychol. Sch. 49, 30–41. doi: 10.1002/pits.20613

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, G. J., and Magnuson, K. (2013). Investing in preschool programs. J. Econ. Perspect. 27, 109–132. doi: 10.1257/jep.27.2.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durlak, J. A. (2010). The importance of doing well in whatever you do: a commentary on the special section, “implementation research in early childhood education”. Early Child Res. Q. 25, 348–357. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Early Head Start National Resource Center (2013). Observation: The heart of individualizing responsive care. Washington, DC: Early Head Start National Resource Center at ZERO TO THREE.

Google Scholar

Escolano-Pérez, E. (2020). Intra-and inter-group differences in the cognitive skills of toddler twins with birth weight discordance: the need to enhance their future from early education. Sustainability 12:10529. doi: 10.3390/su122410529

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Escolano-Pérez, E., Herrero-Nivela, M. L., and Anguera, M. T. (2019). Preschool metacognitive skill assessment in order to promote educational sensitive response from mixed-methods approach: complementary of data analysis. Front. Psychol. 10:1298. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Escolano-Pérez, E., Herrero-Nivela, M. L., Blanco-Villaseñor, A., and Anguera, M. T. (2017). Systematic observation: relevance of this approach in preschool executive function assessment and association with later academic skills. Front. Psychol. 8:2031. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

European Commission (2019). Key data on early childhood education and Care in Europe, 2019 edition, Eurydice report. Luxembourg: Publications Office of the European Union.

Google Scholar

Falk, J. (2002). Mirar al niño: La escala de desarrollo Instituto Pikler (Lóczy) [Look at the child]. Buenos Aires: Ariana.

Google Scholar

Falk, J. (2008). La estabilidad para la continuidad y calidad de las atenciones y relaciones [Stability for continuity and quality of care and relationships]. In-fan-cia 80, 4–10.

Google Scholar

Falk, J. (2018a). “El hecho consciente en lugar de la instintividad. Sustitución eficaz de la relación madre-niño en la casa cuna [conscious act instead of instinct. Effective substitution of the mother-child relationship in the foster home]” in Claves de la Educación Pikler-Lóczy. Compilación de 20 Artículos Escritos por sus Creadoras. ed. E. Herrán (Budapest: Asociación Pikler-Lóczy de Hungría), 179–188.

Google Scholar

Falk, J. (2018b). “Los fundamentos de una verdadera autonomía en el niño pequeño [The foundations of true autonomy in the young child]” in Claves de la Educación Pikler-Lóczy. Compilación de 20 Artículos Escritos por sus Creadoras. ed. E. Herrán (Budapest: Asociación Pikler-Lóczy de Hungría), 89–114.

Google Scholar

García-Fariña, A., Jiménez-Jiménez, F., and Anguera, M. T. (2018). Observation of physical education teachers’ communication: detecting patterns in verbal behavior. Front. Psychol. 9:334. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallam, R. A., Fouts, H. N., Bargreen, K. N., and Hallam, K. P. (2016). Teacher-child interactions during mealtimes: observations of toddlers in high subsidy child care settings. Early Child. Educ. J. 44, 51–59. doi: 10.1007/s10643-014-0678-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernández-Mendo, A., Castellano, J., Camerino, O., Jonsson, G. K., Blanco-Villaseñor, A., Lopes, A., et al. (2014). Programas informáticos de registro, control de calidad del dato, y análisis de datos [Software for registration, data quality control, and data analysis]. Rev de Psicol del Deporte 23, 111–121.

Google Scholar

Hernández-Mendo, A., López-López, J., Castellano, J., Morales-Sánchez, V., and Pastrana-Brincones, J. L. (2012). Hoisan 1.2: Programa informático Para uso en metodología observacional [Hoisan 1.2: software for use in observational methodology]. Cuad de Psicol del Deporte 12, 55–78. doi: 10.4321/S1578-84232012000100006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrán, E. (2008). Observe the movement: construction of the field format “jumping in psychomotor education during the third year of life”. Rev de Psicodidactica 13, 33–43.

Google Scholar

Herrán, E. (2013). La educación Pikler-Lóczy. Cuando educar empieza por cuidar [Pikler-Lóczy education: when education starts with caregiving]. Revis. Latinoam. Educ. Infant. 2, 37–56.

Google Scholar

Herrán, E. (2014). Metodología Observacional en la Escuela infantil Emmi Pikler de Budapest [Video]. EHUtb. Bilbao: UPV/EHU. Available online at: https://ehutb.ehu.eus/video/61a5092630fdd1b1fe721ff0

Google Scholar

Herrán, E. (2016). Metodología observacional en la Escuela Infantil Pikler de Budapest [Observational methodology at Pikler nursery school from Budapest]. Revis. Latinoam. Educ. Infant. 5, 91–98.

Google Scholar

Herrán, E. (2018). Claves de la educación Pikler-Lóczy: Compilación de 20 artículos escritos por sus creadoras [keys to Pikler-Lóczy education: Compilation of 20 articles written by its creators]. Budapest: Asociación Pikler-Lóczy de Hungría.

Google Scholar

Herrán, E., Orejudo, S., Martínez de Morentín, J. I., and Ordeñana, M. B. (2014). Actitudes docentes y autonomía en Educación Infantil 0-2: un estudio exploratorio en la Comunidad Autónoma del País Vasco [educators’ attitudes and autonomy in childhood education age 0-2: an exploratory study in the autonomous Community of the Basque Country (BAC)]. Rev. de Educ. 365, 150–176. doi: 10.4438/1988-592X-RE-2014-365-268

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hevesi, K. (2018). “Verdadera cooperación con el bebé [True cooperation with babies]” in Claves de la Educación Pikler-Lóczy. Compilación de 20 Artículos Escritos por sus Creadoras. ed. E. Herrán (Budapest: Asociación Pikler-Lóczy de Hungría), 189–196.

Google Scholar

Hitchcock, J. H., and Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2022). The Routledge handbook for advancing integration in mixed methods research, 1st edition. London: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780429432828

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kàllò, E. (2014). Educar sin violencia [Video]. EHUtb. Bilbao: UPV/EHU. Available online at: https://ehutb.ehu.eus/video/6189523430fdd18e1c46faf7

Google Scholar

Kàllò, E. (2016). Sobre la unidad de los cuidados y la educación, una vez más [On the unity of care and education, once again]. Rev. Latinoam. Educ. Infant. 5, 15–20.

Google Scholar

Kaufman, M. J., Kaufman, S. R., and Nelson, E. C. (2015). Beginning together: reforming schools by investing in early childhood education. Schools 12, 133–149. doi: 10.1086/680698

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelemen, J. (2016). El trayecto profesional de una cuidadora pikleriana [The profesional career of a Pikler educator]. Rev. Latinoam. Educ. Infant. 5, 36–40.

Google Scholar

Krippendorff, K. (2013). Content analysis. An introduction to its methodology, 3rd edition. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Lapresa, D., Gutiérrez, I., Pérez-de-Albéniz, A., Merino, P., and Anguera, M. T. (2020). Teacher-student-task-interactions in a motor skills programme for an adolescent boy with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic observation study / Interacción profesor-alumno-tarea en un programa de desarrollo de capacidades motrices en un adolescente con TEA: un estudio de observación sistemática. Journal for the Study of Education and Development / Infancia y Aprendizaje 44, 553–585. doi: 10.1080/02103702.2020.1802148

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnusson, M. S. (1996). Hidden real-time patterns in intra-and inter-individual behavior: description and detection. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 12, 112–123. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.12.2.112

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnusson, M. S. (2000). Discovering hidden time patterns in behavior: t-patterns and their detection. Behav. Res. Meth. Instrum. Comput. 32, 93–110. doi: 10.3758/BF03200792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnusson, M. S. (2016). “Time and self-similar structure in behavior and interactions: from sequences to symmetry and fractals” in Discovering hidden temporal patterns in behavior and interaction: T-pattern detection and analysis with THEME™. eds. M. S. Magnusson, J. K. Burgoon, and M. Casarrubea (New York: Springer), 3–35.

Google Scholar

Magnusson, M. S. (2017). THEME 6 User’s manual, June 2017 edition. Reykjavik: PatternVision. Available online at: https://patternvision.com/products/theme/

Google Scholar

Magnusson, M. S. (2020). T-patterns, external memory and mass-societies in proteins and humans: in an eye-blink the naked ape became a string-controlled citizen. Physiol. Behav. 227:113146. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCall, R. B., Groark, C. J., Fish, L., Harkins, D., Serrano, G., and Gordon, K. (2010). A socioemotional intervention in a Latin American orphanage. Infant Ment. Health J. 31, 521–542. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLachlan, C., Marilyn, F., and Edwards, S. (2018). Early childhood curriculum: Planning, assessment and implementation, 3rd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, doi: 10.1017/9781108131810

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onwuegbuzie, A. J., and Johnson, R. B. (2021). The Routledge Reviewer’s guide to mixed methods analysis, 1st edition. London: Routledge, doi: 10.4324/9780203729434

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pic, M., and Jonsson, G. K. (2021). Professional boxing analysis with T-patterns. Physiol. Behav. 232:113329. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2021.113329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pic, M., Navarro-Adelantado, V., and Jonsson, G. K. (2021). Exploring playful asymmetries for gender-related decision-making through T-pattern analysis. Physiol. Behav. 236:113421. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2021.113421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pikler, E. (1968). Some contributions to the study of the gross motor development of children. J. Genet. Psychol. 113, 27–39. doi: 10.1080/00221325.1968.10533806

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pikler, E. (1971). Examinations on developmental and educational psychology in national methodological institute for infant care and education. Magyar Pszichol. Szemle 28, 46–51.

Google Scholar

Pikler, E. (2018). “La competencia del bebé [the baby’s competence]” in Claves de la Educación Pikler-Lóczy. Compilación de 20 Artículos Escritos por sus Creadoras. ed. E. Herrán (Budapest: Asociación Pikler-Lóczy de Hungría), 59–71.

Google Scholar

Portell, M., Anguera, M. T., Chacón-Moscoso, S., and Sanduvete-Chaves, S. (2015). Guidelines for reporting evaluations based on observational methodology. Psicothema 27, 283–289. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2014.276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portell, M., Sene-Mir, A. M., Anguera, M. T., Jonsson, G. K., and Losada, J. L. (2019). Support system for the assessment and intervention during the manual material handling training at the workplace: contributions from the systematic observation. Front. Psychol. 10:1247. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quera, V. (2018). “Analysis of interaction sequences” in The Cambridge handbook of group interaction analysis. eds. E. Brauner, M. Boos, and M. Kolbe (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 295–322. doi: 10.1017/9781316286302.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodriguez, C., and Moro, C. (1999). “El mágico número tres” in Cuándo los niños aún no hablan [the magic number three. When children don't talk yet] (Barcelona: Paidós)

Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Medina, J., Rodríguez-Navarro, H., Arias, V., Arias, B., and Anguera, M. T. (2018). Non-Reciprocal Friendships in a School-Age Boy with Autism: The Ties that Build? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 48, 2980–2994. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3575-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sackett, G. P. (1980). “Lag sequential analysis as a data reduction technique in social interaction research” in Exceptional infant. Psychosocial risks in infant-environment transactions. eds. D. B. Sawin, R. C. Hawkins, L. O. Walker, and J. H. Penticuff (New York: Brunner/Mazel), 300–340.

Google Scholar

Sagastui, J., Herrán, E., and Anguera, M. T. (2020). A systematic observation of early childhood educators accompanying young children’s free play at Emmi Pikler nursery school: instrumental behaviors and their relational value. Front. Psychol. 11:1731. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sagastui, J., Herrán, E., and Anguera, M. T. (2022). Adult intervention levels in young children’s free play: an observational study on how Pikler educators combine the instrumental and relational dimensions of their educational activity. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 37, 707–728. doi: 10.1007/s10212-021-00560-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salamon, A., and Harrison, L. (2015). Early childhood educators’ conceptions of infants’ capabilities: the nexus between beliefs and practice. Early Years 35, 273–288. doi: 10.1080/09575146.2015.1042961

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salcuni, S., Mazzeschi, C., and Capella, C. (2017). Editorial: the role of play in child assessment and intervention. Front. Psychol. 8:1098. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santoyo, C., Jonsson, G. K., Anguera, M. T., Portell, M., Allegro, A., Colmenares, L., et al. (2020). T-patterns integration strategy in a longitudinal study: a multiple case analysis. Physiol. Behav. 222:112904. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.112904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarmento, R., and Ruiz, E. (2011). Educación temprana: desarrollo normativo autonómico en la etapa 0-3 [Early education: regional regulatory development in the stage 0-3]. Rev. Educ. Inclusiva 4, 27–41.

Google Scholar

Sene-Mir, A. M., Portell, M., Anguera, M. T., and Chacón-Moscoso, S. (2020). Manual material handling training: the effect of self-observation, hetero-observational and intrinsic feedback on workers’ knowledge and behaviour. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:8095. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17218095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tardos, A. (2007). Bringing up and providing care for infants and toddlers in an institution. Budapest: Hungarian Pikler-Lóczy Association.

Google Scholar

Tardos, A. (2014). Dando de desayunar al llegar a la Escuela Infantil y empezar bien la mañana [Video]. EHUtb. Bilbao: UPV/EHU. Available online at: https://ehutb.ehu.eus/video/61a50bc630fdd1b20d139cf4

Google Scholar

Tardos, A., and David, M. (2018). “Del valor de la actividad libre del bebé en la elaboración del yo [on the value of the baby's free activity in the elaboration of one’s self]” in Claves de la Educación Pikler-Lóczy. Compilación de 20 Artículos Escritos por sus Creadoras. ed. E. Herrán (Budapest: Asociación Pikler-Lóczy de Hungría), 347–376.

Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. (2016). Cultural learning redux. Child Dev. 87, 643–653. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., and Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 675–691. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turnbull, K. P., Anthony, A. B., Justice, L. M., and Bowles, R. (2009). Preschoolers' exposure to language stimulation in classrooms serving at-risk children: the contribution of group size and activity context. Early Educ. Dev. 20, 53–79. doi: 10.1080/10409280802206601

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valero-Valenzuela, A., Gregorio-García, D., Camerino, O., and Manzano, D. (2020). Hybridization of the teaching personal and social responsibility model and Gamification in physical education. Apunt. Educ. Fis. y Deportes 141, 63–74. doi: 10.5672/apunts.2014-0983.es.(2020/3).141.08

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vincze, M. (2018). “Del biberón a la autonomía [from the bottle (baby’s bottle) to autonomy]” in Claves de la Educación Pikler-Lóczy. Compilación de 20 Artículos Escritos por sus Creadoras. ed. E. Herrán (Budapest: Asociación Pikler-Lóczy de Hungría), 207–232.

Google Scholar

Wallon, H. (1980). Psicología del niño. Una Comprensión Dialéctica Del Desarrollo Infantil [child psychology. A dialectical understanding of child development]. Madrid: Pablo del Río.

Google Scholar

Wallon, H. (1985). La Vida Mental [Mental Life]. Barcelona: Crítica.

Google Scholar

Yin, R. K. (2014). Case study research. Design and methods, 5th edition. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Keywords: Pikler-Lóczy education, observational methodology, T-pattern, THEME, observation instrument, early childhood

Citation: Belza H, Portell M, Herrán E and Anguera MT (2023) New insights into the behavioral structure of Pikler educators: An application of T-pattern detection and analysis with THEME. Front. Psychol. 14:1081785. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1081785

Received: 07 November 2022; Accepted: 08 February 2023;
Published: 27 February 2023.

Edited by:

Hamidreza Namazi, Monash University Malaysia, Malaysia

Reviewed by:

Miguel Pic, South Ural State University, Russia
Carlos Santoyo, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico

Copyright © 2023 Belza, Portell, Herrán and Anguera. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mariona Portell, ✉ bWFyaW9uYS5wb3J0ZWxsQHVhYi5jYXQ=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.