- 1Faculty of Nursing, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
- 2Owerko Centre for Children’s Neurodevelopment and Mental Health, University of Calgary Alberta Children’s Hospital Research Institute, Calgary, AB, Canada
- 3Department of Pediatrics and Community Health Sciences, Hotchkiss Brain Institute, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
- 4Department of Pediatrics, Psychiatry, and Community Health Sciences, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
- 5Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
Children’s cognitive abilities (e.g., working memory) are associated with mental health, adaptive behaviors, and academic achievement, and may be enhanced by parental reflective function (i.e., capacity to reflect on mental states, feelings, thoughts, and intentions in one’s child and oneself). We evaluated associations between maternal reflective function and children’s cognitive abilities alone and while controlling for parent-child attachment and interaction quality, and psychosocial (i.e., maternal depressive symptoms, adverse childhood experiences) and sociodemographic (e.g., socioeconomic status) factors. Our sample, recruited in Canada, was primarily white and included 73 mothers and their 4–5 year old preschool children. Maternal reflective function was measured with the Reflective Functioning Scale applied to the Parent Development Interview and the Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire. Multiple regression analyses revealed that maternal reflective function was associated with children’s cognitive abilities. The Parent Development Interview rated child-reflective function was associated with children’s higher verbal comprehension alone and while adjusting for covariates (e.g., parent-child interaction quality, socioeconomic status), and the Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire Interest and Curiosity with higher verbal comprehension while adjusting for parent-child interactions and attachment pattern. The Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire Certainty in Mental States was associated with higher working memory scores for children while adjusting for covariates. Full Scale IQ and Visual Spatial Index were not significantly associated with maternal reflective function. Associations were found between secure and disorganized attachment with higher verbal comprehension and lower working memory, respectively. These findings highlight the importance of high maternal reflective function to cognitive abilities in early childhood.
Introduction
A child’s cognitive ability underlies their capacity to process, store, incorporate, and recover information in memory, as well as sustain attention, make decisions, perceive information, learn knowledge, skills, and behaviors, and acquire language such as receptive and expressive skills (Kiely, 2014). Meta-analyses show positive associations between childhood intelligence and adaptive (e.g., communication, daily living, motor skills) behaviors in preschool-aged children (Alexander and Reynolds, 2020), and later academic achievements (Roth et al., 2015). Meta-analytic findings also highlight positive correlations between working memory and educational outcomes, such as reading (Peng et al., 2018) and mathematical performance (Friso-van den Bos et al., 2013). Children with major depressive disorder have demonstrated lower intelligence scores (e.g., full scale, verbal), executive functioning skills (e.g., working memory), and verbal memory compared to their peers (see Wagner et al., 2015 for meta-analytic review). Given associations between children’s cognitive abilities and academic outcomes and mental health, developing a better understanding of early environmental factors that could modify or improve cognitive abilities is necessary to ensure a positive developmental trajectory.
Parental reflective function (RF), is the ability to “mentalize” or envision mental states in oneself and one’s child (e.g., thoughts, feelings, desires, and intentions) and verbalize them (Fonagy et al., 2002, p. 363; Fonagy, 2006; Sharp, 2006; Midgely and Vrouva, 2012). It is a modifiable trait (Byrne et al., 2020; Letourneau et al., 2020; Slade et al., 2020) that is theorized to improve children’s developmental attainment, including cognitive abilities (Steele et al., 1996; Fonagy and Target, 1997; Fonagy et al., 2002; Allen et al., 2008; Sharp and Fonagy, 2008). Interventions designed to improve parental RF have demonstrated improvements in infants’ speech development (Bain, 2014), reductions in school-aged children’s externalizing (e.g., aggressiveness) behaviors (Londono Tobon et al., 2020), and improvements in preschool-aged children’s developmental outcomes (Anis et al., 2020a). Given the importance of children’s cognitive abilities and modifiability of parental RF, further research is needed to examine whether parental RF is associated with preschool children’s cognitive abilities.
Parental reflective function
RF is the capacity that allows individuals to make sense of one another; parents with high RF have the ability to see their child as an autonomous individual with a mind of their own (Fonagy et al., 1991, 1993). For example, when a child is upset, a highly reflective parent can recognize and accurately address the source of the child’s distress by considering how the child is thinking and feeling. In contrast, a parent with low RF would possibly ignore or misinterpret the source of the child’s distress. The construct of RF is the operationalization of mentalization (i.e., ability to understand others and oneself in terms of mental states) in the context of attachment relationships and is frequently used synonymously with mentalizing (Fonagy et al., 1998; Sharp and Fonagy, 2008). RF is distinguishable from many related terms including “mindblindedness,” “mindreading,” “theory of mind,” “metacognition,” “mindfulness,” “empathy,” and “emotional intelligence” (Fonagy and Target, 1997; Fonagy et al., 2002; Sharp et al., 2006; Allen et al., 2008). The related concept of “mind mindedness,” defined as the “proclivity to treat one’s child as an individual with a mind” (Meins and Fernyhough, 1999, p. 364), is a commonly used construct. While parental RF refers to a parent’s ability to mentalize, mind mindedness focuses on how frequently parents spontaneously mentalize (Medrea and Benga, 2021). Although the constructs are not synonymous, Sharp and Fonagy (2008) suggest that mind mindedness may tap into the “same underlying neurobiological socio-cognitive system” as RF (p. 737).
Parental reflective function and children’s cognitive abilities
There is a paucity of research investigating the associations between parental RF and children’s verbal comprehension, IQ, and visual spatial skills. Therefore, we broadened our literature review to include research on parental RF and other cognitive abilities, such as executive function, and found some support for an association. A longitudinal path analysis in Canada (n = 385) found that maternal cognitive sensitivity (e.g., parental ability to cognitively stimulate their child) mediated the association between maternal RF (referred to as maternal reflective capacity), measured when children were 18 months old, and 4-year-old children’s executive functioning, theory of mind, and receptive language (Wade et al., 2018). Studies have also reported that maternal RF is positively associated with school-aged children’s RF (Ensink et al., 2015; Rosso and Airaldi, 2016), and as RF demands cognitive abilities, such as perceiving and processing mental states and emotions (Slade, 2005; Benbassat, 2020), the results of these studies support a link between maternal RF and children’s cognitive abilities.
Despite limited research on empirical associations between parental RF and children’s cognitive abilities, programs designed to promote parental RF are proliferating, and research is emerging on the effect of parental RF-based programs on children’s cognitive abilities (e.g., Sleed et al., 2013; Bain, 2014; Longhi et al., 2019). Programs designed to foster maternal RF improved infant’s cognitive outcomes in two studies, yet maternal RF was not reported (Sleed et al., 2013) and associations between maternal RF and children’s cognitive outcomes were not evaluated (Bain, 2014). In one study, mother-infant dyads experiencing homelessness were recruited to evaluate a psychotherapeutic baby clinic (Sleed et al., 2013). Infants participated in cognitive assessments using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, 2nd edition (e.g., memory, verbal communication, sensory/perceptual acuity; Bayley-II; Bayley, 1993). Compared to the control group (n = 29), infants in the intervention group (n = 30) showed significant improvements in cognitive and motor scores from pre- to post-intervention (Sleed et al., 2013). Similarly, mother-infant dyads were recruited from shelters for families experiencing homelessness in Johannesburg to evaluate the effect of Best Beginnings on infant speech development (Bain, 2014). Evaluation of 16 mother-child dyads in the intervention group and 6 mother-child dyads in the control group revealed that infants in the intervention group attained significantly higher scores on a measure of speech development post-intervention compared to infants in the control group. However, the association between maternal RF and children’s cognitive abilities was not assessed.
In two other studies, RF-based programs did not improve children’s cognitive outcomes (Fonagy et al., 2016b; Longhi et al., 2019), measured with the Bayley-III (Bayley, 2006). First-time, young mothers experiencing high psychosocial risk (e.g., adolescent parent, eligible for income support) participated in a randomized controlled trial to evaluate the effect of Minding the Baby® on child developmental outcomes (Longhi et al., 2019). The intervention commenced prenatally and continued until the children were 2 years of age (Slade et al., 2010, 2020; Sadler et al., 2013), and results revealed no significant difference in children’s cognitive abilities between the intervention and control groups at 2 years (Longhi et al., 2019). In contrast, 76 mother-infant dyads were randomized to receive a parent-infant psychotherapy (PIP) program or treatment-as-usual (Fonagy et al., 2016b). PIP involved at a minimum, bi-weekly sessions that had the mother discuss their own mental state and factors affecting it. At 1 year of age, no statistically significant differences were found between the intervention and control groups with children’s cognitive abilities, maternal RF assessed using the Parent Development Interview (Slade et al., 2004), maternal-child attachment, or maternal-infant interactions. The association between maternal RF and children’s cognitive abilities was also not examined (Fonagy et al., 2016b).
Although correlational and experimental studies have been conducted on maternal RF and children’s cognitive abilities, several gaps exist in the literature. Despite showing some promise of an association, correlational research is lacking on children’s verbal comprehension, visual spatial skills, and Full Scale IQ. Randomized controlled trials have been conducted, which offer numerous benefits, such as minimizing bias and increasing capacity to establish causality (Hariton and Locascio, 2018). However, interventional research on parental RF-based programs and children’s cognitive abilities to date has focused on infants, and has either not measured parental RF or not evaluated the association between parental RF and children’s cognitive abilities. Therefore, additional research, focused on preschool-aged children, is needed to understand the association between maternal RF and children’s cognitive abilities, including Full Scale IQ, working memory, verbal comprehension, and visual spatial skills.
Reflective function and parent-child interaction quality
Slade (2005) contended that parents with high RF understand how mental states influence their children’s and their own behaviors, thus they can anticipate their child’s actions, and utilize this awareness to guide their caregiving interactions with their children. Indeed, a narrative review of 47 studies concluded that parental RF was positively associated with caregiving qualities, such as maternal sensitivity toward their child (Camoirano, 2017). Caregiving interactions characterized by parental sensitivity at 10–12 months have been associated with higher cognitive scores for children at 18 months on the Bayley-II (Bayley, 1993; Malmberg et al., 2016). Parental sensitivity at 24 months (N = 630) was also associated with higher scores on the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of Intelligence (Wechsler, 1974) in children 36 months of age (Mills-Koonce et al., 2015). Therefore, caregiving qualities such as parental sensitivity could confound observed associations between parental RF and preschool children’s cognitive abilities and need to be considered as a covariate in analyses of parental RF and children’s cognitive abilities.
Parental reflective function, parent-child attachment, and cognitive abilities
An association between parents’ RF and attachment security has also been reported in several studies (e.g., Fonagy et al., 1991; Stacks et al., 2014). For example, Slade et al. (2005a) evaluated maternal RF with Fonagy et al. (1998) RF scoring of the Parent Development Interview (Slade et al., 2004, 2005b) in a sample of 40 mothers with infants. Infants with secure attachment patterns at 14 months had mothers with significantly higher RF scores when the infant was 10 months of age than infants with disorganized or resistant attachment patterns (Slade et al., 2005a). Similarly, maternal RF, measured with the Parent Development Interview-Revised Short Form (Slade et al., 2003, 2005b) and infant attachment were assessed at 16 months in a sample of 83 mothers with histories of maltreatment (Stacks et al., 2014). Maternal RF was positively correlated with secure (vs. avoidant or disorganized) attachment patterns, and parental sensitivity mediated the link between maternal RF and infant attachment security (Stacks et al., 2014). Further, higher parental RF, measured using the Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (Luyten et al., 2017), predicted secure (vs. insecure) infant attachment patterns (Fonagy et al., 2016a). Parent-child attachment patterns have also been associated with executive function (e.g., Bernier et al., 2015; Regueiro et al., 2020), cognitive abilities on the Bayley-II (Bayley, 1993; Ding et al., 2014), IQ on Wechsler’s Intelligence Scale for Children—IV (Wechsler, 2003; Almas et al., 2016), and children’s mentalizing, especially “thinking-about-feeling,” abilities (see Zeegers et al., 2019 for review). Therefore, parent-child attachment is an important covariate to consider when examining the association between parental RF and children’s cognitive abilities.
Sociodemographic and psychosocial influences on children’s cognitive abilities
There are also significant associations between socioeconomic status (SES), child sex, parental depression, and children’s cognitive abilities. Interrelated factors such as maternal education (Harding, 2015; Harding et al., 2015), income (Dickerson and Popli, 2016; Khanam and Nghiem, 2016), marital status (Son and Peterson, 2017), as well as overall SES can have significant effects on children’s cognitive abilities, including executive functioning, language development, and academic achievement (see Korous et al., 2020 for a systematic review of meta-analyses). Extensive research has linked maternal depression to poorer cognitive abilities in children, such as lower receptive vocabulary (Letourneau et al., 2013; Kingston et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2017). Child sex has also been associated with children’s cognitive abilities; for example, boys have performed lower on a measure of cognitive abilities (Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development and McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities; McCarthy, 1972), than girls in the context of maternal depression (see Ahun et al., 2021 for meta-analysis). Consequently, SES, maternal depression, and child sex should be considered as a covariate in analyses of pre-schoolers’ cognitive abilities.
Although significantly less studied, maternal adverse childhood experiences (ACEs), such as abuse and neglect, have been associated with children’s development. Maternal ACEs are associated with smaller intracranial volumes in infants (Moog et al., 2018), increased risk for physical, emotional (Madigan et al., 2017), and behavioral concerns (Letourneau et al., 2019), and suspected developmental delays (Folger et al., 2018). Therefore, maternal ACEs are an important covariate to consider in studies examining the association between maternal RF and children’s cognitive abilities.
The current study addresses the following question: What is the association between maternal RF and preschool children’s cognitive abilities alone and after adjusting for important covariates, such as parent-child interaction, attachment pattern, psychosocial (i.e., depression, ACEs), and sociodemographic (e.g., SES) variables? We predicted that higher parental RF would associate with higher children’s cognitive abilities, alone and accounting for: (1) relationship qualities (i.e., parent-child interaction and attachment) and (2) psychosocial (i.e., depression, maternal ACEs) and sociodemographic (e.g., socioeconomic status) variables.
Materials and methods
Study design and sample
This longitudinal correlational study utilized a subsample of parents with preschool children from the Alberta Pregnancy Outcomes and Nutrition (APrON) longitudinal pregnancy cohort. Comprehensive APrON study details have previously been published (Kaplan et al., 2014). In brief, pregnant women, 16 years and older with a gestation less than 27 weeks were recruited from maternity care and ultrasound clinics in Alberta, Canada to participate in the APrON study. Women who could not complete questionnaires in English or who planned to relocate from the study region within 6 months were excluded. In this sub-study, families with children who had intellectual or motor disabilities, or a genetic disorder were excluded. In addition, only families for whom we had parental RF, parent-child interaction quality, parent-child attachment, maternal ACEs, demographic and psychosocial data, and child cognitive outcomes at 4–6 years were included. This resulted in a subsample of 73 mother-child pairs with RF and child cognitive outcome data. Ethics approval was obtained from the University of Calgary Conjoint Health Research Ethics Board (ID: REB15-1200) and mothers provided informed consent for their participation and that of their children.
Data on children’s cognitive abilities were measured with the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence–Fourth Edition (WPPSI-IV) when children were on average 53.6 months (SD = 5.1) and maternal RF was collected during a clinic visit when children and mothers were, on average, 60.0 (SD = 3.82) months and 37.6 (4.23) years, respectively. Maternal data on ethnic identity, education, income, marital status, country of birth, child sex, gestational age, and birth weight was collected at a clinic visit that occurred when the child was 3 months of age or via mail, and maternal ACEs were collected cross-sectionally via email when children were 27.35 months (SD = 4.06). Data on maternal-child attachment pattern and interaction quality were collected at 21.09 months (SD = 3.47) and 5.61 (SD = 0.34 months), respectively.
Measures
We measured maternal RF by (1) coding the Parent Development Interview (Slade et al., 2004) with the Reflective Functioning Scale (Fonagy et al., 1998; Slade et al., 2005b) and (2) attaining parental self-reported RF with the Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (PRFQ) (Luyten et al., 2017) to determine if maternal RF measured with the gold standard interview-based tool and newer self-report RF questionnaire both showed associations with children’s cognitive abilities. Observational methods were employed to obtain covariate data on maternal-child interaction quality and attachment patterns. Parental surveys provided covariate data for psychosocial variables (i.e., maternal ACEs and depressive symptoms). Demographic data were also collected via surveys to describe the sample and as covariates. Children’s cognitive abilities were assessed with the WPPSI-IV (Wechsler, 2012).
Independent variable
Reflective function
We assessed maternal RF with the Reflective Functioning Scale, which was developed by Fonagy et al. (1998), and was later adapted (Slade et al., 2005b) to code semi-structured Parent-Development Interviews (PDI) (Slade et al., 2004). During the 20-item semi-structured PDI interviews, parents describe their own and their child’s behaviors, thoughts, and feelings in different parenting situations (Slade et al., 2004). The interviews were audio-recorded and transcribed verbatim. Trained coders (i.e., author 6 and a master coder/trainer from The New School University, NYC, USA), scored each question using the Reflective Functioning Scale (Fonagy et al., 1998; Slade et al., 2005b) to assess parental insight into their own and their child’s thoughts, feelings, mental states, and intentions as a parent in the parent-child relationship. Scores are derived, each ranging from -1 (anti-reflective; parent attempts to evade question, responds with hostility, or offers bizarre and incoherent answers) to +9 (exceptionally reflective; parent provides sophisticated, complex, coherent answers) to evaluate (1) self RF, representing the parent’s ability to recognize their own thoughts, feelings, intentions, and mental states, and (2) child RF, demonstrating the parent’s ability to recognize their child’s thoughts, feelings, mental states, and intentions. Two points were added to each score for a range of 1–11, to aid in analysis interpretation. To ensure coders were blind to other participant data, deidentified transcripts were provided. We evaluated inter-rater reliability via percentage agreement between the coders in 15 interviews and observed 86 and 80% agreement for the self and child RF scale scores.
The PRFQ is an 18-item self-reported questionnaire that evaluates parental RF (Luyten et al., 2017). Individuals rate each item on a seven-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). This measure takes approximately 5 min to complete. We employed the Certainty in Mental States scale, which assesses parental ability to recognize and comprehend the opacity of mental states, and the Interest and Curiosity scale, which investigates parental interest and curiosity in their child’s mental states. For Certainty in Mental States and Interest and Curiosity, mid Likert-scale answers indicate high RF. Scores were transformed as per Anis et al. (2020b) method to ensure that lower scores corresponded to lower RF and higher scores indicated higher RF on the 1–7 scale. The PRFQ has demonstrated good internal consistency, with the following reported Cronbach’s alphas: Certainty in Mental States = 0.82 and Interest and Curiosity = 0.75 (Luyten et al., 2017). Anis et al. (2020b) demonstrated that PRFQ and PDI-Reflective Functioning Scale scores were significantly associated, with original and transformed coding (e.g., r’s = 0.22–0.30).
Covariates
Attachment
Maternal-child attachment security was measured with the Strange Situation Procedure, which is considered the gold-standard measure for attachment in children (Ainsworth et al., 1978; van IJzendoorn et al., 1992), and has been utilized in children up to 40 months of age (Śliwerski et al., 2020). The Strange Situation Procedure was administered in a laboratory setting, and children’s behaviors were assessed throughout eight stressful, yet brief episodes, such as the parent leaving their child in a room with a stranger present. A trained coder (author 6) classified dyads according to four attachment categories, Avoidant (A), Secure (B), Resistant/Ambivalent (C) and Disorganized (D), attaining 100% intra-rater reliability on assessments coded 1 month apart (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Main and Solomon, 1986). Author 6 was trained and certified in ABC coding by Alan Sroufe at the University of Minneapolis (achieving > 80% reliability) as well as by Marinus van IJzendoorn at the University of Cambridge in D coding (achieving > 80% reliability). She also attained 80% inter-rater reliability on ABCD with this SSP dataset with Elizabeth Carlson at the University of Minneapolis. Similar with previous research, individuals were dichotomized as secure vs. insecure and disorganized vs. organized (Cassidy et al., 2011; Letourneau et al., 2015).
Parent-child interaction quality
This was measured with the Total Scale score of the observational Parent-Child Interaction Teaching Scale (PCITS) (Oxford and Findlay, 2013). The PCITS is considered the gold standard measure of parent-child interaction quality, which is assessed while a parent teaches their child a novel skill. Seventy three binary items (yes, no) are used to assess parental (e.g., sensitivity to cues, response to distress) and child (e.g., clarity of cues) behaviors. Scores range from 0 to 73, with higher scores indicating higher quality interactions. The PCITS can take up to 5 min to complete and parent-child dyads were recorded to enhance coding accuracy. A certified coder (i.e., attained > 90% reliability with University of Washington standardized coding), blind to other participant data, coded videos for parental sensitivity to cues, responsiveness to distress, social-emotional growth fostering, cognitive growth fostering, and children’s clarity of cues and responsiveness to their parent. Intra-rater reliability was assessed with every 10th video, and 95% intra-rater agreement was maintained.
Psychosocial variables
We employed the widely utilized 10-item self-report ACE Questionnaire to determine mothers’ exposure to ACEs from birth to 18 years (Felitti et al., 1998/2019). ACEs include exposure to child maltreatment (i.e., abuse and neglect) and household dysfunction (e.g., domestic violence). Individuals receive a score of one for each type of exposure, and scores can range from 0 to 10. Excellent internal consistency (α = 0.88) has been demonstrated with the ACE Questionnaire (Murphy et al., 2014). Maternal ACEs were classified as 0, 1, 2, or ≥ 3. Maternal depressive symptoms were measured with the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS), which is a self-reported 10-item questionnaire that requires parents to recall depressive symptoms during the previous week (Cox et al., 1987). Scores can range from 0 to 30, with higher scores associated with more severe depression. Although the tool is not utilized to diagnose postpartum depression, it is the most widely employed screening tool for postpartum depression (Gibson et al., 2009). Maternal depressive symptoms were measured at 3, 6, 12, 18, 24, and 36 months postpartum, and we utilized mothers’ mean EPDS scores over time to capture mothers’ symptoms in the years prior to the child’s cognitive assessment.
Demographic variables
We collected child demographic data on sex, gestational age, and birth weight, and maternal data on education, ethnicity, income, marital status, and country of birth. Composite measures of SES often include educational level, income, and marital status constructs (Letourneau et al., 2011). Similar to other research (e.g., Ali et al., 2020), SES was calculated from maternal income, education, and marital status by summing the values according to the follow dichotomies: income < $70,000 (0) or ≥ $70,000 (1), educational attainment of < university degree (0) or ≥ a university degree (1), and not married (0) or married (1). The sample’s SES scores ranged from 1 to 3, with higher scores indicative of a higher SES.
Outcome
Children’s cognitive abilities
We measured children’s cognitive abilities with the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of Intelligence–Fourth Edition, Canadian (WPPSI-IVCND), a broad measure of cognitive abilities for children aged 2 years 6 months to 7 years 7 months (Wechsler, 2012). For all children, Full Scale IQ and three composite index scores were calculated (i.e., Verbal Comprehension Index, Visual Spatial Index, and Working Memory Index). Scores have a mean of 100 (SD = 15; range: 40–160), and higher scores indicate better performance. Excellent internal consistency has been observed for Full Scale IQ (r = 0.95), Verbal Comprehension Index (r ≥ 0.93), Visual Spatial Index (r = 0.88), and Working Memory Index (r ≥ 0.91) (Soares and McCrimmon, 2013).
Statistical analysis
The sample characteristics were described using frequencies, means, and standard deviations, as appropriate. Regression modeling was employed to determine the relation between maternal RF and cognitive abilities (i.e., Full Scale IQ, Verbal Comprehension, Visual Spatial, Working Memory). Three models were run to (1) evaluate direct associations between maternal RF and children’s cognitive abilities and (2) determine if the association held when accounting for important covariates (Pourhoseingholi et al., 2012). Model 1 examined the associations between maternal RF and preschool children’s cognitive abilities. Model 2 examined the association between maternal RF and children’s cognitive abilities while controlling for parent-child interaction quality and attachment pattern. Model 3 examined the association between maternal RF and preschool children’s cognitive abilities while controlling for parent-child interaction quality and attachment patterns, and maternal psychosocial and sociodemographic variables. The variables met assumptions for normality, linearity, homoscedasticity, and independence. Variable selection for inclusion was based on a combination of assessment of theoretical importance and empirical observations from bivariate correlations. Regardless of the significance in bivariate correlations with measures of children’s cognitive abilities, theoretically important covariates for model two included parent-child interaction quality and attachment pattern (i.e., secure vs. insecure, organized vs. disorganized) and for model three included maternal depressive symptoms and ACEs. In contrast, sociodemographic variables for inclusion in model three were identified by covariate correlations of ≥0.20 to ensure evidence of an association and retain statistical power to detect associations (Bursac et al., 2008). Given that we had five measures of RF, to reduce the possibility of multicollinearity, we relied on bivariate correlations to assist with RF variable selection (see Table 2).
Models were tested for multicollinearity with variance inflation factor (VIF) analyses. A VIF of 5–10 or greater indicates the presence of collinearity concerns (Shrestha, 2020); all VIF values were between 1 and 1.5, suggesting absence of multicollinearity. We also conducted an a priori power analysis with G*Power Analysis for a fixed linear multiple regression model (Faul et al., 2007). Given alpha = 0.05, power = 0.80, and an effect size of r = 0.23 derived from data showing a significant association between maternal RF and receptive language, an exemplar of cognitive abilities (Wade et al., 2018), our sample size of 73 provided the power to include 9 variables in each model. There was no missing data in our sample of 73 individuals. Given the nature of this study and that we included a limited number of specific RF variables selected a priori and with directional hypotheses, a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was not applied (Rothman, 1990).
Results
Sample characteristics
Demographic and descriptive data are presented in Table 1. Mothers were an average of 37.6 (SD = 4.2) years of age at the RF assessment. The majority were born in Canada (78%), married/common law (95%), had attained a minimum of a university degree (79%), and reported ≥ $70,000 yearly household income (81%). Most mothers did not meet the EPDS threshold (i.e., EPDS score ≥ 10) for mild depressive symptoms (71%) and approximately half of the sample (51%) reported no ACEs. On average maternal scores on the PRFQ Interest and Curiosity and Certainty in Mental States scales were 6.6 and 6.0, respectively. Average PDI child and self RF scores were 5.3 and 5.4, respectively. Child sex was approximately equivalent (55% female). Most children were born full-term (M = 39.5 weeks gestation) and were classified with not disorganized (90%) attachment patterns. Approximately half (52%) presented with secure attachment patterns. On average, children had a Full Scale IQ of 104.8. The mean score on the Verbal Comprehension Index was 107, Visual Spatial Index was 104.4, and Working Memory Index was 103.1. Bivariate correlations are presented in Table 2. Of the associations between RF measures and children’s cognitive abilities outcomes, all demonstrated significant associations except for the PDI self RF scale, thus this variable was not included in models.
Association between maternal reflective function and preschool children’s cognitive abilities
Linear regression models are presented in Tables 3, 4. Model 1 examined the association between maternal RF (i.e., PRFQ scales and PDI-RF scales) and cognitive abilities. No significant associations were found between RF and Full Scale IQ, the Visual Spatial Index score, or the Working Memory Index score. However, significant associations were found between the PDI child RF scale and Verbal Comprehension [b = 2.55 (1.21), p = 0.039]. Model 1 explained 12% of the variance in Verbal Comprehension.
Model 2 adjusted for parent-child interaction quality and attachment pattern. No associations were found between maternal RF (i.e., PRFQ scales and PDI-RF scales) and Full Scale IQ or Visual Spatial Index scores. The PDI child RF [b = 2.42 (1.21), p = 0.047] and PRFQ Interest and Curiosity [b = 7.25 (3.51), p = 0.043] scores were associated with Verbal Comprehension. PRFQ Certainty in Mental States [b = 3.32 (1.52), p = 0.034] was associated with Working Memory. Parent-child interaction quality was positively associated with Verbal Comprehension [b = 0.47 (0.19), p = 0.019] and Full Scale IQ [b = 0.41 (0.18), p = 0.031], and parent-child disorganized attachment patterns were negatively associated with Working Memory [b = -11.87 (5.65), p = 0.04]. Model 2 explained 21% of the variance in Verbal Comprehension and 17% in Working Memory.
Model 3 additionally adjusted for maternal depressive symptoms, ACEs, and SES. No associations were found between the PRFQ or PDI-RF scales and Full Scale IQ or the Visual Spatial Index scores. The associations between PDI child RF and Verbal Comprehension [b = 2.42 (1.20), p = 0.049], and PRFQ Certainty in Mental States [b = 3.70 (1.57), p = 0.022] with Working Memory remained. Parent-child interaction quality remained significantly associated with Verbal Comprehension [b = 0.49 (0.19), p = 0.014] and Full Scale IQ [b = 0.40 (0.17), p = 0.031]. Associations were observed between parent-child secure attachment and Verbal Comprehension [b = 6.75 (3.14), p = 0.036], and socioeconomic status with Full Scale IQ [b = 6.52 (2.23), p = 0.005]. The present model accounted for 28% of the variance in Verbal Comprehension and 21% in Working Memory scores.
Discussion
The purpose of our study was to determine the association between maternal RF and preschool children’s cognitive abilities, alone and while controlling for parent-child attachment and interaction quality, and relevant psychosocial and sociodemographic variables, such as SES and maternal depression. In model one, we evaluated the association between maternal RF and children’s cognitive abilities. Maternal RF, specifically PDI child RF, that is the mother’s ability to recognize their child’s thoughts, feelings, mental states, and intentions, was positive associated with children’s verbal comprehension. In model two, we assessed the association between maternal RF and children’s cognitive abilities while controlling for parent-child interaction quality and attachment pattern; PDI child RF remained positively associated, and PRFQ Interest and Curiosity showed a positive association, with children’s verbal comprehension. PRFQ Certainty in Mental States was positively associated with children’s working memory. Findings also showed associations between maternal-child interaction quality and children’s verbal comprehension. While controlling for maternal-child interaction quality and attachment, maternal depressive symptoms and ACEs, and SES in model three, PDI child RF and PRFQ Certainty in Mental States remained positively associated with children’s verbal comprehension and working memory, respectively. In the third model, maternal-child interaction quality was positively associated with verbal comprehension, and also associated with Full Scale IQ. No measures of maternal RF were significantly associated with children’s Full Scale IQ or visual spatial skills in any of the models. Overall, our hypothesis that maternal RF would associate with children’s cognitive abilities alone and while adjusting for relevant covariates was supported for verbal comprehension skills and partially supportive for children’s working memory.
Our prospective cohort findings demonstrate a positive association between maternal RF and preschool children’s cognitive abilities, specifically verbal comprehension and working memory. Previous research has shown links between maternal RF and childhood constructs that require complex cognitive abilities, such as school-aged children’s mentalizing (i.e., RF) capacity (e.g., Rosso et al., 2015; Scopesi et al., 2015). Additionally, maternal mind-mindedness (i.e., identification and treatment of their child as an individual with their own mind), a related construct to RF, has been associated with preschool children’s executive function, specifically, inhibitory control (Cheng et al., 2018), child language skills at age two (Bernier et al., 2017), and theory of mind at 5 and 6 years (Kirk et al., 2015). Our findings support the important link between maternal RF and preschool-aged children’s cognitive abilities, specifically, verbal comprehension and working memory. Parental RF has been shown to be a modifiable construct (e.g., Letourneau et al., 2020; Slade et al., 2020); therefore, parental RF interventions have potential to promote children’s cognitive abilities, yet additional research is needed.
The results of this study also demonstrate that RF, whether assessed via a coding scheme applied to a comprehensive interview or parental self-report, is associated with children’s cognitive abilities. This is important for future research and clinical evaluations of parental RF as the PRFQ is a short and accessible questionnaire that can be used to measure parental RF; it takes approximately 5 min to complete, is freely available, requires no specific training, and has potential for widespread use. In contrast, although the PDI-RF is well validated, it is time intensive and requires training.
However, we found differences in associations between the two measures of RF and children’s cognitive abilities. The PDI child RF, which measures parental ability to recognize feelings, thoughts, and intentions underlying their child’s behavior, was associated with verbal comprehension alone and while adjusting for parent-child relationship qualities and psychosocial and demographic variables. Research has shown positive associations between children’s language development and parental contingency (Topping et al., 2013), and parents with high child RF are likely attuned with their child’s readiness (e.g., engagement, disengagement) for interactions. However, maternal PDI child RF remained significantly associated while controlling parent-child interaction quality, which suggests that other aspects of maternal RF encourage children’s verbal comprehension. Research has also shown that exposure to more adult words is associated with children’s higher verbal comprehension (e.g., Huttenlocher et al., 1991; Gilkerson et al., 2018). It is possible that parents who receive high PDI child RF scores also engage in more verbalizations with their child than other parents, especially related to their child’s feelings, thoughts, and mental states. In contrast, the PRFQ Certainty in Mental States was correlated with working memory while adjusting for parent-child relationship qualities and psychosocial and demographic variables. Optimal PRFQ Certainty in Mental States scoring is characterized by moderate certainty (over certainty is seen as intrusive) in their child’s mental states (Luyten et al., 2017). Research has shown that optimal parenting (e.g., low intrusiveness) predicts working memory at 36 months (Wu and Schutte, 2021), which may explain the relation between PRFQ Certainty in Mental States and working memory. This association may also be explained with children’s stress reactivity since preschool children with high cortisol levels have shown lower executive functioning (Blair et al., 2011) and studies have reported associations between low parental RF and children’s emotional regulation abilities (Camoirano, 2017). More research is needed to understand how the PDI-RF and PRFQ differ in relation to children’s cognitive abilities.
Research using Fonagy et al. (1998) Reflective Functioning Scale coding of the PDI (Slade et al., 2004, 2005b) has shown differing associations between parental self RF and child RF. For example, Borelli et al. (2016) found that only child RF was related to child attachment security. In contrast, Suchman et al. (2010) found that self RF was positively correlated with caregiving behaviors (i.e., sensitivity to their child’s cues, social-emotional growth fostering, cognitive growth fostering) in a sample of mothers with drug use disorders. In our analysis, PDI self RF was not significantly associated with children’s cognitive abilities; therefore, we excluded this variable from the regression analysis. Additional research using RF coding of the PDI is needed to understand the associations between parental self and child RF on caregiving behaviors and children’s development.
Although not a central finding, our study also imparts additional knowledge on the association between attachment and children’s cognitive abilities. Disorganized attachment was associated with a noteworthy decrease in working memory by 12 points on a scale that ranges from 40 to 160, which is close to one standard deviation (SD = 15) in normative samples (Wechsler, 2012) and nearly one standard deviation (13.05) in working memory scores in our sample; given that a change in scores by half of a standard deviation is frequently considered a clinically significant finding in interventional research (Polit, 2017), our results highlight the clinical relevance of disorganized attachment on children’s working memory. This finding is consistent with Thorell et al. (2012), who reported that disorganized, but not insecure, attachment patterns in school-aged children were significantly related to children’s executive functioning skills, which include working memory. Further, secure attachment was associated with verbal comprehension scores that were nearly 7 points (on a scale with a range of 120 points) higher, when sociodemographic and psychosocial variables were controlled for; secure attachments were not associated with other cognitive abilities, such as visual spatial skills or working memory. Our findings are also consistent with van IJzendoorn et al. (1995) meta-analysis, which reported that insecure attachment patterns were weakly related to lower cognitive (d = 0.17) performance and moderately related to language performance (d = 0.59). Although other research has shown that RF-centered programs can improve parent-child attachment patterns (Slade et al., 2020), we did not find associations between parental RF and parent-child attachment. However, our samples differed demographically, and we observed a smaller proportion of children with disorganized attachment patterns than Slade et al. (2020) sample. Further research is needed to understand if RF focused interventions support children’s attachment and cognitive abilities.
Previous research has reported associations between sociodemographic variables and cognitive abilities (e.g., Letourneau et al., 2011; Merz et al., 2019). We found that SES was related to Full Scale IQ, which is consistent with extensive literature demonstrating a positive association between SES and cognitive abilities (e.g., Letourneau et al., 2011; Merz et al., 2019). Surprisingly, we did not find significant negative associations between maternal depression and cognitive abilities, despite an abundance of literature showing poorer cognitive abilities among children of mothers experiencing depression (e.g., Liu et al., 2017). However, our sample included very few mothers (29%) with EPDS scores indicative of possible depression. Although no significant relation was found between maternal ACEs and preschool children’s cognitive abilities, this could be due to a modest sample with only 51% of mothers reporting ACEs. Given emerging associations between maternal ACEs and children’s health and development (e.g., Madigan et al., 2017; Folger et al., 2018), the link between ACEs and cognitive abilities should be evaluated with larger and more diverse samples.
Strengths and limitations
The APrON longitudinal cohort provided an opportunity for prospective analyses (including important covariates) using reliable and valid measures, many of which were observational (i.e., Strange Situation Procedure to measure parent-child attachment patterns, PCITS to assess parent-child interaction quality) as opposed to parent report. Observational assessments of parent-child interaction quality and attachment are considered gold standard measures and we attained high intra-rater reliability for both tools. Administration of the WPPSI-IV provided a rigorous and broad assessment of preschool-aged children’s cognitive abilities. We also employed multiple valid and reliable measures of parental RF. Although other research has shown correlations between the PDI and PRFQ (Anis et al., 2020b), we did not observe these findings, which is a limitation in our research. Further, to our knowledge, an evaluation of association between maternal RF and a broad range of preschool aged children’s cognitive abilities have only been reported by Wade et al. (2018), and their cognitive ability outcomes (i.e., theory of mind, executive function, receptive language) differed from our study. To minimize participant burden, we collected maternal RF and cognitive ability data at differing clinic visits. However, the measurement of children’s cognitive abilities before assessments of parental RF in our study and the correlational analysis pose challenges to inferences about the direction of associations. Given the measurement timing and correlational analysis, our results do not establish causality, and additional research is needed to confirm our findings. Our study included a relatively homogenous, modest sample of low-risk families (i.e., high SES, low ACEs, low depressive scores, primarily Caucasian, married/cohabiting, few young mothers), and it is possible that associations could be stronger in higher-risk populations. Our sample also lacked ethnic diversity and most mothers were born in Canada, which limits generalizability. Further, as the research included only mothers, future research needs to determine if these findings generalize to fathers.
Conclusion
In conclusion, this study suggests that maternal RF is associated with verbal comprehension and working memory in preschool aged children. Our research also demonstrated the importance of children’s secure and disorganized attachment patterns on verbal comprehension and working memory, respectively. Given the importance of cognitive abilities to children’s mental health, education, and behavioral outcomes, and the modifiability of parental RF and parent-child attachment patterns, these findings underscore the importance of developing early parenting supports to optimize parental RF and parent-child attachment and evaluating the effects of these programs on children’s cognitive abilities.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Calgary Conjoint Health Research Ethics Board (ID: REB15-1200). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
JK assisted with formulating the research question and data analysis, interpreted results, and wrote the original draft. DD and GFG attained funding for the study, oversaw data collection and coding of select variables, contributed substantive knowledge and methodological expertise, and edited final manuscript. MH collected and coded data and edited final manuscript. LA collected data and edited final manuscript. HN analyzed data and edited final manuscript. JLC provided supervision of JK and edited manuscript. As senior author, NL formulated the research question, attained funding for the study as Principal Investigator, and oversaw data collection, coding, cleaning and analysis, ethics approval and writing of manuscript.
Funding
Funding to establish the APrON study was provided by Alberta Innovates Health Solutions. This sub-study was funded by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council, supplemented by support from the Alberta Children’s Hospital Foundation.
Acknowledgments
We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study and the entire APrON team (https://apronstudy.ca), investigators, research assistants, graduate and undergraduate students, volunteers, clerical staff, and managers.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Ahun, M. N., Gapare, C., Gariépy, G., and Côté, S. M. (2021). Sex differences in the association between maternal depression and child and adolescent cognitive development: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 51, 1431–1440. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721001689
Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., and Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Alexander, R. M., and Reynolds, M. R. (2020). Intelligence and adaptive behavior: A meta-analysis. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 49, 85–110. doi: 10.1080/2372966X.2020.1717374
Ali, E., Letourneau, N., Benzies, K., Ntanda, H., Dewey, D., Campbell, T., et al. (2020). Maternal prenatal anxiety and children’s externalizing and internalizing behavioral problems: The moderating roles of maternal-child attachment security and child sex. Can. J. Nurs. Res. 52, 88–99. doi: 10.1177/0844562119894184
Allen, J., Fonagy, P., and Batemen, A. W. (2008). Mentalizing in clinical practice. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Almas, A. N., Degnan, K. A., Nelson, C. A., Zeanah, C. H., and Fox, N. A. (2016). IQ at age 12 following a history of institutional care: Findings from the Bucharest early intervention project. Dev. Psychol. 52, 1858–1866. doi: 10.1037/dev0000167
Anis, L., Letourneau, N., Benzies, K., Ewashen, C., and Hart, M. J. (2020a). Effect of the attachment and child health parent training program on parent–child interaction quality and child development. Can. J. Nurs. Res. 52, 157–168. doi: 10.1177/0844562119899004
Anis, L., Perez, G., Benzies, K. M., Ewashen, C., Hart, M., and Letourneau, N. (2020b). Convergent validity of three measures of reflective function: Parent development interview, parental reflective function questionnaire, and reflective function questionnaire. Front. Psychol. 11:574719. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.574719
Bain, K. (2014). “New Beginnings” in South African shelters for the homeless: Piloting of a group psychotherapy intervention for high-risk mother-infant dyads. Infant Ment. Health J. 35, 591–603. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21457
Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley scales of infant development, 2nd Edn. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Bayley, N. (2006). Bayley scales of infant development manual, 3rd Edn. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Benbassat, N. (2020). Reflective function: A move to the level of concern. Theory Psychol. 30, 657–673. doi: 10.1177/0959354320934182
Bernier, A., Beauchamp, M. H., Carlson, S. M., and Lalonde, G. (2015). A secure base from which to regulate: Attachment security in toddlerhood as a predictor of executive functioning at school entry. Dev. Psychol. 51, 1177–1189. doi: 10.1037/dev0000032
Bernier, A., McMahon, C. A., and Perrier, R. (2017). Maternal mind-mindedness and children’s school readiness: A longitudinal study of developmental processes. Dev. Psychol. 53, 210–221. doi: 10.1037/dev0000225
Blair, C., Granger, D. A., Willoughby, M., Mills-Koonce, R., Cox, M., Greenberg, M. T., et al. (2011). Salivary cortisol mediates effects of poverty and parenting on executive functions in early childhood. Child Dev. 82, 1970–1984. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01643.x
Borelli, J. L., St John, H. K., Cho, E., and Suchman, N. E. (2016). Reflective functioning in parents of school-aged children. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 86, 24–36. doi: 10.1037/ort0000141
Bursac, Z., Gauss, C. H., Williams, D. K., and Hosmer, D. W. (2008). Purposeful selection of variables in logistic regression. Source Code Biol. Med. 3, 17–17. doi: 10.1186/1751-0473-3-17
Byrne, G., Murphy, S., and Connon, G. (2020). Mentalization-based treatments with children and families: A systematic review of the literature. Clin. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 25, 1022–1048. doi: 10.1177/1359104520920689
Camoirano, A. (2017). Mentalizing makes parenting work: A review about parental reflective functioning and clinical interventions to improve it. Front. Psychol. 8:14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00014
Cassidy, J., Woodhouse, S. S., Sherman, L. J., Stupica, B., and Lejuez, C. W. (2011). Enhancing infant attachment security: An examination of treatment efficacy and differential susceptibility. Dev. Psychopathol. 23, 131–148. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000696
Cheng, N., Lu, S., Archer, M., and Wang, Z. (2018). Quality of maternal parenting of 9-month-old infants predicts executive function performance at 2 and 3 years of age. Front. Psychol. 8:2293. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02293
Cox, J. L., Holden, J. M., and Sagovsky, R. (1987). Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh postnatal depression scale. Br. J. Psychiatry 150, 782–786. doi: 10.1192/bjp.150.6.782
Dickerson, A., and Popli, G. K. (2016). Persistent poverty and children’s cognitive development: Evidence from the UK Millennium cohort study. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. A Stat. Soc. 179, 535–558. doi: 10.1111/rssa.12128
Ding, Y.-H., Xu, X., Wang, Z.-Y., Li, H.-R., and Wang, W.-P. (2014). The relation of infant attachment to attachment and cognitive and behavioural outcomes in early childhood. Early Hum. Dev. 90, 459–464. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.06.004
Ensink, K., Normandin, L., Target, M., Fonagy, P., Sabourin, S., and Berthelot, N. (2015). Mentalization in children and mothers in the context of trauma: An initial study of the validity of the child reflective functioning scale. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 33, 203–217. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12074
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191.
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., et al. (1998/2019). Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The adverse childhood experiences (ACE) study. Am. J. Prev. Med. 56, 774–786. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2019.04.001
Folger, A. T., Eismann, E. A., Stephenson, N. B., Shapiro, R. A., Macaluso, M., Brownrigg, M. E., et al. (2018). Parental adverse childhood experiences and offspring development at 2 years of age. Pediatrics 141:e20172826. doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-2826
Fonagy, P. (2006). “The mentalization-focused approach to social development,” in The mentalization-focused approach to social development, eds J. G. Allen and P. Fonagy (New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons), 51–99. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2006.20.6.544
Fonagy, P., and Target, M. (1997). Attachment and reflective function: Their role in self-organization. Dev. Psychopathol. 9, 679–700. doi: 10.1017/S0954579497001399
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. L., and Target, M. (2002). Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of self. New York, NY: Other Press.
Fonagy, P., Sleed, M., and Baradon, T. (2016b). Randomized controlled trial of parent-infant psychotherapy for parents with mental health problems and young infants. Infant Ment. Health J. 37, 97–114. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21553
Fonagy, P., Luyten, P., Moulton-Perkins, A., Lee, Y.-W., Warren, F., Howard, S., et al. (2016a). Development and validation of a self-report measure of mentalizing: The reflective functioning questionnaire. PLoS One 11:e0158678. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0158678
Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Moran, G., Steele, H., and Higgitt, A. (1993). Measuring the ghost in the nursery: An empirical study of the relation between parents’ mental representations of childhood experiences and their infants’ security of attachment. J. Am. Psychoanal. Assoc. 41, 957–989. doi: 10.1177/000306519304100403
Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Moran, G. S., and Higgitt, A. C. (1991). The capacity for understanding mental states: The reflective self in parent and child and its significance for security of attachment. Infant Ment. Health J. 12, 201–218. doi: 10.1002/1097-0355(199123)12:3<201::AID-IMHJ2280120307<3.0.CO2-7
Fonagy, P., Target, M., Steele, H., and Steele, M. (1998). Reflective-functioning manual, version 5.0, for application to adult attachment interviews. London: University College London.
Friso-van den Bos, I., van der Ven, S. H. G., Kroesbergen, E. H., and van Luit, J. E. H. (2013). Working memory and mathematics in primary school children: A meta-analysis. Educ. Res. Rev. 10, 29–44. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2013.05.003
Gibson, J., McKenzie-McHarg, K., Shakespeare, J., Price, J., and Gray, R. (2009). A systematic review of studies validating the Edinburgh postnatal depression scale in antepartum and postpartum women. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 119, 350–364. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01363.x
Gilkerson, J., Richards, J. A., Warren, S. F., Oller, D. K., Russo, R., and Vohr, B. (2018). Language experience in the second year of life and language outcomes in late childhood. Pediatrics (Evanston) 142:e20174276. doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-4276
Harding, J. F. (2015). Increases in maternal education and low-income children’s cognitive and behavioral outcomes. Dev. Psychol. 51, 583–599. doi: 10.1037/a0038920
Harding, J. F., Morris, P. A., and Hughes, D. (2015). The relationship between maternal education and children’s academic outcomes: A theoretical framework. J. Marriage Fam. 77, 60–76. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12156
Hariton, E., and Locascio, J. J. (2018). Randomised controlled trials – the gold standard for effectiveness research: Study design: randomised controlled trials. BJOG: Int. J. Obstet. Gynaecol. 125, 1716. doi: 10.1111/1471-0528.15199
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., and Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Dev. Psychol. 27, 236–248. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236
Kaplan, B., Giesbrecht, G., Leung, B., Field, C., Dewey, D., Bell, R., et al. (2014). The alberta pregnancy outcomes and nutrition (APrON) cohort study: Rationale and methods. Matern. Child Nutr. 10, 44–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1740-8709.2012.00433.x
Khanam, R., and Nghiem, S. (2016). Family income and child cognitive and noncognitive development in Australia: Does money matter? Demography 53, 597–621. doi: 10.1007/s13524-016-0466-x
Kiely, K. M. (2014). “Cognitive function,” in Encyclopedia of quality of life and well-being research, ed. A. C. Michalos (Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands), 974–978. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-0753-5_426
Kingston, D., McDonald, S., Austin, M.-P., and Tough, S. (2015). Association between prenatal and postnatal psychological distress and toddler cognitive development: A systematic review. PLoS One 10:e0126929. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0126929
Kirk, E., Pine, K., Wheatley, L., Howlett, N., Schulz, J., and Fletcher, B. (2015). A longitudinal investigation of the relationship between maternal mind-mindedness and theory of mind. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 33, 434–445. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12104
Korous, K. M., Causadias, J. M., Bradley, R. H., Luthar, S. S., and Levy, R. (2020). A systematic overview of meta-analyses on socioeconomic status, cognitive ability, and achievement: The need to focus on specific pathways. Psychol. Rep. 125, 55–97. doi: 10.1177/0033294120984127
Letourneau, N. L., Duffett-Leger, L., Levac, L., Watson, B., and Young-Morris, C. (2011). Socioeconomics status and child development: A meta-analysis. J. Emot. Behav. Disord. 21, 211–224. doi: 10.1177/1063426611421007
Letourneau, N. L., Tramonte, L., and Willms, J. D. (2013). Maternal depression, family functioning and children’s longitudinal development. J. Pediatr. Nurs. 28, 223–234. doi: 10.1016/j.pedn.2012.07.014
Letourneau, N., Anis, L., Ntanda, H., Novick, J., Steele, M., Steele, H., et al. (2020). Attachment & child health (ATTACH) pilot trials: Effect of parental reflective function intervention for families affected by toxic stress. Infant Ment. Health J. 41, 445–462. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21833
Letourneau, N., Dewey, D., Kaplan, B. J., Ntanda, H., Novick, J., Thomas, J. C., et al. (2019). Intergenerational transmission of adverse childhood experiences via maternal depression and anxiety and moderation by child sex. J. Dev. Orig. Health Dis. 10, 88–99. doi: 10.1017/S2040174418000648
Letourneau, N., Tryphonopoulos, P., Giesbrecht, G., Dennis, C. L., Bhogal, S., and Watson, B. (2015). Narrative and meta-analytic review of intervention aiming to improve maternal-child attachment security. Infant Ment. Health J. 36, 366–387. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21525
Liu, Y., Kaaya, S., Chai, J., McCoy, D. C., Surkan, P. J., Black, M. M., et al. (2017). Maternal depressive symptoms and early childhood cognitive development: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 47, 680–689. doi: 10.1017/S003329171600283X
Londono Tobon, A., Condon, E., Sadler, L. S., Holland, M. L., Mayes, L. C., and Slade, A. (2020). School age effects of minding the baby-an attachment-based home-visiting intervention on parenting and child behaviors. Dev. Psychopathol. 34, 55–67. doi: 10.1017/S0954579420000905
Longhi, E., Murray, L., Wellsted, D., Hunter, R., MacKenzie, K., Taylor-Colls, S., et al. (2019). Minding the Baby® (MTB) home-visiting programme for vulnerable young mothers: Results of a randomised controlled trial in the UK. London: NSPCC.
Luyten, P., Nijssens, L., Fonagy, P., and Mayes, L. C. (2017). Parental reflective functioning: Theory, research, and clinical applications. Psychoanal. Study Child. 70, 174–199. doi: 10.1080/00797308.2016.1277901
Madigan, S., Wade, M., Plamondon, A., Maguire, J. L., and Jenkins, J. M. (2017). Maternal adverse childhood experience and infant health: Biomedical and psychosocial risks as intermediary mechanisms. J. Pediatr. 187, 282–289.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.04.052.
Main, M., and Solomon, J. (1986). “Discovery of an insecure-disorganized/disoriented attachment pattern,” in Affective development in infancy, eds T. B. Brazelton and M. W. Yogman (Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing), 95–124.
Malmberg, L. E., Lewis, S., West, A., Murray, E., Sylva, K., and Stein, A. (2016). The influence of mothers’ and fathers’ sensitivity in the first year of life on children’s cognitive outcomes at 18 and 36 months. Child Care Health Dev. 42, 1–7. doi: 10.1111/cch.12294
McCarthy, D. (1972). Manual for the McCarthy scales of children’s abilities. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Medrea, F. L., and Benga, O. (2021). Parental mentalization: A critical literature review of mind-mindedness, parental insightfulness and parental reflective functioning. Cogn. Brain Behav. Interdiscip. J. 25, 69–105. doi: 10.24193/cbb.2021.25.05
Meins, E., and Fernyhough, C. (1999). Linguistic acquisitional style and mentalising development: The role of maternal mind-mindedness. Cogn. Dev. 14, 363–380. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(99)00010-6
Merz, E. C., Wiltshire, C. A., and Noble, K. G. (2019). Socioeconomic inequality and the developing brain: Spotlight on language and executive function. Child Dev. Perspect. 13, 15–20. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12305
Midgely, N., and Vrouva, I. (2012). Mentalization-based interventions with children, young people and their families. London: Routledge.
Mills-Koonce, W. R., Willoughby, M. T., Zvara, B., Barnett, M., Gustafsson, H., and Cox, M. J. (2015). Mothers’ and fathers’ sensitivity and children’s cognitive development in low-income, rural families. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 38, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2015.01.001
Moog, N. K., Entringer, S., Rasmussen, J. M., Styner, M., Gilmore, J. H., Kathmann, N., et al. (2018). Intergenerational effect of maternal exposure to childhood maltreatment on newborn brain anatomy. Biol. Psychiatry 83, 120–127. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.07.009
Murphy, A., Steele, M., Dube, S. R., Bate, J., Bonuck, K., Meissner, P., et al. (2014). Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) questionnaire and adult attachment interview (AAI): Implications for parent child relationships. Child Abuse Negl. 38, 224–233. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.09.004
Oxford, M. L., and Findlay, D. M. (2013). NCAST caregiver/parent-child interaction teaching manual. Seattle, WA: NCAST Programs, University of Washington, School of Nursing.
Peng, P., Barnes, M., Wang, C., Wang, W., Li, S., Swanson, H. L., et al. (2018). A meta-analysis on the relation between reading and working memory. Psychol. Bull. 144, 48–76. doi: 10.1037/bul0000124
Polit, D. F. (2017). Clinical significance in nursing research: A discussion and descriptive analysis. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 73, 17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2017.05.002
Pourhoseingholi, M. A., Baghestani, A. R., and Vahedi, M. (2012). How to control confounding effects by statistical analysis. Gastroenterol. Hepatol. Bed Bench 5, 79–83.
Regueiro, S., Matte-Gagné, C., and Bernier, A. (2020). Patterns of growth in executive functioning during school years: Contributions of early mother–child attachment security and maternal autonomy support. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 200, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104934
Rosso, A. M., and Airaldi, C. (2016). Intergenerational transmission of reflective functioning. Front. Psychol. 7:1903. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01903
Rosso, A. M., Viterbori, P., and Scopesi, A. M. (2015). Are maternal reflective functioning and attachment security associated with preadolescent mentalization? Front. Psychol. 6:1134. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01134
Roth, B., Becker, N., Romeyke, S., Schäfer, S., Domnick, F., and Spinath, F. M. (2015). Intelligence and school grades: A meta-analysis. Intelligence 53, 118–137. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2015.09.002
Rothman, K. J. (1990). No adjustments are needed for multiple comparisons. Epidemiology 1, 43–46. doi: 10.1097/00001648-199001000-00010
Sadler, L. S., Slade, A., Close, N., Webb, D. L., Simpson, T., Fennie, K., et al. (2013). Minding the baby: Enhancing reflectiveness to improve early health and relationship outcomes in an interdisciplinary home-visiting program. Infant Ment. Health J. 34, 391–405. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21406
Scopesi, A. M., Rosso, A. M., Viterbori, P., and Panchieri, E. (2015). Mentalizing abilities in preadolescents’ and their mothers’ autobiographical narratives. J. Early Adolesc. 35, 467–483. doi: 10.1177/0272431614535091
Sharp, C. (2006). “Mentalizing problems in childhood disorders,” in The mentalization-focused approach to social development, eds J. G. Allen and P. Fonagy (New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons), 101–121.
Sharp, C., and Fonagy, P. (2008). The parent’s capacity to treat the child as a psychological agent: Constructs, measures and implications for developmental psychopathology. Soc. Dev. 17, 737–754. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00457.x
Sharp, C., Fonagy, P., and Goodyer, I. M. (2006). Imagining your child’s mind: Psychosocial adjustment and mothers’ ability to predict their children’s attributional response styles: The role of conversations in children’s social, emotional and cognitive development. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 24, 197–214.
Shrestha, N. (2020). Detecting multicollinearity in regression analysis. Am. J. Appl. Maths. Stat. 8, 29–42. doi: 10.12691/ajams-8-2-1
Slade, A. (2005). Parental reflective functioning: An introduction. Attach. Hum. Dev. 7, 269–281. doi: 10.1080/14616730500245906
Slade, A., Aber, J. L., Berger, B., Bresgi, I., and Kaplan, M. (2003). Parent development interview revised short version. New York, NY: City College of New York.
Slade, A., Bernbach, E., Grienenberger, J., Levy, D. W., and Locker, A. (2004). The parent development interview and the pregnancy interview: Manuals for scoring. New York, NY: City College of New York and Yale Child Study Center.
Slade, A., Grienenberger, J., Bernbach, E., Levy, D., and Locker, A. (2005a). Maternal reflective functioning, attachment, and the transmission gap: A preliminary study. Attach. Hum. Dev. 7, 283–298.
Slade, A., Grienenberger, J., Berbach, E., Levy, D., and Locker, A. (2005b). Manual for scoring reflection functioning on the parent development interview. New York, NY: City University of New York.
Slade, A., Holland, M. L., Ordway, M. R., Carlson, E. A., Jeon, S., Close, N., et al. (2020). Minding the Baby ® : Enhancing parental reflective functioning and infant attachment in an attachment-based, interdisciplinary home visiting program. Dev. Psychopathol. 32, 123–137. doi: 10.1017/S0954579418001463
Slade, A., Sadler, L. S., Close, N., Webb, D., de Dios-Kenn, C., Ezepchick, J., et al. (2010). Minding the baby treatment manual. New Haven, CT: Yale University.
Sleed, M., James, J., Baradon, T., Newbery, J., and Fonagy, P. (2013). A psychotherapeutic baby clinic in a hostel for homeless families: Practice and evaluation. Psychol. Psychother. 86, 1–18. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.2011.02050.x
Śliwerski, A., Kossakowska, K., Jarecka, K., Świtalska, J., and Bielawska-Batorowicz, E. (2020). The effect of maternal depression on infant attachment: A systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:2675. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17082675
Soares, M. A., and McCrimmon, A. W. (2013). Test review: Wechsler Preschool and primary scale of intelligence, fourth edition: Canadian. Can. J. Sch. Psychol. 28, 345–351. doi: 10.1177/0829573513497343
Son, S. C., and Peterson, M. F. (2017). Marital status, home environments, and family strain: Complex effects on preschool children’s school readiness skills. Infant Child Dev. 26, 1–26. doi: 10.1002/icd.1967
Stacks, A. M., Muzik, M., Wong, K., Beeghly, M., Huth-Bocks, A., Irwin, J. L., et al. (2014). Maternal reflective functioning among mothers with childhood maltreatment histories: Links to sensitive parenting and infant attachment security. Attach. Hum. Dev. 16, 515–533. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2014.935452
Steele, H., Steele, M., and Fonagy, P. (1996). Associations among attachment classifications of mothers, fathers, and their infants. Child Dev. 67, 541–555. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01750.x
Suchman, N. E., DeCoste, C., Leigh, D., and Borelli, J. (2010). Reflective functioning in mothers with drug use disorders: Implications for dyadic interactions with infants and toddlers. Attach. Hum. Dev. 12, 567–585. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2010.501988
Thorell, L. B., Rydell, A.-M., and Bohlin, G. (2012). Parent-child attachment and executive functioning in relation to ADHD symptoms in middle childhood. Attach. Hum. Dev. 14, 517–532. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2012.706396
Topping, K., Dekhinet, R., and Zeedyk, S. (2013). Parent-infant interaction and children’s language development. Educ. Psychol. 33, 391–426. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2012.744159
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Dijkstra, J., and Bus, A. G. (1995). Attachment, intelligence, and language: A meta-analysis. Soc. Dev. 4, 115–128. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.1995.tb00055.x
van IJzendoorn, M., Goldberg, S., Kroonenberg, P., and Frenkel, O. (1992). The relative effects of maternal and child problems on the quality of attachment: A meta-analysis of attachment in clinical samples. Child Dev. 63, 840–858. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01665.x
Wade, M., Madigan, S., Plamondon, A., Rodrigues, M., Browne, D., and Jenkins, J. M. (2018). Cumulative psychosocial risk, parental socialization, and child cognitive functioning: A longitudinal cascade model. Dev. Psychol. 54, 1038–1050. doi: 10.1037/dev0000493
Wagner, S., Müller, C., Helmreich, I., Huss, M., and Tadiæ, A. (2015). A meta-analysis of cognitive functions in children and adolescents with major depressive disorder. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 24, 5–19. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0559-2
Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler intelligence scale for children-revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (2003). WISC-IV technical and interpretive manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (2012). Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence, 4th Edn. London: Pearson.
Wu, Y., and Schutte, A. R. (2021). Attentional Shifting Mediates the Relationship between parenting and working memory. J. Cogn. Dev. 22, 667–677. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2021.1976781
Keywords: maternal reflective function, cognitive abilities, attachment security, maternal depression, preschool, regression, APrON study
Citation: Komanchuk J, Dewey D, Giesbrecht GF, Hart M, Anis L, Ntanda H, Cameron JL and Letourneau N (2022) Association between maternal reflective function and preschool children’s cognitive abilities. Front. Psychol. 13:995426. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.995426
Received: 15 July 2022; Accepted: 24 October 2022;
Published: 16 November 2022.
Edited by:
Naomi Sweller, Macquarie University, AustraliaReviewed by:
Emily Merz, Colorado State University, United StatesNanhua Cheng, Chinese Academy of Sciences (CAS), China
Copyright © 2022 Komanchuk, Dewey, Giesbrecht, Hart, Anis, Ntanda, Cameron and Letourneau. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Nicole Letourneau, Nicole.Letourneau@ucalgary.ca