Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 23 May 2022
Sec. Evolutionary Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic A 150 Years’ Celebration of Darwin’s Book on Human Evolution and Sexual Selection: Its Legacy and Future Prospects View all 21 articles

The Road Not Taken: What Developmental Psychology Might Learn From Darwin’s Insights Concerning Sexual Selection

  • 1Peace Research Institute Oslo, Oslo, Norway
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Detroit Mercy, Detroit, MI, United States
  • 3Department of Psychology, Wayne State University, Detroit, MI, United States

Developmental Psychology is the branch of psychology that studies, not only human behavior, but how and why human behavior changes over time. This essay seeks to review to what extent Developmental Psychology has failed to perceive human behavior through the lens of evolutionary theory in general, and in particular sexual selection as first described by Darwin and later elaborated on by many, including Robert Trivers and Geoffrey Miller; the essay asserts that this failure has resulted in many wrong turns and missed opportunities. In some cases, major developmental theorists (e.g., Freud, Erikson) were bedeviled by sex-based differences which they saw but could not explain and which compromised the parsimony of their stage theories. In the case of stage theories of moral development, some major theorists (e.g., Piaget, Kohlberg) were able to offer simpler explanations of moral development only by limiting their studies to male subjects. And, while Developmental Psychology textbooks thoroughly describe sex differences in the timing of morphological changes in puberty, writers seldom discuss why the timing is different in the two sexes, universally, and functionally. On the other hand, several domains of developmental focus, including play, mate choice, parenting, and spatial cognition, have seen successful research efforts that utilized sexually selected predispositions as foundational assumptions. The essay concludes with a discussion of how a more evolutionary and functional view of human behavior might move the field of Developmental Psychology to an even more robust and accurate understanding of how humans change over the course of a lifetime.

Introduction

As we mark 150 years since Darwin’s publication of The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex, it is fitting that we reflect on Psychology’s limited ability to benefit from Darwin’s insights. Eloquent essays have been written on the broad failure to integrate Evolutionary Theory into the Social Sciences, including Psychology (Tinbergen, 1963; Pinker, 2004; Barkow, 2006; Segerstråle, 2006). Expectations for integrating Evolutionary Theory might have been particularly high for Developmental Psychology, which aims to explain changes in human behavior over the lifespan; those expectations have not been met either (MacDonald, 1988; Charlesworth, 1992). Looking at Sexual Selection as contrasted with Evolutionary Theory more generally, one is even more struck by Developmental Psychology’s stubborn resistance; Sexual Selection has been a fearsome lightning rod indeed.

There are many reasons for Developmental Psychology’s failure to integrate ideas from Sexual Selection Theory. These causes include Western philosophy’s history of dualism, a combination, culturally, of ignorance and arrogance about sexual matters, a focus on practical measures to improve people’s lives (meliorism), and suspicion of biology coming from both United States political wings. The result has been an emphasis on socialization and other culture-specific environmental factors in the development of human behavior, as opposed to species-wide biological factors. Above all, developmental psychology has failed to explore functional explanations for universal human behaviors and sex differences. Though the reasons for this failure are numerous, one prominent cause may be the misperception that because functional explanations imply genetic causation, extragenetic influences on phenotype development are relatively trivial, a position which is inconsistent with a dynamic systems theory perspective (Lickliter and Honeycutt, 2003; Badcock, 2012; Al-Shawaf et al., 2021; Narvaez et al., 2021)1. While a focus on ultimate explanations can lead to an overemphasis on biological causation (Narvaez et al., 2021), it is important to note also the benefits of theory in building a coherent science (Eronen and Bringmann, 2021). Like many before us (e.g., Ploeger et al., 2008; Bjorklund, 2018), we believe that a Darwinian approach holds great potential, in conjunction with other theoretical approaches (e.g., developmental systems theory), as a metatheory for a mature developmental psychological science.

This essay will not dwell on why there is this powerful bias toward nurture-based explanation; the dynamics have been amply described in prior work. Rather, this essay will briefly summarize some basic ideas from Sexual Selection Theory with relevance for Developmental Psychology. The essay will then describe the path taken by Developmental Psychology, as reflected in major textbooks used in higher education courses, and point out failures in theory and research resulting, arguably, from failing to recognize the importance of Sexual Selection. The essay will end with examples of successful integration of Sexual Selection insights into developmental research and call for more such efforts.

Sexual Selection Theory: Concepts Relevant to Developmental Psychology

Darwin was puzzled by the consistency of the sex difference in animals whereby males compete for sexual access to females. In 1972, Trivers solved the conundrum by providing a theory to explain this pattern (Trivers, 1972). He recognized that the female is usually the more nurturant sex, and the males compete for access to this essential, limiting asset, often resulting in the evolution of larger, aggressive males. Males can reproduce merely by inseminating a female, whereas in mammals most notably, the female must invest much more energy, providing internal fertilization, pregnancy, parturition, and lactation. Therefore, females are selected to be judicious in choosing a mate, a pattern discernable in humans as well as most other mammals. Mammals’ internal fertilization also means that pair-bonding males are prone to uncertainty about paternity, so selection favors males’ guarding their mates against sexual interlopers (Mealey, 2000). Correspondingly, pair-bonding females’ interests are more threatened by desertion by their mate.

Evolutionary theorists seek functional explanations for species-wide traits, including sex differences in behavior. They are interested in these “why” (ultimate; Mayr, 1961; MacDougall-Shackleton, 2011) questions that even intrigue young children but don’t seem to puzzle mainstream psychologists. Developmental psychology textbooks seem not to recognize the extent to which human behavioral sex differences can be explained by extensions of what Trivers calls the theory of sexual selection and parental investment.

While some have questioned the usefulness of posing “why” questions (e.g., Laland et al., 2011), we see value in their inclusion. First, functional explanations provide heuristic and inductive value for hypothesis generation at the proximate (how) level of explanation. Knowing that the heart acts as a pump immediately suggests how components of the system might interrelate. Second, functional explanations can help define categories of behavior providing ontological clarity. For instance, the form of a behavior often belies its function. Both an infant’s smile and cry, while differing in form (and state) function to reduce the distance between him and his caregiver. Likewise, the form of status-seeking behavior can include both prosocial and antisocial behaviors, yet serve functionally identical purposes (Hawley, 1999, 2016). Functional explanations can also distinguish between behaviors that share form (e.g., maternal aggression vs. reactive aggression). Third, functional explanations can provide an additional type of support (or lack thereof). Both proximate and ultimate explanation ought to be concordant. Forth, a functional understanding can improve targeting of developmental guidance (Ellis et al., 2012). Finally, our understanding of a given phenomenon is simply much richer if both levels of analysis are addressed. Knowing that male birds sing due to rising testosterone levels during the breeding season (proximate explanation) is categorically different than knowing that male birds sing to attract mates (ultimate explanation), but knowing both can be quite satisfying. See MacDougall-Shackleton (2011) for further discussion.

Returning to ultimate explanations derived from Sexual Selection theory, patterns of size dimorphism can be understood from this perspective. For instance, as in other species with male sexual competition, men are taller and stronger than women on average in all populations, maturing a year or two after girls. Adolescent boys and men are more aggressive and competitive than girls and women across cultures (e.g., Schlegel, 1995). Around the world, boys exhibit more play fighting than girls in order to practice combat and to establish dominance relations.

Men’s greater desire for status and resources for acquiring mates is linked to men’s higher violent crime rates (Wilson and Daly, 1985) and putatively responsible for men’s greater strength, especially upper body strength useful for fighting. Whereas men often react to a threat of attack by mobilizing physiologically for a fight, women and other female mammals often act to protect their young or to seek help, a reaction partly mediated by oxytocin (Taylor, 2006).

The sex differentiation in reproductive behavior is mediated in part by hormones. Accordingly, Sexual Selection Theory has come to incorporate elements of endocrinology, utilizing more elements of endocrinology than were available to Darwin.

Female mammals are exposed to little or no androgenic influence before birth, resulting in less play fighting than males. Girls with abnormally high exposure to androgens before birth show more play fighting than control girls, and less interest in doll play and marriage (Hooven, 2021). At puberty, interest in babies rises in both sexes, especially among females (Goldberg et al., 1982). At puberty, girls are feminized, mostly by estrogens. In both sexes, high levels of sex-typical gonadal hormones contribute to sex differentiation and hence in traditional evolutionary models are assumed to contribute to sexual attractiveness (Law Smith et al., 2006; Lidborg et al., 2022).

A woman’s level of several parental hormones during pregnancy or after birth is correlated with her bonding and nurturant tendencies. In mammals, maternal motivation is driven by hormones, exposure to young, filial emotional displays, competence, learning, and imitation. Men’s level of testosterone falls, and their level of prolactin rises when their mate becomes pregnant and when the baby is born (Storey et al., 2000). The greater these changes, the more paternal behavior the father exhibits (e.g., Gettler et al., 2011). Thus, parental behavior is shaped partly by hormones and experience in both sexes, but the hormones differ.

The parental roles are somewhat specialized by gender. Around the world, fathers tend to defend the nuclear family from attack (Opie et al., 2013), and fathers are expected to be more productive economically. Women, however, perform labor outside the home, prehistorically having gathered plant food (Friedl, 1975). Tribal cultures exhibit clear sex differentiation of labor, avoiding disputes over task assignment and redundancy and gaps in training during childhood.

Girls living in stressful or father-absent homes tend to reach menarche earlier than controls (Ellis, 2004). Boys in abusive homes and who possess a certain gene on the X chromosome, are at risk of antisocial behavior, and more at risk than girls. Abuse, including sexual abuse and homicide, is more likely when an unrelated male, such as a boyfriend or stepfather, resides in the home (Daly and Wilson, 1988). In general, development by any measure is better when both biological parents are in the home.

Many other human reproductive behaviors do not differ between the sexes but are universal and hence may have an evolved basis. Almost all tribal cultures are extended-family cultures in which three generations live together or in proximity. These universals are of interest because of their relevance to survival and reproductive success. Comparatively, biparental species tend to show marked reductions in sexual dimorphism compared to promiscuous or polygynous ones, a trend visible in our hominin ancestors (Grabowski et al., 2015). Pair-bonding emotions e.g., amorousness seems to be monotropic and qualitatively isomorphic and correlates of marital satisfaction are shared (Weisfeld et al., 2018). Both sexes compete for dominance, albeit males more so (Weisfeld, 1986), with forms of aggression also differing (Campbell, 1999; Pellegrini and Archer, 2005). Correlates of divorce, such as infertility are shared between the sexes, although earnings, financial independence, and infidelity differentially impact divorce across the sexes (Betzig, 1989). By many indications, including the fact that almost all people, around the world, seek to marry, marriage is a reproductive adaptation (Weisfeld et al., 2018).

Teaching Life-Span Developmental Psychology

We three authors have altogether taught various courses in Developmental Psychology for over 85 years. In teaching Developmental Psychology, we have sought to use texts that integrate causal explanations for behavior, reasoning that heredity and socialization interact throughout the lifespan. That is, after all, the charge of Developmental Psychology, understood from the time that the journal by that name began publication in the 1960’s: to explain changes in behavior as a function of time. As the first editor, Boyd R. McCandless, wrote, this would surely require integrating nature and nurture, including accounting for sex differences (McCandless, 1969). Yet published research and textbook writing certainly veered off that path early and often. Our own difficulty in finding truly integrative Developmental Psychology textbooks is reflected in the data provided in Table 1. In preparing this table, we created a list of concepts from Sexual Selection Theory that are relevant to Developmental Psychology, and we searched the subject index from 10 mainstream texts to see if the topic appeared (Fitzgerald, 1986; Berk, 2001; Rice, 2001; Broderick and Blewitt, 2003; Boyd and Bee, 2009; Feldman, 2009; Sigelman and Rider, 2018; Kuther, 2020; Kail and Cavanaugh, 2021; Kraynock et al., 2022). In many cases, whether or not the topic was found in the index, we searched further in the text to see whether the connection to sexual selection was made; if yes, we marked it as included, and if not, we marked it not included. For example, “breastfeeding” was mentioned in some books in terms of how long it’s recommended by U.S. pediatricians, but the functional reasoning, in terms of how milk varies with the nursling’s age and gender, was not presented. We included one text published before the year 2000 (Fitzgerald, 1986, which never went into second edition), because we considered it to include good integrative reasoning for its time. The text box below enumerates, for each concept, what percentage of the 10 texts made connections to the relevant concepts from Sexual Selection Theory.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Topics for sexual selection usually missing from mainstream texts on developmental psychology.

As is apparent, 9 of the 10 texts acknowledge that cross-cultural research and naturalistic observation are important methodologies in Developmental Psychology. Only 3 books discussed human evolution as a source of useful information. All 10 of the books describe gender-related similarities and differences in play, but explaining why they occur cross-culturally varied widely, in terms of integrating biological and socialization explanations. For example, Boyd and Bee (2009) present sex differences in toddlers’ play as the product of males’ identity development; in contrast, Fitzgerald (1986) and Kuther (2020) discuss the roles of prenatal hormones and environmental effects as inseparable influences. Out of the 36 concepts, 14 are not mentioned at all, and 9 are mentioned in one text. In addition to what appears in Table 1, the term Sexual Selection Theory (Darwin, 1871) was not found in any text; Parental Investment Theory (Trivers, 1972) was presented incorrectly in 1 text. As Surbey put it in her 1998 essay, our intention is not to admonish teachers of Developmental Psychology, but, rather, to encourage teachers to be open to integrating additional information into the way they think about development and the way they present it to students.

What if? How Theories Might Have Benefited From Sexual Selection Ideas

Stage theories are a major contribution from Developmental Psychology, as they function as path models, aiding our ability to predict behavioral changes over time. Dozens of stage theories have been proposed, but only a handful are generally familiar: those proposed by Freud (psychosexual development); Erikson (psychosocial development); Piaget (cognitive development); and Kohlberg (moral development). In each case, save perhaps Piaget’s, the theorist was handicapped by a failure to deal with sex differences.

Freud’s great contribution may be seeing that much human behavior is driven by unconscious forces (Miller, 2016); but Freud’s developmental stages (oral, anal, phallic, latency, genital) are generally not supported by empirical data (Rutter, 1971). Indeed, Freud admitted his frustration at not being able to fit female experience into his theory of the Oedipal Complex—through which a boy’s castration anxiety leads to identification with the father figure and elaborated moral functioning via a mature superego. Eventually, Freud (1925) proposed (1925) that because females never experience castration anxiety, females have compromised superegos, and therefore females’ moral reasoning and civil functioning can never be as mature as what males offer. As Gilligan concluded, “Thus a problem in theory became cast as a problem in women’s development, and the problem in women’s development was located in their experience of relationships” (Gilligan, 1982, p. 7). Several neoanalytic theorists (e.g., Chodorow, 1974) endeavored to reframe Freud’s theory in a way that does less violence to female development, by focusing less on anatomical differences and more on sex role differences.

One major example of such reframing is seen in the stage theory of Erikson (1980), who departed from Freud’s focus on anatomy and sex, again turning to relationships. But Erikson’s fifth psychosocial stage (identity vs. role confusion) and sixth stage (intimacy vs. isolation) seemed to lose their clarity when he discussed females; as Gilligan (1982) pointed out, identity and intimacy may be simultaneous developmental processes, and this seems especially so for females. Gilligan’s insights into weaknesses in Eriksonian theory make even more functional sense if one considers that females everywhere tend to marry at a younger age than males do partially due to limitations on female reproductive potential.

Piaget (1936, 1945) constructed a theory of cognitive development in children and adolescence that has largely stood the test of time, cross-culturally (Bjorklund, 1995; Miller, 2016) and provided the groundwork for research on adult cognition (Sinnott, 1996). As MacDonald observes, Flavell once commented that, “if one wants to understand how Piaget conceived the child, one simply has to ask how evolution would have designed an optimal learning machine” (MacDonald, 1988, p. 14). MacDonald points to the young child’s intrinsic motivation and responsiveness to general features of the environment. It would be difficult to argue that Piaget’s cognitive development theory would have substantially benefited from his paying more attention to Sexual Selection. It is a different matter, however, with Piaget’s theory of moral development (Piaget, 1932). Both Piaget and his intellectual heir, Kohlberg, were so bedeviled by gender differences in their findings that they chose to do most, if not all, of their morality research on males (Gilligan, 1982). Baron-Cohen (2005) addressed gender differences in social decision-making directly, when he modified his own theory of mindreading to make a “model of empathizing.” This model describes development in children up to age four, but its predictions fit with patterns of findings across ages and cultures. Both sexes are more likely to empathize than to systemize. However, males, on average, systemize more (attend to physical detail, construct, and organize systems) than females do. Females empathize (use eye contact, decode non-verbal expression, and respond empathetically) more than males do. Baron-Cohen (2005, p. 481) concluded that, “From an evolutionary perspective, sex difference in empathizing and systemizing are likely to have been shaped by sexual selection and follow, at least in part, from sex differences in reproductive strategies.” More research is warranted, but this is a promising start toward addressing questions about moral development.

One could argue that early theorists were anxious to see Developmental Psychology recognized as an independent science, distinct from the biological disciplines that were refining their methods at the same time (Charlesworth, 1992). But, once the path was defined as one that did not include biological influences, most developmentalists marched down the path without looking back, leaving the landscape littered with missed opportunities. Surely important opportunities were missed when stage theorists failed to recognize the implications of sexually dimorphic characteristics (often related to Life History processes such as gestation, lactation, and weaning) seen throughout human development.

What if? How Research Might Have Benefited From Sexual Selection Ideas

Generally, discussion of human sex differences has been met with resistance from Developmental Psychologists. Consider that the number of citations (according to Google Scholar) for Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) volume The Psychology of Sex Differences is close to 15,000. The book emphasizes socialization factors involved in sex differences in most behaviors, including aggression. By 1998, Maccoby (1998) had reconsidered biology’s role, describing an interactive process whereby biological factors nudged children toward sex segregation, which increased sex differences in aggression, etc. Yet despite the earlier edition being twice as old, it has garnered five times the number of citations. Similarly, books by Mealey (2000); Lippa (2005), and Geary (2020) have not found wide acceptance within Developmental Psychology.

Typically, Developmental Psychology textbooks document sex differences in the timing of morphological changes in puberty. With few exceptions (Weisfeld and Billings, 1988; Weisfeld, 1999), writers seldom discuss why the timing is different in the two sexes, universally, and functionally. Boys retain their child-like morphology longer, to gain experience and strength before their physical characteristics engender competitive responses from mature males (see Dunsworth, 2020, for an alternative, proximate explanation). Darwin had seen this sex difference in functional terms, when he described how young boys around the world practice wrestling, often not marrying before the age of twenty “as before that age they cannot conquer their rivals” (Darwin, 1871, p. 872). Only a handful of developmentalists (Aldis, 1976; LaFrenière, 2013) have pointed out that, while boys do more rough-and-tumble play, both sexes engage equally in chase games, honing escape abilities—demonstrating the utility of extending Sexual Selection Theory to understand functional patterns of universal and divergent patterns of play.

Again, the play literature tends to see bullying behavior in cases of what is arguably normal playfighting in preadolescent children. Weisfeld and Weisfeld (2013) refer to the work of Aldis (1976) who described playfighting between siblings as the typical scenario, where actors represent different ages and sizes, and where the larger actor frequently switches roles to let the smaller actor “win”—all with smiles and laughter as expressions of functional play. Since the work of Olweus (1978) this has been confused with bullying. The result is not only poorly conceptualized research leading to wasted resources and ineffectual programs (Merrell et al., 2008); the confusion has also led to reducing recess and punishing behaviors that may be developmentally normal and functional.

Where Has Sexual Selection Theory Benefited Developmental Psychology?

One can find intriguing examples of research that recognizes sex differences and explores those differences as part of the Developmental Psychologist’s challenge. Both Charlesworth and his student, LaFrenière, did observational research on children’s play and observed marked sex differences (Charlesworth and Lafreniere, 1983). Applying a functional lens, LaFrenière et al. (2002) were able to document those sex differences in 10 cultures showing that girls expressed more empathy and social competence, while boys’ behavior exhibited more physical aggression and dominance. Later LaFrenière (2013, p. 183) would write, “For boys especially, rough-and-tumble play in early childhood provides a scaffold for learning emotion regulation skills related to managing anger and aggression.” Thus, LaFrenière identified both an ultimate function for the behavior (male-male competition for future reproductive success) and a proximate function (social success with peers in childhood). As Mealey had observed earlier, “…the existence of two physical sexes virtually ensures the existence of two psychological sexes” (Mealey, 2000, p. 40).

Recognizing sex differences is fundamental for understanding the development of visual-spatial skills (Maccoby and Jacklin, 1974; Hoyenga and Hoyenga, 1993; Kimura, 1999; Geary, 2020). Silverman and Eals (1992) and Eals and Silverman (1994), in a series of observational experiments, found that male undergraduate students were superior at wayfinding, while female undergrads were superior at remembering objects and their placement in a two-dimensional array. They explained this through the “hunter-gatherer hypothesis” which attributed the differences to survival-related skills developed in humans’ hunter-gatherer past and inherited as predispositions seen in males and females today. As hunters, the theory explains, men utilized three-dimensional skills traveling great distances in search of prey and then finding their way back home. As gatherers, women covered shorter distances but remembered where they saw signs of edible plants on the ground—so that they could return and harvest what was ready to eat. Thus, the differences in visual-spatial skills are highly specialized, for functional reasons. Later, this theory was confirmed with data from 40 countries (Silverman et al., 2007). Interested in when these differences appear developmentally, Choi and Silverman (2003) designed experiments showing that some sex differences are apparent in 9-year-olds, but elaborated sex differences do not appear until about 3 years later. This suggests roles both for pubertal hormones, and for refining differentiated strategies through practice.

Life History Theory, developed in evolutionary ecology in the 1950’s (Stearns, 1976), and later applied to the human lifecycle (Hill and Kaplan, 1999), attempts to explain life-history strategies (e.g., sexual debut) in terms of selective forces, some of which challenge the sexes differentially. Falling within Life History Theory, the concept of sex ratio, the ratio of males to females, affects a wide range of behaviors. While the human primary (before birth) and secondary (at birth) sex ratios are approximately 1:1, the tertiary sex ratio, by early adulthood, is skewed toward females, because males often suffer higher mortality in childhood and adolescence, partially due to intrasexual competition (Mealey, 2000) and “risky behavior” (Daly and Wilson, 1983). Being the scarcer sex, males are then better able to impose their mating priorities. When females are in short supply, the converse applies. For instance, Pollet and Nettle (2008) did archival research on a U.S. population with low numbers of women; these women were able to marry young and marry well, securing husbands who had a higher SES than competing males. Conversely, when young males were scarce, they were less likely to be married, presumably because they were better positioned to pursue short-term mating; however, a higher proportion of these scarce men are married later in life (Kruger and Schlemmer, 2009). This topic deserves more research, especially given current patterns of sex differences in educational attainment (US Census Bureau, 2020), considered alongside tendencies for homophily and hypergamy (c.f., Sassler and Lichter, 2020). The sex ratio is skewed even more when fewer males present as attractive choices. Furthermore, patterns of marriage mobility—female educational hypergamy and male hypogamy (marrying someone with more vs. less education)—are no longer apparent (Qian, 2017; Van Bavel et al., 2018). Data from 28 countries drawn from the European Social Survey indicate that women, rather than remaining unpartnered, will often chose to “cast a wider net,” partnering with men whose socioeconomic status falls short of their own (De Hauw et al., 2017). Similar patterns have emerged in Latin America (Esteve et al., 2016) and the United States (Qian, 2017; Lichter and Qian, 2019).

Conclusion

Many books document human sex differences ably (e.g., Maccoby and Jacklin, 1974; Hoyenga and Hoyenga, 1993; Mealey, 2000; Lippa, 2005; Geary, 2020). We see heuristic value when a book provides, not just a massive listing of such sex differences, but a story told by the differences. It is the “orderliness” of behavior that is of interest, as Archer (1992) put it. We benefit most from research that recognizes patterns in sex differences and offers an overarching explanatory theory.

We need honest discussion regarding sex roles and implications for salaries and work environments, while simultaneously avoiding rigid normative judgments and outright dismissal of potential bias. Sex segregation in the labor force and gender inequality in pay continues, with the gender gap narrowing slowly in most countries that provide relevant data (Ponthieux and Meurs, 2015). One could argue for breaking down all professional sex segregation; but a stronger argument might be made for raising salaries of predominantly female occupations and reducing barriers in the workplace, so that atypical boys and girls feel comfortable pursuing divergent paths.

For those Developmental Psychologists who would like to read more about Evolutionary Theory and its application to human development, particularly in childhood, The Origins of Human Nature (Bjorklund and Pellegrini, 2002) is an important introduction, as are Human Infancy (Freedman, 1974), Ethology and Human Development (Archer, 1992), and Adaptive Origins (LaFrenière, 2010; see also Geary and Bjorklund, 2000; Salmon and Shackelford, 2007; Ellis et al., 2009; Konner, 2010; Geary and Berch, 2016; Bjorklund, 2020; Geary, 2020; Hart and Bjorklund, 2022). Many articles offer good introductions to Sexual Selection theory and its relevance to human development (e.g., Buss, 1995; Miller, 1998; Bjorklund and Pellegrini, 2000; Schmitt, 2005; Puts, 2016; Wilson et al., 2017).

What might be gained by incorporating Darwin’s insights into our work in Developmental Psychology? More complete understanding? More satisfying understanding? Better positioning for making decisions on a personal and societal level? More of a sound basis for arguing for equality of opportunity if we are so inclined? A sounder footing for educating the next generations of teachers, nurses, psychologists, and others who study Developmental Psychology? We argue, simply, yes.

Author Contributions

SG, GW, and CW conceived of the manuscript idea. GW designed the manuscript outline. GW and CW prepared the first draft. SG restructured and added additional content in the subsequent drafts, proofread, formatted, and submitted the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to express our gratitude to Lynda Boothroyd for her helpful comments on the current state of research regarding attraction and endocrine linked traits.

Footnotes

  1. ^ It is important to note that a functional explanation does not imply (hard) genetic determinism. While it does posit selection of genes, and a role of genes in development, it does not speak to the questions of canalization, that is robustness or plasticity, of phenotypes. It merely asserts some role of genetic causation and can explain species-typical patterns of behavior. It has long been observed that extragenetic developmental adjustments can alter behavioral phenotypes. For instance, Kuo (1960) demonstrated that breed-typical behavior could be extinguished via developmental adjustments.

References

Al-Shawaf, L., Zreik, K., and Buss, D. M. (2021). “Thirteen misunderstandings about natural selection,” in Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science, eds T. Shackelford and V. Weekes-Shackelford (Cham: Springer), 1–14. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2158-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aldis, O. (1976). Play Fighting. New York: Academic Books.

Google Scholar

Archer, J. (1992). Ethology and Human Development. London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.

Google Scholar

Badcock, P. B. (2012). Evolutionary systems theory: A unifying meta-theory of psychological science. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 16, 10–23. doi: 10.1037/a0026381

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkow, J. H. (2006). “Introduction: Sometimes the Bus Does Wait,” in Missing the Revolution: Darwinism for Social Scientists, ed. J. H. Barkow (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). “The Empathizing System: A Revision of the 1994 Model of the Mindreading System,” in Origins of the Social Mind, eds B. J. Ellis and D. F. Bjorklund (New York: American Psychological Association).

Google Scholar

Berk, L. E. (2001). Development Through the Lifespan. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Google Scholar

Betzig, L. (1989). Causes of Conjugal Dissolution - a Cross-Cultural-Study. Curr. Anthropol. 30, 654–676. doi: 10.1086/203798

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bjorklund, D. F. (1995). Children’s Thinking: Developmental Function and Individual Differences. Pacific Grove: Brooks Cole.

Google Scholar

Bjorklund, D. F. (2018). A metatheory for cognitive development (or “Piaget is dead” revisited). Child Dev. 89, 2288–2302. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bjorklund, D. F. (2020). How children Invented Humanity: The role of Development in Human Evolution. Oxford: University Press.

Google Scholar

Bjorklund, D. F., and Pellegrini, A. D. (2000). Child development and evolutionary psychology. Child Dev. 71, 1687–1708. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bjorklund, D. F., and Pellegrini, A. D. (2002). The Origins of Human Nature: Evolutionary Developmental Psychology. Washington, DC: APA.

Google Scholar

Boyd, D., and Bee, H. (2009). Lifespan. Upper Saddle River: Pearson

Google Scholar

Broderick, P. C., and Blewitt, P. (2003). The Life Span: Human Development for Helping Professionals. Upper Saddle River: Pearson.

Google Scholar

US Census Bureau (2020). Gender Differences In Education Attainment. Available online at https://data.census.gov/cedsci/table?q=gender%20differences%20in%20higher%20education%20graduation&tid=ACSST1Y2019.S1501 [accessed on Mar 14, 2022].

Google Scholar

Buss, D. M. (1995). Psychological Sex-Differences - Origins through Sexual Selection. Am. Psychol. 50, 164–168. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.50.3.164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, A. (1999). Staying alive: Evolution, culture, and women’s intrasexual aggression. Behav. Brain Res. 22, 203–214. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x99001818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charlesworth, W. R. (1992). Darwin and Developmental-Psychology - Past and Present. Dev. Psychol. 28, 5–16. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.1.5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charlesworth, W. R., and Lafreniere, P. (1983). Dominance, Friendship, and Resource Utilization in Preschool Childrens Groups. Ethol. Sociobiol. 4, 175–186. doi: 10.1016/0162-3095(83)90028-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chodorow, N. (1974). “Family Structure and Feminine Personality,” in Woman, Culture, and Society, eds M. Z. Rosaldo and L. Lamphere (Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press).

Google Scholar

Choi, J., and Silverman, I. (2003). Processes underlying sex differences in route-learning strategies in children and adolescents. Pers. Individ. Differ. 34, 1153–1166. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(02)00105-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daly, M., and Wilson, M. (1983). Sex, Evolution, and Behavior. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Institute.

Google Scholar

Daly, M., and Wilson, M. (1988). Evolutionary Social-Psychology and Family Homicide. Science 242, 519–524. doi: 10.1126/science.3175672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. New York: Random House Modern Library.

Google Scholar

De Hauw, Y., Grow, A., and Van Bavel, J. (2017). The reversed gender gap in education and assortative mating in Europe. Eur. J. Popul. 33, 445–474. doi: 10.1007/s10680-016-9407-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunsworth, H. M. (2020). Expanding the evolutionary explanations for sex differences in the human skeleton. Evol. Anthropol. 29, 108–116. doi: 10.1002/evan.21834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eals, M., and Silverman, I. (1994). The Hunter-Gatherer Theory of Spatial Sex-Differences - Proximate Factors Mediating the Female Advantage in Recall of Object Arrays. Ethol. Sociobiol. 15, 95–105. doi: 10.1016/0162-3095(94)90020-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellis, B. J. (2004). Timing of pubertal maturation in girls: An integrated life history approach. Psychol. Bull. 130, 920–958. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellis, B. J., Del Giudice, M., Dishion, T. J., Figueredo, A. J., Gray, P., Griskevicius, V., et al. (2012). The evolutionary basis of risky adolescent behavior: implications for science, policy, and practice. Dev. Psychol. 48:598. doi: 10.1037/a0026220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., and Schlomer, G. L. (2009). Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk. Hum. Nat. 20, 204–268. doi: 10.1007/s12110-009-9063-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erikson, E. H. (1980). Identity and the Life Cycle. New York: Norton.

Google Scholar

Eronen, M. I., and Bringmann, L. F. (2021). The theory crisis in psychology: How to move forward. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 16, 779–788. doi: 10.1177/1745691620970586

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esteve, A., Schwartz, C. R., Van Bavel, J., Permanyer, I., Klesment, M., and Garcia, J. (2016). The end of hypergamy: Global trends and implications. Popul. Dev. Rev. 42:615. doi: 10.1111/padr.12012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. S. (2009). Discovering the Lifespan. Upper Saddle River: Pearson

Google Scholar

Fitzgerald, J. M. (1986). Lifespan Human Development. Belmont, CA: Sage

Google Scholar

Freedman, D. G. (1974). Human Infancy: An Evolutionary Perspective. New York: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Freud, S. (1925). “Some Psychical Consequences of the Anatomical Distinction between the Sexes,” in The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, ed. J. Strachey (London: The Hogarth Press). doi: 10.1177/000306516601400402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedl, E. (1975). Women and Men: An Anthropologist’s View. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

Google Scholar

Geary, D. C. (2020). Male, Female: The Evolution of Human Sex Differences. Washington, D. C: APA.

Google Scholar

Geary, D. C., and Berch, D. B. (2016). Evolutionary Perspectives on Education and Child Development. Germany: Springer.

Google Scholar

Geary, D. C., and Bjorklund, D. F. (2000). Evolutionary developmental psychology. Child Dev. 71, 57–65.

Google Scholar

Gettler, L. T., Mcdade, T. W., Feranil, A. B., and Kuzawa, C. W. (2011). Longitudinal evidence that fatherhood decreases testosterone in human males. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 108, 16194–16199. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105403108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilligan, C. (1982). In a Different Voice. Boston, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Goldberg, S., Blumberg, S. L., and Kriger, A. (1982). Menarche and Interest in Infants - Biological and Social Influences. Child Dev. 53, 1544–1550. doi: 10.2307/1130082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grabowski, M., Hatala, K. G., Jungers, W. L., and Richmond, B. G. (2015). Body mass estimates of hominin fossils and the evolution of human body size. J. Hum. Evol. 85, 75–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2015.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hart, S. L., and Bjorklund, D. F. (2022).. Evolutionary Perspectives on Infancy. Germany: Springer.

Google Scholar

Hawley, P. H. (1999). The ontogenesis of social dominance: A strategy-based evolutionary perspective. Dev. Rev. 19, 97–132. doi: 10.1006/drev.1998.0470

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawley, P. H. (2016). “Eight myths of child social development: An evolutionary approach to power, aggression, and social competence,” in Evolutionary Perspectives on Child Development and Education, eds D. C. Geary and D. B. Berch (Germany: Springer).

Google Scholar

Hill, K., and Kaplan, H. (1999). Life history traits in humans: Theory and empirical studies. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 28, 397–430. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.28.1.397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hooven, C. (2021).. T: The Story of Testosterone, the Hormone that Dominates and Divides. New York: Holt.

Google Scholar

Hoyenga, K. B., and Hoyenga, K. T. (1993). Gender-Related Differences. New York: Allyn and Bacon.

Google Scholar

Kail, R. V., and Cavanaugh, J. C. (2021). Human Development: A Life-Span View. Boston: Cengage.

Google Scholar

Kimura, D. (1999). Sex and Cognition. Boston, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Konner, M. (2010). The Evolution of Childhood: Relationships, Emotion, Mind. Boston: Harvard Belknap.

Google Scholar

Kraynock, M. C., Seifert, K. L., Hoffnung, R. J., and Hoffnung, M. (2022). Lifespan Development: A Chronological Approach. America: Academic Media Solutions.

Google Scholar

Kruger, D. J., and Schlemmer, E. (2009). Male scarcity is differentially related to male marital likelihood across the life course. Evol. Psychol. 7:147470490900700210.

Google Scholar

Kuther, T. L. (2020). Lifespan Development: Lives in Context. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Kuo, Z. Y. (1960). Studies on the basic factors in animal fighting: VII. Inter-species coexistence in mammals. J. Genet. Psychol. 97, 211–225.

Google Scholar

LaFrenière, P. J. (2010). Adaptive Origins: Evolution and Human Development. New York: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

LaFrenière, P. J. (2013). Children’s play as a context for managing physiological arousal and learning emotion regulation. Psihol. Teme 22, 183–204.

Google Scholar

LaFrenière, P. J., Masataka, N., Butovskaya, M., Chen, Q., Dessen, M. A., Atwanger, K., et al. (2002). Cross-cultural analysis of social competence and behavior problems in preschoolers. Early Educ. Dev. 13, 201–220. doi: 10.1207/s15566935eed1302_6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laland, K. N., Sterelny, K., Odling-Smee, J., Hoppitt, W., and Uller, T. (2011). Cause and effect in biology revisited: is Mayr’s proximate-ultimate dichotomy still useful? Science 334, 1512–1516. doi: 10.1126/science.1210879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Law Smith, M. J., Perrett, D. I., Jones, B. C., Cornwell, R. E., Moore, F. R., Feinberg, D. R., et al. (2006). Facial appearance is a cue to oestrogen levels in women. Proc. Royal Soc. B P Roy. Soc. B-Biol. Sci. 273, 135–140. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lichter, D. T., and Qian, Z. (2019). “The study of assortative mating: Theory, data, and analysis,” in Analytical Family Demography, ed. R. Schoen (Germany: Springer).

Google Scholar

Lickliter, R., and Honeycutt, H. (2003). Developmental dynamics: toward a biologically plausible evolutionary psychology. Psychol. Bull. 129:819. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.6.819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lidborg, L. H., Cross, C. P., and Boothroyd, L. G. (2022). A meta-analysis of the association between male dimorphism and fitness outcomes in humans. Elife 11:e65031. doi: 10.7554/eLife.65031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lippa, R. A. (2005). Gender, Nature, and Nurture. New York: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The Two Sexes: Growing Up Apart, Coming Together. Boston: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Maccoby, E. E., and Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The Psychology of Sex Differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Google Scholar

MacDonald, K. B. (1988). “The interfaces between Sociobiology and Developmental Psychology,” in Sociobiological Perspectives on Human Development, ed. K. B. Macdonald (New York: Springer-Verlag).

Google Scholar

MacDougall-Shackleton, S. A. (2011). The levels of analysis revisited. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 366, 2076–2085. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayr, E. (1961). Cause and effect in biology: kinds of causes, predictability, and teleology are viewed by a practicing biologist. Science 134, 1501–1506. doi: 10.1126/science.134.3489.1501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCandless, B. R. (1969). Editorial. Dev. Psychol. 1:1. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294x.2004.02411.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mealey, L. (2000). Sex Differences: Developmental and Evolutionary Strategies. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Merrell, K. W., Gueldner, B. A., Ross, S. W., and Isava, D. M. (2008). How effective are school bullying intervention programs? A meta-analysis of intervention research. Sch. Psychol. Q. 23, 26–42. doi: 10.1037/1045-3830.23.1.26

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, G. F. (1998). “How mate choice shaped human nature: A review of sexual selection,” in Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology: Ideas, Issues, and Applications, eds C. Crawford and D. Krebs (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.). doi: 10.1017/s1464793102006085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, P. H. (2016). Theories of Developmental Psychology. New York: Macmillan Learning.

Google Scholar

Narvaez, D., Moore, D. S., Witherington, D. C., Vandiver, T. I., and Lickliter, R. (2021). Evolving evolutionary psychology. Am. Psychol. Ebup ahead of print] doi: 10.1037/amp0000849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olweus, D. (1978). Aggression in the Schools: Bullies and Whipping Boys. Washington, DC: Hemisphere (Wiley).

Google Scholar

Opie, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Dunbar, R. I., and Shultz, S. (2013). Male infanticide leads to social monogamy in primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 110, 13328–13332. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1307903110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellegrini, A. D., and Archer, J. (2005). “. Sx differences in competitive and aggressive behavior,” in Origins of the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Child Develoment, eds B. J. Ellis and D. F. Bjorklund (New York: Guilford.Press). doi: 10.2527/jas.2006-074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piaget, J. (1932). The Moral Judgment of the Child. London: Kegan Paul.

Google Scholar

Piaget, J. (1936). The Origins of Intelligence in Children. New York: International Universities Press.

Google Scholar

Piaget, J. (1945). Play, Dreams, and Imitation in Childhood. New York: Norton.

Google Scholar

Pinker, S. (2004). Why nature & nurture won’t go away. Daedalus 133, 5–17.

Google Scholar

Ploeger, A., Van Der Maas, H. L., and Raijmakers, M. E. (2008). Is evolutionary psychology a metatheory for psychology? A discussion of four major issues in psychology from an evolutionary developmental perspective. Psychol. Inq. 19, 1–18. doi: 10.1080/10478400701774006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollet, T. V., and Nettle, D. (2008). Driving a hard bargain: sex ratio and male marriage success in a historical Us population. Biol. Lett. 4, 31–33. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ponthieux, S., and Meurs, D. (2015). “. Gender inequality,” in Handbook of Income Distribution, eds A. B. Atkinson and F. Bourguignon (Amsterdam: Elsevier).

Google Scholar

Puts, D. (2016). Human sexual selection. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 7, 28–32.

Google Scholar

Qian, Y. (2017). Gender asymmetry in educational and income assortative marriage. J. Marriage Fam. 79, 318–336. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12372

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rice, E. P. (2001). Human Development. Upper Saddle River: Prentice-Hall.

Google Scholar

Rutter, M. (1971). Normal psychosexual development. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 11, 259–283. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1970.tb01044.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salmon, C. A., and Shackelford, T. K. (2007). Family Relationships: An Evolutionary Perspective. Oxford: University Press.

Google Scholar

Sassler, S., and Lichter, D. T. (2020). Cohabitation and marriage: Complexity and diversity in union-formation patterns. J. Marriage Fam. 82, 35–61. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12617

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlegel, A. (1995). A cross-cultural approach to adolescence. Ethos 23, 15–32. doi: 10.1525/eth.1995.23.1.02a00020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitt, D. P. (2005). Sociosexuality from Argentina to Zimbabwe: A 48-nation study of sex, culture, and strategies of human mating. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 247–275. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x05000051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Segerstråle, U. (2006). “. Evolutionary explanation: Between science and values,” in Missing the Revolution: Darwinism for Social Scientists, ed. J. H. Barkow (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Sigelman, C., and Rider, E. A. (2018). Life-Span Human Development. Boston, MA: Cengage.

Google Scholar

Silverman, I., Choi, J., and Peters, M. (2007). The hunter-gatherer theory of sex differences in spatial abilities: Data from 40 countries. Arch. Sex. Behav. 36, 261–268. doi: 10.1007/s10508-006-9168-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silverman, I., and Eals, M. (1992). “Sex differences in spatial abilities: Evolutionary theory and data,” in The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, eds J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby (New York: Oxford Press). doi: 10.1007/s12110-007-9022-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinnott, J. (1996). “The developmental approach: Postformal thought as adaptive intelligence,” in Perspectives on Cognitive Change in Adulthood and Aging, eds F. Blanchard-Fields and T. M. Hess (New York: McGraw-Hill).

Google Scholar

Stearns, S. C. (1976). Life-history tactics: a review of the ideas. Q. Rev. Biol. 51, 3–47. doi: 10.1086/409052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Storey, A. E., Walsh, C. J., Quinton, R. L., and Wynne-Edwards, K. E. (2000). Hormonal correlates of paternal responsiveness in new and expectant fathers. Evol. Hum. Behav. 21, 79–95. doi: 10.1016/s1090-5138(99)00042-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S. E. (2006). Tend and befriend: Biobehavioral bases of affiliation under stress. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 273–277. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00451.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Z. Tierpsychol. 20, 410–433. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1963.tb01161.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trivers, R. (1972). “Parental investment and sexual selection,” in Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man, ed. B. C. Campbell (New York: Aldine de Gruyter).

Google Scholar

Van Bavel, J., Schwartz, C. R., and Esteve, A. (2018). The reversal of the gender gap in education and its consequences for family life. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 44, 341–360. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-073117-041215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisfeld, C. C. (1986). Female behavior in mixed-sex competition: A review of the literature. Dev. Rev. 6, 278–299. doi: 10.1016/0273-2297(86)90015-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisfeld, C. C., and Weisfeld, G. (2013). Defining Normal on the Playground: What Would Tinbergen Do? Hum. Ethol. Bull. 28, 12–22.

Google Scholar

Weisfeld, C. C., Weisfeld, G. E., and Dillon, L. M. (2018). The Psychology of Marriage: An Evolutionary and Cross-Cultural View. Lanham, MD: Lexington Books.

Google Scholar

Weisfeld, G. E. (1999). Evolutionary Principles of Human Adolescence. New York: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Weisfeld, G. E., and Billings, R. L. (1988). “Observations on adolescence,” in Sociobiological Perspectives on Human Development, ed. K. B. Macdonald (New York: Springer-Verlag).

Google Scholar

Wilson, M., and Daly, M. (1985). Competitiveness, risk taking, and violence: The young male syndrome. Ethol. Sociobiol. 6, 59–73. doi: 10.1016/0162-3095(85)90041-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, M. L., Miller, C. M., and Crouse, K. N. (2017). Humans as a model species for sexual selection research. Proc. Royal Soc. B P Roy Soc B-Biol Sci. 284:20171320. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2017.1320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sexual selection, sex differences, developmental psychology, developmental pedagogy, stage theories, adaptationism

Citation: Goetz SMM, Weisfeld CC and Weisfeld GE (2022) The Road Not Taken: What Developmental Psychology Might Learn From Darwin’s Insights Concerning Sexual Selection. Front. Psychol. 13:900799. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.900799

Received: 21 March 2022; Accepted: 02 May 2022;
Published: 23 May 2022.

Edited by:

Hui Jing Lu, Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hong Kong SAR, China

Reviewed by:

David Bjorklund, Florida Atlantic University, United States
David Witherington, University of New Mexico, United States

Copyright © 2022 Goetz, Weisfeld and Weisfeld. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stefan M. M. Goetz, stegoe@prio.org

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.