Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 17 April 2023
Sec. Forensic and Legal Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Impact of Internal and External Influences on Memory and their Relevance to Legal Decisions View all 13 articles

Correcting myths about stress and memory: a commentary on Pezdek and Reisberg, 2022

  • 1School of Clinical Medicine, Discipline of Psychiatry and Mental Health, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia
  • 2Department of Clinical Psychological Science, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands
  • 3Leuven Institute of Criminology, Catholic University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
  • 4Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands
  • 5Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, United Kingdom

Introduction

The question of how acute stress might affect memory has applied value because witnesses, victims, and perpetrators often report experiencing stress or associated emotions (e.g., fear) during a crime. They might also experience acute stress when they are interviewed by the police. It is therefore important that legal professionals and memory scientists, particularly those acting as expert witnesses, can rely on evidence-based knowledge concerning the acute effects of stress on memory.1 Pezdek and Reisberg (2022) recently published an article aimed at debunking six psychological myths about evidence in the legal system. In their article, they argued that the idea that high stress improves the accuracy of memory is a myth (Myth #2). We take issue with this assertion on the basis that such a conclusion is not empirically warranted and does not accurately reflect the current state of research. In this commentary, we lend some critical nuance regarding the complex stress-memory relationship in eyewitness contexts.

Discussion

In their article, Pezdek and Reisberg noted that they “focused on myths of which the contrary evidence seems particularly clear” (p. 144) and that the evidence they provided showed that “these widely held beliefs are (at least) without basis and, in many cases, flatly false” (p. 143). Although research on stress and memory has been ongoing over several decades, evidence or consensus on this topic is not as clear-cut as suggested. In a recent survey of 73 memory experts, Marr et al. (2021a) showed that 95% of eyewitness experts and 81% of fundamental memory experts generally agreed that “Very high levels of stress impair the accuracy of eyewitness testimony.” However, in their study, only 61% of these experts deemed the statement reliable enough to present in court (see also Kassin et al., 2001). Importantly, the opinions of eyewitness memory experts and fundamental memory experts diverged widely regarding stress effects during encoding. While 78% of fundamental memory experts agreed that “Experiencing stress during an event (i.e., at encoding) enhances memory for that event,” only 32% of eyewitness experts did, highlighting the lack of consensus even amongst memory experts.

Pezdek and Reisberg acknowledged the complexity of the stress-memory relationship by referring to a meta-analysis (Shields et al., 2017) that suggests that encoding stress may enhance memory for stressor-relevant information when there is no or little delay between encoding and the stressor. However, Pezdek and Reisberg concluded that these conditions for encoding stress improving memory were limited to a “narrow focus” (p. 145) and implied that situations where stress impaired memory were more common. In reality, though, eyewitnesses frequently experience stress and encoding simultaneously, and the type of to-be-remembered information is often directly related to the stressor in a crime situation. These factors are in line with the moderating conditions for memory enhancements within the meta-analytic findings. Both of these factors also align with neurobiological theories and findings of many acute stress studies in the fundamental memory field suggesting memory enhancements (e.g., Joëls et al., 2006; Marr et al., 2021b, for a review).

To provide evidence against Myth #2, Pezdek and Reisberg cited findings from the eyewitness memory field suggesting that encoding stress impairs memory. However, this past work suffers from serious methodological limitations (Sauerland et al., 2016; Marr et al., 2021b). Many eyewitness studies conduct the memory retrieval test within minutes after the stressor/encoding phase (e.g., Brigham et al., 1983; Stanny and Johnson, 2000; Davis et al., 2019; Pezdek et al., 2020; Price et al., 2022). Because stress has an opposite effect on memory encoding (i.e., enhancing) and retrieval (i.e., impairing), this lack of sufficient retention interval obstructs any conclusions about the effects of encoding stress effects on memory.2 Additionally, the majority of eyewitness studies (e.g., Davis et al., 2019; most studies in Deffenbacher et al., 2004; Pezdek et al., 2020) have relied on self-reports of stress rather than more objective, physiological measures, such as blood pressure or cortisol. Self-reported measures are valuable for application to real life, where physiological, objective measures are often unobtainable. However, for experimental lab studies, this measurement issue raises the question of whether the effect of encoding stress on memory was actually captured—or merely an effect of arousal (or a number of other cognitive phenomena). Researchers should strive to ensure that stress is properly induced and verified by using objective measures wherever possible, alongside self-report measures (cf. Shields et al., 2017; Marr et al., 2021b). If physiological measures of stress cannot be included, researchers should be cautious in using the term “stress” with respect to its effects on memory without noting this limitation. This care in terminology is particularly important for eyewitness studies, which often involve complex scenarios that likely produce many other effects, including the impact of arousal, divided attention, perceptual phenomena, or cognitive load. More studies examining links between self-reported levels of stress and physiological states of stress would be helpful for improving the construct validity of self-report measures, and in turn, will improve application to reality (e.g., Weber et al., 2022).

Pezdek and Reisberg (2022) also discredited the (ecological) validity of stress induced by the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST; see footnote 2 on p. 146). Dozens of studies collecting physiological measures alongside self-reports and recent meta-analyses have confirmed the validity of the TSST for inducing a full stress response (e.g., Goodman et al., 2017; Seddon et al., 2020; Gu et al., 2022). In contrast, it is currently unclear how “stressors” used in many eyewitness studies score on these dimensions (e.g., emotional pictures, violent videos, false fire alarms, Joëls et al., 2006; Marr et al., 2021b). Given that to-be-remembered materials that are directly related to the stressor elicit stronger effects (Shields et al., 2017), this should motivate eyewitness memory and stress researchers to collaborate in designing studies that combine the best of both fields to study the effects of encoding stress on memory (cf. Marr et al., 2021a). However, the fact that stress elicited in the TSST is not directly related to the to-be-remembered material does not justify discarding all findings that derive from its use—or effectively throwing the baby out with the bath water.

Conclusion and implications

We conclude that the empirical research base to date does not allow for any strong conclusions about the effect of encoding stress on memory. Rather, whether acute stress impairs, enhances, or does not reliably affect memory performance is dependent on many moderators, most of which still need to be more thoroughly investigated in future research (Marr et al., 2021b). Eyewitness reports from those who have been through a stressful experience should not be immediately accepted or discounted without examining the surrounding context and keeping the findings from both the eyewitness and fundamental memory fields in mind.

Future research on this topic will provide a clearer understanding of the factors that critically contribute to the relationship between stress and memory and the direction of that relationship. In the meantime, however, it is important to acknowledge the existing shades of gray when discussing stress effects on memory, particularly in applied legal settings. That being said, certain sub-topics relevant to the stress-memory relationship in eyewitness settings do show greater expert consensus than others (e.g., those related to stress severity and detail type; Marr et al., 2021a). Additionally, strong expert consensus exists regarding the inaccuracy of certain widespread layperson beliefs, including ideas that police officers are less influenced by acute stress or that stressful experiences can cause memory repression. These incorrect beliefs can and should be countered where relevant by expert witnesses in court.

Author contributions

CM drafted the commentary. All other authors provided critical feedback and contributed to the development and finalization of the commentary.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^In this commentary—like Pezdek and Reisberg—we focus on acute rather than chronic stress, where a stressor is long-lasting and continuous. Though not discussed further in this commentary, future studies should consider the unique effects of chronic stress on memory performance (Wolf, 2008; Finsterwald and Alberini, 2014) and the interaction between acute and chronic stress, which may be relevant to legal settings when considering ongoing or repeated events such as family violence crimes.

2. ^Note that this type of research is important in its own right—if the research question aims to examine how acute stress may affect immediate memory performance (e.g., Krix et al., 2016). However, if researchers specifically aim to examine effects of encoding stress, a retention interval of at least 24 h is needed to properly separate the encoding and retrieval memory stages due to the lengthy timeline of a physiological stress response (Joëls and Baram, 2009).

References

Brigham, J. C., Maass, A., Martinez, D., and Wittenberger, G. (1983). The effect of arousal on facial recognition. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 4, 279–293. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp0403_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, S. D., Peterson, D. J., Wissman, K. T., and Slater, W. A. (2019). Physiological stress and face recognition: Differential effects of stress on accuracy and the confidence-accuracy relationship. J. Appl. Res. Memory Cognit. 8, 367–375. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2019.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deffenbacher, K. A., Bornstein, B. H., Penrod, S. D., and McGorty, E. K. (2004). A meta-analytic review of the effects of high stress on eyewitness memory. Law Hum. Behav. 28, 687–706. doi: 10.1007/s10979-004-0565-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finsterwald, C., and Alberini, C. M. (2014). Stress and glucocorticoid receptor-dependent mechanisms in long-term memory: from adaptive responses to psychopathologies. Neurobiol. Learn. Memory 112, 17–29. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, W. K., Janson, J., and Wolf, J. M. (2017). Meta-analytical assessment of the effects of protocol variations on cortisol responses to the Trier Social Stress Test. Psychoneuroendocrinology 80, 26–35. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.02.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gu, H., Ma, X., Zhao, J., and Liu, C. (2022). A meta-analysis of salivary cortisol responses in the Trier Social Stress Test to evaluate the effects of speech topics, sex, and sample size. Compreh. Psychoneuroendocrinol. 16, 100125. doi: 10.1016/j.cpnec.2022.100125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joëls, M., and Baram, T. Z. (2009). The neuro-symphony of stress. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 459–466. doi: 10.1038/nrn2632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joëls, M., Pu, Z., Wiegert, O., Oitzl, M. S., and Krugers, H. J. (2006). Learning under stress: how does it work? Trends Cognit. Sci. 10, 152–158. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kassin, S. M., Tubb, V. A., Hosch, H. M., and Memon, A. (2001). On the “general acceptance” of eyewitness testimony research: a new survey of the experts. Am. Psychol. 56, 405–416. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.5.405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krix, A. C., Sauerland, M., Raymaekers, L. H. C., Memon, A., Quaedflieg, C. W. E. M., and Smeets, T. (2016). Eyewitness evidence obtained with the self-administered interview is unaffected by stress. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 30, 103–112. doi: 10.1002/acp.3173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marr, C., Otgaar, H., Sauerland, M., Quaedflieg, C. W. E. M., and Hope, L. (2021a). The effects of stress on eyewitness memory: a survey of memory experts and laypeople. Memory Cognit. 49, 401–421. doi: 10.3758/s13421-020-01115-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marr, C., Sauerland, M., Otgaar, H., Quaedflieg, C., and Hope, L. (2021b). The effects of acute stress on eyewitness memory: an integrative review for eyewitness researchers. Memory 29, 1091–1100. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2021.1955935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pezdek, K., Abed, E., and Cormia, A. (2020). Elevated stress impairs the accuracy of eyewitness memory but not the confidence-accuracy relationship. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 158–169. doi: 10.1037/xap0000316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pezdek, K., and Reisberg, D. (2022). Psychological myths about evidence in the legal system: how should researchers respond? J. Appl. Res. Memory Cognit. 11, 143. doi: 10.1037/mac0000037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, H. L., Tottenham, L. S., Hatin, B., Fitzgerald, R. J., and Rubínov,á, E. (2022). Effects of stress on eyewitness identification in the laboratory. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 36, 191–202. doi: 10.1002/acp.3910

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauerland, M., Raymaekers, L. H. C., Otgaar, H., Memon, A., Waltjen, T. T., Nivo, M., et al. (2016). Stress, stress-induced cortisol responses, and eyewitness identification performance. Behav. Sci. Law 34, 580–594. doi: 10.1002/bsl.2249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seddon, J. A., Rodriguez, V. J., Provencher, Y., Raftery-Helmer, J., Hersh, J., Labelle, P. R., et al. (2020). Meta-analysis of the effectiveness of the trier social stress test in eliciting physiological stress responses in children and adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology 116, 104582. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.104582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shields, G. S., Sazma, M. A., McCullough, A. M., and Yonelinas, A. P. (2017). The effects of acute stress on episodic memory: a meta-analysis and integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 143, 636–675. doi: 10.1037/bul0000100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanny, C. J., and Johnson, T. C. (2000). Effects of stress induced by a simulated shooting on recall by police and citizen witnesses. Am. J. Psychol. 113, 359–386. doi: 10.2307/1423364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weber, J., Angerer, P., and Apolinário-Hagen, J. (2022). Physiological reactions to acute stressors and subjective stress during daily life: a systematic review on ecological momentary assessment (EMA) studies. PLoS ONE 17, e0271996. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0271996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, O. T. (2008). The influence of stress hormones on emotional memory: Relevance for psychopathology. Acta Psychol. 127, 513–531. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: acute stress, eyewitness memory, psychological myths, emotional memory, forensic settings

Citation: Marr C, Otgaar H, Quaedflieg CWEM, Sauerland M and Hope L (2023) Correcting myths about stress and memory: a commentary on Pezdek and Reisberg, 2022. Front. Psychol. 14:1078021. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1078021

Received: 23 October 2022; Accepted: 29 March 2023;
Published: 17 April 2023.

Edited by:

Bernhard Pastötter, University of Trier, Germany

Reviewed by:

Charlie Frowd, University of Central Lancashire, United Kingdom
Marcus Möschl, Technical University Dresden, Germany

Copyright © 2023 Marr, Otgaar, Quaedflieg, Sauerland and Hope. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Carey Marr, Y2FyZXkubWFyciYjeDAwMDQwO3Vuc3cuZWR1LmF1

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.