Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 16 September 2022
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

Cross-national measurement invariance of the Purpose in Life Test in seven Latin American countries

  • 1Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, Universidad Privada del Norte, Lima, Peru
  • 2South American Center for Education and Research in Public Health, Universidad Norbert Wiener, Lima, Peru
  • 3Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Nacional de Rosario, Rosario, Argentina
  • 4Centro de Investigación en Neurociencias de Rosario, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Nacional de Rosario, Rosario, Argentina
  • 5Laboratorio de Cognición y Emoción, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Nacional de Rosario, Rosario, Argentina
  • 6Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas, Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 7Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, Universidad Católica del Maule, Talca, Chile
  • 8Programa de Pós-Graduação em Psicología, Pontificia Universidade Católica de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Brasil
  • 9Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Pedagógica y Tecnológica de Colombia, Tunja, Colombia
  • 10Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Cuenca, Cuenca, Ecuador
  • 11Escuela de Psicología, Pontificia Universidad Católica del Ecuador, Ambato, Ecuador
  • 12Escuela de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad Tecnológica de El Salvador, San Salvador, El Salvador
  • 13Universidad de Guanajuato, Guanajuato, Mexico
  • 14Facultad de Ciencias del Comportamiento, Universidad Autónoma del Estado de México, Toluca, Mexico
  • 15Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, Universidad Nacional del Este, Ciudad del Este, Paraguay
  • 16Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, Universidad Católica de Asunción, Asunción, Paraguay
  • 17Centro de Estudios Adlerianos, Montevideo, Uruguay
  • 18Facultad de Ciencias Humanas y Educación, Universidad Peruana Unión, Lima, Peru
  • 19Instituto de Investigación en Ciencias Sociales y Educación, Universidad de Atacama, Copiapó, Chile

The Purpose in Life Test (PIL) is a measure of purpose in life widely used in many cultures and countries; however, cross-cultural assessments are scarce. The present study aimed to evaluate the cross-cultural measurement invariance of the PIL in the general population of seven Latin American countries (Colombia, Ecuador, El Salvador, Mexico, Paraguay, Argentina, and Uruguay). A total of 4306 people participated, selected by non-probabilistic convenience sampling, where Uruguay has the highest mean age (M = 41.8; SD = 16.6 years); while Ecuador has the lowest mean age (M = 24.6; SD = 7.8 years). Furthermore, in each country, there is a higher proportion of women (>60%) than men (<40%). Using Multi-Group Confirmatory Factor Analysis, the factorial structure does not show evidence of invariance among the included countries. However, based on the Multi-Group Factor Analysis Alignment, there is evidence that a three-dimensional structure of the PIL (Meaning of existence, Freedom to make meaning in daily life and Will to find meaning in the face of future challenges) is the same in the participating countries. Results based on item response theory indicate that most PIL items can significantly differentiate responses according to the level of life purpose. In addition, people with low life purpose will tend to choose the lower response alternatives on the PIL; while people with higher life purpose will choose higher response alternatives. The findings indicate that the PIL has the potential to increase knowledge about how people conceive and experience their purpose in life in different countries.

Introduction

The relevance of purpose or meaning in life in the psychological literature increases progressively from the strong connections with positive mental health that research begins to report since 1960 (Yaccarini and Furman, 2017; Bronk, 2020). Within existential philosophy, life is considered to have no single predetermined meaning, as individuals construct meaning throughout their lives (Fabry, 1988; Sezer, 2012). This theoretical idea gained considerable relevance within psychology due to the experience of Victor Frankl, an Austrian physician who suffered confinement in concentration camps during World War II (Furman, 2021; Schimmoeller and Rothhaar, 2021). Frankl believed that humans are driven by a “will to meaning,” which involves the desire to find purpose in life, even in the most miserable circumstances (Frankl, 1984). Following Frankl’s work; Crumbaugh and Maholick (1964) define purpose in life as a subjective sense of one’s life as meaningful. According to the literature, there are three central components in the definition of purpose underlying different conceptualizations: (1) commitment, which refers to the capacity for development, a stable link to certain beliefs, values and orientations, giving a coherent and unified sense of self; (2) goal orientation, which refers to the perception that current activities are related to future outcomes and; (3) personal meaning, defined as the extent or degree of ubiquity of purpose in an individual’s life and its impact on their behavior, thoughts and emotions (Bronk, 2014, 2020). Although the terms “meaning” and “purpose” have been used interchangeably, it has been suggested that there is a differentiation in the use of both terms. Thus, purpose refers to an engagement with the wider world-beyond the self, involving an intention to act for the benefit of others - or, in other words, for a greater cause - something that would differentiate it from merely assigning meaning to one’s own life (Damon et al., 2003; Furman, 2021). However, there is still not enough solid theoretical or empirical evidence to support such a differentiation (Furman, 2021); moreover, Frankl himself, throughout his work, used the terms “purpose” and “meaning” to refer to the same construct (Bronk, 2014).

Lack of meaning or purpose in life is associated with the presence of existential emptiness which, in turn, is related to symptoms of depression, anxiety, stress, addictions or aggressive tendencies (Schulenberg and Melton, 2010). Currently, purpose or meaning in life is a protective factor from the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on well-being (Arslan and Allen, 2022; Echeverria et al., 2021; Karataş et al., 2021). Despite the changes caused by confinement during the current pandemic, individuals have demonstrated a strong purpose in life (Romero-Ramos et al., 2021), which has been linked to a reduction in symptoms of anxiety, depression, stress, and worry associated with the pandemic (Echeverria et al., 2021; Humphrey and Vari, 2021; Samios et al., 2021; Zuo et al., 2021).

While it is possible to find some exceptions (Ratner et al., 2021; Wang et al., 2021), most research leans toward a quantitative approach to the study of purpose in life (Bronk, 2014, 2020). The first measure was introduced by Frankl (1966) who developed a questionnaire comprised of 13 items, created to assess purpose in a clinical sample. Although Frankl initially developed this scale to provide a clinical assessment, and not for research purposes (Bronk, 2014), Crumbaugh and Maholick (1964) administered their questionnaire in a sample of psychiatric patients and general population. As the results showed reliability and validity below their expectations, with Frankl’s support they developed a new version of the technique (Crumbaugh and Maholick, 1964; Hutzell, 1988). Since its development, the Purpose in Life Test (PIL; Crumbaugh and Maholick, 1964) has been considered the most relevant measure for assessing the degree to which people perceive their own lives as meaningful (Walters and Klein, 1980; Bronk, 2020). Although the PIL is considered to be an eminently quantitative measure, in reality it is composed of Part A, made up of 20 questions, which provide information that can be quantified and compared between different samples; and other parts B, made up of 13 incomplete sentences, and C, which presents paragraphs referring to future goals and past experiences, which provide qualitative information that can be of greater use at the therapeutic level. However, part A is the most used section of the three-part PIL in research studies and can be easily quantified, so this section is frequently referred to as the PIL (Schulenberg and Melton, 2010).

Different versions of the PIL have provided adequate psychometric properties in different contexts such as Australia (Dyck, 1987; Marsh et al., 2003), Argentina (Simkin et al., 2018), Brazil (Hayashi and Esmerelles, 2017; Nascimento and Dias, 2019), Canada (Dufton and Perlman, 1986), Colombia (Ortiz et al., 2012), China (Chang and Dodder, 1983; Law, 2012), Cuba (Ochoa et al., 2018), Korea (Kim et al., 2001), Spain (García-Alandete et al., 2013, 2018), Italy (Brunelli et al., 2012), Hungary (Konkolÿ Thege and Martos, 2006), Japan (Okado, 1998), Mexico (Magaña Valladares et al., 2004), Norway (Haugan and Moksnes, 2013), and Sweden (Jonsén et al., 2010), among others. Despite its wide acceptance within the academic community, one of the main concerns about measuring purpose in life with the PIL lies in the factor structure of the scale. In fact, the PIL has received heavy criticism due to the presence of different underlying factor structures, which limits suggesting a consistently replicable model (Schulenberg and Melton, 2010). Most studies have recommended assessing purpose as a unidimensional concept (Crumbaugh and Maholick, 1964; Marsh et al., 2003; Steger, 2006; Simkin et al., 2018), while others consider a multifactorial structure (García-Alandete et al., 2016).

There are also debates about the number of items that should be included in the scale (García-Alandete et al., 2013). Thus, for example, although the unidimensional structure has been widely replicated (Brunelli et al., 2012; Simkin et al., 2018), other studies have suggested eliminating some items from this model (Marsh et al., 2003; Steger, 2006; Tibaldi Nascimento and Lebre Dias, 2019). Among those who propose a two-dimensional factor structure, there is also a wide disparity of criteria regarding the items and the names of these dimensions (Walters and Klein, 1980; Dufton and Perlman, 1986; Molcar and Stuempfig, 1988; Shek, 1988; McGregor and Little, 1998; Waisberg and Starr, 1999; Morgan and Farsides, 2009; García-Alandete et al., 2013). There is also no consensus among the items and names of the dimensions within those authors who propose a three-factor structure (Magaña Valladares et al., 2004; Jonsén et al., 2010; Martínez Ortiz et al., 2012).

Many of the studies that have suggested a multidimensional structure of PIL indicated the presence of high interfactor correlations (García-Alandete et al., 2013; Hayashi and Esmerelles, 2017). The presence of high correlations has led researchers to test models with a second-order factor underlying all the items (García-Alandete et al., 2013) which can evaluate the existing overlap between the different factors or the degree of independence between them. However, a study with the Spanish version of the PIL indicated that a second-order model was inadequate, due to the presence of incompatible estimates in the standardized equation (García-Alandete et al., 2013). Few studies, such as the one mentioned above, have evaluated second-order or Bifactor models that express general constructs made up of different highly related domains (Chen et al., 2006). Second order models are used when there are highly related dimensions and there is a higher order factor that could explain the relationship between them, while bifactor models, in addition to also assuming the presence of a general factor that would explain the similarity of the dimensions, each item loads on the specific factors and on the general factor, which allows for estimating the proportion of the variance which is a product of the general factor and that which is from the specific factors (Mansolf and Reise, 2017; Raykov and Pohl, 2013). Thus, the second-order and bifactor models would allow for evaluating the effects of the multidimensionality of the PIL on the total score and on each factor (Reise, 2012).

The different results about the factor structure of the PIL exemplify how difficult it is to have measurement instruments with adequate psychometric evidence (García-Alandete et al., 2017). Problems about the structure of the PIL are associated with the use of different factor analysis techniques (exploratory factor analysis, principal components analysis, confirmatory factor analysis) and the different characteristics of the samples (university students, older adults, patients with chronic diseases, among others). Moreover, although the PIL was constructed for clinical purposes, most studies have been conducted on non-clinical samples. Likewise, different cultural and linguistic aspects may influence the factor structure of the scale. When a measurement instrument developed in one cultural context is to be applied in another, it cannot be assumed that the factor structure will be the same (Sharma et al., 2009). This suggests a need for performing a review of different models proposed for the PIL, comparing them and identifying the best and most useful one.

Additionally, culture is an important source of individual values and expectations (Steger et al., 2008), which shapes different experiences that people may consider enriching and meaningful (Kitayama et al., 2000). Some suggest that having a meaningful life is associated with higher levels of well-being within some cultures (Steger et al., 2008); whereas, from well-being theories, it is suggested that, levels of meaning may be consistent across cultures (Ryff and Singer, 1998; Deci and Ryan, 2000). Specifically, if dimensions of meaning in life are associated with basic psychological needs, then these dimensions should manifest and be equally important across cultures. However, other studies suggest cultural variation in life purpose (Lindfors et al., 2006). At the Latin American level, as far as is known from the literature, there are no studies comparing the meaning of life construct among different countries. However, some individual studies have been reported (Francke, 2011; Martínez Ortiz and Castellanos Morales, 2013; Barrero et al., 2020). In Colombia, it has been suggested that additional analysis is needed to identify whether the results are produced by biases in the instrument or the characteristics of the sample (Barrero et al., 2020). Due to the paucity of scientific literature on cultural variation in life purpose, a cross-cultural study is necessary. In these cases, the comparative approach has been useful to identify differences and similarities of a psychological construct between different cultural groups with the aim of making theoretical generalizations. However, as this approach is based on the assumption of comparability; the following question arises: is the same construct truly being compared between different groups? (Lomazzi, 2018). This concern is related to the problem of measurement invariance (MI) and the methodological approaches used to assess it.

Measurement invariance is a procedure that aims to demonstrate to what extent a self-report measure expresses the same meaning and whether the responses to the items are the result of the same factors, in all groups where it was applied (Vandenberg and Lance, 2000). The absence of MI can generate misinterpretations in the results and conclusions possibly derived from methodological errors (Moors, 2004). Traditionally, multigroup confirmatory factor analysis (MGCFA) has been the most widely used method to assess MI (Davidov et al., 2014). This method is based on the concept of “exact equivalence,” where comparisons between different groups will be adequate if the instrument used is exactly the same (Lomazzi, 2018). The MGCFA tests three levels of measurement invariance: configural (where it is evaluated whether or not the construct responds to the same factor structure in all groups), metric (where the unit of measurement needs to be equal, such that factor loadings are equal in all groups) and scalar (which is the most demanding level and requires equality in factor loadings and item intersections) (Steenkamp and Baumgartner, 1998). The presence of metric invariance would allow for the comparison of covariances and unstandardized regression coefficients between groups; whereas, the comparison of latent means would be achievable with the presence of scalar invariance (Davidov, 2010). However, it is also suggested that the presence of partial invariance is sufficient to compare means (Chen, 2007).

Despite the importance of the MGCFA, criticisms of this classical MI approach have emerged for some years (Muthén and Asparouhov, 2013; Van De Schoot et al., 2013; Asparouhov and Muthén, 2014). For example, in large-scale cross-national studies, where a large number of groups can be compared, the MGCFA method may be too stringent and reject models that may be comparable across groups (Lomazzi, 2018). In this sense, it is difficult to achieve full MI as the probability of violating some equivalence principles is higher as the number of groups increases (Davidov et al., 2014). While there are procedures to identify partially invariant models, this requires large modification rates that may lead to produce inadequate models, which are far from the real models and do not guarantee that the means are unbiased; moreover, the selection of the parameters to be released may be arbitrary and miss potentially adequate models (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2014; Marsh et al., 2018).

As an alternative to MGCFA, the Multi-Group Factor Analysis Alignment (CFA-MIAL; Asparouhov and Muthén, 2014) method has been developed and it estimates means without equality restrictions for factor loadings and intersections between groups, thus identifying the most appropriate invariant measurement pattern. In addition, the CFA-MIAL assesses the invariance of factor loadings and intersections simultaneously and considers that both need not necessarily be identical across culturally diverse groups (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2014). The CFA-MIAL assumes that the number of non-invariant parameters and the degree of non-invariance can be minimal. This allows for identifying an invariant pattern between different groups, as well as estimating means and variances by considering the actual differences in factor loadings and intercepts. Thus, CFA-MIAL becomes an alternative which allows for automating and simplifying MI (Marsh et al., 2018). However, CFA-MIAL is still rarely applied (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2014), with only a few relevant studies before (Jang et al., 2017) and during the pandemic (Caycho-Rodríguez et al., 2021a).

While there are some studies that attempt to explore the psychometric properties of the PIL in Latin America (Martínez Ortiz et al., 2012; Simkin et al., 2018) more research evaluating MI across different countries is needed. This could contribute to improved cross-cultural research in the field and provide findings that are more generalizable than previous studies. Therefore, the present study aimed to evaluate cross-cultural MI of PIL in samples from seven Latin American countries (Colombia, Ecuador, El Salvador, Mexico, Paraguay, Argentina, and Uruguay). This included the evaluation of PIL unidimensionality, reliability and cross-cultural MI using the CFA-MIAL and MG-CFA methods. The use of two methods to test MI allows for more solid conclusions and to identify non-invariant items and non-invariant countries. Finally, it is expected that the results will contribute to having a Latin American version of the instrument that can be used in different countries (Yela, 1996).

Materials and methods

Design

An instrumental design was used, since the psychometric properties of a psychological measurement instrument were evaluated (Ato et al., 2013).

Participants

The total sample consisted of 4306 people from seven Latin American countries (Colombia, Ecuador, El Salvador, Mexico, Paraguay, Argentina, and Uruguay), selected by non-probability convenience sampling based on the following inclusion criteria: (1) be of legal age as stipulated in each participating country; (2) be a national of one of the participating countries; (3) be able to answer online surveys and; (4) have given informed consent to participate in the study. Everyone who did not meet the inclusion criteria were not allowed to be part of the study, i.e., people who were minors, who were not nationals of any of the participating countries, who did not have Internet access, and/or who did not give informed consent to participate. Due to movement and interaction restrictions worldwide related to the COVID-19 pandemic during the data collection period, the use of non-probability sampling has been a widely used procedure in the past year (Pierce et al., 2020). Individuals who did not give informed consent to participate were not considered in the study. The highest mean age was found in participants residing in Uruguay (M = 41.8; SD = 16.6 years); while the lowest mean age was in participants from Ecuador (M = 24.6; SD = 7.8 years). Furthermore, it can be seen that in all countries there is a higher proportion of women (>60%) than men (<40%). Regarding the educational level of the participants, the majority have completed university studies (>60%). It can also be seen that most of the participants have a professional career (>50%), except in the countries of Ecuador (38.5%) and El Salvador (27.8%), where there is a higher proportion of unskilled work (45.1 and 37.5%, respectively). Table 1 presents the demographic characteristics of the sample for each country.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of the participants in the Americas.

Instruments

Sociodemographic Survey

Participants completed a sociodemographic survey covering aspects related to age, sex, educational level and occupation.

Purpose in Life Test

The PIL assesses meaning in life from a set of 20 items that are answered on a Likert-type scale of seven response categories (Crumbaugh and Maholick, 1964). Categories 1 and 7 have specific response anchors for each item (a score of 1 represents low life purpose and 7 represents high life purpose), and category 4 represents a neutral position. The total score is obtained from the sum of the value of the chosen response on each item, ranging from 20 to 140, where higher scores express clearer meaning and purpose in life. There are no time restrictions, and most respondents answer the 20 questions in 10 to 15 min. The Spanish version by Simkin et al. (2018) was used in this study.

Procedure

This study is part of a larger project aimed at obtaining brief, cross-culturally valid measures of mental health indicators in Latin America during the COVID-19 pandemic (see, for example, Caycho-Rodríguez et al., 2021a,2022, b). The study was conducted between June 12 and September 14, 2020. In all countries, the collection process was the same. A self-administered online survey was prepared on the Google Forms digital platform, which was disseminated through social networks (Facebook, Twitter, WhatsApp, and Instagram) and email. In the self-administered online survey, the objectives of the study, the coordinator’s contact information, and informed consent were presented first. Only after providing informed consent was the self-reported PIL scale accessed. The confidentiality of the data collected was guaranteed; in addition, all participants could withdraw from the study whenever they wished. The project was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Neuroscience Research Center of Rosario and the Laboratory of Cognition and Emotion, belonging to the Faculty of Psychology of the National University of Rosario in Argentina. In addition, the study followed the ethical guidelines of the American Psychological Association (2010).

Data analysis

Three methodological approaches were used to evaluate the internal structure of the scale: (a) Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA), including a Bifactor model, (b) Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling (ESEM) and (c) Bifactor Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling (B-ESEM). All previous psychometric studies of this scale have used classical CFA models (i.e., two- or three-factor related models) to assess the factor structure of the PIL; however, as mentioned above, different studies suggested the presence of multidimensional structures and high interfactor correlations. This may be attributed to the different ways in which people understand life purpose in different cultures or age groups or it might suggest that an adequate analytical model has not been used. In this sense, for multidimensional data, typical CFA models may be too restrictive; whereas, bifactor CFA can give information on multidimensionality due to the coexistence of general and specific factors. CFA models have statistical advantages such as the inclusion of latent variables with measurement error correction (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2009; Marsh et al., 2014). The ESEM model is another viable alternative for a more adequate evaluation of the factorial structure, since it allows cross-loadings (typical of exploratory factor analysis [EFA]) and the use of advanced statistical methods, typical of CFA. Thus, it has been suggested that the ESEM model generates a better fit and fewer correlated factors than the CFA models (Guay et al., 2015). In addition, the ESEM model can be easily and directly compared with previous CFA models. Finally, the B-ESEM model was developed to combine two relevant multidimensionality sources (Bifactor and ESEM) and to have a more accurate assessment of multidimensional measures (Morin et al., 2016). However, when the ESEM model does not present an adequate fit to the data or does not provide smaller interfactor correlations, the CFA model is preferable due to its parsimony (Marsh et al., 2009). Thus, the use of CFA, ESEM and B-ESEM models could contribute significantly to the debate on the structure of the PIL. So far, no study has used all the above-mentioned methods for the evaluation of the factor structure of the PIL.

In the three methodological approaches, the Diagonally Weighted Least Squares with Mean and Variance corrected (WLSMV) estimator was used because the items were at the ordinal level (Brown, 2015). In the CFA models, items were only loaded on their respective factor, while cross-loadings were restricted to zero. In contrast, in the ESEM and B-ESEM models, items loaded on their main factors, while cross-loadings were “targeted,” but not forced, to be as close to zero as possible. For this purpose, a confirmatory rotation approach was employed: (a) GeominQ rotation for the ESEM model and (b) target rotation, specifically targetT, for the B-ESEM model. This makes it possible to specify a priori the indicators of each factor and the free estimation of cross-loadings (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2009). Both models were based on the three-factor related model by Jonsén et al. (2010), as it was the model with the best fit and theoretical support with respect to the other CFA models. In addition, following the recommendation of Marsh et al. (2014) the ESEM and B-ESEM models were compared with the CFA models.

To evaluate the fit of the models, we followed the typical interpretation guidelines: the chi-squared test (χ2), the RMSEA index and the SRMR index, where values below 0.05 indicate a good fit of the model; whereas values between 0.05 and 0.08 express an acceptable fit (Kline, 2015). Likewise, the CFI and TLI indices were used, where values above 0.90 and 0.95 indicate an acceptable and good fit, respectively (Schumacker and Lomax, 2015). The difference between the 23 estimated models was not tested as the models employed different numbers of items and factors (from one factor to four factors). This makes the results of the chi-square difference test uninterpretable. Even in terms of information criterion indices such as AIC, it might be difficult to obtain a value from a comparison, as models with more items would have a higher power of rejection than models with fewer items. To evaluate the internal consistency of the scale, the omega coefficient was used, where a value greater than 0.70 was considered acceptable (McDonald, 1999, Viladrich et al., 2017). Figure 1 shows the graphical representations of the models tested (one-dimensional, two-dimensional, three-dimensional, four-dimensional, bifactor model, ESEM models and ESEM-Bifactor model).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PIL models evaluated.

Measurement invariance of PIL by participant nationality (country) was assessed through two methodological approaches: (a) exact measurement invariance (traditional approach) and (b) approximate measurement invariance (AMI). The traditional approach considers that the factorial weights and intercepts between groups are zero, so that the parameters must be equal to show invariance between groups. On the other hand, for the AMI approach, the factorial weights and intercepts are not necessarily equal between groups that have different cultural characteristics, allowing small differences in the parameters (Byrne and van de Vijver, 2017; Lomazzi, 2018; Fischer and Karl, 2019).

For the evaluation of invariance based on the traditional approach, we used Multi-Group Confirmatory Factor Analysis (MGCFA), which proposes a set of hierarchical variance models: configural invariance (reference model), metric invariance (where factor loadings are equal), scalar invariance (where factor loadings and intercepts are equal) and strict invariance (where factor loadings, intercepts and residuals are equal). The comparison of the sequence of models was carried out based on two strategies: first, a formal statistical test, based on the chi-square difference (Δχ2), where non-significant values (p > 0.05) indicate invariance between groups. Second, we used the differences in the CFI index (ΔCFI), where values less than <0.010 suggest model invariance between groups (Chen, 2007). We also used the RMSEA differences (ΔRMSEA), where values less than <0.015 indicate model invariance (Chen, 2007).

On the other hand, the evaluation of the invariance based on the AMI method used the Multi-Group Factor Analysis Alignment (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2014). Here, an unrestricted configural model was initially fitted to each of the groups. This configural model was then optimized with a component loss function to minimize the invariance in factor means and factor variances in each group (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2014). The invariance tolerance criteria were established as follows, for the factorial weights (λ = 0.40) and the intercepts (ν = 0.20) (Robitzsch, 2020). Likewise, the power of alignment was set at 0.25 for both parameters (Fischer and Karl, 2019). Parameter invariance was evaluated based on the R2 index, which is the average of the specific R2 of each parameter, where values close to 1 indicate a high degree of invariance, while values close to 0 indicate lower invariance (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2014). To consider a scale as non-invariant, a limit of 25% was established to evaluate the percentage of non-invariant parameters (λ and ν) (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2014).

All statistical analyses were carried out using the “lavaan” package (Rosseel, 2012) for the CFA, the “semTools” package (Jorgensen et al., 2018) for the factorial invariance, the “sirt” package (Robitzsch, 2020) for the Alignment method. In all cases, the RStudio environment was used (RStudio Team, 2018). The R codes and data are available in the open access repository OSF: https://osf.io/pd8n4/.

Results

Validity based on the internal structure and reliability of the Purpose in Life Test

The item 13 (“I am: Not very responsible—Very responsible”) has the highest average score in all countries and item 7 (“After retirement, I would like to: Do some things that have interested me—Laze around for the rest of my life”) has the lowest average score in most countries. Regarding the skewness and kurtosis indices, it can be seen that the items present adequate indices in most countries (As < ± 2; Ku < ± 7) according to the criteria of Finney and DiStefano (2013). This can be seen in the Supplementary material 1.

Table 2 shows the different models reported in the scientific literature for the PIL scale. As can be seen, the original model and its variants (1a–1d) do not show adequate adjustment indices in all countries. Similarly, a model of two related factors in its various variants (2a–2d) also does not show adequate fit indices across countries. With respect to a model of three related factors, only models 3c and 3f show acceptable fit indices in all countries. However, model 3f presents estimation problems (“covariance matrix of latent variables is not positive definite”) in the countries of Argentina, Colombia, El Salvador, and Uruguay. It can also be seen that a model with four related factors does not show adequate adjustment indexes in the countries. Table 3 shows that the items of model 3c have a high factorial weight in their corresponding dimension (>0.70) in most countries. It can also be seen that there is a significant relationship between the dimensions of the scale in all the countries. Therefore, model 3c was used as the basis for estimating the Bi-factor, ESEM and B-ESEM models.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Fit indices, factorial weights and reliability of the models by country of participants.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Factorial weight and reliability of the PIL scale by country.

As shown in Table 2, the Bi-factor model could only be estimated in Mexico, since in the other countries it presented estimation problems (“covariance matrix of latent variables is not positive definite”). With respect to the last two models, it can be seen that the ESEM model shows acceptable fit indices and the B-ESEM model shows excellent fit indices in all countries. Nevertheless, it is important to evaluate the factor loadings estimated in the models before choosing the best model. Regarding the ESEM model, it can be seen in Table 3 that the specific factor meaning of existence is well defined in most countries, with 1 to 4 items that evidenced a cross-loading above 0.25 on other factors. The specific factor Freedom to make meaning in daily life is poorly defined in Argentina (|λ| = 0.17 a.40; M| λ| = 0.18), Colombia (|λ| = 0.08 a.53; M| λ| = 0.26) and Uruguay (|λ| = 0.15 a.41; M| λ| = 0.28). In addition, it can be seen that there are 2 to 5 items with cross-loadings greater than 0.25 in other factors. The third factor, Will to find meaning before future challenges, is poorly defined in Argentina (|λ| = 0.21 a.25; M| λ| = 0.23), Colombia (|λ| = 0.19 a.61; M| λ| = 0.36), Ecuador (|λ| = 0.04 a.77; M| λ| = 0.49), Paraguay (|λ| = 0.38 a −0.57; M| λ| = −0.19), and Uruguay (|λ| = −0.13 a −0.20; M| λ| = −0.16). It also shows 3 to 9 items with cross-loadings greater than 0.25 on other factors.

With respect to the B-ESEM model, Table 3 shows that the general factor is well defined in most of the countries, except in Ecuador and El Salvador, where the weight of the items of factor 1 was higher than the general factor. It can also be seen that the first specific factor is not well defined in Argentina (|

λ| = 0.00 a −0.56; M| λ| = 0.17), Colombia (|λ| = −0.05 a −0.61; M| λ| = 0.30), Mexico (|λ| = −0.01 a.75; M| λ| = 0.20) and Uruguay (|λ| = 0.10 a.45; M| λ| = 0.33). Similarly, the second specific factor is not well defined in Argentina (|λ| = 0.19 a.53; M| λ| = 0.30), Ecuador (|λ| = −0.14 a −0.39; M| λ| = 0.28), El Salvador (|λ| = −0.01 a.31; M| λ| = 0.22) and Uruguay (|λ| = −0.02 a −0.51; M| λ| = 0.26). With respect to the third specific factor, item 7 has a very low factorial weight in most of the countries, therefore, it shows problems of representativeness since only two items are left from it. In view of the above, model 3c was chosen to perform the following statistical analyses.

For the estimation of the reliability of the scale (model 3c), the results of the CFA were used. As can be seen in Table 4, the dimensions of the Meaning of existence (ω ≥ 0.92), Freedom to make meaning in daily life (ω ≥ 0.69) and Will to find meaning before future challenges (ω ≥ 0.60) show acceptable reliability estimates in most of the countries, except in Paraguay, specifically for the third factor (ω = 0.52).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Fit indices of the invariance models by country of the participants.

Factor invariance by country

Table 4 shows that the factor structure of the PIL did not show evidence of strict invariance based on MGCFA for the different countries evaluated, based on the sequence of invariance models proposed: metric invariance (ΔCFI = 0.04; ΔRMSEA = −0.02), scalar invariance (ΔCFI = −0.02; ΔRMSEA = 0.00) and strict invariance (ΔCFI = −0.01; ΔRMSEA = 0.00).

On the other hand, the analysis with the Alignment method showed that the factor structure of the PIL is invariant for the factor loadings (R2 = 0.99) and the intercepts of the items (R2 = 0.99) (Table 5). It is also observed that all factor weights are invariant (0%) across countries; although there are 12 non-invariant parameters with respect to the intercepts. However, at the general level, the percentage of non-invariant parameters is low (10.1%). The findings suggest the presence of approximate invariance at the PIL metric and scalar levels.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. ML invariance alignment (IA) in all seven countries.

Discussion

In seeking to respond to the need for a measure of life purpose that can be used in different countries, the present study used a data set from seven Latin American countries (Argentina, Colombia, Ecuador, El Salvador, Mexico, Paraguay, and Uruguay) to evaluate the MI and other psychometric properties of the PIL in Spanish. Overall, the results indicate that the PIL can be used to compare life purpose across some Latin American countries. This is particularly valuable because of the growing need to understand the life purpose of populations in different contexts. Assessing the psychometric properties and cross-cultural utility of the PIL contributes to a better understanding of the theoretical underpinnings of life purpose and provides researchers with information to guide the choice of appropriate indicators to validly and reliably measure this construct.

The PIL has received numerous criticisms due to the inconsistency in the factorial structure (Jonsén et al., 2010), which have their origin in the fact that initially Crumbaugh and Maholick (1964) did not provide information on the method of formulation and selection of the items nor on the statistical treatment of the items (Davies et al., 2014). Thus, after comparing different models, the model of three related factors (Sense of existence, Freedom to give meaning to daily life, and Will to find meaning in the face of future challenges) proposed by Jonsén et al. (2010) is the one with the best fit in all countries. This finding suggests that, in the seven Latin American countries, the PIL is a multidimensional measure that is in line with the assumptions of Frankl’s existential theory, which involves the presence of three assumptions: freedom of will, the will to sense or meaning, and the meaning of life. The com bination of freedom of will and will to meaning generates the freedom a person has to pursue meaning in life. Likewise, the freedom to give meaning to life and the will to find meaning in future challenges involve creative values which are acquired through people’s experiences and attitudes (Frankl, 1958, 1963).

Unlike the original PIL model (Crumbaugh and Maholick, 1964), the Jonsén et al. (2010) model did not consider three items: (1) “When considering the world in relation to my life, the world: totally confuses me-Has meaning for my life,” (2) “I Am: hardly responsible-very responsible,” (3) “Regarding suicide: I have seriously thought it is a way out-I have never thought about it.” Regarding the first question, it has also been removed from the models reported in other studies (McGregor and Little, 1998; Morgan and Farsides, 2009; García-Alandete et al., 2013; Hayashi and Esmerelles, 2017). Apparently, this is a difficult question to answer due to the fact that it expresses the presence of a complex world to which it is not easy to make sense; while, in the second question, the difficulty may lie in the lack of knowledge and the need to see oneself as responsible (Jonsén et al., 2010). Finally, regarding the last question, it is always delicate to answer aspects associated with suicide (Desseilles et al., 2012), which would explain, in some way, its elimination from the model.

From a methodological point of view, the RMSEA values of the three-factor model are at the limit of what is typically considered as acceptable (Kline, 2015), as in the case of Mexico and Paraguay, or above, as in Argentina, Colombia, Ecuador, El Salvador, and Uruguay. At this point, it would have been possible to evaluate the presence of correlated errors among some of the items, which would have improved the model fit, and specifically the RMSEA value. The presence of correlated errors in factor models may suggest similarity in item content, higher task demands, measurement errors and response style (Brown, 2015). However, this assessment was not performed since it may overestimate or underestimate reliability due to the presence of variance that is not related to the construct (in this case, purpose in life) and generate a bias in the interpretation of measurement accuracy (Yang and Green, 2010).

Similarly, the reliability in the dimensions of the proposed three-factor model in most countries was higher than 0.65 (range 0.67 to 0.94), which is in a higher range than the range of 0.54 to 0.83 reported by Jonsén et al. (2010). However, in Paraguay the dimension “making sense of future challenges” had the lowest omega coefficient value (ω = 0.52), which was also reported previously (Jonsén et al., 2010). Therefore, it is suggested that the PIL is a relatively consistent measure of life purpose across samples. In the case of Paraguay, the relevance or not of using the third dimension would have to be evaluated, due to the low measurement precision.

Once the factor structure that best fit the data was determined, we proceeded to determine the MI among the seven countries. Applying the MGCFA method, no MI was reported (neither at the metric: ΔCFI = 0.04 or scalar level: ΔCFI = −0.02) of the PIL for the 7 countries involved, which would not allow for comparing the means between countries without absence of bias. As mentioned above, the classical MGCFA method for assessing MI presents some problems for total invariance, because possible equivalence violations increase as the number of groups to be compared increases, as is often the case for cross-national studies (Davidov et al., 2008, 2014; Muthén and Asparouhov, 2013; Asparouhov and Muthén, 2014). While it is possible to release parameters to achieve good fit and partial invariance, these modifications may produce an inadequate model that may be far from the real model (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2014). Releasing parameters does not guarantee that the means are unbiased; moreover, due to the multicollinearity present in the modification indices, the choice of released parameters could be arbitrary (Marsh et al., 2018).

Although there is a lack of evidence to support MI using the MGCFA method, invariance was also evaluated with the CFA-MIAL method (Marsh et al., 2018), which is suitable for comparing groups of countries, where MI is not expected due to cultural differences between them (Muthén and Asparouhov, 2018). In the present study, all factor weights are invariant, showing metric invariance; whereas 10.1% of intercept parameters are not invariant. Given that less than 25% of the non-invariant parameter estimates is the suggested cut-off point, it can be concluded that most of the PIL items showed scalar invariance. However, due to the novelty of the CFA-MIAL method, this criterion should be considered with caution until more evidence is obtained (Jang et al., 2017). Moreover, there is an absence of studies on the acceptable magnitude of non-invariance. Therefore, it is advisable to accumulate knowledge on the magnitude of non-invariance by reporting the estimates of factor loadings and intercepts, as well as their differences between groups, and to assess the impact of non-invariance in subsequent analyses (Kim et al., 2017). Having fulfilled the requirements based on a three-factor model for the PIL, the results of the more flexible CFA-MIAL method, based on realistic assumptions that relax invariance constraints, indicated that the factor structure of the PIL exhibits a pattern of approximate measurement invariance in the data (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2014). The analysis identified that the intersections of items 3, 4, 7, 10, 14, 15, and 20 show the highest degree of non-invariance. Thus, there seems to be variations in the way some indicators of life purpose are understood and interpreted in populations from different countries. This leads to the suggestion that these items may not be as robust in representing the life purpose of different populations and leads to the suggestion that possible comparisons should be interpreted with caution. On the other hand, the invariant items across the seven countries may be used as anchor or reference items when it is desired to construct a CFA model across countries. Achieving approximate invariance helps to estimate path coefficients and latent means, even if there are non-invariant items (Pokropek et al., 2019).

Although this is the first study on MI of PIL among samples from seven Latin American countries, it is important to recognize some limitations. First, the present study used non-probability convenience sampling. Therefore, the representativeness of the participants in each country is low, which limits the generalization of results to the entire general population of the participating countries. Second, although the total sample size is large (N = 4306), the selection was restricted to only 7 Latin American countries (Argentina, Colombia, Ecuador, El Salvador, Mexico, Paraguay, and Uruguay), which makes it even more difficult to generalize results beyond the case of Latin America. Third, it is also important to note the presence of an unequal representation of participants from Latin American countries. The current group of participants is dominated by samples from South American countries (Argentina, Colombia, Ecuador, Paraguay, and Uruguay) with little presence of participants from other regions of the Americas, such as Central America (El Salvador) and North America (Mexico). It is also important to carry out studies of this type in countries on other continents, such as Europe or Africa, in order to have more evidence of the universality or cultural relativism of the PIL. Fourth, it is important to consider the possibility that the data collection method overestimated the similarities between countries, since information was obtained from a population with similar characteristics, such as being Internet users and having experience in answering online surveys. However, some characteristics among the seven countries were not directly comparable at all, so comparisons of PIL scores may be somewhat biased. For example, Ecuadorians were under 25 years old on average compared to Uruguayans with an average age over 40 years old. With an average age difference of 15 years, the Uruguayans may be more predisposed by age to higher PIL scores. This has been shown previously, where people between the ages of 30 to 55 achieved significantly higher scores on the Spanish version of the PIL than younger people, between the ages of 18 to 29 (García-Alandete et al., 2013). Similarly, the samples from all countries were predominantly female (more than 60%) and the PIL findings may be more representative for women. Furthermore, the method of data collection did not allow for a cultural distinction between countries, as it did not obtain representative data on the subcultures within each country. Fifth, the study had a cross-sectional design, so a causal interpretation of the relationships between meaning in life and fear of COVID-19 cannot be established. Sixth, self-report measures were used to assess meaning in life and fear of COVID-19, which could lead to response bias due to social desirability and reduce the accuracy of the data. However, all participants were assured of their confidentiality and encouraged to answer all questions honestly. Seventh, although cross-cultural MI of the PIL was demonstrated, we were unable to examine why the intersections of some items were not invariant. It is possible that there are other potential predictors for the non-invariance of some items that were not considered in this study. It would be important to investigate factors contributing to life purpose and their effects on future behavior and life outcomes. Additionally, evidence of convergent or discriminant validity of the PIL with other constructs was not assessed. Finally, it is worth noting that the different factorial structures also reflect a marked deficiency in the specialized literature that does not allow for conceptual clarity of the dimensions that make up the PIL. In this sense, independently of investigating the metric properties, future studies should deepen efforts to reach a relative consensus on a more precise theoretical definition that clarifies the purpose in life construct and its facets.

Despite these limitations, this study has some important strengths to mention. First, this is the first study to evaluate the MI of PIL across different countries, specifically in this case, a large sample of seven Latin American countries. Second, the study is the first to employ the CFA-MIAL method to assess PIL invariance. As mentioned before, this is a novel statistical approach to perform joint invariance assessments in a large number of groups, in this case 7 countries. Third, beyond the general MI information, specific information on non-invariant items was provided. By identifying the non-invariant items, a better understanding of the individual PIL items is achieved. Fourth, the findings provide a strong contribution to increase the scope and application of the PIL in Latin America, which is particularly important due to the evidence that this scale is strongly related to aspects associated with well-being (Stoyles et al., 2015; Mei et al., 2021).

In conclusion, the results indicate that the PIL demonstrates MI in seven Latin American countries (Argentina, Colombia, Ecuador, El Salvador, Mexico, Paraguay, and Uruguay). This not only adds empirical evidence for the usefulness of the PIL in the Latin American context, but also contributes to the possibility of cultural comparison of life purpose using the same tool. Thus, important information on the similarity and or difference of life purpose between countries could be obtained. In addition, the understanding of cultural differences can be broadened by focusing on the identification of non-invariant items, non-invariant countries and sources of non-invariance. This is valuable in situations where cultural differences are apparently expected. Also, the results can guide researchers in developing culturally invariant items that measure aspects associated with life purpose. However, if the PIL is used in cross-cultural studies that measure and compare means of life purpose in different Latin American countries, it can be done with approximate invariance. This practice should be used with caution, as the impact of approximate invariance on factor means needs more research, and furthermore, there is still no consensus on whether approximate invariance is close enough to conclude that a measure is invariant (Jang et al., 2017).

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found in the article/Supplementary material.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee of the Neuroscience Research Center of Rosario and the Laboratory of Cognition and Emotion, belonging to the Faculty of Psychology of the National University of Rosario in Argentina. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

TC-R and LV provided initial conception, organization, and main writing of the text. LV and CL-C analyzed the data and prepared all figures and tables. MCe, MG, PM, MCa, CR, CL-C, RM-H, EC-A, ML-R, PC, YQ, ER, MP, OG, AB, MW, and CB-V were involved in data collection for their respective countries and acted as consultants and contributors to research design, data analysis, text writing, and read and approved the draft. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.974133/full#supplementary-material

References

American Psychological Association (2010). Publications manual of the american psychological association. México: Manual Moderno.

Google Scholar

Armas, M., Castedo, A., and Cabaco, A. S. (2018). Fiabilidad y validez del “Purpose in life” (PIL) en una muestra clínica. Eur. J. Health Res. 4, 43–51. doi: 10.30552/ejhr.v4i1.89

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arslan, G., and Allen, K. A. (2022). Exploring the Association Between Coronavirus Stress, Meaning in Life, Psychological Flexibility, and Subjective Well-Being. Psychol. Health Med. 27, 803–814. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2021.1876892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asparouhov, T., and Muthén, B. (2009). Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling. Struct. Equat. Model. 16, 397–438. doi: 10.1080/10705510903008204

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asparouhov, T., and Muthén, B. (2014). Multiple-Group Factor Analysis Alignment. Struct. Equat. Model. A Multidiscip. J. 21, 495–508. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2014.919210

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ato, M., López-García, J. J., and Benavente, A. (2013). Un sistema de clasificación de los diseos de investigación en psicología. Anal. Psicol. 29, 1038–1059. doi: 10.6018/analesps.29.3.178511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrero, J. A. C., Polanco, J. G. C., and Patiño, D. P. S. (2020). Sentido de vida, depresión y ansiedad en población rural de Cundinamarca, Colombia. Rev. Cien. Soc. 26, 206–216.

Google Scholar

Bronk, K. C. (2014). Purpose in life: a critical component of optimal youth development. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-7491-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bronk, K. C. (2020). “The importance of purpose: theory, research, and application,” in Positive psychological science. improving everyday life, well-being, work, education, and societies across the globe, eds S. I. Donaldson, M. Csikszentmihalyi, and J. Nakamura (Milton Park: Routledge), 64–80. doi: 10.4324/9780203731833-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T. A. (2015). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research, Second Edn. New York, NY: Guilford Publications.

Google Scholar

Brunelli, C., Bianchi, E., Murru, L., Monformoso, P., Bosisio, M., Gangeri, L., et al. (2012). Italian validation of the Purpose In Life (PIL) test and the Seeking Of Noetic Goals (SONG) test in a population of cancer patients. Support. Care Cancer 20, 2775–2783. doi: 10.1007/s00520-012-1399-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, B. M., and van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2017). El enfoque de alineamiento de máxima verosimilitud para evaluar de forma aproximada la invarianza de medida: Una aplicación intercultural paradigmática. Psicothema 29, 539–551. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2017.178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caycho-Rodríguez, T., Valencia, P. D., Vilca, L. W., Cervigni, M., Gallegos, M., Martino, P., et al. (2022). Cross-Cultural Measurement Invariance of the Fear of COVID-19 scale in seven Latin American Countries. Death Stud. 48, 2003–2017. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2021.1879318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caycho-Rodríguez, T., Valencia, P. D., Vilca, L. W., Lee, S. A., Carbajal-León, C., Vivanco-Vidal, A., et al. (2021b). COVID-19 Bereavement in Ten Latin American Countries: Measurement Invariance of the Pandemic Grief Scale and Its Relation to Suicidal Ideation. OMEGA-J. Death Dying 302228211048566. doi: 10.1177/00302228211048566. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caycho-Rodríguez, T., Vilca, L. W., Valencia, P. D., Carbajal-León, C., Vivanco-Vidal, A., Saroli-Araníbar, D., et al. (2021a). Cross-Cultural Validation of a New Version in Spanish of Four Items of the Preventive COVID-19 Infection Behaviors Scale (PCIBS) in Twelve Latin American Countries. Front. Psychol. 12:763993. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.763993

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, R. H., and Dodder, R. A. (1983). The Modified Purpose in Life Scale: A Cross-National Validity Study. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 18, 207–217. doi: 10.2190/QKGN-E1HW-CA88-X6K3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct. Equat. Model. 14, 464–504. doi: 10.1080/10705510701301834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, F. F., West, S. G., and Sousa, K. H. (2006). A comparison of bifactor and second-order models of quality of life. Multiv. Behav. Res. 41, 189–225. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr4102_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crumbaugh, J. C., and Maholick, L. T. (1964). An experimental study in existentialism: The psychometric approach to Frankl’s concept ofnoogenic neurosis. J. Clin. Psychol. 20, 200–207. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(196404)20:2<200::AID-JCLP2270200203<3.0.CO;2-U

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damon, W., Menon, J., and Cotton Bronk, K. (2003). The development of purpose during adolescence. Appl. Dev. Sci. 7, 119–128. doi: 10.1207/S1532480XADS0703_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidov, E. (2010). Testing for comparability of human values across countries and time with the third round of the European social survey. Int. J. Comparat. Soc. 51, 171–191. doi: 10.1177/0020715210363534

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidov, E., Meuleman, B., Billiet, J., and Schmidt, P. (2008). Values and support for immigration: A cross-country comparison. Eur. Sociol. Rev. 24, 583–599. doi: 10.1093/esr/jcn020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidov, E., Meuleman, B., Cieciuch, J., Schmidt, P., and Billiet, J. (2014). Measurement equivalence in cross-national research. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 40, 55–75. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-071913-043137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, G., Klaassen, D., and Längle, A. (2014). “Purpose in life test,” in Encyclopedia of quality of life and well-being research, ed. A. Michalos (Heidelberg: Springer), 5238–5243. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-0753-5_23

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol. Inq. 11, 227–268. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desseilles, M., Perroud, N., Guillaume, S., Jaussent, I., Genty, C., Malafosse, A., et al. (2012). Is it valid to measure suicidal ideation by depression rating scales? J. Affect. Disord. 136, 398–404. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dufton, B. D., and Perlman, D. (1986). The association between religiosity and the purpose-in-life test: does it reflect purpose or satisfaction? J. Psychol. Theol. 14, 42–48. doi: 10.1177/009164718601400105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dyck, M. J. (1987). Assessing logotherapeutic constructs: Conceptual and psychometric status of the purpose in life and seeking of noetic goals tests. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 7, 439–447. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(87)90021-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Echeverria, I., Peraire, M., Haro, G., Mora, R., Camacho, I., Almodóvar, I., et al. (2021). Healthcare Kamikazes” during the COVID-19 pandemic: Purpose in life and moral courage as mediators of psychopathology. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:7235. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18147235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabry, J. B. (1988). Guideposts to meaning: discovering what really matters. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Pubns Inc.

Google Scholar

Finney, S. J., and DiStefano, C. (2013). “Nonnormal and categorical data in structural equation modeling,” in Structural equation modeling: A second course, eds G. R. Hancock and R. O. Mueller (Charlotte, NC: IAP Information Age Publishing), 439–492.

Google Scholar

Fischer, R., and Karl, J. A. (2019). A primer to (cross-cultural) multi-group invariance testing possibilities in R. Front. Psychol. 10:1507. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Francke, M. E. (2011). El sentido de la vida y el adulto mayor en el área metropolitana de Monterrey. Available online at: http://psicogerontologia.maimonides.edu/wp-content/uploads/2011/12/PAPERINV-AMA-Y-TRASCIENDE-pdf (accessed April 12, 2022).

Google Scholar

Frankl, V. E. (1958). The will to meaning. J. Pastoral Care 12, 82–88.

Google Scholar

Frankl, V. E. (1963). Man’s search for meaning: an introduction to logotherapy. New York, NY: Washington Square Press.

Google Scholar

Frankl, V. E. (1966). Self-transcendence as a human phenomenon. J. Hum. Psychol. 6, 97–106. doi: 10.1177/002216786600600201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frankl, V. E. (1984). Man’s search for meaning. Washington, DC: Washington Square Press.

Google Scholar

Furman, H. (2021). Propósito, satisfacción y proyecto de vida: una relación dialéctica. Subjet. Proces. Cogn. 25, 73–83.

Google Scholar

García-Alandete, J., Marco, J. H., and Pérez, S. (2017). Purpose-in-life test: Comparison of the main models in patients with mental disorders. Spanish J. Psychol. 20:E31. doi: 10.1017/sjp.2017.28

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Alandete, J., Martínez, E. R., and Nohales, P. S. (2013). Estructura factorial y consistencia interna de una versión española del Purpose-In-Life Test. Univ. Psychol. 12, 517–530. doi: 10.11144/Javeriana.UPSY12-2.efci

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Alandete, J., Ros, M. C., Salvador, J. H. M., and Rodríguez, S. P. (2018). Psychometric properties of the purpose-in-life test and age-related differences among women diagnosed with eating disorders. Psychiatry Res. 261, 161–167. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.12.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Alandete, J., Rosa, E., Sellés, P., Bernabé, G., and Soucase, B. (2016). “The structural validity and internal consistency of a spanish version of the purpose-in-life test,” in Logotherapy and existential analysis. Proceedings of the viktor frankl institute, Vol. 1, ed. A. Batthyány (New York, NY: Springer International Publishing), 75–83.

Google Scholar

Gottfried, A. (2016). Adaptación argentina del PIL Test (Test de Sentido en la Vida) de Crumbaugh y Maholick. Rev. Psicol. 12, 49–65.

Google Scholar

Guay, F., Morin, A. J., Litalien, D., Valois, P., and Vallerand, R. J. (2015). Application of exploratory structural equation modeling to evaluate the academic motivation scale. J. Exp. Educ. 83, 51–82. doi: 10.1080/00220973.2013.876231

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haugan, G., and Moksnes, U. K. (2013). Meaning-in-life in nursing home patients: A validation study of the purpose-in-life test. J. Nurs. Meas. 21, 296–319. doi: 10.1891/1061-3749.21.2.296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayashi, J. R. P., and Esmerelles, M. T. (2017). Análise fatorial exploratória e de consistência interna do purpose-in-life test com estudantes brasileiros. Eur. J. Appl. Bus. Manage. 3, 136–153.

Google Scholar

Huamani, J. C., and Arias, W. L. (2018). Análisis psicométrico de la Prueba de Sentido de Vida en escolares de la ciudad de Arequipa (Perú). Rev. Invest. Psicol. 21, 51–66. doi: 10.15381/rinvp.v21i1.15112

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Humphrey, A., and Vari, O. (2021). Meaning matters: self-perceived meaning in life, its predictors and psychological stressors associated with the COVID-19 pandemic. Behav. Sci. 11:50. doi: 10.3390/bs11040050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutzell, R. R. (1988). A review of the purpose in life test. Int. Forum Logother. 11, 89–101.

Google Scholar

Jang, S., Kim, E. S., Cao, C., Allen, T. D., Cooper, C. L., Lapierre, L. M., et al. (2017). Measurement invariance of the satisfaction with life scale across 26 countries. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 48, 560–576. doi: 10.1177/0022022117697844

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jonsén, E., Fagerström, L., Lundman, B., Nygren, B., Vähäkangas, M., and Strandberg, G. (2010). Psychometric properties of the Swedish version of the Purpose in Life scale. Scand. J. Caring Sci. 24, 41–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6712.2008.00682.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jorgensen, T. D., Pornprasertmanit, S., Schoemann, A. M., and Rosseel, Y. (2018). semTools: Useful tools for structural equation modeling. R package version 0.5-1. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=semTools (accessed May 25, 2022).

Google Scholar

Karataş, Z., Uzun, K., and Tagay, Ö (2021). Relationships between the life satisfaction, meaning in life, hope and COVID-19 Fear for Turkish adults during the COVID-19 Outbreak. Front. Psychol. 12:778. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.633384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, E. S., Cao, C., Wang, Y., and Nguyen, D. T. (2017). Measurement invariance testing with many groups: A comparison of five approaches. Struct. Equat. Model. 24, 524–544. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2017.1304822

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H. S., Jung, S. K., Ko, Y., and Song, J. W. (2001). Reliability and validity of Korean version of ‘purpose in life (PIL) test’ scale. J. Korean Assoc. Soc. Psychiatry 6, 155–165.

Google Scholar

Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., and Kurokawa, M. (2000). Culture, emotion, and well-being: Good feelings in Japan and the United States. Cogn. Emot. 14, 93–124. doi: 10.1080/026999300379003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2015). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling, 4th Edn. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Konkolÿ Thege, B., and Martos, T. (2006). Purpose in life test – characteristics of the hungarian adaptation. Mentál. Pszichos. 7, 153–169. doi: 10.1556/Mental.7.2006.3.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Law, B. M. F. (2012). Psychometric properties of the existence subscale of the purpose in life questionnaire for Chinese adolescents in Hong Kong. Sci. World J. 12, 1–5. doi: 10.1100/2012/685741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindfors, P., Berntsson, L., and Lundberg, U. (2006). Factor structure of Ryff’s psychological well-being scales in Swedish female and male white-collar workers. Personal. Individ. Differ. 40, 1213–1222. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lomazzi, V. (2018). Using alignment optimization to test the measurement invariance of gender role attitudes in 59 Countries. Methods Data Analy. 12, 77–103. doi: 10.12758/mda.2017.09

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magaña Valladares, L., Zavala, M. A., Ibarra Tarango, I., Gómez Medina, M. M. T., and Gómez Medina, M. M. T. (2004). El Sentido de vida en Estudiantes de Primer Semestre de la Universidad de la Salle Bajío. Rev. Centro Invest. Univ. Salle 6, 5–13.

Google Scholar

Mansolf, M., and Reise, S. P. (2017). When and why the second-order and bifactor models are distinguishable. Intelligence 61, 120–129. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2017.01.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, A., Smith, L., Piek, J., and Saunders, B. (2003). The purpose in life scale: Psychometric properties for social drinkers and drinkers in alcohol treatment. Educ. Psychol. Measur. 63, 859–871. doi: 10.1177/0013164403251040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Guo, J., Parker, P. D., Nagengast, B., Asparouhov, T., Muthén, B., et al. (2018). What to do when scalar invariance fails: The extended alignment method for multi-group factor analysis comparison of latent means across many groups. Psychol. Methods 23, 524–545. doi: 10.1037/met0000113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Morin, A. J., Parker, P. D., and Kaur, G. (2014). Exploratory structural equation modeling: An integration of the best features of exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 10, 85–110. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., Muthén, B., Asparouhov, T., Lüdtke, O., Robitzsch, A., Morin, A. J., et al. (2009). Exploratory structural equation modeling, integrating CFA and EFA: Application to students’ evaluations of university teaching. Struct. Equat. Model. 16, 439–476. doi: 10.1080/10705510903008220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez Ortiz, E., and Castellanos Morales, C. (2013). Percepción de sentido de vida en universitarios colombianos. Pensamiento Psicol. 11, 71–82.

Google Scholar

Martínez Ortiz, E., Trujillo Cano, ÁM., and Trujillo, C. A. (2012). Validación del test de propósito vital (PIL test – Purpose in life test) para Colombia. Rev. Argent. Clín. 21, 85–93.

Google Scholar

McDonald, R. P. (1999). Test theory: a unified treatment. Milton Park: Taylor & Francis.

Google Scholar

McGregor, I., and Little, B. R. (1998). Personal projects, happiness, and meaning: On doing well and being yourself. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 74, 494–512. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mei, Z., Lori, A., Vattathil, S. M., Boyle, P. A., Bradley, B., Jin, P., et al. (2021). Important correlates of purpose in life identified through a machine learning approach. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 29, 488–498. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2020.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molcar, C. C., and Stuempfig, D. W. (1988). Effects of world view on purpose in life. J. Psychol. 122, 365–371. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1988.9915523

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moors, G. (2004). Facts and artefacts in the comparison of attitudes among ethnic minorities. A multigroup latent class structure model with adjustment for response style behavior. Eur. Sociol. Rev. 20, 303–320. doi: 10.1093/esr/jch026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, J., and Farsides, T. (2009). Measuring meaning in life. J. Happ. Stud. 10, 197–214. doi: 10.1007/s10902-007-9075-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morin, A. J., Arens, A. K., and Marsh, H. W. (2016). A bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling framework for the identification of distinct sources of construct-relevant psychometric multidimensionality. Struct. Equat. Model. 23, 116–139. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2014.961800

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthén, B., and Asparouhov, T. (2013). BSEM measurement invariance analysis. Mplus Web Notes 17, 1–48.

Google Scholar

Muthén, B., and Asparouhov, T. (2018). Recent methods for the study of measurement invariance with many groups: Alignment and random effects. Sociol. Methods Res. 47, 637–664. doi: 10.1177/0049124117701488

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nascimento, R. B. T., and Dias, T. L. (2019). Teste propósito de vida: propriedades psicométricas e evidências de validade. Rev. Aval. Psicol. 18.

Google Scholar

Ochoa, Y. M., Abreu, M., del, C. L., and Vea, H. D. B. (2018). Validación del Test Sentido de Vida en pacientes adultos con cáncer. Rev. Habanera Cienc. Méd. 9, 156–166.

Google Scholar

Okado, T. (1998). The Japanese version of the purpose in life test. Ciudad de México: System Publica.

Google Scholar

Ortiz, E. M., Cano, ÁM. T., and Trujillo, C. A. (2012). Validación del Test de Propósito Vital (pil test-purpose in life test) para Colombia. Rev. Argent. Clín. Psicol. 21, 85–93.

Google Scholar

Pierce, M., McManus, S., Jessop, C., John, A., Hotopf, M., Ford, T., et al. (2020). Says who? The significance of sampling in mental health surveys during COVID-19. Lancet Psychiatry 7, 567–568. doi: 10.1016/S2215-366(20)30237-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pokropek, A., Davidov, E., and Schmidt, P. (2019). A Monte Carlo simulation study to assess the appropriateness of traditional and newer approaches to test for measurement invariance. Struct. Equat. Model. 26, 724–744. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2018.1561293

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratner, K., Burrow, A. L., Burd, K. A., and Hill, P. L. (2021). On the conflation of purpose and meaning in life: A qualitative study of high school and college student conceptions. Appl. Dev. Sci. 25, 364–384. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2019.1659140

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raykov, T., and Pohl, S. (2013). Essential unidimensionality examination for multicomponent scales: An interrelationship decomposition approach. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 73, 581–600. doi: 10.1177/0013164412470451

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reise, S. P. (2012). The rediscovery of bifactor measurement models. Multiv. Behav. Res. 47, 667–696. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2012.715555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Risco, A. (2009). Sentido de la vida: evolución e implicaciones para la Educación Superior. (Tesis Doctoral inédita). Salamanca: Universidad de Salamanca.

Google Scholar

Robitzsch, A. (2020). sirt: Supplementary Item Response Theory Models (R package version 3.9-4). Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/package=sirt (accessed May 25, 2022).

Google Scholar

Romero-Ramos, N., Romero-Ramos, Ó, and Suarez, A. J. G. (2021). Purpose in life during COVID-19 confinement: Effect of physical activity and meditation. Polish J. Sport Tour. 28, 25–31. doi: 10.2478/pjst-2021-0011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. J. Statist. Softw. 48, 1–36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

RStudio Team. (2018). RStudio: integrated development environment for R. Boston, MA: RStudio, Inc.

Google Scholar

Ryff, C. D., and Singer, B. (1998). The contours of positive human health. Psychol. Inq. 9, 1–28. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0901_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samios, C., Praskova, A., and Radlinska, B. (2021). The relationship between COVID-19 pandemic-related stress and meaning in life: testing the moderating effects of self-compassion and savoring. Anxiety Stress Coping 35, 9–24. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2021.1974408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schimmoeller, E. M., and Rothhaar, T. W. (2021). Searching for meaning with victor frankl and walker percy. Linacre Q. 88, 94–104. doi: 10.1177/0024363920948316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulenberg, S. E., and Melton, A. M. (2010). A confirmatory factor-analytic evaluation of the purpose in life test: Preliminary psychometric support for a replicable two-factor model. J. Happ. Stud. 11, 95–111. doi: 10.1007/s10902-008-9124-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schumacker, R. E., and Lomax, R. G. (2015). A beginner’s guide to structural equation modeling, 4th Edn. Milton Park: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Sezer, S. (2012). A view to the subject of the meaning of life in terms of theoretical and psychometric studies. J. Faculty Educ. Sci. 45, 209–227. doi: 10.1501/Egifak_0000001242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharma, S., Deller, J., Biswal, R., and Mandal, M. K. (2009). Emotional intelligence: Factorial structure and construct validity across cultures. Int. J. Cross Cult. Manage. 9, 217–236. doi: 10.1177/1470595809335725

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shek, D. T. L. L. (1988). Reliability and factorial structure of the Chinese version of the purpose in life questionnaire. J. Clin. Psychol. 44, 384–392. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198805)44:3<384::AID-JCLP2270440312<3.0.CO;2-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simkin, H., Matrángolo, G., and Azzollini, S. (2018). Argentine validation of the Purpose in Life Test. Estud. Psicol. 39, 104–126. doi: 10.1080/02109395.2017.1407903

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steenkamp, J. B. E., and Baumgartner, H. (1998). Assessing measurement invariance in cross-national consumer research. J. Consum. Res. 25, 78–90. doi: 10.1086/209528

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steger, M. F. (2006). An illustration of issues in factor extraction and identification of dimensionality in psychological assessment data. J. Personal. Assess. 86, 263–272. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa8603_03

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steger, M. F., Kawabata, Y., Shimai, S., and Otake, K. (2008). The meaningful life in Japan and the United States: Levels and correlates of meaning in life. J. Res. Personal. 42, 660–678. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2007.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoyles, G., Chadwick, A., and Caputi, P. (2015). Purpose in life and well-being: The relationship between purpose in life, hope, coping, and inward sensitivity among first-year university students. J. Spiritual. Mental Health 17, 119–134. doi: 10.1080/19349637.2015.985558

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tibaldi Nascimento, R. B., and Lebre Dias, T. (2019). Teste Propósito de Vida: Propriedades Psicométricas e Evidências de Validade. Avaliçãao Psicol. 18, 176–182. doi: 10.15689/ap.2019.1802.15459.08

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van De Schoot, R., Kluytmans, A., Tummers, L., Lugtig, P., Hox, J., and Muthén, B. (2013). Facing off with Scylla and Charybdis: a comparison of scalar, partial, and the novel possibility of approximate measurement invariance. Front. Psychol. 4:770. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandenberg, R. J., and Lance, C. E. (2000). A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: Suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organ. Res. Methods 3, 4–70. doi: 10.1177/109442810031002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viladrich, C., Angulo-Brunet, A., and Doval, E. (2017). A journey around alpha and omega to estimate internal consistency reliability. Ann. Psychol. 33, 755–782. doi: 10.6018/analesps.33.3.268401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waisberg, J. L., and Starr, M. W. (1999). Psychometric properties of the purpose in life test with a sample of substance abusers. Int. Forum Logother. 22, 22–26. doi: 10.17795/ijpbs.3753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walters, L. H., and Klein, A. E. (1980). A cross-validated investigation of the Crumbaugh purpose-in-life test. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 40, 1065–1071. doi: 10.1177/001316448004000434

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, T., You, X. Q., Li, Y., and Huang, X. T. (2021). Understanding how youth develop a purpose: A qualitative study among Chinese emerging adults. Curr. Psychol. 1–11. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-01273-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yaccarini, C., and Furman, H. (2017). Materialism, spirituality and purpose of life. {PSOCIAL} 3, 31–38.

Google Scholar

Yang, Y., and Green, S. B. (2010). A note on structural equation modeling estimates of reliability. Struct. Equat. Model. 17, 66–81. doi: 10.1080/10705510903438963

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yela, M. (1996). Los tests. Psicothema 8, 249–263.

Google Scholar

Zuo, B., Yang, K., Yao, Y., Han, S., Nie, S., and Wen, F. (2021). The relationship of perceived social support to feelings of hopelessness under COVID-19 pandemic: The effects of epidemic risk and meaning in life. Personal. Individ. Differ. 183:111110. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: measurement invariance, cross-cultural, purpose in life, Latin American, Analysis Alignment

Citation: Caycho-Rodríguez T, Vilca LW, Cervigni M, Gallegos M, Martino P, Calandra M, Rey Anacona CA, López-Calle C, Moreta-Herrera R, Chacón-Andrade ER, Lobos-Rivera ME, del Carpio P, Quintero Y, Robles E, Panza Lombardo M, Gamarra Recalde O, Buschiazzo Figares A, White M and Burgos-Videla C (2022) Cross-national measurement invariance of the Purpose in Life Test in seven Latin American countries. Front. Psychol. 13:974133. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.974133

Received: 20 June 2022; Accepted: 18 August 2022;
Published: 16 September 2022.

Edited by:

João P. Marôco, University Institute of Psychological, Social and Life Sciences (ISPA), Portugal

Reviewed by:

Luis Anunciação, Pontifical Catholic University of Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
Daniel Vicentini Oliveira, University Center of Maringá (UniCesumar), Brazil

Copyright © 2022 Caycho-Rodríguez, Vilca, Cervigni, Gallegos, Martino, Calandra, Rey Anacona, López-Calle, Moreta-Herrera, Chacón-Andrade, Lobos-Rivera, del Carpio, Quintero, Robles, Panza Lombardo, Gamarra Recalde, Buschiazzo Figares, White and Burgos-Videla. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tomás Caycho-Rodríguez, dG9tYXMuY2F5Y2hvQHVwbi5wZQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.