Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 01 December 2022
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Opportunities on Improving Student Motivation at All Levels of Education View all 13 articles

The relationship between basic psychological needs satisfaction and university students’ academic engagement: The mediating effect of emotional intelligence

Hongxia Chen
Hongxia Chen1*Morning Hon ZhangMorning Hon Zhang2
  • 1School of Marxism, Zhoukou Normal University, Zhoukou, Henan Province, China
  • 2Institute for Education Science, Zhoukou Normal University, Zhoukou, Henan Province, China

Introduction: Basic psychological needs satisfaction (BPNS) and Emotional intelligence (EI) have been underscored as helpful psychological constructs in explaining academic engagement. However, the joint interaction of BPNS with EI abilities to explain academic engagement has not been tested. Therefore, the present study aimed to investigate the interactive role of BPNS with EI abilities in the prediction of academic engagement in a sample of Chinese university students.

Methods: A questionnaire survey was administered to a sample of 466 university students. The data were analyzed using the SPSS (version 21.0) software. The first analysis consisted of descriptive statistics (including mean and standard deviation) and Pearson’s correlations among BPNS, EI, and academic engagement. Through structural equation modeling (SEM), direct and indirect effects were calculated.

Results: The results showed that BPNS was positively associated with academic engagement and that only the Use of emotion dimension of EI mediated these associations.

Discussion: These results suggest that important interventions incorporated with BPNS and EI abilities, especially the use of emotion ability, may be performed to promote university students’ academic engagement.

Introduction

Academic engagement in the current situation

As a psychologically satisfying mental state, academic engagement refers to a persistent affective and cognitive state of contentment toward studying and learning, rather than a momentary and specific condition (Salanova et al., 2010; Casuso-Holgado et al., 2013; Chen et al., 2021). Extensive research literature has indicated that academic engagement is a key facilitator of higher academic achievements, and also can effectively reduce the levels of dropout rates (e.g., Newmann et al., 1992; Chipchase et al., 2017; Kwon et al., 2018). Recently, facilitating engagement has also been revealed to reduce the adversarial impact of sociodemographic predictors on student performance (Lei et al., 2018). So far, research have showed that both personal variables and social-contextual variables could influence the student’ academic engagement (Patrick et al., 2007; Wu et al., 2010; Stoeber et al., 2011; King and Ganotice, 2014; Kilday and Ryan, 2019; MacCann et al., 2020; Virtanen et al., 2020; García-Martínez et al., 2022; Luo and Luo, 2022; Saleem et al., 2022).

Positive psychology, which is a rising field in psychology, has given us new insights into the personal and contextual variables which influenced academic engagement (Carmona-Halty et al., 2021; Dewaele and Li, 2021; Kang et al., 2021; Luthans et al., 2021; Saleem, 2022). Among the scientific existing literature, two theories need to be noticed particularly. One is the self-determination theory (SDT), which emphasizes the importance of organic interaction between internal individual factors and external social-contextual factors (Ryan and Deci, 2017). As such, only if the influence of external social-contextual factors is transformed into individual intrinsic factors through autonomous motivation, academic engagement could be sustainable and effective. The other is the demands-resources theory (JD-R), which concerns the extent to which people apply their personal resources in their everyday life (Schaufeli and Taris, 2014). According to JD-R, some personal psychological resources could influence performance by affecting engagement. For example, adaptability, compassion, mindfulness, psychological capital, self-concept, etc., being personal resources, could create a motivational process leading to academic engagement, and, in turn, improve academic achievement and students’ life satisfaction (e.g., Armenteros et al., 2021; Carmona-Halty et al., 2021; Putwain et al., 2021).

Based on these two theories, recently, a growing body of literature has been focusing on the simultaneous effects between the personal and social-contextual factors, such as teacher-student relationship, flourishing, and academic performance (Chamizo-Nieto et al., 2021); academic performance, academic engagement, and psychological capital resources (Martínez et al., 2019); academic buoyancy, adaptability, and engagement (Martin et al., 2017); and achievement goals, achievement emotions, and academic engagement (Putwain et al., 2022). Despite the growing body of literature that indicates a complex interplay between personal and social-contextual factors, however, most of the studies on the influencing factors of academic engagement have been identified and recognized in the outcome variables such as academic achievement, life satisfaction, and mental health. Among them, academic engagement is mainly regarded as an independent variable, a mediating variable, or a moderating variable, and few studies directly consider academic engagement as a dependent variable. Furthermore, the simultaneous effects were found mainly in the field of Primary, Secondary, and high-school contexts, but less attention has been paid to university settings. Thus, there is a need to further investigate whether or not the patterns of relationships found in previous literature are also found at the university level.

In a sample of university students, the present study would take academic engagement as a dependent variable, in line with previous studies from the field of positive psychology, focusing on individual psychological factors that affect academic engagement and its influencing mechanism. Among all these variables, basic psychological needs satisfaction (BPNS) and emotional intelligence (EI) would be examined simultaneously. To the best of our knowledge, the joint interaction of BPNS with EI abilities to explain academic engagement has not been tested in university students. The identification of these associations allows for a more reliable understanding of how the motivational variables and individual psychological resources are related to the academic engagement with university students. This understanding will provide the basis for the implementation of programs that help to improve academic engagement in the university environment.

BPNS and academic engagement

Academic engagement describes the degree to which students engage themselves in learning-related activities. Traditionally, academic engagement is considered a multidimensional construct, comprising behavioral, cognitive, and emotional dimensions (e.g., Fredricks et al., 2004; Appleton et al., 2008; Pietarinen et al., 2014). More recently, some studies have articulated a fourth dimension, i.e., agentic engagement, which highlights student’s constructive contribution toward the flow of the instruction he receives (Reeve, 2013).

Due to the following two reasons, the present study used Schaufeli et al.’s definition of engagement, in which academic engagement is described as a “positive, fulfilling, work-related state of mind that is characterized by vigor, dedication, and absorption” (Schaufeli et al., 2002, p. 74). The first reason is that the Schaufeli et al.’s model was proposed directly from university students based on their research on employee engagement. The second reason is that, compared with other educational phase, although university students are not formally employed by the university, students’ studies are involved in coercive, structured tasks and activities (e.g., attending class, cooperating and competing with others, and submitting assignments), which makes academic engagement in university more similar with work (Walker et al., 2006). For example, Saleem et al. (2022) recently posited that post-graduate student’ s educational journey is much different than high-school students in terms of length of the degree, course work, examination, assessment, educational outcomes, teaching methodology, and so on.

According to Schaufeli et al.’s model, academic engagement is a highly motivated and satisfying mental state. Given the motivational nature of academic engagement, a lot of scientific literature in the field of education have investigated the motivational predictors of academic engagement, such as self-efficacy, achievement emotions, and achievement goals (Patrick et al., 2007; Diseth et al., 2012; Song et al., 2015; Stahlberg et al., 2019; Zysberg and Schwabsky, 2021). Among the variables, according to SDT, the fulfillment of the basic psychological needs (i.e., autonomy, competence, and relatedness) is critical to students intrinsic motivation, as these three basic needs are the necessary conditions for individual psychological growth, internalization, and mental health (Jang et al., 2012). For example, in a sample of 648 university teachers in China, Jin et al. (2022) found that meeting the three types of BPNS correlated positively with work engagement. Furthermore, a large number of empirical studies have shown that the satisfaction of the three psychological basic needs-either separately or in combination-could have a positive and significant impact on learning engagement and academic performance (Hofer and Busch, 2011; Madjar et al., 2013; Carmona-Halty et al., 2019; Martin and Collie, 2019). For example, in a sample of 366 Korean high-school students using a three-wave longitudinal research, Jang et al. (2016) found that students tend toward a semester-long trajectory of rising engagement when they perceive their teachers to be autonomy supportive and need satisfying. Similarly, Carmona-Halty et al. (2019) also found that students whose basic psychological needs are satisfied at school experience more academic psychological capital, which, in turn, leads to better academic performance.

Furthermore, empirical studies have linked discrete psychological needs to learning and achievement. In a study conducted by Gasiewski et al. (2012) with 2,873 students across 15 Colleges and universities, it was indicated that students who reported feeling comfortable asking questions in class and seeking out tutoring, i.e., autonomy need was respected and satisfied, tended to be more engaged in courses where the instructor persistently displayed an openness to student questions. Regarding to the competency need, which concerns the feeling that individuals experience the control of their environment and ability development. Based on competence motivation theory, Wong et al. (2002) identified self-worth as a significant predictor of motivational orientation and academic outcomes. Related need refers to the feelings of being connected to others. Studies have identified that a supportive teacher-student relationship may provide students with a sense of security that promotes their free and active participation in classroom academic activities (Quin, 2017). More recently, a meta-analysis conducted by Roorda et al. (2017), based on 99 studies with preschool to high-school students, has shown that the total effect size for the associations between both positive teacher-student relationships and engagement was r = 0.39 (p < 0.01) and negative relationships and engagement was r = −0.32 (p < 0.01).

Considering past empirical research herein described, this study will propose the following hypotheses:

Hypothesis 1 (H1): BPNS is positively correlated with academic engagement.

The mediating role of EI

Engagement is not only highly motivated state, but also strongly affected by emotions, which are an inherent part of the human existence in any context (Brackett et al., 2021). Thus, while examining intrinsic factors, researchers also have found emotional intelligence (EI) to be an important predictor of academic engagement (e.g., Sinclair et al., 2003; Mavroveli et al., 2009; Durlak et al., 2011; Huang et al., 2022).

In the scientific literature EI was usually defined in two different ways, i.e., ability model and trait model (Zeidner et al., 2008). Both models have been used in many domains, such as nursing, teaching, physical activity, and teleworking (Law et al., 2004; Smitha et al., 2009; Cebrian et al., 2020). In the current study, EI is conceptualized from the ability model developed by Mayer et al. (2008), which is defined as a mental ability for perceiving, understanding, regulating, and using one’s own and others’ emotions in thinking and action. Research literature suggests that emotionally intelligent people with higher EI also show more positive mood, higher levels of life satisfaction, well-being, flourishing, better psychological adjustment, and lower levels of psychological stress across different samples (e.g., Chu, 2010; Su and Reeve, 2011; Szczygieł and Mikolajczak, 2017; Mérida-López and Extremera, 2020; Karapetyan, 2021).

In the field of education, there is an increasing consensus on the idea that EI is an important skill that teachers and students must develop (Sha et al., 2022). For example, with a total of 702 teachers working at different educational levels in southern Spain, Mérida-López et al. (2020) found that EI and self-efficacy were positively related to teachers’ work engagement and negatively related to withdrawal intentions. As to students, as MacCann et al. (2011) suggest that EI is characterized as the third most important predictor after Intelligence and Conscientiousness in academic achievement. Recently, in a study conducted by Estrada et al. (2021) with 550 students from four higher education institutions and one secondary school, it was observed that EI was shown to be positively related to compassion and higher levels of commitment, which, consequently, led to better academic performance.

Furthermore, some previous research also suggested that EI would positively relate to BPNS (Emery et al., 2016). According to the SDT, psychological needs contain both cognitive and affective elements and BPNS appears as an important motivation factor for the development of EI (Raufelder et al., 2016). For example, in a study conducted with 16 Coaches and 171 youth athletes by Watson and Kleinert (2019), it was observed that coaches’ EI was related to basic need satisfaction in athletes. More recently, in a sample of 1,332 students in Southwest Spain, Rivera-Pérez et al. (2021) suggested that positive and significant associations were found between cooperative academic and EI in all school stages. The results indicated that people with higher BPNS would tend to develop a better EI.

In addition, the mediating role of EI between individual variables and positive psychological outcomes has been proposed in the field of workplace (e.g., Côté, 2014; De Clercq et al., 2014; Castillo-Gualda et al., 2019). For example, in a sample of 201 Italian workers, Di Fabio et al. (2018) indicated that EI mediated the relationship between personality traits and both hope and optimism. Similarly, the role of individual differences on the strength of implicit motives in the relationship between needs for relatedness and well-being has also been observed (Di Fabio and Kenny, 2016). For instance, in a study conducted by Callea et al. (2019) with 216 Italian participants, it was observed that those who showed the higher levels of psychological need for relatedness were more positively associated with both happiness and flourishing and that EI mediated these associations.

Taken together, based upon past empirical studies and the current knowledge on the role of EI herein described, the present study will propose the following two hypotheses:

Hypothesis 2 (H2): BPNS is positively correlated with Emotional intelligence (EI).

Hypothesis 3 (H3): EI plays a mediating role in the relationship between BPNS and academic engagement.

The present study

Based on previous findings, this research aimed to investigate relationships among BPNS, EI, and academic engagement in a sample of university students; specifically, the mediated role of the different dimensions of EI would be examined. The present study may contribute to the literature in two ways. First, no studies have simultaneously considered the relationships of both BPNS and EI on academic engagement. By identifying the indirect effects of BPNS on increasing academic engagement as mediated through EI, our results can contribute to the positive psychology literature. Once the relationships were identified, effective interventions could be designed to improve academic engagement among university students.

Second, although the mediated role of EI between individual variables and positive psychological outcomes had been found in the field of workplace, as to which dimension of EI having the mediated role was still a controversial issue (Extremera et al., 2020). By assessing the independent roles of different dimensions of EI between BPNS and academic engagement, the present study may shed some light on the importance of considering EI skills as potential mediated factors in the associations between academic engagement and its correlates.

Materials and methods

Participants and procedure

The present study conducted a questionnaire survey at a single university in Zhoukou city from middle China. Students were recruited by means of convenience sampling. This research was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, written informed consent was given to all participants and their privacy, feelings, and intentions were fully considered (Goodyear et al., 2007). With necessary guidance and support, participants voluntarily filled in the questionnaire at the classroom. The electronic Questionnaires were distributed on the spot, and questionnaires would not be submitted until all questions had been answered. Thus, there were no uncompleted questionnaires, and a total of 506 questionnaires were collected. The questionnaire took about 15 min, so questionnaires that took too short, i.e., less than 5 min, or significantly inconsistent, were excluded. Finally, we got 466 valid questionnaires for final analysis. While questionnaire survey was collected, as senior students were on an off-campus internship, they were not included in the sample. Among participants, 88 were men (18.9%) and 378 were women (81.1%), with a predominantly female student sample. Overall, there were 332 first-year students (71.2%), 111 s-year students (23.8%), and 23 juniors (4.9%). As to major, there were 308 students (66.1%) in arts and social science, 66 students (14.2%) in science, and 92 students (19.7%) in engineering.

Measures

Academic engagement scale

Academic engagement was assessed by the Work Engagement Student Scale (UWES-SS) created by Schaufeli et al. (2002). The scale consists of 14 items which evaluate three dimensions of academic engagement: (1) vigor, with 5 items (e.g., When studying I feel strong and vigorous); (2) dedication, with 5 items (e.g., My studies inspire me); and (3) absorption, with 4 items (e.g., I can get carried away by my studies). All items were scored on a five-point Likert scale, from 1 (never) to 5 (always). A higher score on this scale indicates a higher level of academic engagement.

Basic psychological needs satisfaction scale

Basic Psychological needs satisfaction was assessed with Basic Psychological Needs Satisfaction scales (BPNSs), proposed by Ryan and Deci (2017). The scale consists of 14 items which evaluate three dimensions of BPNS: (1) Autonomy, with 6 items (e.g., I can try to solve tasks my own way); (2) Competence, with 6 items (e.g., I am considered capable of difficult tasks); and (3) Social relatedness, with 4 items (e.g., I feel accepted by my classmates). This scale adopts a 5-point Likert scoring system, from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5(strongly agree). A higher score on this scale in university students indicates a higher level of the satisfactions of their basic psychological needs.

Emotional intelligence scale

Emotional intelligence was assessed with the Wong and Law Emotional Intelligence Scale (WLEIS). This scale is a self-report measure, composed of 16 items, with a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (completely agree). The Chinese validation (Sha et al., 2022) proposed a factor solution in four dimensions: (1) Self-emotion appraisal (SEA), with 4 items (e.g., I really understand what I feel); (2) Others’ emotion appraisal (OEA), with 4 items (e.g., I am a good observer of others’ emotions); (3) Use of emotion (UOE), with 4 items (e.g., I am a self-motivated person); and (4) Regulation of emotion (ROE), with 4 items (e.g., I have good control of my own emotions). A higher score on this scale indicates a greater degree of emotional intelligence.

Demographic variables

Participants’ demographic data, including information about gender, grade level, and major, were collected with our questionnaire. Previous research showed that those demographic variables may have a direct or mediated effect on basic Psychological needs satisfaction and emotional intelligence (Madjar et al., 2013; Martin and Collie, 2019; Cebrian et al., 2020; Karapetyan, 2021), so they were considered as controls in the present study.

Reliability and validity analysis

To evaluate the measurement model, the following indicators were used in the present study: standardized factor loadings, component reliability (CR), average variance extracted (AVE), Cronbach’s alpha coefficients (α), and discriminant validity. The results of reliability and validity analysis are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Results of reliability and validity analysis.

At the beginning, item analysis was used to eliminate inappropriate questions from the questionnaire. There were totally 46 items in the original questionnaire, the items with factor loadings below 0.5 were deleted. After this process, three items in the Basic Psychological Needs Satisfaction Scale (i.e., “we are taught to work independently” in the Autonomy Subscale, “I have already learned a lot with this teacher” and “I feel challenged in class” in the Competence Subscale), and three in the Academic Engagement Scale (i.e., “When I get up in the morning, I feel like going to class” in the Vigor Subscale, “I find my studies challenging” in the Dedication Subscale and “Time flies when I′ m studying” in the Absorption Subscale), the factor loadings of which were below 0.5, were deleted. Finally, 40 items in total remained for further analysis, including 11 for the Academic Engagement scale (i.e., 4 items in the Vigor Subscale, 4 items in the Dedication Subscale, and 3 items in the Absorption Subscale), 16 for the Emotional intelligence scale (i.e., 4 items in the Self-emotion appraisal Subscale, 4 items in the Others’ emotion appraisal Subscale, 4 items in the Use of emotion Subscale, and 4 items in the Regulation of emotion Subscale), and 13 for the Basic Psychological Needs Satisfaction scale (i.e., 5 items in the Autonomy Subscale, 4 items in the Competence Subscale, and 4 items in the Social relatedness Subscale). As Table 1 illustrates, the factor loadings of all instrument items were all above 0.5.

In addition, results showed that all CR values were greater than 0.6, and all AVE values, except autonomy and competence items, were greater than 0.5. Even though the AVE is less than 0.5, according to Fornell and Larcker (1981), if the CR value exceeds the criteria of 0.6, the scale’s convergent validity is still acceptable. Meanwhile, internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s alpha) exceeded 0.8 for all items, indicating good reliability of this study’s constructs.

Finally, the square root of AVE was performed to examine the discriminant validity of all the research instruments. If the square root value of AVE was greater than the correlation coefficient in each dimension (Fornell and Larcker, 1981), the discriminant validity of the constructs was suitable. As shown in Table 2, the results met the criteria for assessing discriminant validity.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Discriminant validity of the research instruments.

Data analysis

The data were analyzed using the SPSS (version 21.0) software. The first analysis consisted of descriptive statistics (including mean and standard deviation) and Pearson’s correlations among BPNS, EI, and academic engagement (Hayes, 2013). Then, the SPSS Amos program (version 21.0) was used to evaluate the mediating effect of EI on BPNS and academic engagement. The present study is based on the structural equation modeling (SEM) technique, which is a nominal research analysis approach (Saleem et al., 2022). For the mediation analyses, a bootstrapping method was further used to obtain bias-corrected 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) with 5,000 re-samples. If the 95% CI did not contain zero, an effect was considered significant.

Results

Common method bias

As all variables used in the present study were measured by a self-report questionnaire, there may have a common method deviation. Before analysis, Harman’s single-factor test was used (Podsakoff et al., 2003). In the present study, there were 10 factors with feature values greater than 1 extracted. The explanatory variance of the first factor was 23.738%, less than the 50% threshold. Thus, the common method deviation of data in the present study was not serious.

Descriptive statistics and correlation analysis of main variables

Prior to the assessment of the hypotheses, descriptive statistics and Pearson’s correlations among variables were conducted. As shown in Table 3, BPNS was positively related with academic engagement (r = 0.458). As expected, BPNS correlated significantly with the four dimensions of EI (SEA: r = 0.240; OEA: r = 0.163; UOE: r = 0.482; ROE: r = 0.212). In the same way, the four subscales of EI were also positively correlated with academic engagement (SEA: r = 0.209; OEA: r = 0.101; UOE: r = 0.542; ROE: r = 0.187). In addition, the correlation coefficients between the variables range from 0.101 to 0.542, less than 0.700, indicating that there is no serious collinearity between the three variables.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Descriptive statistics and correlations among the study variables.

Measurement and structural model

We assessed both measurement and structural models for all of the study variables, i.e., BPNS, EI, and academic engagement through different fitness indexes such as absolute, incremental, and parsimonious fit indices (Hu and Bentler, 1999). Per the guidelines, measures like normed chi-square (X2/df); RMR (root mean residual); RMSEA (root mean square error of approximation); CFI (comparative fit index); GFI (goodness of fit index); TLI (Tucker-Lewis index); IFI (incremental fit index); and SRMR (standardized root mean residual) were utilized (Schweizer, 2010). The measurement model and structural model outcomes are shown in Table 4. While performing a confirmatory factor analysis of the scale, an excessive sample size may cause the increased Chi-square values; therefore, other adaptation indicators were considered the model fit (Hu and Be Ntler, 1998; Hsiao et al., 2015). Overall, the results revealed a good fit of the measurement model for each scale in the present study.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Measurement model and structural model validity.

Hypothesis testing

To analyze the influence of BPNS on academic engagement and the role of EI, SEM approach was used, the results are listed in Figure 1. Specifically, these results showed that university students’ BPNS significantly predicted academic engagement (β = 0.294, p < 0.001). Therefore, the research hypothesis H1 was supported. The second hypothesis H2 (BPNS is positively correlated with Emotional intelligence on EI) also was observed. The standardized coefficient of BPNS on SEA was β = 0.339 (p < 0.001), on OEA was β = 0.210 (p < 0.001), on UOE was β = 0.630 (p < 0.001), and on ROE was β = 0.269 (p < 0.001). That is, the research hypothesis H2 was supported. We also tested the mediated effect of four dimensions of EI between BPNS and academic engagement, the results showed BPNS still has a significant positive effect on academic engagement. However, three of the mediators, i.e., SEA, OEA, and ROE, had non-significant effects on academic engagement, only one dimension of EI, that is UOE, had a significant positive effect on academic engagement (β = 0.441, p < 0.001). Therefore, the research hypothesis H3 was supported, i.e., it was the UOE dimension of EI that played a partially mediating role in the relationship between BPNS and academic engagement.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Single mediation model shows the effect of BPNS on AE through the dimensions of EI. Standardized coefficients are presented. BPNS, basic psychological needs satisfaction; AE, academic engagement; SEA, Self-emotion appraisal; OEA, Others’ emotion appraisal; UOE, Use of emotion; ROE, Regulation of emotion.***p<0.001; ns, non-significant effect.

Test of mediation

In order to further test the mediating effect, the bootstrap method was used in a procedure of 5,000 re-samples to analyze the mediating effects via the SPSS Amos program. In this process, a 95% confidence interval (95% CI) would be generated to test the significance of the indirect effect between BPNS and academic engagement through mediating roles of the four dimensions of EI. If zero does not exist between the lower and upper confidence intervals, the direct effects would be identified as significant. Table 5 shows the Bootstrapping analysis results.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Test of multiply mediation effects.

As shown in Table 5, the direct effect value of BPNS on academic engagement was 0.294, accounting for 50.95% of the total effect, and the 95% CI was [0.128, 0.455], indicating that the direct effect was significant. After including the four dimensions of EI as mediators in the process, the total indirect effect was still significant, but only the dimension of UOE served as a significant mediating variable. The indirect effect value of UOE on BPNS and academic engagement was 0.283, accounting for 49.04% of the total effect, with a 95% CI [0.183, 0.411]. Therefore, these results indicated that not all dimensions of EI, but only UOE dimension, played a partially mediating role in the effect of BPNS on academic engagement.

Discussion

Main findings

This research aimed to determine the BPNS-academic engagement association in university students and the mediating effects of EI abilities. The results of the present study showed that BPNS could positively affect academic engagement through EI abilities.

Firstly, consistent with findings in previous psychology studies, the correlation suggested that both basic psychological needs and emotional intelligence positively related to university students’ academic engagement. On the one hand, this finding is consistent with self-determination theory, i.e., satisfying all three needs is critical for psychological well-being, in turn, promoting mental health and significantly impacting engagement. As previous studies suggest, when students report a high perception of autonomy support from teachers, they view their class activities as volitional and self-determined and engage in class willing and eagerly (e.g., Madjar et al., 2013; Martin and Collie, 2019). The positive effects associated with BPNS allow university students to be more energized, proactive, and motivated, and these positive mental states as resources could enhance the higher levels of engagement. On the other hand, the correlation results are also in line with the JD-R theory, previous research mainly focused on the relationship between EI abilities and individual and work-related well-being domains, such as job and life satisfaction in teachers, in nurses, and in healthcare (e.g., Law et al., 2004; Smitha et al., 2009; Cebrian et al., 2020). In line with this, this study further extended the model to university students.

In this sense, our study is coherent with the need to return to a more humanistic education that incorporates a new language and new content. Added to this perspective is the need to incorporate emotions into the teaching-academic process and give them a greater role by establishing the relationship between emotional factors and motivational reasons as an additional way of strengthening and developing the individual academic variables. When the needs of students are satisfied or fulfilled with contextual support, students also show higher levels of EI, and these interpersonal and psychological reciprocal effects could positively predict the engagement.

Secondly, the results showed that Hypothesis 2 was supported, stating that BPNS would positively relate to EI, which would add new light on the relationship between motivational systems and EI (Bechter et al., 2021). Despite the mediated role of motivational variables and individual psychological resources had been examined, respectively, in current scientific literature, the explicit role of emotions in the motivation-generative mechanism was still poorly investigated. Recently, some research had explored the issue among different populations. For example, Watson and Kleinert (2019) showed that coaches’ EI was related to basic need satisfaction in athletes. More recently, in a study conducted by Callea et al. (2019) with 216 Italian participants, suggested that the need for relatedness would positively relate to EI.

In line with those studies, the results obtained from this study could support a new evidence in the relationship between BPNS and EI, indicating that people with higher levels of BPNS will tend to develop a better EI. Thus, it is possible to envisage that the most emotionally intelligent people who are satisfied in basic psychological needs display higher levels of individual well-being. That is, university students who show higher levels of BPNS are not only more motivated and full of energy, but also more emotionally intelligent. In turn, being more emotionally intelligent did increase the levels of academic engagement.

Finally, the present study also seemed to support Hypothesis 3, which stated that the contribution of BPNS to academic engagement would be mediated by EI abilities, but only through the UOE dimension of EI abilities among Chinese university students. These findings are consistent with previous work that show the mediating effect of EI between personal resources and work engagement/outcomes. However, as to which dimension of EI abilities may work in enhancing positive mental state within the JD-R theory still remains unclear. Among these, the often mentioned dimension of EI was Emotion regulation ability (ERA) and/or UOE. For example, a study conducted by Mérida-López and Extremera (2020), with 190 teachers in Spain, found that only ERA was significantly associated with work engagement, job satisfaction, and life satisfaction. Other studies found both ERA and UOE of EI could play the mediated role. In a study with 380 Chinese adults, Bao et al. (2015) showed that mindfulness was positively associated with four components of EI abilities, and negatively associated with perceived stress. Additionally, the regulation and use of emotion components of EI could act as mediators of the association between mindfulness and perceived stress.

Notwithstanding this, the present study did not provide evidence to consider the dimension of ROE as a mediator in the link to EI performance. Compared with other dimensions, the results suggested that only the dimension of UOE could play a significant mediating role between BPNS and academic engagement. The finding was in line with a study more recently carried out by Parent-Lamarche (2022). Based on a sample of 254 Canadian employees from 18 small and medium organizations during the COVID-19 pandemic, Parent-Lamarche (2022) found that except for skill utilization and recognition, use of emotion could appear to be key considerations for organizations that wish to increase work engagement and decrease intention to quit.

The reason why it was UOE not ROE that played the mediating role in relationships between BPNS and academic engagement may be inferred from the essential characteristics of both. Judging from the essential characteristics of ROE, it emphasizes the capability to control and regulate emotions, which is conductive to relieving and mitigating one’s psychological distress, recovering from negative situations, and adjusting their emotions flexibly (Cheung and Ng, 2019). However, judging from the essential characteristics of UOE, it mainly focuses on using and maximizing existing individual psychological resources, and it is of importance to enhancing one’s psychological functionings and well-being, which is crucial for improving positive state and outcomes (Chen et al., 2020). From the perspective of positive psychology, once the basic psychological needs were satisfied, several beneficial academic outcomes, such as the achievement of positive emotions at school (e.g., joy, interest, contentment, and school satisfaction), and more effort would be activated. These emotions, based on the broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, 2001), may further broaden students’ momentary thought-action repertoires and build their enduring personal resources. In this respect, using the emotions produced from BPNS could help students to open their minds to different thoughts and problem-solving approaches, and, in turn, enhance academic engagement.

Like other research, the results from here are not conclusive; hence, the need for more research should be added up so as to understand this issue.

Limitations and future research

Some potential limitations should be mentioned in this study. First, the present research used a cross-sectional questionnaire, so the findings may have been affected by unpredictable social interactions, which prevents us from drawing causal conclusions. In order to fully disentangle reciprocal causal relations, an additional, longitudinal design study is required in the future. Second, there is a limitation directly connected to how EI is measured. In this study, the WLEIS is a self-report instrument. Although the scale has been shown to be both reliable and valid, it is recommended to use both self-reports and performance tests to measure EI (Brackett et al., 2006). Third, the sample included only university students in China, so the results cannot be generalized. To further test the findings, future studies should incorporate participants from other cultures and other countries to form a bigger and more representative sample.

Practical implications

Despite these limitations, the present results have crucial implications for teachers and university administrators. First, considering the direct effect of BPNS on academic engagement, autonomy-supportive learning environments are of great significance of teachers to foster students’ positive mental state. For instance, teachers should carry out certain instructional behavior, such as attending to the students’ perspective, vitalizing inner motivational resources, appreciating and accepting negative affect, and displaying patience (Diseth et al., 2018). More recently, one intervention named Peer Assisted Study Sessions (PASS), a structured peer-led study group where students collectively share knowledge and solve course-related tasks, both cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses showed its positive outcomings to academic engagement and performance (Rivera-Pérez et al., 2021).

Second, although scholars and administrators acknowledge the importance of EI to academic engagement and achievement, a lot of intervention techniques aimed at improving the students’ EI have been designed (Vesely et al., 2013; Mérida-López et al., 2021), most training programs were mainly focused on developing the emotional skills of students, especially the abilities of emotional regulation and management, so as to cope with the problems of academic burnout, pressure and test anxiety. All these measures are necessary to improve students’ academic engagement, but not sufficient, at least when positive psychological resources already exist. The results from this study suggested that when students basic psychological needs were satisfied, it was the UOE dimension not ROE dimension of EI that enhanced higher levels of academic engagement. Therefore, EI training programs should aim not only to develop the abilities of emotional regulation and management, but also to improve the awareness of one’s emotion, to be an active interest in using and exploring of one’s positive emotions.

Conclusion

To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first to explore the means by which EI potentiates the power of basic psychological needs satisfaction to enhance academic engagement among university students. That is, university students who showed higher levels of BPNS were more emotionally intelligent. In turn, being more emotionally intelligent would enhance the levels of academic engagement. In sum, the present study paves the way for future research on the importance of EI as a mediator in the relationship between BPNS and academic engagement in the perspective of positive psychology of sustainable development.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by The Ethics Review Committee of Zhoukou Normal University. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

MZ and HC: conceptualization, methodology, software, investigation, data curation, writing-original draft preparation, writing—review and editing, and informed consent statement. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., and Furlong, M. J. (2008). Student engagement with school: critical conceptual and methodological issues of the construct. Psychol. Schools 45, 369–386. doi: 10.1002/pits.20303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armenteros, S. M., Gómez, R. C., Sánchez, A. M. R., and Barghouti, Z. (2021). Mindfulness and academic performance: the role of compassion and engagement. Innov. Educ. Teach. Int. 58, 3–13. doi: 10.1080/14703297.2019.1676284

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bao, X., Xue, S., and Kong, F. (2015). Dispositional mindfulness and perceived stress: the role of emotional intelligence. Pers. Individ. Differ. 78, 48–52. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.01.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechter, B. E., Whipp, P. R., Dimmock, J. A., and Jackson, B. (2021). Emotional intelligence and interpersonal relationship quality as predictors of high school physical education teachers’ intrinsic motivation. Curr. Psychol. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02096-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., and Salovey, P. (2021). Emotional intelligence: implications for personal, social, academic, and workplace success. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 5, 88–103. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00334.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., Shiffman, S., Lerner, N., and Salovey, P. (2006). Relating emotional abilities to social functioning: a comparison of self-report and performance measures of emotional intelligence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 780–795. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Callea, A., De Rosa, D., Ferri, G., Lipari, F., and Costanzi, M. (2019). Are more intelligent people happier? Emotional intelligence as mediator between need for relatedness, happiness and flourishing. Sustainability 11:1022. doi: 10.3390/su11041022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmona-Halty, M., Salanova, M., Llorens, S., and Schaufeli, W. B. (2021). Linking positive emotions and academic performance: the mediated role of academic psychological capital and academic engagement. Curr. Psychol. 40, 2938–2947. doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-00227-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmona-Halty, M., Schaufeli, W. B., Llorens, S., and Salanova, M. (2019). Satisfaction of basic psychological needs leads to better academic performance via increased psychological capital: a three-wave longitudinal study among high school students. Front. Psychol. 10:2113. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castillo-Gualda, R., Herrero, M., Rodríguez-Carvajal, R., Brackett, M. A., and Fernández-Berrocal, P. (2019). The role of emotional regulation ability, personality, and burnout among Spanish teachers. Int. J. Stress. Manag. 26, 146–158. doi: 10.1037/str0000098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casuso-Holgado, M. J., Cuesta-Vargas, A. I., Moreno-Morales, N., LabajosManzanares, M. T., Barón-López, F. J., and Vega-Cuesta, M. (2013). The association between academic engagement and achievement in health sciences students. BMC Med. Educ. 13:33. doi: 10.1186/1472-6920-13-33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cebrian, G., Junyent, M., and Mulà, I. (2020). Competencies in education for sustainable development: emerging teaching and research developments. Sustainability 12:579. doi: 10.3390/su12020579

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamizo-Nieto, M. T., Arrivillaga, C., Rey, L., and Extremera, N. T. (2021). The role of emotional intelligence, the teacher-student relationship, and flourishing on academic performance in adolescents: a moderated mediation study. Front. Psychol. 12:695067. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.695067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, P., Bao, C., and Gao, Q. (2021). Proactive personality and academic engagement: the mediating effects of teacher-student relationships and academic self-efficacy. Front. Psychol. 12:652994. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.652994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, G., Zhang, J., Chan, K., Michaels, S., Resnick, L. B., and Huang, X. (2020). The link between student-perceived teacher talk and student enjoyment, anxiety and discursive engagement in the classroom. Br. Educ. Res. J. 46, 631–652. doi: 10.1002/berj.3600

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, R. Y. M., and Ng, M. C. Y. (2019). Mindfulness and symptoms of depression and anxiety: the underlying roles of awareness, acceptance, impulse control, and emotion regulation. Mindfulness 10, 1124–1135. doi: 10.1007/s12671-018-1069-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chipchase, L., Davidson, M., Blackstock, F., Bye, R., Clothier, P., Klupp, N., et al. (2017). Conceptualising and measuring student disengagement in higher education: a synthesis of the literature. Int. J. High. Educ. 6, 31–42. doi: 10.5430/ijhe.v6n2p31

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chu, L. C. (2010). The benefits of meditation Vis-a-Vis emotional intelligence, perceived stress and negative mental health. Stress. Health 26, 169–180. doi: 10.1002/smi.1289

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Côté, S. (2014). Emotional intelligence in organizations. Annu. Rev. Organ. Psychol. Organ. Behav. 1, 459–488. doi: 10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-031413-091233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Clercq, D., Bouckenooghe, D., Raja, U., and Matsyborska, G. (2014). Unpacking the goal congruence-organizational deviance relationship: the roles of work engagement and emotional intelligence. J. Bus. Ethics 124, 695–711. doi: 10.1007/s10551-013-1902-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewaele, J.-M., and Li, C. (2021). Teacher enthusiasm and students’ socialbehavioral learning engagement: the mediating role of student enjoyment and boredom in Chinese EFL classes. Lang. Teach. Res. 25, 922–945. doi: 10.1177/13621688211014538

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Fabio, A., and Kenny, M. E. (2016). Promoting well-being: the contribution of emotional intelligence. Front. Psychol. 7:1182. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Fabio, A., Palazzeschi, L., Bucci, O., Guazzini, A., Burgassi, C., and Pesce, E. (2018). Personality traits and positive resources of workers for sustainable development: is emotional intelligence a mediator for optimism and hope? Sustainability 10:3422. doi: 10.3390/su10103422

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diseth, A., Breidablik, H. J., and Meland, E. (2018). Longitudinal relations between perceived autonomy support and basic need satisfaction in two student cohorts. Educ. Psychol. 38, 99–115. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2017.1356448

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diseth, Å., Danielsen, A. G., and Samdal, O. (2012). A path analysis of basic need support, self-efficacy, achievement goals, life satisfaction and academic achievement level among secondary school students. Educ. Psychol. 32, 335–354. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2012.657159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, R. D., and Schellinger, K. (2011). The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: a meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Dev. 82, 405–432. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01564.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emery, A. A., Heath, N. L., and Mills, D. J. (2016). Basic psychological need satisfaction, emotion dysregulation, and non-suicidal self-injury engagement in young adults: an application of self-determination theory. J. Youth Adolesc. 45, 612–623. doi: 10.1007/s10964-015-0405-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Estrada, M., Monferrer, D., Rodríguez, A., and Moliner, M. Á. (2021). Does emotional intelligence influence academic performance? The role of compassion and engagement in education for sustainable development. Sustainability 13:1721. doi: 10.3390/su13041721

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Extremera, N., Mérida-López, S., Quintana-Orts, C., and Rey, L. (2020). On the association between job dissatisfaction and employee’s mental health problems: does emotional regulation ability buffer the link? Pers. Individ. Dif. 155:109710. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.109710

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fornell, C., and Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 18, 39–50. doi: 10.1177/002224378101800104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., and Paris, A. H. (2004). School engagement: potential of the concept, state of the evidence. Rev. Educ. Res. 74, 59–109. doi: 10.3102/00346543074001059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Am. Psychol. 56, 218–226. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Martínez, I., Augusto-Landa, J. M., Quijano-López, R., and León, S. P. (2022). Self-concept as a mediator of the relation between university students’ resilience and academic achievement. Front. Psychol. 12:747168. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.747168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gasiewski, J. A., Eagan, M. K., Garcia, G. A., Hurtado, S., and Chang, M. J. (2012). From Gatekeeping to Engagement: A Multicontextual, Mixed Method Study of Student Academic Engagement in Introductory STEM Courses. Res. High. Educ. 53, 229–261. doi: 10.1007/s11162-011-9247-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodyear, M. D., Krleza-Jeric, K., and Lemmens, T. (2007). The declaration of Helsinki. Br. Med. J. Publ. Group 335, 624–625. doi: 10.1136/bmj.39339.610000.BE

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach ; New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Hofer, J., and Busch, H. (2011). Satisfying one’s needs for competence and relatedness: consequent domain-specific well-being depends on strength of implicit motives. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 37, 1147–1158. doi: 10.1177/0146167211408329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsiao, C., Lee, Y.-H., and Chen, H.-H. (2015). The effects of internal locus of control on entrepreneurship: the mediating mechanisms of social capital and human capital. Int. J. Hum. Resour. Manage. 27, 1158–1172. doi: 10.1080/09585192.2015.1060511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Be Ntler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychol. Methods 3, 424–453. doi: 10.1037//1082-989X.3.4.424

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Modeling 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, J., Sang, G., and Chao, T. (2022). Self-worth as a mediator and moderator between teacher-student relationships and student engagement in rural schools. Front. Psychol. 12:777937. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.777937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jang, H., Kim, E. J., and Reeve, J. (2012). Longitudinal test of self-determination theory’s motivation mediation model in a naturally occurring classroom context. J. Educ. Psychol. 104, 1175–1188. doi: 10.1037/a0028089

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jang, H., Kim, E., and Reeve, J. (2016). Why students become more engaged or more disengaged during the semester: a self-determination theory dual-process model. Learn. Instr. 43, 27–38. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2016.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, W., Zheng, X., Gao, L., Cao, Z., and Ni, X. (2022). Basic psychological needs satisfaction mediates the link between strengths use and teachers’ work engagement. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:2330. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19042330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, X., Wu, Y., and Li, L. (2021). Validation and prediction of the school psychological capital among Chinese college students. Front. Psychol. 12:697703. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.697703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karapetyan, L. V. (2021). Research on the relationship of students’ emotional and personal well-being with intelligence indicators. Perspect. Sci. Educ. 49, 414–426. doi: 10.32744/PSE.2021.1.28

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilday, J. E., and Ryan, A. M. (2019). Personal and collective perceptions of social support: implications for classroom engagement in early adolescence. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 58, 163–174. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, R. B., and Ganotice, F. A. (2014). The social underpinnings of motivation and achievement: investigating the role of parents, teachers, and peers on academic outcomes. Asia-Pacific Educ. Res. 23, 745–756. doi: 10.1007/s40299-013-0148-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwon, K., Kupzyk, K., and Benton, A. (2018). Negative emotionality, emotion regulation, and achievement: cross-lagged relations and mediation of academic engagement. Learn. Indiv. Dif. 67, 33–40. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2018.07.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Law, K. S., Wong, C. S., and Song, L. J. (2004). The construct and criterion validity of emotional intelligence and its potential utility for management studies. J. Appl. Psychol. 89, 483–496. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.89.3.483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lei, H., Cui, Y., and Zhou, W. (2018). Relationships between student engagement and academic achievement: a meta-analysis. Soc. Behav. Pers. 46, 517–528. doi: 10.2224/sbp.7054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, Z., and Luo, W. (2022). Discrete achievement emotions as mediators between achievement goals and academic engagement of Singapore students. Educ. Psychol. 42, 749–766. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2022.2048795

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luthans, B. C., Luthans, K. W., and Chaffin, T. D. (2021). Character matters: the mediational impact of self-regulation on PsyCap and academic performance. J. Educ. Bus. 97, 1–7. doi: 10.1080/08832323.2021.1874856

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacCann, C., Fogarty, G. J., Zeidner, M., and Roberts, R. D. (2011). Coping mediates the relationship between emotional intelligence (EI) and academic achievement. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 36, 60–70. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacCann, C., Jiang, Y., Brown, L. E. R., Double, K. S., Bucich, M., and Minbashian, A. (2020). Emotional intelligence predicts academic performance: a meta analysis. Psychol. Bull. 146, 150–186. doi: 10.1037/bul0000219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madjar, N., Nave, A., and Hen, S. (2013). Are teachers’ psychological control, autonomy support and autonomy suppression associated with students’ goals? Educ. Stud. 39, 43–55. doi: 10.1080/03055698.2012.667871

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, A. J., and Collie, R. J. (2019). Teacher-student relationships and students’ engagement in high school: does the number of negative and positive relationships with teachers matter? J. Educ. Psychol. 111, 861–876. doi: 10.1037/edu0000317

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, A. J., Yu, K., Ginns, P., and Papworth, B. (2017). Young people’s academic buoyancy and adaptability: a cross-cultural comparison of China with North America and the United Kingdom. Educ. Psychol. 37, 930–946. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2016.1202904

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez, I. M., Youssef-Morgan, C. M., Chambel, M. J., and Marques-Pinto, A. (2019). Antecedents of academic performance of university students: academic engagement and psychological capital resources. Educ. Psychol. 39, 1047–1067. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2019.1623382

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mavroveli, S., Petrides, K., Sangareau, Y., and Furnham, A. (2009). Exploring the relationships between trait emotional intelligence and objective socio-emotional outcomes in childhood. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 79, 259–272. doi: 10.1348/000709908X368848

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., and Caruso, D. R. (2008). Emotional intelligence. Am. Psychol. 63, 503–517. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.63.6.503

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mérida-López, S., and Extremera, N. (2020). The interplay of emotional intelligence abilities and work engagement on job and life satisfaction: which emotional abilities matter Most for secondary-school teachers? Front. Psychol. 11:563634. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.563634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mérida-López, S., Extremera, N., and Chambel, M. J. (2021). Linking self and other-focused emotion regulation abilities and occupational commitment among pre-service teachers: testing the mediating role of study engagement. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:5434. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18105434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mérida-López, S., Sánchez-Gómez, M., and Extremera, N. (2020). Leaving the teaching profession: examining the role of social support, engagement and emotional intelligence in teachers’ intentions to quit. Psychosoc. Interv. 29, 141–151. doi: 10.5093/pi2020a10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newmann, F. M., Wehlage, G. G., and Lamborn, S. D. (1992). “The significance and sources of student engagement” in Student Engagement and Achievement in American Secondary Schools. ed. F. M. Newmann (New York, NY: Teachers College Press), 11–39.

Google Scholar

Parent-Lamarche, A. (2022). Teleworking, work engagement, and intention to quit during the COVID-19 pandemic: same storm, different boats? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:1267. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19031267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patrick, H., Ryan, A. M., and Kaplan, A. (2007). Early adolescents’ perceptions of the classroom social environment, motivational beliefs, and engagement. J. Educ. Psychol. 99, 83–98. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.83

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pietarinen, J., Soini, T., and Pyhältö, K. (2014). Students’ emotional and cognitive engagement as the determinants of well-being and achievement in school. Int. J. Educ. Res. 67, 40–51. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2014.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Podsakoff, N. P., and Lee, J. Y. (2003). The mismeasure of man(agement) and its implications for leadership research. Leadersh. Q. 14, 615–656. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2003.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putwain, D. W., Schmitz, E. A., Wood, P., and Pekrun, R. (2021). The role of achievement emotions in primary school mathematics: control-value antecedents and achievement outcomes. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 91, 347–367. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putwain, D. W., Wood, P., and Pekrun, R. (2022). Achievement emotions and academic achievement: reciprocal relations and the moderating influence of academic buoyancy. J. Educ. Psychol. 114, 108–126. doi: 10.1037/edu0000637

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quin, D. (2017). Longitudinal and contextual associations between teacher-student relationships and student engagement: a systematic review. Rev. Educ. Res. 87, 345–387. doi: 10.3102/0034654316669434

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raufelder, D., Regner, N., Drury, K., and Eid, M. (2016). Does self-determination predict the school engagement of four different motivation types in adolescence? Educ. Psychol. 36, 1242–1263. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2015.1008405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reeve, J. (2013). How students create motivationally supportive learning environments for themselves: the concept of agentic engagement. J. Educ. Psychol. 105, 579–595. doi: 10.1037/a0032690

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivera-Pérez, S., Fernandez-Rio, J., and Iglesias Gallego, D. (2021). Effects of an 8-week cooperative learning intervention on physical education students’ task and self-approach goals, and emotional intelligence. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:61. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18010061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roorda, D. L., Jak, S., Zee, M., Oort, F. J., and Koomen, H. M. (2017). Affective teacher–student relationships and students’ engagement and achievement: a meta-analytic update and test of the mediating role of engagement. School Psychol. Rev. 46, 239–261. doi: 10.17105/SPR-2017-0035.V46-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-Determination Theory: Basic Psychological Needs in Motivation, Development, and Wellness. New York, NY: Guilford Publications.

Google Scholar

Salanova, M., Schaufeli, W. B., Martinez, I. M., and Bresó, E. (2010). How obstacles and facilitators predict academic performance: the mediating role of study burnout and engagement. Anxiety Stress Coping 23, 53–70. doi: 10.1080/10615800802609965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saleem, M. S. (2022). The role of psychological capital and work engagement in enhancing construction workers’ safety behavior. Front. Public Health 10:19. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.810145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saleem, M. S., Isha, A. S. N., Awan, M. I., Yusop, Y. B., and Naji, G. M. A. (2022). Fostering academic engagement in post-graduate students: assessing the role of positive emotions, positive psychology, and stress. Front. Psychol. 13:920395. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.920395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaufeli, W. B., Salanova, M., González-romá, V., and Bakker, A. B. (2002). The measurement of engagement and burnout: a two sample confirmatory factor analytic approach. J. Happiness Stud. 3, 71–92. doi: 10.1023/A:1015630930326

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaufeli, W. B., and Taris, T. W. (2014). “A critical review of the job demandsresources model: implications for improving work and health,” in Bridging Occupational, Organizational and Public Health: A Transdisciplinary Approach. eds. G. F. Bauer and O. Hämmig (Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer), 43–68. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-5640-3_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schweizer, K. (2010). Some guidelines concerning the modelling of traits and abilities in test construction. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 26, 1–2. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sha, J., Tang, T., Shu, H., He, K., and Shen, S. (2022). Emotional intelligence, emotional regulation strategies, and subjective well-being among university teachers: a moderated mediation analysis. Front. Psychol. 12:811260. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.811260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinclair, M. F., Christenson, S. L., Lehr, C. A., and Anderson, A. R. (2003). Facilitating student engagement: lessons learned from check & connect longitudinal studies. Calif. School Psychol. 8, 29–41. doi: 10.1111/tct.13437

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smitha, K. B., Profetto-McGratha, J., and Cummings, G. G. (2009). Emotional intelligence and nursing: an integrative literature review. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 46, 1624–1636. doi: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2009.05.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, J., Bong, M., Lee, K., and Kim, S. I. (2015). Longitudinal investigation into the role of perceived social support in adolescents’ academic motivation and achievement. J. Educ. Psychol. 107, 821–841. doi: 10.1037/edu0000016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stahlberg, J., Tuominen, H., Pulkka, A., and Niemivirta, M. (2019). Maintaining the self? Exploring the connections between students’ perfectionistic profiles, self-worth contingency, and achievement goal orientations. Pers. Individ. Dif. 151:109495. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoeber, J., Childs, J. H., Hayward, J. A., and Feast, A. R. (2011). Passion and motivation for studying: predicting academic engagement and burnout in university students. Educ. Psychol. 31, 513–528. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2011.570251

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Su, Y. L., and Reeve, J. (2011). A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of intervention programs designed to support autonomy. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 23, 159–188. doi: 10.1007/s10648-010-9142-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szczygieł, D., and Mikolajczak, M. (2017). Why are people high in emotional intelligence happier? They make the most of their positive emotions. Pers. Individ. Dif. 117, 177–181. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.05.051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vesely, A. K., Saklofske, D. H., and Leschied, A. D. W. T. (2013). Teachers-the vital resource: the contribution of emotional intelligence to teacher efficacy and well-being. Can. J. School Psychol. 28, 71–89. doi: 10.1177/0829573512468855

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Virtanen, T. E., Vasalampi, K., Kiuru, N., Lerkkanen, M. K., and Poikkeus, A. M. (2020). The role of perceived social support as a contributor to the successful transition from primary to lower secondary school. Scand. J. Educ. Res. 64, 967–983. doi: 10.1080/00313831.2019.1639816

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, C. O., Greene, B. A., and Mansell, R. A. (2006). Identification with academics, intrinsic/extrinsic motivation, and self-efficacy as predictors of cognitive engagement. Learn. Individ. Differ. 16, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2005.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, M., and Kleinert, J. (2019). The relationship between coaches’ emotional intelligence and basic need satisfaction in athletes. Sports Coach. Rev. 8, 224–242. doi: 10.1080/21640629.2018.1491669

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, E. H., Wiest, D. J., and Cusick, L. B. (2002). Perceptions of autonomy support, parent attachment, competence and self-worth as predictors of motivational orientation and academic achievement: an examination of sixth and ninth-grade regular education students. Adolescence 37, 255–266. Available at: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/12144158/

Google Scholar

Wu, J. Y., Hughes, J. N., and Kwok, O. M. (2010). Teacher student relationship quality type in elementary grades: effects on trajectories for achievement and engagement. J. Sch. Psychol. 48, 357–387. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2010.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeidner, M., Roberts, R. D., and Matthews, G. (2008). The science of emotional intelligence: current consensus and controversies. Eur. Psychol. 13, 64–78. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040.13.1.64

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zysberg, L., and Schwabsky, N. (2021). School climate, academic self-efficacy and student achievement. Educ. Psychol. 41, 467–482. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2020.1813690

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: academic engagement, positive psychology, basic psychological needs, emotional intelligence, use of emotion

Citation: Chen H and Zhang MH (2022) The relationship between basic psychological needs satisfaction and university students’ academic engagement: The mediating effect of emotional intelligence. Front. Psychol. 13:917578. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.917578

Received: 11 April 2022; Accepted: 10 November 2022;
Published: 01 December 2022.

Edited by:

Woon Chia Liu, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore

Reviewed by:

María De Los Dolores Valadez Sierra, University of Guadalajara, Mexico
Gehad Mohammed Ahmed Naji, University of Technology Petronas, Malaysia

Copyright © 2022 Chen and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hongxia Chen, NTQ0MzQ2NDQwQHFxLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.