Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 11 October 2022
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

COVID-19 related conspiracy beliefs and their relationship with defense strategies, emotions, powerlessness, attitudes, and time perspective

  • 1Department of Humanities, Literature, Cultural Heritage, Education Sciences, University of Foggia, Foggia, Italy
  • 2Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 3Department of Human, Philosophical and Educational Sciences, University of Salerno, Fisciano, Italy

The COVID-19 pandemic has greatly impacted individual’s life and society, and such an emergency has increased the likelihood of recurring conspiratorial thinking. There is much research on broader conspiratorial thinking and studies on COVID-19-related conspiratorial thinking has been growing worldwide, moreover, the negative consequences of COVID-19 specific conspiratorial beliefs for people’s health are clear. However, person-centered research aiming at identify groups of individuals who share patterns of relations between COVID-19 specific conspiratorial beliefs and other psychological features is still scarce. A sample of 1.002 people (18–40 years old, M = 23; SD = 5.19) responded to a questionnaire administered online. The aim was to identify groups of individuals based on their beliefs about COVID-19 conspiracy theories and to compare the groups identified in terms of psychological characteristics associated such as automatic defense mechanisms, coping strategies, powerlessness, emotions, emotional regulation, attitudes toward the COVID-19, social distancing discontent, perceptions of COVID-19 severity and temporal perspective. A k-mean cluster analysis identified the groups of Believers (22.26%), Ambivalent believers (34.3%), and Non-believers (43.21%). The three groups differ particularly in terms of defense mechanisms, and time perspective. Results suggested the need to tailor interventions for individuals believing in COVID-19 conspiratorial theories based on differences in the psychological characteristics among the three groups.

Introduction

The COVID-19 pandemic has greatly impacted individual’s life and society, imposing many limits on individual’s actions. In a time of serious and tragic social crisis, people look for ways to deal with fear, uncertainty, and lack of control, and, as research showed, this increases the likelihood of recurring to conspiratorial thinking (van Prooijen and Douglas, 2017) to provide a causal explanation to events to feel protected uncertainty and offers some compensatory sense of control (Douglas et al., 2017). Conspiracy theories can be described as “a subset of false beliefs in which the ultimate cause of an event is believed to be due to a plot by multiple actors working together with a clear goal in mind, often unlawfully and in secret” (Swami et al., 2014, pp. 220). It has been shown that conspiratorial beliefs have harmful consequences in the health domain. For example, among African-Americans, the conspiratorial beliefs that birth control and HIV/AIDS are forms of genocide against them were associated with negative attitudes toward contraceptive behaviors, which may have exposed people to unwanted pregnancies and sexually transmitted illnesses (Thorburn Bird and Bogart, 2003; Bogart and Thorburn, 2006; Hoyt et al., 2012). Moreover, it has been shown that endorsement of a variety of unrelated conspiracy theories is associated with negative attitudes toward vaccination (Jolley and Douglas, 2014; Lewandowsky et al., 2015). In the last 15 years many studies have investigated broader conspiratorial theories and found that they respond to at least three individual sorts of needs: epistemic needs, reflecting the desire to satisfy curiosity and to avoid uncertainty in understanding individuals’ environment; existential needs, as the desire to restore a threatened sense of security and control (see Kruglanski et al., 2021); social needs, including the desire to maintain a positive image of the self and the social group (see Douglas, 2021 for a review).

In the context of the actual emergency, COVID-19 conspiracy theories regarded the beliefs that COVID-19 is part of a government bioweapons program, that 5G cell towers are spreading COVID-19, or that pharmaceutical companies are encouraging the spread of COVID-19 for profit. Such beliefs are also associated with unhealthy or negative behaviors, for example supporting alternative and inefficacious remedies to fight COVID-19 as hydroxychloroquine (Bertin et al., 2020) or garlic and colloidal silver (Teovanović et al., 2021); it has also been seen that believing that 5G phone masts spread COVID-19 predicted intention to vandalize 5G masts and, more generally, to commit violence (Jolley and Paterson, 2020). Numerous conspiracy hypotheses may have been probably amplified by social media platforms which provide direct access to an unprecedented number of questionable contents (Cinelli et al., 2020; Rovetta and Bhagavathula, 2020; Rovetta and Castaldo, 2022).

Research has reported the negative consequences of COVID-19 specific conspiracy beliefs for people’s health: for instance, they were negatively associated with preventative behaviors such as wearing a mask, maintaining physical distancing, and willingness to vaccinate (Earnshaw et al., 2020; Allington et al., 2021). Studies also showed some psychological factors that could be associated with COVID-19-specific conspiratorial thinking, such as personality traits, emotions, lack of individual control, threat perception, perception of risk or mortality, and attitudes toward government actions (Biddlestone et al., 2020; Oleksy et al., 2021; Pellegrini et al., 2021).

Although many studies have investigated the factors and processes associated with both broader conspiratorial thinking and COVID-19-related conspiratorial thinking (variable-centered approaches), there are few person-centered research focused on identifying groups of individuals based on their levels of broader conspiratorial thinking, and, so far as we know, neither study identifies groups of individuals in terms of COVID-19-related conspiratorial thinking. For person-centered approach we mean studies that identify groups of individuals who share particular attributes or relations among attributes. Person-centered approaches are well suited for addressing questions that concern group differences in patterns, whereas variable-centered approaches describe associations between variables (Laursen and Hoff, 2006). Based on a person-centered approach, the first aim of the present study is to identify groups of individuals who share similar patterns in terms of conspiratorial beliefs about COVID-19 (e.g., the degree to which they think the virus was purposefully created in a lab in Wuhan or by pharmaceutical companies to sell their medications and vaccines) by means of a cluster analysis. The second aim was to compare the groups, identified by the cluster analysis, in terms of the psychological characteristics associated such as psychological defense mechanisms, coping styles, powerlessness, attitudes toward the norms, perception of coronavirus severity, social distancing discontent, and time perspective. As we stated above, although most of these factors were found to be associated with broader conspiratorial thinking or with specific COVID-19 conspiratorial beliefs, this has only been investigated through variables-center approaches. The present study aims to compare for the first time groups of individuals who share patterns of relations between conspiratorial beliefs about COVID-19 and psychological characteristics. Understanding these patterns plays a crucial role in identifying treatment targets and assigning appropriate interventions to people. In the next sections, we will outline the psychological factors we have considered in our study and provide a basis for their inclusion consistently with previous work on COVID-19-related or broader conspiracy beliefs.

COVID-19 -related conspiracy beliefs and defense mechanisms

Defense mechanisms are automatic psychological processes that mediate an individual’s reaction to emotional conflict and internal and external stressors (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). Usually operating without individual awareness, they are not a definite attempt to solve an issue, but a mental process to minimize feelings of anxiety. They can be classified in a hierarchy ranging from high adaptive to less adaptive levels (Thobaben, 2005; Metzger, 2014). The high-adaptive levels of defense mechanisms result in optimal adaptation in the handling of stressors and usually maximize feelings of well-being (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). The defense mechanisms grouped in this level allow for the conscious awareness of feelings, ideas, and their consequences (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). Humor and anticipation are two examples. An individual uses the defense mechanism of humor when he deals with internal/external stressors by emphasizing amusing and ironic aspects. Anticipation involves dealing with stress by experiencing or anticipating consequences and emotional reactions in advance and considering realistic alternative responses or solutions (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). At a lower level of adaptiveness, dissociation is one other common defense level we refer to when an individual separates from reality by a temporary alteration in consciousness or identity (Thobaben, 2005). At an even lower level of adaptiveness, two examples of defense functioning are projection and denial (gross impairment in reality testing). They are characterized by the failure of defensive regulation to contain the individual’s reaction to stressors, leading to a pronounced break with objective reality (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). An individual uses denial when unconsciously refuses to admit the existence of an unpleasant reality that is readily apparent to others, whereas he uses projection when unconsciously attributes unacceptable thoughts, feelings, or actions to another (e.g., blaming) (Thobaben, 2005). Finally, one example of defense mechanisms at the lowest level of adaptiveness is acting out. It is characterized by the use of physical actions instead of dealing with challenges directly by reflecting on and discussing feelings (Thobaben, 2005). Although it appears that there is no evidence so far of associations with COVID-19 specific conspiratorial beliefs, some studies showed that broader conspiracy theories were associated with psychological defense mechanisms (Albarracín, 2020), therefore, we found promising to include these factors in the comparison between the groups identified in terms of COVID-19 conspiratorial beliefs.

COVID-19 -related conspiracy beliefs and coping

Coping is defined as “the cognitive and behavioral efforts made to master, tolerate, or reduce external and internal demands and conflicts among them” (Folkman and Lazarus, 1980). While psychological defense mechanisms are considered partially or largely unconscious and automatic, literature on coping emphasizes the conscious and volitional aim and behavioral and cognitive methods to attain those aims (Skinner et al., 2003), that represent possible malleable factors (Alivernini et al., 2019). Studies have shown that maladaptive coping strategies for example pseudo-epistemic coping (Swami et al., 2016), or avoidance coping strategies (Grant et al., 2013) could be related to broader conspiratorial thinking. Therefore, coping seems a potential factor to include when it comes to comparing the groups identified in terms of conspiratorial beliefs about COVID-19.

COVID-19 -related conspiracy beliefs and emotions

As highlighted by Šrol et al. (2021), people’s emotional responses to the COVID-19 pandemic are crucial for understanding related conspiracy beliefs. People are more likely to believe in broader conspiracy theories when they experience negative emotions (Grzesiak-Feldman, 2013; Freeman and Bentall, 2017). Limited studies showed associations with some emotions, such as fear, anxiety, and worry, with COVID-19-related conspiracy beliefs. In particular, higher levels of anxiety, fear and worry about COVID-19 were associated with the belief that the disease is part of a conspiracy (Jovančević and Milićević, 2020; Sallam et al., 2020; Bruder and Kunert, 2021; Pellegrini et al., 2021). However, no studies so far have compared group of individuals who share patterns of relations between conspiratorial beliefs about COVID-19 and emotions (e.g., worry, surprise, fear, hope) or emotional regulations.

COVID-19-related conspiracy beliefs and powerlessness

Another factor associated with conspiratorial beliefs relates to the capacity of such beliefs to make people feel more able to have a meaningful impact on important issues, an ability that is greatly under threat in such a pandemic crisis. Powerlessness – the perception of individuals about their incapacity to have an impact on relevant issues (Xiang et al., 2019) – was found to be associated both with general conspiracy theories (Douglas et al., 2017) and with conspiratorial thinking specific on COVID-19 (Biddlestone et al., 2020). However, so far, no studies have analyzed powerlessness through a person-centered approach in order to compare different groups of individuals identified in terms of conspiratorial beliefs about COVID-19.

COVID-19 -related conspiracy beliefs: Associations with attitudes, perception of coronavirus severity, and social distancing discontent

Studies have also shown that COVID-19-related conspiratorial beliefs were associated with more negative attitudes toward government responses, perception of coronavirus severity, and discontent toward preventative measures (Georgiou et al., 2020; Pellegrini et al., 2021). However, no studies so far have investigated these factors in relations to conspiratorial beliefs about COVID-19 through a person-centered approach.

COVID-19 -related conspiracy beliefs and time perspective

Many studies suggest that the way we view our past, present, and future affected a multitude of our daily behaviors. The typical way in which individuals segment the flow of their personal experiences into time categories can be used to describe individual differences (Zimbardo and Boyd, 2015). The researchers empirically distinguished five dimensions to describe individual time perspectives. The Past-Negative dimension reflects a generally negative, pessimistic, and regretful view of the past. The Past Positive refers to a nostalgic, warm, and sentimental attitude toward the past. The Present Hedonistic is characterized by an orientation toward present enjoyment and excitement ignoring future consequences. The Present Fatalistic factor reveals a belief in predestined future, the importance of fate, and a lack of control over life events. Future refers to a general future orientation characterized by planning for future aims and prospects of achievements (Zimbardo and Boyd, 2015). According to Zimbardo and Boyd (2015), time perspective allows flexible transition among the temporal orientations in particular situations, however, a specific orientation can be dominant for an individual. Especially present-oriented individuals may be best able to enjoy the moment as they would not be distracted by past worries, but they also may not be able to delay gratification. Individuals with high future orientation usually are good at setting and achieving goals and restraining themselves from engaging in risky behaviors. On the other hand, their ambitions may lead to neglect of personal and social relationships. Future orientation has been seen as related to several positive consequences for individuals, such as higher socioeconomic status, superior academic achievement, and fewer risk-taking behaviors (Fraisse, 1957; de Volder and Lens, 1982; Nuttin, 1985; Strathman et al., 1994; Zaleski, 1994; Levine, 1997; Petruccelli et al., 2014). According to Zimbardo and Boyd (2015), people-oriented toward the Present Fatalistic feel their lives dominated by external forces rather than by their behaviors, whereas Future-oriented people are supposed to be more self-responsible, look more after their health, and to seek long-term gratification. From the argument exposed above, we found time perspective a promising factor to include when it comes to comparing the groups identified in terms of conspiratorial beliefs about COVID-19.

The current study

The present study contributes to this research area by identifying meaningful groups of individuals based on their conspiratorial beliefs about COVID-19. Moreover, the groups identified will be compared in terms of psychological characteristics, related to conspiratorial thinking. Consequently, two main research questions (RQ) guide this study: RQ1: Which groups of individuals are identified in terms of COVID-19 conspiratorial beliefs? RQ2: Are there any differences between the groups identified in terms of their psychological characteristics?

Materials and methods

Participants

Data were collected from December 2020 to January 2021, through an online questionnaire distributed throughout Italian universities. The questionnaire was spread through the Qualtrics platform (Qualtrics.XM1), including the requirement to fill in all questions in order not to have missing data. Participants could refuse their consent to fill in the questionnaire and drop out at any time. This study was conducted by the ethical standards of the Helsinki Declaration and was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Department of Psychology of “Sapienza” University of Rome, with the following title: Psychological aspects of individual concerns and attitudes against Coronavirus in university students and prot. N. 0000305.

One thousand and ninety-nine questionnaires were collected; 1002 participants joined the research and accepted informed consent, while 87 refused (91.2% retention); 75.7% were female (N = 759); the age ranged from 18-to-40 years (M = 23; SD = 5.19). Further characteristics of the participants are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the sample.

Materials

For the present study, an online questionnaire consisting of different sections was developed; first, a summary of demographic data (i.e., age, gender, living conditions); then, the questionnaire included the measures presented below:

COVID-19 conspiracy scale

This 10-item scale (Biddlestone et al., 2020) was designed to measure beliefs in COVID-19 conspiracy theories (e.g., “Much information about the Coronavirus is deliberately kept from the public”); items are assessed on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from “Strongly disagree” (1) to “Strongly agree” (7). Scale reliability was strong (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.906; S.E. = 2.28).

The reduced version of “The Defense Style Questionnaire 40”

This scale, through 14 items (e.g., “Often, when I am annoyed by something, I react impulsively”), aims to measure the level of defense mechanisms (Bovey and Hede, 2001); items are assessed on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from “Strongly disagree” (1) to “Strongly agree” (7). The reliability of this scale was 0.671 (S.E. = 5.52), and subscales’ reliability ranged from 0.482 (S.E. = 1.72) to 0.724 (S.E. = 1.57).

The brief approach/avoidance coping questionnaire

This instrument assesses the “approach-avoidance” dichotomy in coping style through 12 items (e.g., “I am actively engaged in finding a solution to my problems”) on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from “Disagree completely” (1) to “Agree completely” (5) (Finset et al., 2002); reliability of this scale was good in both factors, in accordance with the original study (Approach Cronbach’s alpha = 0.536; S.E. 2.37; Avoidance Cronbach’s alpha = 0.614; S.E. = 2.44).

The difficulties in emotion-regulation strategies

This scale measures individual difficulties in engaging in emotional regulation strategies through factors such as the recognition of one’s own emotions, the acceptance of negative emotions, the ability to regulate one’s own emotions, and to direct one’s behavior toward goals despite negative emotions (Lausi et al., 2020); it is a self-reported questionnaire, consisting in 20 items (e.g., “I pay attention to how I feel”) on a 5-point Likert scale, running from “Almost never” (1) to “Almost always” (5). Subscales’ reliability ranged from 0.741 (Awareness; S.E. = 1.70) to 0.905 (Impulse; S.E. = 1.26).

The positive and negative affect schedule – shortened version

This scale is a self-reported that measures positive and negative affect through 10-items (5-item Positive Affect and 5-item Negative Affect); participants are asked to rate adjectives of varying mood states (e.g., “happy,” “cheerful,” “worried,” “sad”), based on how often they have felt that way during the past week; on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “Never” (1) to “Always” (5) (Ebesutani et al., 2012). The scale has also been used in Italian subjects (Cozzolino et al., 2021). Scale’s reliability was good in both factors (Positive Affect Cronbach’s alpha = 0.800; S.E. = 1.74; Negative Affect Cronbach’s alpha = 0.786; S.E. = 1.84).

Powerlessness

A three-item scale (e.g., “I feel that the Coronavirus is too big a problem for my actions to have an impact”) has been used in order to measure the sense of being unable to make a meaningful impact on important issues (Biddlestone et al., 2020); the responses are on a 5-point Likert Scale, ranging from “Strongly disagree” (1) to “Strongly agree” (5). Scale’s reliability was good (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.738; S.E. = 1.32).

The attitudes and moods about the new coronavirus

This scale aimed to investigate the attitudes and moods of respondents about the new Coronavirus (Mari et al., 2020). The items on the scale (e.g., “The Coronavirus is a mysterious and highly lethal virus capable of decimating the world’s population”) were evaluated on a 5-point Likert scale, from “Completely disagree” (1) to “Completely agree” (5). The reliability of this scale ranged from 0.449 (Negative Attitudes; S.E. = 1.04) to 0.792 (Positive Attitudes; S.E. = 1.55).

Social distancing discontent

Participants were asked to indicate on a 5-point Likert-scale, ranging from “Not at all” (1) to “Completely” (5) how discontent they were with following the rules of virus containment (Burrai et al., 2021); items were built following the World Health Organization recommendations (e.g., How unhappy are you with the recommendation of: “Reducing outside activities”, “Having to maintain a physically safe distance from people you meet”) and adapted from previous studies conducted in Italy (Mallia et al., 2020; Alivernini et al., 2021; Cavicchiolo et al., 2021); the scale’s reliability was good (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.770; S.E. = 1.62).

Perception of coronavirus severity

This set of six items investigates beliefs related to Coronavirus, including the likelihood of contracting the virus; contagiousness and severity of the virus; and concern related to the possibility of contracting the virus (Burrai et al., 2020). Items are assessed on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from “Not at all” (1) to “Completely” (5).

Stanford time perspective inventory

This instrument provides a simple way to measure multiple temporal perspectives of individuals and it is built on a theoretical basis that examines the emotional, social, cognitive, and motivational processes that are supposed to contribute to, and are in turn influenced by, the functioning of the temporal perspective (e.g., “Every morning one should make a plan of the day”; D’Alessio et al., 2003); all the answers were 5-point response scales ranging from “Never” (0) to “Very often” (4). Subscales’ reliability ranged from 0.295 (Hedonistic Present; S.E. = 3.08) to 0.736 (Future; S.E. = 2.63).

Data analysis

One thousand and two questionnaires were collected from students attending Italian universities. Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS (Statistical Package for Social Sciences), Version 25.0 (IBM Corp. Released, 2021). The socio-demographic characteristics of the sample were investigated; the normality of the data distribution for each variable was investigated through Q-Q Plots; no non-normal data were found; values of internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) were calculated for each factorial scale and the total (Taber, 2018). Zero-order correlations using Pearson coefficient were computed among all the variables of the study. Moreover, a Linear Regression Analysis was conducted to investigate which variables best predict COVID-19 conspiracy scale scores; the stepwise method was selected with Confidence Intervals at 95%. COVID-19 conspiracy scale was selected as a dependent variable and variables showing a significant correlation with the COVID-19 conspiracy scale were selected as predictors. The standardized values of the scales showing significant correlations were used to perform a k-mean cluster analysis (Quick Cluster in SPSS; Steinley and Brusco, 2008) with the aim of identifying groups of individuals based on the labeling variable “COVID-19 conspiracy scale.” As we do not know of any study on this kind in the field of conspiratorial thinking, an explorative approach was adopted: it was determined a priori that iteration of the data should lead to three final clusters, according to the selection criteria leading to a sufficiently large number of people in each cluster (Milligan and Cooper, 1985; Kodinariya and Makwana, 2013). Iteration among the scale scores led to the identification of the three cluster profiles based on the proximity of the scores to the centroids of the clusters themselves. The mean score of the labeling variable (i.e., COVID-19 conspiracy scale) was used to name the three clusters: a group with higher scores (i.e., COVID-conspiracy believers), a group with the average scores (COVID-conspiracy ambivalent believers), and a group with the lowest scores (COVID-conspiracy non-believers). The resulting clusters were then compared with each other in terms of psychological characteristics by performing ANOVAs using Bonferroni’s post hoc analysis with significance at p < 0.001.

Results

Correlation analysis

Pearson’s r correlation analyses (Table 2) showed both positive and negative scores between Conspiracy scores and several of the examined subscales. The correlation effect size ranged from small to medium. Only low significant correlations (Greenland et al., 2016) were found between Conspiracy scores and STPI subscale “Hedonistic Present” (p = 0.299); DSQ subscales “Anticipation” (p = 0.137) and “Isolation” (p = 0.931); BACQ “Approach” (p = 0.064) subscale; DERS “Goals” (p = 0.626), “Clarity” (p = 0.392) and “Awareness” (p = 0.204) subscales and PANAS negative affect (p = 0.851) (See Supplementary Tables 1, 2 for total correlations and their confidence intervals).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlation analysis among the COVID-19 conspiracy scale and the examined subscales.

Regression analysis

A stepwise regression analysis was conducted to identify whether COVID-19 Conspiracy scale could be predicted by any of the variables which showed statistically significant correlations; seven models were found (Table 3), with an R Square ranging from 0.184 to 0.283. The following subscales were excluded: Acting Out, Denial and Projection subscales of DSQ; Avoidance BACQ subscale, Non-Acceptance and Impulse subscales of DERS-20, both PANAS subscales, Positive Attitudes and Moods toward Coronavirus, beliefs toward coronavirus contagiousness, severity and worrying.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Stepwise regression analysis among the COVID-19 conspiracy scale and the examined subscales.

Cluster analysis

The iteration and classification of standardized scores of the cluster analysis produced three groups of individuals in terms of conspiratorial beliefs about COVID-19, therefore they were labeled as (1) The COVID-conspiracy believers; (2) The COVID-conspiracy ambivalent believers; (3) The COVID-conspiracy non-believers. (See Figure 1 and Table 4) according to the scores of COVID-19 conspiracy scale.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Graphical Representation of Clusters. DSQ_AO, Acting Out subscale of DSQ; DSQ_H, Humor subscale of DSQ; DSQ_DEN, Denial subscale of DSQ; DSQ_DISS, Dissociation subscale of DSQ; DSQ_P, Projection subscale of DSQ; BACQ_AV, Avoidance subscale of BACQ; DERS_NA, Non-Acceptance subscale of DERS-20; DERS_I, Impulse subscale of DERS-20; POS_A, Positive Affect subscale of PANAS; POW, Powerlessness; AMAC_P, Positive Attitudes and Mood toward Coronavirus; AMAC_N, Negative Attitudes and Mood toward Coronavirus; DISC, Discontent for COVID-19; COVID_S, Beliefs toward Coronavirus Severity; COVID_ C, Beliefs toward Coronavirus Contagiousness; COVID_ W, Worrying for Coronavirus; FUTURE, Future subscale of STPI; FATALIST, Fatalistic Present subscale of STPI.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Sample clusters characterization.

The first cluster, named “COVID-conspiracy believers” represented the 22.26% of the sample, people belonging to this cluster showed the highest scores on the conspiracy scale and negative attitudes toward Coronavirus (AMAC_P M = 10.14, SD = 2.17; AMAC_N M = 13.22, SD = 2.77); this is also the group that most underestimated the severity and contagiousness of COVID-19. Regarding the sense of powerlessness, people belonging to this cluster showed the highest scores (M = 8.56, SD = 2.51) while regarding defense mechanisms, this is the cluster showing highest scores in the denial (M = 6.80, SD = 2.65) and dissociation (M = 6.47, SD = 2.70) subscales.

The second cluster was composed by 34.53% of the sample, named “COVID-conspiracy ambivalent believers”, showing middle scores on conspiracy scale. These participants showed the highest scores on the AMAC positive subscale (M = 12.50, SD = 1.73) but also on discontent due to social distancing norms (M = 17.75, SD = 3.27). It was also the cluster that perceived COVID-19 as more severe (COVID_S M = 4.14, SD = 0.64), contagious (COVID_C M = 4.32, SD = 0.62), and worrying (COVID_W M = 4.22, SD = 0.62) than seasonal flu. Within this group we found the highest scores on the DERS-20, both in the Non-Acceptance subscale (M = 13.62, SD = 5.56) and in the Impulse subscale (M = 10.82, SD = 4. 39); regarding defense mechanisms, members of this cluster seemed to use Acting Out (M = 9.84, SD = 2.51) and Projection (M = 7.43, SD = 2.79) strategies more; moreover, they showed the highest scores on the Avoidance subscale of the BACQ (M = 17.75, SD = 3.54), and lower scores on the PANAS Positive Affect subscale (M = 11.37, SD = 3.67). Regarding temporal perspective, this group showed the highest scores on the present fatalistic (M = 16.14, SD = 3.10).

Finally, the third group, labeled “COVID-conspiracy non-believers,” which shows the lowest mean scores on the conspiracy scale, was comprised of 43.21% of the sample. People within this cluster showed medium to low scores on the Coronavirus-related scales, compared to the other two clusters (Table 2), while showing the lowest scores on Emotional Dysregulation, Powerlessness and Avoidance Coping Strategies, and the highest scores on using humor as a defense mechanism (M = 10.78, SD = 2.37), the PANAS Positive Affect subscale (M = 13.29, SD = 3.78), and temporal orientation to the future (M = 34.38, SD = 4.85). Table 5 summarizes the significant results of the post hoc analyses performed among the groups to highlight the differences found among the three clusters and the sizes of the effects. Results showed a small effect for COVID-19 severity, humor subscale of DSQ, PANAS positive affect, and future subscale of STPI, while a large effect in the other dimensions.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Differences between groups on COVID-related measures.

Discussion

The COVID-19 pandemic has greatly impacted individual’s life and society and such emergency has increased the likelihood of recurring to conspiratorial thinking. Although several studies have identified the factors and processes associated to COVID-19 conspiratorial thinking, research on this topic, based on a person-centered approach, is still limited. Our first aim was to identify groups of individuals, based on conspiratorial beliefs about COVID-19, who share similar patterns; our second aim was to compare these groups in terms of psychological characteristics. Concerning our first aim, the cluster analysis identified three groups of individuals: the “COVID-conspiracy believers,” the “COVID-conspiracy ambivalent believers”, and the “COVID-conspiracy non-believers.” The “Believers” group, apart from showing the highest scores on the conspiracy scale, also showed the highest score on the negative attitude toward Coronavirus. This group also underestimated the most the severity and contagiousness of COVID-19 and showed the highest score on powerlessness, meant as the individual perception about their incapacity to have an impact on relevant issues. These results are in line with previous studies that found COVID-19-related conspiratorial beliefs associated with more sense of powerlessness and more negative attitudes toward COVID-19 (Georgiou et al., 2020; Pellegrini et al., 2021). This group also show highest scores in the use of defense mechanisms as “denial” (e.g., “People say I tend to ignore unpleasant facts as if they didn’t exist”) and “dissociation” (e.g., “I ignore danger as if I were Superman”) which have the goal to keeping unpleasant or unacceptable stressors, impulses, ideas, affects, or responsibility out of awareness. The “denial” represents the unconscious mechanism of disavowal that it is behind the willing suspension of disbelief and the temporarily abandon critical faculties. The “dissociation” allows to this kind of people to deal with internal/external stressors with a breakdown in the usually integrated functions of consciousness, memory, and perception of self or the environment. They also strongly use defense mechanisms such as “acting out” (e.g., “I get openly aggressive when I feel hurt”) and “projection” (e.g., “People tend to mistreat me”). The “acting out” is positioned at the lowest level of the hierarchy based on their level of adaptiveness (Metzger, 2014). Referring when “an individual deals with internal/external stressors by actions rather than reflections or feelings” (Bovey and Hede, 2001, p. 537). In support of this finding, the “Believers” group also seem to experience difficulties in engaging in emotional regulation strategies due to factors such as “high impulsivity” (e.g., “When I’m upset, I have difficulty controlling my behaviors”) and “low acceptance of negative emotions” (e.g., “When I’m upset, I become embarrassed for feeling that way”). “Projection” instead is a defense mechanism which involves our own unacceptable qualities or feelings and ascribing them to other people in order to reduce anxiety. For these reasons we can state that “Believers” tend to react to stress, anxiety, fear and worry by automatically and unconsciously activating a very archaic, primitive, and not very adaptive defense mechanisms pattern. This will have important effects on the way they perceive reality and/or others and it will orient them toward specific attitudes and behaviors that in some ways, even if they are dysfunctional and bizarre, are also very predictable. This extends the results of previous studies, who found associations between conspiracy theories with psychological defense mechanisms (Albarracín, 2020), to the COVID-19 specific conspiratorial beliefs.

Moreover, the Believers group has shown to be very oriented to present fatalistic, the so-called “What will be, will be” (e.g., “It doesn’t make sense to worry about the future since there is nothing to do about it anyway”). Based on this result, the Believers group think that everything is already determined by fate, that their lives are dominated by a predetermined plan over which they have little or no control (Zimbardo et al., 2012). Although no studies so far have examined the relationships between time perspective and COVID-19 conspiratorial thinking, according to the conceptualization of the fatalistic present, the “Believers” feel their lives dominated by external forces rather than by their behaviors.

The “Ambivalent believers” group showed intermediate scores on the conspiracy scale, and it was the group that perceived COVID-19 as more severe, contagious, and worrying than seasonal flu. These people experienced the highest scores in the positive attitudes and moods about the Coronavirus. On the other side, they also experienced the strongest discontent due to social distancing norms. Members of this group seem to use defense mechanisms such as “acting out” and “projection” which we know to be two types of defense mechanisms with a low level of adaptiveness (Metzger, 2014). They also use the avoidance coping strategy the most. As we have defined, they seem uncertain and ambivalent, almost indefinite, unlike “Believers” and “Non-believers” groups. The “Ambivalent believers” group showed the highest score in present fatalistic temporal orientation. Many studies evidenced that this kind of temporal perspective is correlated to low emotional well-being and high-risk level of feeling anxiety and depression (Zimbardo et al., 2012). For all that we can state that right, the “Ambivalent believers” group seem to show solid maladjustment to the COVID-19 issue compared to the “Believers” and the “Non-believers” group.

Finally, people within the “Non-believers” group (e.g., the cluster with the lowest scores on the conspiracy scale) showed medium to low scores on the Coronavirus-related scales, compared to the “Ambivalent believers” and the “Believers.” They showed the lowest scores on Emotional Dysregulation, Powerlessness, and Avoidance Coping Strategies evidencing better overall mind-body functioning which can also be noted from Intensive Positive Affectivity. Moreover, we found that they have the highest scores on using “humor,” known as one of the defense mechanisms at the highest levels of adaptiveness (Metzger, 2014).

Alongside this positive attitude to the difficult stage where the world is, they also have a very useful temporal perspective. They are more oriented toward the future than the “Believers” and the “Ambivalent believers.” As some other research showed, future-oriented people make decisions based on a reasoned evaluation of the consequences, they plan and are confident that their choices will work, and they tend to live longer thanks to their conscientiousness (Zimbardo et al., 2012).

Overall, the findings of the present study confirm the key role of emotions, attitudes, and powerlessness in COVID-19 conspiratorial thinking (Biddlestone et al., 2020; Jovančević and Milićević, 2020; Sallam et al., 2020; Bruder and Kunert, 2021; Pellegrini et al., 2021), and extend the results of previous studies who found defense mechanisms to be correlated with broader conspiratorial thinking to the COVID-19 specific conspiratorial beliefs (Albarracín, 2020), by using a person-centered approach focused on identifying groups of individuals based on their levels of conspiratorial thinking.

Potential limitations and directions for future research

Some limitations of this study need to be considered. First, the number of clusters was chosen a priori using a selection method that considered solutions with a sufficiently large number of people within each group. Second, the majority of the participants in our sample were female, future studies should be conducted with a more balanced sample. Third, although this cluster analysis included some indicators of psychological well-being, future research could include other indicators of emotional aspects, particularly stress and anxiety (Rossi et al., 2011; Venuleo et al., 2018). Typologies based on valid psychometric measures of these critical aspects would be more informative for policy interventions. Finally, we should specify that our results are correlational in nature therefore caution should be taken in drawing causal inferences. Nonetheless, this study contributes to understanding COVID-19-related conspiracy beliefs, even considering these limitations. The study also provides empirical bases for developing more tailored programs and interventions to reduce COVID-19-related conspiracy beliefs prevalence and impacts by adopting a person-centered approach and identifying diverse and qualitative distinct groups of individuals based on specific psychological features.

Conclusion

The COVID-19 pandemic has greatly impacted individuals’ lives and society (Bruno et al., 2021), imposing many limits on individuals’ actions that increased the likelihood of recurring conspiratorial thinking that has harmful consequences in the health domain. For instance, they were negatively associated with preventative behaviors such as wearing a mask, maintaining social distancing, and willingness to vaccinate. This is the first study conducted in this field through a person-centered approach, that identifies groups of individuals who share particular patterns in terms of conspiratorial beliefs about COVID-19 and psychological characteristics associated.

The study answered two different research questions: first, based on conspiratorial thinking about COVID-19, three different groups of individuals can be identified: “Believers,” “Ambivalent believers” and “Non-believers.” The second: the groups identified differ in terms of psychological characteristics, specifically in defense mechanisms, coping strategies, and temporal orientation. These results can be useful for structuring different prevention and communication paths concerning the specific profile.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Department of Psychology of “Sapienza” University of Rome (N. 0000305). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

GC, MC, and AMG developed the theoretical framework of the present study, designed the study, and developed the methodological approach. GL, EC, and LG performed all the analyses and designed tables and figure. GC and LG led the literature search and interpretation of data. GC, MC, AMG, and PL critically revised the manuscript. MC contributed to the scientific supervision of the whole work. All authors made a substantial contribution to the work, read, and approved the final version of the work.

Funding

This study was funded by Learning Science Hub.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.939615/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.qualtrics.com/

References

Albarracín, D. (2020). “The psychology of fake news,” in The Psychology of Fake News, eds R. Greifeneder, M. E. Jaffé, E. J. Newman, and N. Schwarz (New York, NY: Routledge). doi: 10.4324/9780429295379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alivernini, F., Manganelli, S., Cavicchiolo, E., Chirico, A., and Lucidi, F. (2019). The use of self-regulated cognitive strategies across students with different immigrant backgrounds and gender. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 37, 652–664. doi: 10.1177/0734282918785072

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alivernini, F., Manganelli, S., Girelli, L., Cozzolino, M., Lucidi, F., and Cavicchiolo, E. (2021). Physical distancing behavior: The role of emotions, personality, motivations, and moral decision-making. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 46, 15–26. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsaa122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allington, D., Duffy, B., Wessely, S., Dhavan, N., and Rubin, J. (2021). Erratum: Health-protective behaviour, social media usage and conspiracy belief during the COVID-19 public health emergency (Psychological Medicine (1–7)DOI: 10.1017/S003329172000224X) Psychol. Med. 51:1770. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721000593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association [APA] (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Bertin, P., Nera, K., and Delouvée, S. (2020). Conspiracy beliefs, rejection of vaccination, and support for hydroxychloroquine: A conceptual replication-extension in the COVID-19 pandemic context. Front. Psychol. 11:2471. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.565128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biddlestone, M., Green, R., and Douglas, K. M. (2020). Cultural orientation, power, belief in conspiracy theories, and intentions to reduce the spread of COVID-19. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 59, 663–673. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogart, L. M., and Thorburn, S. (2006). Relationship of African Americans’ sociodemographic characteristics to belief in conspiracies about HIV/AIDS and birth control. J. Natl. Med. Assoc. 98:1144.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bovey, W. H., and Hede, A. (2001). Resistance to organisational change: The role of defence mechanisms. J. Manag. Psychol. 16, 534–548. doi: 10.1108/EUM0000000006166

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruder, M., and Kunert, L. (2021). The conspiracy hoax? Testing key hypotheses about the correlates of generic beliefs in conspiracy theories during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int. J. Psychol. 57, 43–48. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruno, G., Panzeri, A., Granziol, U., Alivernini, F., Chirico, A., Galli, F., et al. (2021). The italian COVID-19 psychological research consortium (IT C19PRC): General overview and replication of the UK study. J. Clin. Med. 10:52. doi: 10.3390/jcm10010052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burrai, J., Barchielli, B., Cricenti, C., Borrelli, A., D’Amato, S., Santoro, M., et al. (2021). Older adolescents who did or did not experience COVID-19 symptoms: Associations with mental health, risk perception and social connection. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:5006. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18095006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burrai, J., Roma, P., Barchielli, B., Biondi, S., Cordellieri, P., Fraschetti, A., et al. (2020). Psychological and emotional impact of patients living in psychiatric treatment communities during Covid-19 lockdown in Italy. J. Clin. Med. 9:3787. doi: 10.3390/jcm9113787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavicchiolo, E., Manganelli, S., Girelli, L., Cozzolino, M., Lucidi, F., and Alivernini, F. (2021). Adolescents at a distance: The importance of socio-cognitive factors in preventive behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. Eur. J. Health Psychol. 28, 161–170. doi: 10.1027/2512-8442/a000083

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cinelli, M., Quattrociocchi, W., and Galeazzi, A. (2020). The covid-19 social media infodemic. Sci. Rep. 10:16598. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-73510-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cozzolino, M., Celia, G., Girelli, L., and Limone, P. (2021). Effects of the brain wave modulation technique administered online on stress, anxiety, global distress, and affect during the first wave of the COVID-19 pandemic: A randomized clinical trial. Front. Psychol. 12:635877. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.635877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Alessio, M., Guarino, A., De Pascalis, V., and Zimbardo, P. G. (2003). Testing Zimbardo’s stanford time perspective inventory (STPI) - short form. Time Soc. 12, 333–347. doi: 10.1177/0961463X030122010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Volder, M. L., and Lens, W. (1982). Academic achievement and future time perspective as a cognitive-motivational concept. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 42, 566–571. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.42.3.566

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Douglas, K. M. (2021). COVID-19 conspiracy theories. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 24, 270–275. doi: 10.1177/1368430220982068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., and Cichocka, A. (2017). The psychology of conspiracy theories. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 26, 538–542. doi: 10.1177/0963721417718261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Earnshaw, V. A., Eaton, L. A., Kalichman, S. C., Brousseau, N. M., Hill, E. C., and Fox, A. B. (2020). COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs, health behaviors, and policy support. Transl. Behav. Med. 10, 850–856. doi: 10.1093/tbm/ibaa090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebesutani, C., Regan, J., Smith, A., Reise, S., Higa-McMillan, C., and Chorpita, B. F. (2012). The 10-item positive and negative affect schedule for children, child and parent shortened versions: Application of item response theory for more efficient assessment. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 34, 191–203. doi: 10.1007/s10862-011-9273-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finset, A., Steine, S., Haugli, L., Steen, E., and Laerum, E. (2002). The brief approach/avoidance coping questionnaire: Development and validation. Psychol. Health Med. 7, 75–85. doi: 10.1080/13548500120101577

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folkman, S., and Lazarus, R. S. (1980). An analysis of coping in a middle-aged community sample. J. Health Soc. Behav. 21:219. doi: 10.2307/2136617

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraisse, P. (1957). La période réfractaire psychologique. L’année Psychol. 57, 315–328. doi: 10.3406/psy.1957.26609

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, D., and Bentall, R. P. (2017). The concomitants of conspiracy concerns. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 52, 595–604. doi: 10.1007/s00127-017-1354-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgiou, N., Delfabbro, P., and Balzan, R. (2020). COVID-19-related conspiracy beliefs and their relationship with perceived stress and pre-existing conspiracy beliefs. Pers. Individ. Differ. 166:110201. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, D. M., Wingate, L. R., Rasmussen, K. A., Davidson, C. L., Slish, M. L., Rhoades-Kerswill, S., et al. (2013). An examination of the reciprocal relationship between avoidance coping and symptoms of anxiety and depression. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 32, 878–896. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2013.32.8.878

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenland, S., Senn, S. J., Rothman, K. J., Carlin, J. B., Poole, C., Goodman, S. N., et al. (2016). Statistical tests, P values, confidence intervals, and power: A guide to misinterpretations. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 31, 337–350. doi: 10.1007/s10654-016-0149-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grzesiak-Feldman, M. (2013). The effect of high-anxiety situations on conspiracy thinking. Curr. Psychol. 32, 100–118. doi: 10.1007/s12144-013-9165-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoyt, M. A., Rubin, L. R., Nemeroff, C. J., Lee, J., Huebner, D. M., and Proeschold-Bell, R. J. (2012). HIV/AIDS-related institutional mistrust among multiethnic men who have sex with men: Effects on HIV testing and risk behaviors. Health Psychol. 31, 269–277. doi: 10.1037/a0025953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

IBM Corp. Released (2021). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 27.0. (Armonk, NY: IBM Corp), 1–8.

Google Scholar

Jolley, D., and Douglas, K. M. (2014). The effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on vaccination intentions. PLoS One 9:e89177. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jolley, D., and Paterson, J. L. (2020). Pylons ablaze: Examining the role of 5G COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs and support for violence. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 59, 628–640. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jovančević, A., and Milićević, N. (2020). Optimism-pessimism, conspiracy theories and general trust as factors contributing to COVID-19 related behavior – A cross-cultural study. Pers. Individ. Differ. 167:110216. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kodinariya, T. M., and Makwana, P. R. (2013). Review on determining number of cluster in K-means clustering. Int. J. 1, 90–95.

Google Scholar

Kruglanski, A. W., Molinario, E., and Lemay, E. P. (2021). Coping with COVID-19-induced threats to self. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 24, 284–289. doi: 10.1177/1368430220982074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laursen, B., and Hoff, E. (2006). Person-centered and variable-centered approaches to longitudinal data. Merrill Palmer Q. 52, 377–389. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2006.0029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lausi, G., Quaglieri, A., Burrai, J., Mari, E., and Giannini, A. M. (2020). Development of the DERS-20 among the Italian population: A study for a short form of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 8, 1–8. doi: 10.6092/2282-1619/mjcp-2511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levine, R. (1997). A Geography of Time: The Temporal Misadventure of a Social Psychologist, or How Every Culture Keeps Time Just a Little Bit Differently. New York, NY: Basic Books, 258.

Google Scholar

Lewandowsky, S., Gignac, G. E., and Oberauer, K. (2015). Correction: The role of conspiracist ideation and worldviews in predicting rejection of science. PLoS One 10:e0134773. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mallia, L., Chirico, A., Zelli, A., Galli, F., Palombi, T., Bortoli, L., et al. (2020). The implementation and evaluation of a media literacy intervention about PAES use in sport science students. Front. Psychol. 11:368. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00368

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mari, E., Fraschetti, A., Lausi, G., Pizzo, A., Baldi, M., Paoli, E., et al. (2020). Forced cohabitation during coronavirus lockdown in Italy: A study on coping, stress and emotions among different family patterns. J. Clin. Med. 9:3906. doi: 10.3390/jcm9123906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metzger, J. A. (2014). Adaptive defense mechanisms: Function and transcendence. J. Clin. Psychol. 70, 478–488. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milligan, G. W., and Cooper, M. C. (1985). An examination of procedures for determining the number of clusters in a data set. Psychometrika 50, 159–179.

Google Scholar

Nuttin, J. M. (1985). Narcissism beyond Gestalt and awareness: The name letter effect. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 15, 353–361. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420150309

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oleksy, T., Wnuk, A., Maison, D., and Łyś, A. (2021). Content matters. different predictors and social consequences of general and government-related conspiracy theories on COVID-19. Pers. Individ. Differ. 168:110289. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellegrini, V., Giacomantonio, M., De Cristofaro, V., Salvati, M., Brasini, M., Carlo, E., et al. (2021). Is Covid-19 a natural event? Covid-19 pandemic and conspiracy beliefs. Pers. Individ. Differ. 181:111011. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petruccelli, F., Diotaiuti, P., Verrastro, V., Petruccelli, I., Carenti, M. L., De Berardis, D., et al. (2014). Obsessive-compulsive aspects and pathological gambling in an Italian sample. BioMed Res. Int. 2014:167438.

Google Scholar

Rossi, E. L., Cozzolino, M., Mortimer, J., Atkinson, D., and Rossi, K. L. (2011). A brief protocol for the creative psychosocial genomic healing experience: The 4-stage creative process in therapeutic hypnosis and brief psychotherapy. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 54, 133–152. doi: 10.1080/00029157.2011.605967

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rovetta, A., and Bhagavathula, A. S. (2020). COVID-19-related web search behaviors and infodemic attitudes in Italy: Infodemiological study. JMIR Public Health Surveill. 6:e19374. doi: 10.2196/19374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rovetta, A., and Castaldo, L. (2022). Are we sure we fully understand what an infodemic is? A global perspective on infodemiological problems. JMRIX Med. 3:e36510.

Google Scholar

Sallam, M., Dababseh, D., Yaseen, A., Al-Haidar, A., Ababneh, N. A., Bakri, F. G., et al. (2020). Conspiracy beliefs are associated with lower knowledge and higher anxiety levels regarding covid-19 among students at the university of Jordan. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:4915. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17144915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skinner, E. A., Edge, K., Altman, J., and Sherwood, H. (2003). Searching for the structure of coping: A review and critique of category systems for classifying ways of coping. Psychol. Bull. 129, 216–269. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Šrol, J., Ballová Mikušková, E., and Èavojová, V. (2021). When we are worried, what are we thinking? Anxiety, lack of control, and conspiracy beliefs amidst the COVID-19 pandemic. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 35, 720–729. doi: 10.1002/acp.3798

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinley, D., and Brusco, M. J. (2008). A new variable weighting and selection procedure for K-means cluster analysis. Multivariate Behav. Res. 43, 77–108. doi: 10.1080/00273170701836695

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strathman, A., Gleicher, F., Boninger, D. S., and Edwards, C. S. (1994). The consideration of future consequences: Weighing immediate and distant outcomes of behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 66, 742–752. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.4.742

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swami, V., Furnham, A., Smyth, N., Weis, L., Lay, A., and Clow, A. (2016). Putting the stress on conspiracy theories: Examining associations between psychological stress, anxiety, and belief in conspiracy theories. Pers. Individ. Differ. 99, 72–76. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swami, V., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., Tran, U. S., and Furnham, A. (2014). Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition 133, 572–585. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taber, K. S. (2018). The use of Cronbach’s alpha when developing and reporting research instruments in science education. Res. Sci. Educ. 48, 1273–1296.

Google Scholar

Teovanović, P., Lukić, P., Zupan, Z., Lazić, A., Ninković, M., and Žeželj, I. (2021). Irrational beliefs differentially predict adherence to guidelines and pseudoscientific practices during the COVID -19 pandemic. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 35, 486–496. doi: 10.1002/acp.3770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thobaben, M. (2005). Defense mechanisms and defense levels. Home Health Care Manag. Pract. 17, 330–332. doi: 10.1177/1084822304274097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thorburn Bird, S., and Bogart, L. M. (2003). Birth control conspiracy beliefs, perceived discrimination, and contraception among African Americans: An exploratory study. J. Health Psychol. 8, 263–276. doi: 10.1177/1359105303008002669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Prooijen, J.-W., and Douglas, K. M. (2017). Conspiracy theories as part of history: The role of societal crisis situations. Mem. Stud. 10, 323–333. doi: 10.1177/1750698017701615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Venuleo, C., Mangeli, G., Mossi, P., Amico, A. F., Cozzolino, M., Distante, A., et al. (2018). The cardiac rehabilitation psychodynamic group intervention (CR-PGI): An explorative study. Front. Psychol. 9:976. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiang, P., Zhang, H., Geng, L., Zhou, K., and Wu, Y. (2019). Individualist–collectivist differences in climate change inaction: The role of perceived intractability. Front. Psychol. 10:187. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaleski, Z. E. (1994). Psychology of Future Orientation. Lublin: Towarzystwa Naukowego Katolickiego Uniwersytetu Lubelskiego.

Google Scholar

Zimbardo, P. G., and Boyd, J. N. (2015). “Putting time in perspective: a valid, reliable individual-differences metric,” in Time Perspective Theory; Review, Research and Application, eds M. Stolarski, N. Fieulaine, and W. van Beek (Berlin: Springer International Publishing), 17–55. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-07368-2_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimbardo, P. G., Sword, R., and Sword, R. (2012). The Time Cure: Overcoming PTSD with the New Psychology of Time Perspective Therapy. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

Keywords: COVID-19 conspiratorial thinking, cluster analysis, defense mechanisms, coping strategies, time perspective

Citation: Celia G, Lausi G, Girelli L, Cavicchiolo E, Limone P, Giannini AM and Cozzolino M (2022) COVID-19 related conspiracy beliefs and their relationship with defense strategies, emotions, powerlessness, attitudes, and time perspective. Front. Psychol. 13:939615. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.939615

Received: 09 May 2022; Accepted: 05 August 2022;
Published: 11 October 2022.

Edited by:

Dan Romer, University of Pennsylvania, United States

Reviewed by:

Federica Galli, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Angela Lambrou-Louca, University of Nicosia, Cyprus
Alessandro Rovetta, R&C Research, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Celia, Lausi, Girelli, Cavicchiolo, Limone, Giannini and Cozzolino. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Laura Girelli, lgirelli@unisa.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.