Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 08 September 2022
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Motivation to participate in secondary science communication

Zhichen HuZhichen HuBaolong MaBaolong MaRubing Bai
Rubing Bai*
  • School of Management and Economics, Beijing Institute of Technology, Beijing, China

The rise of social media provides convenient mechanisms for audiences to participate in secondary science communication (SSC). The present study employs the theory of consumption values and theory of planned behavior to predict audiences’ SSC intentions. The results indicate that emotional value, social value, altruistic value, attitude, internal perceived behavioral control and subjective norm are significant predictors of audiences’ intentions to share or to repost science content on their social media. These results suggest that the theory of consumption values, together with the theory of planned behavior, is a useful framework for understanding SSC behaviors.

Introduction

The rise of social media has dramatically transformed the way the audiences engage or interact with science communicators. For a long time, science organizations and scientists have been delivering knowledge to audiences through science cafes (Dallas, 2014), science festivals (Jensen and Buckley, 2014; Boyette and Ramsey, 2019; Nielsen et al., 2019), Web and e-mails (Duke, 2002), etc. With social media, science communication practitioners’ work has become increasingly fast-paced--leaving less time for investigation, storytelling and curating what information should be disseminated (Massarani et al., 2021). Compared with the traditional science communication platforms, social media is recognized as a more efficient one for science communicators and audiences due to its advantages of widespread, faster speed, deeper interactivity and visibility (Lee and VanDyke, 2015; Su et al., 2017; Lee et al., 2018). For example, institutions and scientists can share their research on Twitter, Instagram, WeChat, and Weibo in a direct and instantaneous fashion (Lee and VanDyke, 2015). And audiences can engage with these updates through a few ways, including “Like,” “Comment,” “Share” and “Repost” (Hwong et al., 2017). More importantly, social media extends the traditional role of audience, typically providing them with roles of the “audience” or the “communicator,” or both of them, according to the stage they are in, as shown in Figure 1. By evaluating and selectively disseminating the message originally posted by an organization or a scientist, audience can help circulate information to a wider group than the organization’s own pool of followers, thus facilitating new rounds of discussions and greatly enhancing science communication effectiveness (Boyd et al., 2010; Lovejoy and Saxton, 2012; Hermida et al., 2014; Su et al., 2017).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The relationship between PSC and SSC.

A growing body of research shows that scientists consider public communication in a relatively simple way (Davies, 2008; Jensen et al., 2008; Peters et al., 2008; Dunwoody et al., 2009; Nisbet and Markowitz, 2015; Besley et al., 2016; Choi and Kim, 2020), indicating that communicators’ interaction with their audience in the digital public sphere requires more cautiousness for fear of criticism and personal attacks, or being framed by suggestive questions and potentially receiving low-quality comments from audience. And these could in turn lead to a decline of trust in similar communicators or scientists on the platform (Dermentzi and Papagiannidis, 2018; Hara et al., 2019; Jones et al., 2019; Sajeev et al., 2019). Moreover, research has shown that scientists often approach their engagement work through a narrow set of skills and considerations (Franks et al., 2013; Peters, 2013; Grand et al., 2015; Dudo and Besley, 2016; Besley et al., 2018; Yuan et al., 2018). Meanwhile, the science communication ecosystem is seen fragmented, with numerous interfaces where professional science communicators and new science communication actors interact with audiences in myriad ways (Bubela et al., 2009; Rutsaert et al., 2013). Therefore, science organizations and scientists show limited influence on social media (McClain, 2019), which further highlights the importance of audiences acting as “communicators” and disseminating science content on social media.

However, according to the academic research and science communication literature, in fact, audiences do not always participate in the online dissemination: Audience engagement in online science content is transient and brief (Thaler et al., 2012), and institutions lack clear plans for the goals and expected outcomes of online communication (Bik et al., 2015). Most audiences only share science content in a single-connected community, but they could have spread it to new audiences or those who in great need of scientific education (Williams et al., 2015). Although the patterns of dissemination differ over time, the impact of science content on the public still remains limited (Alperin et al., 2019), and engagement of each online user tends to decline as the audience size increases (Kahle et al., 2016).

What’s worse, the lack of online gatekeepers to distinguish between correct interpretations of peer-reviewed data and personal opinions, coupled with people’s disposition to communicate in isolated communities, can lead to the spreading and reinforcement of false information, hereafter referred to as misinformation (Bessi et al., 2015; Radzikowski et al., 2016). The misinformation present on social media can spread even more quickly to a broad range of people in a short amount of time, increasing the likelihood that information seekers will notice misinformation and incorporate it into their worldviews (Del Vicario et al., 2016; Vosoughi et al., 2018). These effects are aggravated when media consumers have a low level of media literacy and are not able to recognize misinformation (Scheufele and Krause, 2019). Therefore, a clear understanding of the causes of SSC is crucial for effectively managing the science communication.

This study focuses on the predictors of secondary science communication (SSC) on social media. Specifically, this study entails a survey designed to better understand audiences’ SSC behaviors. Previous literature has defined the transmission of information among audiences as “retransmission” (Kim et al., 2013; Luarn et al., 2014; Sutton et al., 2015), but this definition only applies to the direct repetition of original content, ignoring the audience’s right to screen and evaluate or even reprocess information and the special attributes of social media context. Thus, we define “SSC” as audience’s selective dissemination of the original science information from professional communicators to other audiences according to their social scope and preference, in which an information reprocessing may be included.

Our definition clarifies the following four characteristics of SSC: (1) SSC differs from PSC. In the case of PSC (as shown in Figure 1), professional organizations or individuals post science content which is new to the network. When this science content is shared by audiences, SSC has occurred. More specifically, SSC is the reposting of science content already on the Internet. Both SSC and PSC are forms of science communication, as each process involves communication of science content to others (Dockter et al., 2020). (2) SSC also often occurs offline, but social media provides a lower threshold and a wider platform for SSC. In this study, we focus on the SSC behavior online, mostly on social media, including sharing science content with specific friends or in Chat Group, @mentions friends on Twitter, reposting it to WeChat Moments, and modified reposting, etc. (3) “SSC” concept emphasizes audience’s value-based evaluation and selective dissemination. In other words, audiences will not always participate in the SSC, but will disseminate specific content based on their own time, resources, preferences and evaluations. Compared with “retransmission,” “SSC” represents a conscious shift away from one-way, linear dissemination of scientific information to a bidirectional, participatory, deliberative communication in which the audiences are active participants (Kurath and Gisler, 2009). (4) Resharing the science content shared by an audience is also regarded as SSC, and the information may be reprocessed in this series of processes. The science content produced by professional institutions may be boring and difficult to understand, but it can be more colorful, interesting and credible after being slightly processed by the audience (Welbourne and Grant, 2016; Dockter et al., 2020).

Literature review

Science communication plays a number of vital roles in society (Fischhoff and Scheufele, 2013; Davies, 2021), including effectively engaging with diverse stakeholders (Weingart and Joubert, 2019), combating misinformation (Goldstein et al., 2020), and encouraging wider participation in Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics which is known as STEM (Bevan et al., 2020). Burns et al. (2003) recognize five main purposes of science communication: awareness, enjoyment, interest, opinion formation, and understanding. Through a qualitative interview study with scholars and professors, Davies (2021) proposed six types of roles for science communication within society. It ensures the accountability and legitimacy of publicly funded science, has practical value (such as enabling laypeople and policy makers to make good choices in today’s technologically oriented societies) and enhances democracy by empowering citizens. Moreover, science communication allows access to the beauty of science as an aspect of culture, serves promotional purposes and casts economic effects in recruiting people into scientific careers or preparing a market base for technological innovations. In general, emphasizing the functions of public communication and engagement with science for democracy, science communication is defined as a process where information is transferred or negotiated, as well as a means through which societies can nurture shared sense-making about the issues that face them (Weingart and Joubert, 2019; Davies, 2021).

While science communication literature calls on communicators and audiences to interact and build relationships on social media (Taylor and Kent, 2014; Avidar et al., 2015), there is still a lack of research on the predictors of audience dissemination. Previous research related to social media focuses mostly on scientists’ motivation for choosing online platforms (Poliakoff and Webb, 2007; Mewburn and Thomson, 2013; Jia et al., 2017), how effectively science institutions are using social media (Lee and VanDyke, 2015; Lee et al., 2018), core topics around scientific issues (Pearce et al., 2014; Büchi, 2017; Lee et al., 2020), audience’s trust in online science content (Liang et al., 2014; Huber et al., 2019) as well as the extent to which audiences use social media (Anderson et al., 2010; Su et al., 2015; Hargittai et al., 2018). However, little has examined audience’s SSC behavior. Moreover, given the scarcity of systematic analysis into the mechanisms of SSC, current practices of science communication on social media are based on intuition and experiences rather than empirical evidence. To be more specific, a deeper understanding of the predictors of SSC can help communicators to better manage SSC. Specifically, institutions and scientists can adopt different communication strategies (such as providing either more functional value or altruistic value) for different science content and adjust their strategies in time according to the feedback of the audiences (such as Like, Comment, Share and Repost; Das et al., 2021; Li and Guo, 2021) and finally contributes to a more effective science communication.

Behavioral theories believe that any type of behavior, SSC included, can be predicted by psychological variables. Two theories can be particularly helpful for predicting audiences’ intentions to participate in SSC: Theory of consumption values (TCV) and Theory of planned behavior (TPB).

Theory of consumption values

TCV was proposed by Sheth et al. (1991) to explain why consumers choose one product over the others. Consumption value is the estimate of product utility that consumers received compared to their effort to reach or consume the product, and it is also the key determinant of consumers’ attitude and behavior (Zeithaml, 1988). Marketing scholars believe that there are two types of motivation for consumer behavior: functional needs and nonfunctional needs. Based on this concept, consumption value can be segmented into specific factors, such as functional, emotional, and social value. The importance of consumption value theory lies in the assumption that consumers balance value assessments to make informed internal and external decisions, which is more than just purchasing behavior (Kim et al., 2007).

TCV frameworks has been used in a variety of domains and thus additional value dimensions have been developed within the recent years. For example, Sweeney and Soutar (2001) proposed quality, emotional, price, and social values in a retail purchase situation; Turel et al. (2010) confirmed visual appeal, social, playfulness, and value-for-money as factors that affect word-of-mouth intentions of hedonic digital artifacts; Kim and Chang (2020) applied economic, emotional, social, and altruistic values to investigate the intention to take part in festivals. And in the domain of social media, TCV has been used to study the intention to pay for social networking sites (Lu and Hsiao, 2010), usage construct with regard to Facebook (Aladwani, 2014), and reason of using online social media brand community (Kaur et al., 2018). However, to our best knowledge, the domain of science communication has not been examined from the perspective of TCV.

Science communication has been proposed on the assumption that ignorance is the basis of a lack of societal support for various issues in science and technology. This model is known as the knowledge deficit model of science communication (Simis et al., 2016). Deficit model thinking proposes the belief that public skepticism toward modern science is caused by a lack of adequate knowledge about it. Furthermore, this skepticism, or “knowledge deficit,” can be overcome by providing sufficient information to the public (Besley and Tanner, 2011). This model adopts a one-way, top-down communication process, in which scientists—with all the required information—filled the knowledge vacuum in the scientifically illiterate general public as they saw fit. Scientific facts and methods were the vital components of public understanding for the deficit model (Miller, 2001). For example, science communication studies find that audiences have deficient knowledge about science with a lack of interest and low trust in it (Durant, 1999; Bauer et al., 2007), and that providing the audiences with knowledge about science increases their motivation to process scientific information (Bauer, 2016; Hargittai et al., 2018). Therefore, science content on social media needs to possess high values and interestingness to attract audiences to engage with messages (Hwong et al., 2017).

On the other hand, online science content can be perceived as a service provided by communicators (Su et al., 2015; Bai et al., 2019). And communicators use affordances of social media to engage in presentation of science content to garner public attention to science (Bhargava and Velasquez, 2021). Attention is a key resource for social movements (Davenport and Beck, 2001). Tufekci (2013) suggested that attention is the means through which a social movement can introduce and fight for its preferred framing, convince broader publics of its cause, recruit new members, attempt to neutralize opposition framing, access solidarity, and mobilize its own adherents. Therefore, communicators need to provide audiences with content that interests them as much as possible to get more attention. For example, the science content “liked” by audiences on social media can be widely disseminated, while as for those that audiences disliked tend to have a lower number of comments and retweets, ultimately resulting in poorer communication. In other words, SSC can be partially explained by “Attention Economy” and represents audiences’ attention consumption choice-oriented behavior. Thus, TCV is appropriate for predicting audiences’ intention to participate in SSC.

In the present study, we apply functional, emotional, social, and altruistic value to explain audiences’ SSC intentions. Specifically, functional value refers to “audience perceived utility acquired from the expected performance of the online communicator or science content.” Emotional value stands for “audience perceived utility acquired from the feelings or affective states that science content generates.” Social value represents “audience perceived utility acquired from the enhanced social self-concept by following the online communicator.” Altruistic value denotes “audience perceived utility acquired from helping others through gaining science knowledge” (Sweeney and Soutar, 2001; Kim and Chang, 2020).

Theory of planned behavior

TPB was proposed by Ajzen (1991), suggesting that an individual’s behavior is affected by the attitude toward the behavior, what other people think of the behavior, and how much control has over the expected barriers. TPB has received widespread support as a model of behavior and has been adopted to understand audience’s adoption of communicated information (Paulussen et al., 1995; Longnecker, 2016), environmental civic engagement (Ho et al., 2015; Witzling et al., 2015), educators’ attitudes toward science (van Aalderen-Smeets et al., 2012), and scientists’ communication behavior (Breslin et al., 2001; Poliakoff and Webb, 2007; Dijkstra et al., 2015).

Similarly, TPB frameworks are also useful for understanding SSC behavior. To be more specific, if an audience does not consider it important to repost science knowledge, the audience will hardly participate in SSC no matter how attractive the science content seems to be (Attitude). And with a high level of media literacy (Internal perceived behavioral control) and abundant time (External perceived behavioral control), one can confidently recognize misinformation and consider it easy to repost science content on social media, then the audience will be likely to participate in SSC as well. In addition, whether the audience’s friends frequently repost science content on social media can also influence the SSC intention (Subjective norm and objective norm). In other words, if both an audience and their peers believe SSC is important, then the audience will be more willing to participate in SSC. Williams et al. (2015) found that science content is more easily spread in a single-connected community, which supports the influence of subjective norm and objective norm from the side.

Therefore, this study applies attitude, internal perceived behavioral control, external perceived behavioral control, subjective norm, and descriptive norm to predict SSC intention. Based on the original definitions of the variables in TPB as well as the research background of this study, attitude means “audience’s attitude toward SSC behavior.” Internal perceived behavioral control refers to “audience’s internal perception that he possesses control over personal resources to participate in SSC, such as confidence, adequate planning, and the ability.” External perceived behavioral control stands for “audience’s internal perception that he has control over external conditions and situations to participate in SSC, such as time, channels, and availability.” Subjective norm signifies “audience’s perception of significant referents’ opinions toward SSC behavior.” Descriptive norm means “audience’s perception of significant referents’ typically SSC behavior” (Armitage et al., 1999; Armitage and Conner, 1999; Poliakoff and Webb, 2007).

Hypotheses

This study examines the extent to which audiences’ SSC intentions are predicted by TCV-based predictors (Functional value, Emotional value, Social value, Altruistic value) as well as by TPB-based predictors (Attitude, Internal perceived behavioral control, External perceived behavioral control, Subjective norm, Descriptive norm).

Although, as far as we consider, there is no science communication research based on TCV, audience’s SSC is essentially a form of consumption choice-oriented behavior (Su et al., 2015). Thus, we argue that the higher the values of science content, the higher the audiences’ intentions to be involved in SSC. Precisely speaking, we propose that perceived functional value positively affects SSC intentions (Hypothesis 1a); perceived emotional value positively affects SSC intentions (Hypothesis 1b); perceived social value positively affects SSC intentions (Hypothesis 1c); perceived altruistic value positively affects SSC intentions (Hypothesis 1d).

In addition, Poliakoff and Webb (2007) indicates that attitude, perceived behavioral control, and descriptive norms can positively predict scientist’s intentions to participate in public engagement activities. These effects may be similar between the scientist and the audience. Therefore, we propose that attitude positively affects SSC intentions (Hypothesis 2a); internal perceived behavioral control positively affects SSC intentions (Hypothesis 2b); external perceived behavioral control positively affects SSC intentions (Hypothesis 2c); subjective norm positively affects SSC intentions (Hypothesis 2d); descriptive norm positively affects SSC intentions (Hypothesis 2e).

Materials and methods

Participants

We commissioned a sample service company1 to collect data. Sojump.com has 8.39 million panel members in China. We designated that only participants who followed (at least) a science communicator on social media could participate in the survey. The final sample consisted 489 complete cases, after deleting 74 completions that failed a series of filter questions (including 3 reverse scale items).

In terms of the participants, 63.2% were female, and their average age was 26 years (SD = 5.58). Participants with postgraduate degree account for 12.5% of the entire sample, undergraduate 81.0% while high school or below takes up 6.5%. Furthermore, the majority of the participants earned 5,001–10,000 yuan per month (40.3%). Altogether, 40.1% of the participants earned less than 5,000 yuan per month, 13.3% earned 10,001–15,000 yuan, 4.1% earned 15,001–20,000 yuan, and 2.2% earned more than 20,001 yuan monthly. According to the National Bureau of Statistics (2021), the annual per capita disposable income of China was 35,100 yuan, and the ratio of male to female population in China is around 51.2%. Therefore, the sample distribution is basically consistent with the national conditions.

Questionnaire

According to Huang and Yang (2020); Taragin-Zeller et al. (2020); and Besley et al. (2021), at the beginning of the questionnaire, participants read: “With the development of the Internet, many science organizations or individual communicators have appeared on social media, such as Guokr, Kepuchina.cn, UFO Talk.2 They spread science knowledge through social media like WeChat public account, micro-blog, TikTok and so on. At the same time, people can also share and repost their science content to their friends on these social media.” The above-mentioned science communicators have all got a certification as “popular science communicator” on the social media platforms. Then the subjects were asked to recall a science communicator they had followed on social media and fill in the questionnaire according to their perceptions. Participants were required to write down the name of the communicator, their content (whether the communicator mainly spread science knowledge through articles or videos), and the frequency they went through the content posted by the communicator.

The communicators listed by the participants are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Information of the communicators listed by participants.

Items adopted in this research were all taken from their English versions and translated and them to the Chinese setting. Two different bilingual researchers translated the English versions of the scales into Chinese and translated them back into English to make sure the meanings of the items remain the same in Chinses and English. All the seven-point (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree) measurements of variables are borrowed from previous studies. The questionnaire ends with demographic questions, including gender, age, education and monthly income.

Functional value was measured with three items (Sweeney and Soutar, 2001): “I think the content produced by this communicator is well made,” “I think the content produced by this communicator meets my needs,” “I think this communicator has provided me with good help” (Alpha coefficient = 0.83).

Emotional value was measured with three items (Sweeney and Soutar, 2001): “The content produced by this communicator will make me feel good,” “The content produced by this communicator is something that I would enjoy,” “The content produced by this communicator would give me pleasure” (Alpha coefficient = 0.86).

Social value was measured with three items (Sweeney and Soutar, 2001): “Following this communicator would make a good impression on other people,” “Following this communicator would help me to feel acceptable,” “Following this communicator would give its owner social approval” (Alpha coefficient = 0.82).

Altruistic value was measured with three items (Kim and Chang, 2020): “Gaining science knowledge from this communicator helps me make social contributions,” “Gaining science knowledge from this communicator can provide pure help to other people,” “Gaining science knowledge from this communicator is a kind of social contribution” (Alpha coefficient = 0.80).

Attitude was based on three items (Armitage et al., 1999): “It is a good idea to share or to repost science content on social media,” “It makes sense to share or to repost science content on social media,” “It is important to share or to repost science content on social media” (Alpha coefficient = 0.81).

Internal perceived behavioral control was based on three items (Armitage and Conner, 1999): “I believe I have the ability to share or to repost science content on social media,” “If it were entirely up to me, I am confident that I would be able to share or to repost science content on social media,” “I am confident to participate in sharing or reposting science content on social media” (Alpha coefficient was 0.85).

External perceived behavioral control was based on four items (Armitage and Conner, 1999): “There are likely to be plenty of opportunities for me to share or to repost science content on social media,” “I have many social media channels to share or to repost science content,” “Whether or not I share or repost science content on social media is entirely up to me,” “If I want to, I can share or repost science content on social media”(Alpha coefficient = 0.78).

Subjective norm was based on three items (Armitage et al., 1999): “My friends think I should share or repost science content on social media,” “My friends want me to share or to repost science content on social media,” “My friends would approve of my sharing or reposting science content on social media” (Alpha coefficient = 0.83).

Descriptive norm was based on two items (Poliakoff and Webb, 2007): “Some friends you know best often share or repost science content on social media,” “Many people around you share or repost science content on social media” (Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient = 0.82).

Intention was measured based on Kim and Chang (2020) scale: “I am willing to share and repost the information about this communicator on social media,” “I am willing to share and repost the content produced by this communicator on social media,” “I am willing to share and repost the knowledge learned from this communicator on social media,” “I will share a positive assessment of this communicator on social media with others” (Alpha coefficient = 0.89).

Analysis

Pearson correlations, means, and standard deviations were presented in Table 2. We conducted hierarchical regression analyses by first including control variables (Model 1). Then we included TCV variables (Model 2), followed by TPB variables (Model 3). The maximum Variance Inflation Factor for each variable was 2.84, which was lower than the standard value 5 proposed by Hair et al. (2011), indicating that the results were not disturbed by multicollinearity (see Table 3).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean, Standard Deviations, and correlations.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Regression results.

Results

Hierarchical regression results indicated that the frequency of watching science content (Β = 0.22, SE = 0.04, β = 0.28, p < 0.01) and monthly income (Β = 0.14, SE = 0.06, β = 0.12, p < 0.05) were significant predictors of participants’ SSC intentions (Table 3). To be more specific, higher income and more frequent exposure to scientific content promote audience’s SSC intention. However, age, gender, education, type of communicator (organization or individual) and type of content (article or video) were nonsignificant.

Further, the influence of income on SSC intention is significant in Model 1 and 2, but was nonsignificant in Model 3. Therefore, it is possible that the TPB variables mediated the influence of income on SSC intention. In other words, audience with higher income has stronger SSC intention because of more positive attitude, more internal perceived behavioral control, and more subjective norm. And this finding is not difficult to understand. Consider an engineer in a tech company as an example. Since mastering science and technology brings high income, this engineer will naturally have a more positive attitude towards participating in SSC (Attitude); because of the professionalism and scientific literacy, this engineer will have more confidence, adequate planning, and the ability to recommend professional science knowledge to his relatives and friends (Internal perceived behavioral control); and because of the high income, this engineer would feel a greater responsibility to engage in SSC (Subjective norm).

When TCV variables were included in the model, emotional value (Β = 0.23, SE = 0.06, β = 0.21, p < 0.01), social value (Β = 0.17, SE = 0.05, β = 0.16, p < 0.01), and altruistic value (Β = 0.29, SE = 0.05, β = 0.27, p < 0.01) were significant predictors of SSC intentions, while functional value was nonsignificant. These results indicated that science content with higher emotional, social, and altruistic value made participants more willing to share or to repost science information. Thus, the results support Hypothesis 1b, 1c, and 1d.

With TPB variables included in the model, attitude (Β = 0.23, SE = 0.04, β = 0.21, p < 0.01), internal perceived behavioral control (Β = 0.27, SE = 0.05, β = 0.25, p < 0.01), and subjective norm (Β = 0.17, SE = 0.05, β = 0.15, p < 0.01) were significant predictors of SSC intentions, but external perceived behavioral control and descriptive norm were not. In addition, social value was no longer significant in this model (Β = −0.03, SE = 0.05, β = −0.03, p = 0.50). Thus, Hypotheses 2a, 2b and 2d were supported while Hypothesis 2c and 2e were rejected.

Discussion

Digitalization and the rise of social media platforms have revolutionized the way in which scientists interact with diverse non-scientific publics (Bubela et al., 2009; Rutsaert et al., 2013). A wide range of social media platforms give scientists new means to share scientific insights with citizens directly, but also allow audiences to generate information themselves (Bubela et al., 2009; Rutsaert et al., 2013; Hara et al., 2019). This has extended the range of actors involved in the production and use of scientific knowledge to artists, activists, bloggers, amateur enthusiasts and social media influencers — social media extends the role of audiences, typically considering them roles as both the “audience” and the “communicator.”

This study has important implications for the practices of online science communication. We suggested that a positive SSC can effectively address the problems scientists face when disseminating science knowledge (think about communication in a simple way, fear of criticism and personal attacks, narrow set of skills and considerations), but a negative SSC can lead to more serious outcomes (spreading and reinforcement of false information). Therefore, understanding of the predictors of SSC can help communicators to better manage SSC. We contend that scientists can provide different values for different science content and adjust their strategies in time according to the audiences’ feedback to increase their interest in participating in SSC.

On this background, we first proposed the concept of SSC and then encouraged audiences to actively participate in SSC. Different from the concept of “retransmission” which is only applicable to the simple repetition of original information (Kim et al., 2013; Luarn et al., 2014; Sutton et al., 2015), “SSC” emphasizes audience’s value-based evaluation and selective dissemination, and considers audience’s information reprocessing behavior. Furthermore, the “retransmission” concept reflects the traditional unidirectional, sender-receiver mode of science communication. In contrast, SSC represents a bidirectional, participatory, deliberative communication in which the audiences are active participants (Kurath and Gisler, 2009). And we suggest that the “SSC” concept is more suitable in a social media context, because it builds a bridge between science communicators and lay audiences in equal conversations (Scheufele, 2014).

Previous researches on scientific communication highlights the importance of encouraging audiences’ participation. However, literatures are largely silent on the way to accomplish it on social media or factors leading to audiences’ online SSC behavior as a main form of science participation. For example, reposting and modified reposting on social media lead to audiences’ greater perceived source credibility, perceived content effectiveness, and likelihood to engage in the science content (Dong, 2015; Dockter et al., 2020). To fill in this gap, the current research employs the TCV and TPB to explain audiences’ SSC behaviors, with its results indicating that both TCV-based and TPB-based factors play an important role in predicting audiences’ SSC intention on social media.

Specifically, it is found that science content with higher emotional, social, and altruistic value leads to audiences’ stronger SSC intentions. This suggests that communicators can promote audience SSC behavior by enhancing the color of science content and emphasizing the social and altruistic value of learning science knowledge. Moreover, functional value has little effect on SSC intentions, which, to a degree, can explain why even if some scientific contents seem helpful, they can only be effectively spread in a single-connected community (Williams et al., 2015). In other words, audiences need far more than science content that only meets their basic demand for knowledge acquiring. More efforts should be put in by communicators to make science content more colorful and make it easier for those audiences who give attentions to science to feel its perceived social value and its possible contribution to the whole society. Only in this way, can it be possible to get more audiences involved into SSC.

Although previous research has examined audience attitude towards online science content, they focused more on non-value predictors, such as gender of the science communicator (Chau, 2010; Abisheva et al., 2014), veracity of the information (Pandey et al., 2010; Murugiah et al., 2011; Azer, 2012), type of channel (Kim, 2012; Welbourne and Grant, 2016), topic and sentiment of a tweet (Naveed et al., 2011), etc. However, as science content gains popularity among a broader audience, the scientific communication must be useful and appealing. Therefore, studying the value predictors is vital to understand what drives popularity broadly (Welbourne and Grant, 2016). Therefore, we focus on TCV-based predictors, as opposed to non-value factors because they are very helpful for communicators to produce high-quality science content and also useful for understanding drivers of wide spread online.

Moroeover, attitude, internal perceived behavioral control, and subjective norm can lead to positive SSC intention. These findings are helpful for communicators to improve the communication effectiveness of their science-related content, as audience engagement in science is low due to disinterest and lack of motivation to process science information (Bauer, 2016; Hargittai et al., 2018). On the one hand, we suggest that communicators should encourage audiences to participate in science discussion as well as to repost science content on their social media, in order to promote consumer to form a positive attitude towards SAC and subjective norm. On the other hand, government and online communicators should work together to build a suitable environment for audiences to disseminate SSC. For example, adequate freedom should be provided for scientific discussion and equal attention should be given to comments and reposting on social media.

As with all studies, this research has several limitations that may be addressed by future research. First, as a result of cognitive dissonance, audience might increase the perceived value of science content after they have participated in SSC. Further investigation conducted through lab experiment designs can verify the results of the current research. Second, the participants were followers of science communicator on social media. And if the participants had a degree of interest might lead to “Ceiling Effect,” this could have some impact on findings associated to SSC attitudes. Additionally, the science communication on social media is more fragmented and the online users already have a high willingness to share (Rutsaert et al., 2013; Mingoia et al., 2017), thus the survey focused only on social media may ignore some other information. And there may be some differences in communication behaviors between different social medias. Thus, we call for further research on SSC on social media, especially in cross-media contexts. The survey is based on Chinese samples and a high percentage of the sample are highly educated. Therefore, it should be further tested whether the same effects still exist in other countries or in low educational level audiences in the future.

Despite these limitations, this study points out the necessity of SSC behavior on social media and provides clear evidence that TCV and TPB are useful for predicting audiences’ SSC intention. Results are informative considering it is one of the first few studies that deal with audience dissemination issues, which, if managed efficiently, can contribute to a wider audience engagement with science thus promoting a higher level of science communication environment.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

Ethical review and approval were not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author contributions

ZH and BM conceived the study and wrote the first draft of the article. RB was responsible for revising and proofreading the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

References

Abisheva, A., Garimella, V. R. K., Garcia, D., and Weber, I. (2014). Who watches (and shares) what on youtube? And when? Using twitter to understand youtube viewership. Proceed. 7th ACM intern.l conferen. Web search. data mining, 2014, 593–602. doi: 10.1145/2556195.2566588

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 50, 179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aladwani, A. M. (2014). Gravitating towards Facebook (GoToFB): what it is? And how can it be measured? Comput. Hum. Behav. 33, 270–278. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alperin, J. P., Gomez, C. J., and Haustein, S. (2019). Identifying diffusion patterns of research articles on twitter: a case study of online engagement with open access articles. Public Underst. Sci. 28, 2–18. doi: 10.1177/0963662518761733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, A. A., Brossard, D., and Scheufele, D. A. (2010). The changing information environment for nanotechnology: online audiences and content. J. Nanopart. Res. 12, 1083–1094. doi: 10.1007/s11051-010-9860-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armitage, C. J., and Conner, M. (1999). Distinguishing perceptions of control from self-efficacy: predicting consumption of a low-fat diet using the theory of planned behavior 1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 29, 72–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1999.tb01375.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armitage, C. J., Conner, M., Loach, J., and Willetts, D. (1999). Different perceptions of control: applying an extended theory of planned behavior to legal and illegal drug use. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 21, 301–316. doi: 10.1207/S15324834BASP2104_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avidar, R., Ariel, Y., Malka, V., and Levy, E. C. (2015). Smartphones, publics, and OPR: do publics want to engage? Public Relat. Rev. 41, 214–221. doi: 10.1016/j.pubrev.2014.11.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azer, S. A. (2012). Can “YouTube” help students in learning surface anatomy? Surg. Radiol. Anat. 34, 465–468. doi: 10.1007/s00276-012-0935-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bai, Q., Dan, Q., Mu, Z., and Yang, M. (2019). A systematic review of emoji: current research and future perspectives. Front. Psychol. 10, 2221. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauer, M. W. (2016). Results of the essay competition on the “deficit concept.”. Public Underst. Sci. 25, 398–399. doi: 10.1177/0963662516640650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauer, M. W., Allum, N., and Miller, S. (2007). What can we learn from 25 years of PUS survey research? Liberating and expanding the agenda. Public Underst. Sci. 16, 79–95. doi: 10.1177/0963662506071287

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Besley, J. C., Dudo, A. D., Yuan, S., and Ghannam, N. A. (2016). Qualitative interviews with science communication trainers about communication objectives and goals. Sci. Commun. 38, 356–381. doi: 10.1177/1075547016645640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Besley, J. C., Dudo, A., Yuan, S., and Lawrence, F. (2018). Understanding scientists’ willingness to engage. Sci. Commun. 40, 559–590. doi: 10.1177/1075547018786561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Besley, J. C., Newman, T. P., Dudo, A., and Tiffany, L. A. (2021). American scientists’ willingness to use different communication tactics. Sci. Commun. 43, 486–507. doi: 10.1177/10755470211011159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Besley, J. C., and Tanner, A. H. (2011). What science communication scholars think about training scientists to communicate. Sci. Commun. 33, 239–263. doi: 10.1177/1075547010386972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bessi, A., Zollo, F., Del Vicario, M., Scala, A., Caldarelli, G., and Quattrociocchi, W. (2015). Trend of narratives in the age of misinformation. PLoS One 10:e0134641. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bevan, B., Calabrese Barton, A., and Garibay, C. (2020). Broadening perspectives on broadening participation: professional learning tools for more expansive and equitable science communication. Fron. Commun. 5, 52. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2020.00052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhargava, V. R., and Velasquez, M. (2021). Ethics of the attention economy: the problem of social media addiction. Bus. Ethics Q. 31, 321–359. doi: 10.1017/beq.2020.32

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bik, H. M., Dove, A. D., Goldstein, M. C., Helm, R. R., MacPherson, R., Martini, K., et al. (2015). Ten simple rules for effective online outreach. PLoS Comput. Biol. 11:e1003906. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyd, D., Golder, S., and Lotan, G. (2010). “Tweet, tweet, retweet: conversational aspects of retweeting on twitter,” in 2010 43rd Hawaii international conference on system sciences (New Jersey: IEEE), 1–10.

Google Scholar

Boyette, T., and Ramsey, J. (2019). Does the messenger matter? Studying the impacts of scientists and engineers interacting with public audiences at science festival events. J. Sci. Commun. 18, A02. doi: 10.22323/2.18020202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breslin, C., Li, S., Tupker, E., and Sdao-Jarvie, K. A. T. H. Y. (2001). Application of the theory of planned behavior to predict research dissemination: a prospective study among addiction counselors. Sci. Commun. 22, 423–437. doi: 10.1177/1075547001022004004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bubela, T., Nisbet, M. C., Borchelt, R., Brunger, F., Critchley, C., Einsiedel, E., et al. (2009). Science communication reconsidered. Nat. Biotechnol. 27, 514–518. doi: 10.u1038/nbt0609-514

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Büchi, M. (2017). Microblogging as an extension of science reporting. Public Underst. Sci. 26, 953–968. doi: 10.1177/0963662516657794

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burns, T. W., O'Connor, D. J., and Stocklmayer, S. M. (2003). Science communication: a contemporary definition. Public Underst. Sci. 12, 183–202. doi: 10.1177/09636625030122004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chau, C. (2010). YouTube as a participatory culture. New Dir. Youth Dev. 2010, 65–74. doi: 10.1002/yd.376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, B., and Kim, H. S. (2020). Online customer-to-customer interactions, customer–firm affection, firm-loyalty and participation intention. Asia Pacific J. Market. Logistics, ahead-of-print. 32, 1717–1735. doi: 10.1108/APJML-07-2019-0450

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dallas, D. (2014). “Café Scientifique,” in Communicating science to the public (Dordrecht: Springer), 261–275.

Google Scholar

Das, S., Ahmed, T., Kapadia, A., and Patil, S. (2021). Does this photo make me look good? How social media feedback on photos impacts posters, outsiders, and friends. Proceed. ACM on Human-Computer Interac. 5, 1–32. doi: 10.1145/3449120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davenport, T. H., and Beck, J. C. (2001). The attention economy. Ubiquity 2001:1-es. doi: 10.1145/376625.376626

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, S. R. (2008). Constructing communication: talking to scientists about talking to the public. Sci. Commun. 29, 413–434. doi: 10.1177/1075547008316222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, S. R. (2021). An empirical and conceptual note on science communication’s role in society. Sci. Commun. 43, 116–133. doi: 10.1177/1075547020971642

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Vicario, M., Bessi, A., Zollo, F., Petroni, F., Scala, A., Caldarelli, G., et al. (2016). The spreading of misinformation online. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 113, 554–559. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1517441113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dermentzi, E., and Papagiannidis, S. (2018). UK public’s intention to engage with academia via online technologies. Behav. Inform. Technol. 37, 120–132. doi: 10.1080/0144929X.2017.1410725

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dijkstra, A., Roefs, M. M., and Drossaert, C. H. (2015). The science-media interaction in biomedical research in the Netherlands. Opinions of scientists and journalists on the science-media relationship. J. Sci. Commun. 14, A03. doi: 10.22323/2.14020203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dockter, C. E., Lee, S., Boman, C. D., Hinnant, A., and Cameron, G. T. (2020). The impact of retransmission and modality on communicating Health Research findings via social media. Health Commun. 36, 1–11. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2020.1749354

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, Z. (2015). How to persuade adolescents to use nutrition labels: effects of health consciousness, argument quality, and source credibility. Asian J. Commun. 25, 84–101. doi: 10.1080/01292986.2014.989241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dudo, A., and Besley, J. C. (2016). Scientists’ prioritization of communication objectives for public engagement. PLoS One 11:e0148867. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0148867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duke, S. (2002). Wired science: use of world wide web and e-mail in science public relations. Public Relat. Rev. 28, 311–324. doi: 10.1016/S0363-8111(02)00135-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunwoody, S., Brossard, D., and Dudo, A. (2009). Socialization or rewards? Predicting US scientist-media interactions. J. Mass Commun. Q. 86, 299–314. doi: 10.1177/107769900908600203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durant, J. (1999). Participatory technology assessment and the democratic model of the public understanding of science. Sci. Public Policy 26, 313–319. doi: 10.3152/147154399781782329

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischhoff, B., and Scheufele, D. A. (2013). The science of science communication ii. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 116, 7632–7633. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312080110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franks, B., Bangerter, A., and Bauer, M. W. (2013). Conspiracy theories as quasi-religious mentality: an integrated account from cognitive science, social representations theory, and frame theory. Front. Psychol. 4, 424. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldstein, C. M., Murray, E. J., Beard, J., Schnoes, A. M., and Wang, M. L. (2020). Science communication in the age of misinformation. Ann. Behav. Med. 54, 985–990. doi: 10.1093/abm/kaaa088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grand, A., Davies, G., Holliman, R., and Adams, A. (2015). Mapping public engagement with research in a UK university. PLoS One 10:e0121874. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121874

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Ringle, C. M., and Sarstedt, M. (2011). PLS-SEM: indeed a silver bullet. J. Mark. Theory Pract. 19, 139–152. doi: 10.2753/MTP1069-6679190202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hara, N., Abbazio, J., and Perkins, K. (2019). An emerging form of public engagement with science: ask me anything (AMA) sessions on Reddit r/science. PLoS One 14:e0216789. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0216789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hargittai, E., Füchslin, T., and Schäfer, M. S. (2018). How do young adults engage with science and research on social media? Some preliminary findings and an agenda for future research. Social Media+ Soc. 4:7720. doi: 10.1177/2056305118797720

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermida, A., Lewis, S. C., and Zamith, R. (2014). Sourcing the Arab spring: a case study of Andy Carvin's sources on twitter during the Tunisian and Egyptian revolutions. J. Comput.-Mediat. Commun. 19, 479–499. doi: 10.1111/jcc4.12074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, S. S., Liao, Y., and Rosenthal, S. (2015). Applying the theory of planned behavior and media dependency theory: predictors of public pro-environmental behavioral intentions in Singapore. Environ. Commun. 9, 77–99. doi: 10.1080/17524032.2014.932819

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y., and Yang, C. (2020). A metacognitive approach to reconsidering risk perceptions and uncertainty: understand information seeking during COVID-19. Sci. Commun 42, 616–642. doi: 10.1177/1075547020959818

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, B., Barnidge, M., Gil de Zuniga, H., and Liu, J. (2019). Fostering public trust in science: the role of social media. Public Underst. Sci. 28, 759–777. doi: 10.1177/0963662519869097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hwong, Y. L., Oliver, C., Van Kranendonk, M., Sammut, C., and Seroussi, Y. (2017). What makes you tick? The psychology of social media engagement in space science communication. Comput. Hum. Behav. 68, 480–492. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.11.068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, E., and Buckley, N. (2014). Why people attend science festivals: interests, motivations and self-reported benefits of public engagement with research. Public Underst. Sci. 23, 557–573. doi: 10.1177/0963662512458624

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, P., Rouquier, J. B., Kreimer, P., and Croissant, Y. (2008). Scientists who engage with society perform better academically. Sci. Public Policy 35, 527–541. doi: 10.3152/030234208X329130

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jia, H., Wang, D., Miao, W., and Zhu, H. (2017). Encountered but not engaged: examining the use of social media for science communication by Chinese scientists. Sci. Commun. 39, 646–672. doi: 10.1177/1075547017735114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, R., Colusso, L., Reinecke, K., and Hsieh, G. (2019). R/science: challenges and opportunities in online science communication. Proceedings of the 2019 CHI conferen. human factors in computing syst. 2019, 1–14.

Google Scholar

Kahle, K., Sharon, A. J., and Baram-Tsabari, A. (2016). Footprints of fascination: digital traces of public engagement with particle physics on CERN's social media platforms. PLoS One 11:e0156409. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0156409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaur, P., Dhir, A., Rajala, R., and Dwivedi, Y. (2018). Why people use online social media brand communities. Online Inf. Rev. 42, 205–221. doi: 10.1108/OIR-12-2015-0383

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J. (2012). The institutionalization of YouTube: from user-generated content to professionally generated content. Media Cult. Soc. 34, 53–67. doi: 10.1177/0163443711427199

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H. W., Chan, H. C., and Gupta, S. (2007). Value-based adoption of mobile internet: an empirical investigation. Decis. Support. Syst. 43, 111–126. doi: 10.1016/j.dss.2005.05.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H., and Chang, B. (2020). A study on the effects of crowdfunding values on the intention to visit local festivals: focusing on mediating effects of perceived risk and e-WOM. Sustain. For. 12, 3264. doi: 10.3390/su12083264

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H. S., Lee, S., Cappella, J. N., Vera, L., and Emery, S. (2013). Content characteristics driving the diffusion of antismoking messages: implications for cancer prevention in the emerging public communication environment. J. Natl. Cancer Inst. Monogr. 2013, 182–187. doi: 10.1093/jncimonographs/lgt018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurath, M., and Gisler, P. (2009). Informing, involving or engaging? Science communication, in the ages of atom-, bio-and nanotechnology. Public Underst. Sci. 18, 559–573. doi: 10.1177/0963662509104723

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, N. M., Abitbol, A., and VanDyke, M. S. (2020). Science communication meets consumer relations: an analysis of twitter use by 23andMe. Sci. Commun. 42, 244–264. doi: 10.1177/1075547020914906

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, N. M., and VanDyke, M. S. (2015). Set it and forget it: the one-way use of social media by government agencies communicating science. Sci. Commun. 37, 533–541. doi: 10.1177/1075547015588600

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, N. M., VanDyke, M. S., and Cummins, R. G. (2018). A missed opportunity?: NOAA’s use of social media to communicate climate science. Environ. Commun. 12, 274–283. doi: 10.1080/17524032.2016.1269825

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., and Guo, M. (2021). Scientific literacy in communicating science and socio-scientific issues: prospects and challenges. Front. Psychol. 12:758000. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.769175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, X., Su, L. Y. F., Yeo, S. K., Scheufele, D. A., Brossard, D., Xenos, M., et al. (2014). Building buzz: (scientists) communicating science in new media environments. J. Mass Commun. Q. 91, 772–791. doi: 10.1177/1077699014550092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longnecker, N. (2016). An integrated model of science communication—more than providing evidence. J. Sci. Commun. 15, Y01. doi: 10.22323/2.15050401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovejoy, K., and Saxton, G. D. (2012). Information, community, and action: how nonprofit organizations use social media. J. Comput.-Mediat. Commun. 17, 337–353. doi: 10.1111/j.1083-6101.2012.01576.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, H. P., and Hsiao, K. L. (2010). The influence of extro/introversion on the intention to pay for social networking sites. Inf. Manag. 47, 150–157. doi: 10.1016/j.im.2010.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luarn, P., Yang, J. C., and Chiu, Y. P. (2014). The network effect on information dissemination on social network sites. Comput. Hum. Behav. 37, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.04.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Massarani, L., Entradas, M., Neves, L. F. F., and Bauer, M. W. (2021). Global science journalism report: Working conditions and practices, professional ethos and future expectations. London, United Kingdom: Science and Development Network.

Google Scholar

McClain, C. R. (2019). Likes, comments, and shares of marine organism imagery on Facebook. PeerJ 7:e6795. doi: 10.7717/peerj.6795

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mewburn, I., and Thomson, P. (2013). Why do academics blog? An analysis of audiences, purposes and challenges. Stud. High. Educ. 38, 1105–1119. doi: 10.1080/03075079.2013.835624

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, S. (2001). Public understanding of science at the crossroads. Public Underst. Sci. 10, 115–120. doi: 10.1088/0963-6625/10/1/308

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mingoia, J., Hutchinson, A. D., Wilson, C., and Gleaves, D. H. (2017). The relationship between social networking site use and the internalization of a thin ideal in females: a meta-analytic review. Front. Psychol. 8, 1351. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murugiah, K., Vallakati, A., Rajput, K., Sood, A., and Challa, N. R. (2011). YouTube as a source of information on cardiopulmonary resuscitation. Resuscitation 82, 332–334. doi: 10.1016/j.resuscitation.2010.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Bureau of Statistics (2021). Statistical Communique of the People’s Republic of China on National Economic and Social Development in 2021, Beijing. Available at: http://www.stats.gov.cn/xxgk/sjfb/zxfb2020/202202/t20220228_1827971.html.

Google Scholar

Naveed, N., Gottron, T., Kunegis, J., and Alhadi, A. C. (2011). Bad news travel fast: a content-based analysis of interestingness on twitter. Proceed. 3rd intern. web science conference, 2011, 1–7. doi: 10.1145/2527031.2527052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielsen, K., Gathings, M. J., and Peterman, K. (2019). New, not different: data-driven perspectives on science festival audiences. Sci. Commun. 41, 254–264. doi: 10.1177/1075547019832312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbet, M. C., and Markowitz, E. M. (2015). Expertise in an age of polarization: evaluating scientists’ political awareness and communication behaviors. Ann. Am. Acad. Pol. Soc. Sci. 658, 136–154. doi: 10.1177/0002716214559699

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pandey, A., Patni, N., Singh, M., Sood, A., and Singh, G. (2010). YouTube as a source of information on the H1N1 influenza pandemic. Am. J. Prev. Med. 38, e1–e3. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2009.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulussen, T., Kok, G., Schaalma, H., and Parcel, G. S. (1995). Diffusion of AIDS curricula among Dutch secondary school teachers. Health Educ. Q. 22, 227–243. doi: 10.1177/109019819502200210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearce, W., Holmberg, K., Hellsten, I., and Nerlich, B. (2014). Climate change on twitter: topics, communities and conversations about the 2013 IPCC working group 1 report. PLoS One 9:e94785. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, H. P. (2013). Gap between science and media revisited: scientists as public communicators. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 110, 14102–14109. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1212745110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, H. P., Brossard, D., De Cheveigné, S., Dunwoody, S., Kallfass, M., Miller, S., et al. (2008). Science-media interface: It's time to reconsider. Sci. Commun. 30, 266–276. doi: 10.1177/1075547008324809

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poliakoff, E., and Webb, T. L. (2007). What factors predict scientists' intentions to participate in public engagement of science activities? Sci. Commun. 29, 242–263. doi: 10.1177/1075547007308009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radzikowski, J., Stefanidis, A., Jacobsen, K. H., Croitoru, A., Crooks, A., and Delamater, P. L. (2016). The measles vaccination narrative in twitter: a quantitative analysis. JMIR Public Health Surveill. 2:e5059. doi: 10.2196/publichealth.5059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutsaert, P., Regan, Á., Pieniak, Z., McConnon, Á., Moss, A., Wall, P., et al. (2013). The use of social media in food risk and benefit communication. Trends Food Sci. Technol. 30, 84–91. doi: 10.1016/j.tifs.2012.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sajeev, E. P. M., Mintz-Woo, K., Damert, M., Brunner, L., and Eise, J. (2019). “Blogging climate change: a case study,” in Addressing the challenges in communicating climate change across various audiences (Cham: Springer), 129–142.

Google Scholar

Scheufele, D. A. (2014). Science communication as political communication. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 111, 13585–13592. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1317516111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheufele, D. A., and Krause, N. M. (2019). Science audiences, misinformation, and fake news. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 116, 7662–7669. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1805871115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheth, J. N., Newman, B. I., and Gross, B. L. (1991). Why we buy what we buy: a theory of consumption values. J. Bus. Res. 22, 159–170. doi: 10.1016/0148-2963(91)90050-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simis, M. J., Madden, H., Cacciatore, M. A., and Yeo, S. K. (2016). The lure of rationality: why does the deficit model persist in science communication? Public Underst. Sci. 25, 400–414. doi: 10.1177/0963662516629749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Su, L. Y. F., Akin, H., Brossard, D., Scheufele, D. A., and Xenos, M. A. (2015). Science news consumption patterns and their implications for public understanding of science. J. Mass Commun. Q. 92, 597–616. doi: 10.1177/1077699015586415

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Su, L. Y. F., Scheufele, D. A., Bell, L., Brossard, D., and Xenos, M. A. (2017). Information-sharing and community-building: exploring the use of twitter in science public relations. Sci. Commun. 39, 569–597. doi: 10.1177/1075547017734226

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutton, J., Gibson, C. B., Spiro, E. S., League, C., Fitzhugh, S. M., and Butts, C. T. (2015). What it takes to get passed on: message content, style, and structure as predictors of retransmission in the Boston Marathon bombing response. PLoS One 10:e0134452. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134452

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sweeney, J. C., and Soutar, G. N. (2001). Consumer perceived value: the development of a multiple item scale. J. Retail. 77, 203–220. doi: 10.1016/S0022-4359(01)00041-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taragin-Zeller, L., Rozenblum, Y., and Baram-Tsabari, A. (2020). Public engagement with science among religious minorities: lessons from COVID-19. Sci. Commun. 42, 643–678. doi: 10.1177/1075547020962107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, M., and Kent, M. L. (2014). Dialogic engagement: clarifying foundational concepts. J. Public Relat. Res. 26, 384–398. doi: 10.1080/1062726X.2014.956106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaler, A. D., Zelnio, K. A., Freitag, A., MacPherson, R., Shiffman, D., Bik, H., et al. (2012). “Digital environmentalism: tools and strategies for the evolving online ecosystem,” in Environmental leadership: A reference handbook, Vol. 39 (California, USA: Sage, Thousand Oaks), 364–373.

Google Scholar

Tufekci, Z. (2013). “Not this one” social movements, the attention economy, and microcelebrity networked activism. Am. Behav. Sci. 57, 848–870. doi: 10.1177/0002764213479369

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turel, O., Serenko, A., and Bontis, N. (2010). User acceptance of hedonic digital artifacts: a theory of consumption values perspective. Inf. Manag. 47, 53–59. doi: 10.1016/j.im.2009.10.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Aalderen-Smeets, S. I., Walma van der Molen, J. H., and Asma, L. J. (2012). Primary teachers' attitudes toward science: a new theoretical framework. Sci. Educ. 96, 158–182. doi: 10.1002/sce.20467

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vosoughi, S., Roy, D., and Aral, S. (2018). The spread of true and false news online. Science 359, 1146–1151. doi: 10.1126/science.aap9559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weingart, P., and Joubert, M. (2019). The conflation of motives of science communication—causes, consequences, remedies. J. Sci. Commun. 18, Y01. doi: 10.22323/2.18030401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Welbourne, D. J., and Grant, W. J. (2016). Science communication on YouTube: factors that affect channel and video popularity. Public Underst. Sci. 25, 706–718. doi: 10.1177/0963662515572068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, H. T., McMurray, J. R., Kurz, T., and Lambert, F. H. (2015). Network analysis reveals open forums and echo chambers in social media discussions of climate change. Glob. Environ. Chang. 32, 126–138. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2015.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witzling, L., Shaw, B., and Amato, M. S. (2015). Incorporating information exposure into a theory of planned behavior model to enrich understanding of proenvironmental behavior. Sci. Commun. 37, 551–574. doi: 10.1177/1075547015593085

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, S., Besley, J. C., and Lou, C. (2018). Does being a jerk work? Examining the effect of aggressive risk communication in the context of science blogs. J. Risk Res. 21, 502–520. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2016.1223159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeithaml, V. A. (1988). Consumer perceptions of price, quality, and value: a means-end model and synthesis of evidence. J. Mark. 52, 2–22. doi: 10.1177/002224298805200302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: secondary science communication, audience dissemination/participation, social media, theory of consumption values, theory of planned behavior

Citation: Hu Z, Ma B and Bai R (2022) Motivation to participate in secondary science communication. Front. Psychol. 13:961846. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.961846

Received: 05 June 2022; Accepted: 09 August 2022;
Published: 08 September 2022.

Edited by:

Roberto Cerchione, University of Naples Parthenope, Italy

Reviewed by:

Clare Wilkinson, University of the West of England, United Kingdom
André Bresges, University of Cologne, Germany

Copyright © 2022 Hu, Ma and Bai. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Rubing Bai, 2711794043@qq.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.