Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 16 August 2022
Sec. Movement Science

Stable Sparse Classifiers predict cognitive impairment from gait patterns

\nTania Aznielle-Rodríguez,Tania Aznielle-Rodríguez1,2Marlis Ontivero-Ortega,Marlis Ontivero-Ortega3,4Lídice Galn-GarcíaLídice Galán-García3Hichem Sahli,&#x;Hichem Sahli2,5Mitchell Valds-Sosa
&#x;Mitchell Valdés-Sosa6*
  • 1Department of Electronics, Cuban Center for Neuroscience, Havana, Cuba
  • 2Electronics and Informatics Department, Vrije Universiteit Brussels, Brussels, Belgium
  • 3Department of Neuroinformatics, Cuban Center for Neuroscience, Havana, Cuba
  • 4Department of Data Analysis, Faculty of Psychological and Educational Sciences, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
  • 5Interuniversity Microelectronics Centre, Heverlee, Belgium
  • 6Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Cuban Center for Neuroscience, Havana, Cuba

Background: Although gait patterns disturbances are known to be related to cognitive decline, there is no consensus on the possibility of predicting one from the other. It is necessary to find the optimal gait features, experimental protocols, and computational algorithms to achieve this purpose.

Purposes: To assess the efficacy of the Stable Sparse Classifiers procedure (SSC) for discriminating young and healthy older adults (YA vs. HE), as well as healthy and cognitively impaired elderly groups (HE vs. MCI-E) from their gait patterns. To identify the walking tasks or combinations of tasks and specific spatio-temporal gait features (STGF) that allow the best prediction with SSC.

Methods: A sample of 125 participants (40 young- and 85 older-adults) was studied. They underwent assessment with five neuropsychological tests that explore different cognitive domains. A summarized cognitive index (MDCog), based on the Mahalanobis distance from normative data, was calculated. The sample was divided into three groups (young adults, healthy and cognitively impaired elderly adults) using k-means clustering of MDCog in addition to Age. The participants executed four walking tasks (normal, fast, easy- and hard-dual tasks) and their gait patterns, measured with a body-fixed Inertial Measurement Unit, were used to calculate 16 STGF and dual-task costs. SSC was then employed to predict which group the participants belonged to. The classification's performance was assessed using the area under the receiver operating curves (AUC) and the stable biomarkers were identified.

Results: The discrimination HE vs. MCI-E revealed that the combination of the easy dual-task and the fast walking task had the best prediction performance (AUC = 0.86, sensitivity: 90.1%, specificity: 96.9%, accuracy: 95.8%). The features related to gait variability and to the amplitude of vertical acceleration had the largest predictive power. SSC prediction accuracy was better than the accuracies obtained with linear discriminant analysis and support vector machine classifiers.

Conclusions: The study corroborated that the changes in gait patterns can be used to discriminate between young and healthy older adults and more importantly between healthy and cognitively impaired adults. A subset of gait tasks and STGF optimal for achieving this goal with SSC were identified, with the latter method superior to other classification techniques.

Introduction

Human gait patterns naturally change across the lifetime, adapting to changes in structural and muscular characteristics. The decrease in muscle mass, and aging of the bones and joints, among other musculoskeletal changes, produce a limited range of motion, the avoidance of weight-bearing, asymmetry, and limping in elderly persons (Pirker and Katzenschlager, 2017). Thus elderly adults walk more slowly than the youths, with a shorter step length and increased time in double limb support (Menz et al., 2003; Lønseth, 2016), among other changes. Gait disorders have a prevalence of 10% between 60 and 69 years, this increases to 60% in adults over 80 years (Mahlknecht et al., 2013). These impairments produce recurrent falls, depressed mood, and diminished quality of life (Montero-Odasso et al., 2012; Mahlknecht et al., 2013). The causes of gait disorders include neurological conditions, where cognitive impairments play a central role. Interestingly, gait disturbances in older adults with apparently normal cognition could predict early cognitive decline (Buracchio et al., 2010; Beauchet et al., 2013; Bahureksa et al., 2016; Kikkert et al., 2017; Byun et al., 2018).

The performance of spatio-temporal gait features (STGF) has been explored with different walking tasks in healthy and cognitively impaired adults. Dual-task, fast, and normal walking tasks, are the tasks most frequently reported in the literature. The dual-task paradigm has become the preferred test for assessing interactions between cognition and gait, in addition to the risk of falls (Lundin-Olsson et al., 1997). In this paradigm, the STGF obtained when the subject performs an attention-demanding task and walks at the same time are compared with normal walking (Pashler, 1994). The premises are that two simultaneously performed tasks compete for brain resources (Yogev-Seligmann et al., 2008; Montero-Odasso et al., 2009), and thus interference is greater when these resources are reduced (e.g., by brain damage). Some studies find that dual-task paradigms are good predictors of cognitive decline in healthy older adults (Beauchet et al., 2017; MacAulay et al., 2017), and also predict well progression to dementia in patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) (Montero-Odasso et al., 2017). Moreover, they allow discrimination between cases of Parkinson's disease (PD) that have and do not have MCI (Ricciardi et al., 2020). However, other studies find that walking at a fast pace is enough to increase sensitivity for MCI diagnosis (Beauchet et al., 2013; Bahureksa et al., 2016). Lønseth (2016) reports that walking at a preferred pace is sufficient for predicting cognitive decline from STGF. In addition, Ricciardi et al. (2020) propose that normal gait could be used for screening MCI in PD due to its high sensitivity. As can be seen, there is no agreement regarding the most appropriate walking condition to assess the relationship between gait and cognitive impairment (Bahureksa et al., 2016).

On the other hand, several methodological advances facilitate the use of gait analysis in cognitive assessments. First, inertial measurement units (IMU) enable gait analysis outside laboratories and during long periods (Mannini et al., 2015; Benson et al., 2018; Dasgupta et al., 2018; Gwak et al., 2018; Chen et al., 2020). Second, machine learning techniques (MLT) allow extracting more information from the data and uncovering unforeseen relationships between variables (Graham et al., 2020). A brief review illustrates these possibilities. Gwak et al. (2018) applied a logistic regression classifier to photoplethysmography and STGF and reported an 86% identification accuracy of MCI. Chen et al. (2020) explored several classifiers based on STGF for discriminating cases of Parkinson's disease with MCI and without MCI. They found that feature selection, based on principal component analysis (PCA), followed by classification with a support vector machine (SVM) using a polynomial kernel was the most accurate classifier (91.67%). Another study (Zhou et al., 2020) employed SVM combined with PCA, random forests (RF), and Artificial Neural Network (ANN) on the STGF of normal walking in samples of young adults, healthy elderlies, and geriatric elderlies (with many comorbidities, but without cognitive impairment). They were able to classify the age accurately with AUC values of 0.91 (0.81–0.93), 0.86 (0.63–0.83), and 0.86 (0.72–0.87) for each algorithm. Ghoraani et al. (2021) applied SVM classifiers based on STGF and Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) for the following discriminations: MCI vs. healthy, Alzheimer's disease (AD) vs. healthy, and AD vs. MCI. They obtained average accuracies of 78% (77% F1-score) and 83% (84% F1-score) with STGF and MoCA scores, respectively. Dasgupta et al. (2018) were able to accurately predict small changes in gait patterns due to cognitive load in a small sample of healthy adults using either logistic regression, RF, learning vector quantization, or SVM.

Unfortunately, most attempts to select the optimal experimental/STGF paradigms have compared only a few variants of walking tasks and STGF combinations, and only in small subject samples. This entails slow progress in defining the relative merits of the many potential combinations of tasks/features that exist. Consequently, there are still no standardized clinical tools for predicting the cognitive status from STGF, with a great variety of experiments and variables waiting to be explored. Solving this with traditional approaches would imply many experiments, each ideally with a large sample of subjects (with many more cases than the number of STGF). The Stable Sparse Classifiers procedure (SSC) proposed Bosch-Bayard et al. (2018) could help overcome this limitation because it can work with samples of a size greater than or similar to the number of STGF, allowing different combinations of these features to be obtained without having to increase the sample size.

SSC is applied to find more robust biomarkers by using resampling techniques. SSC estimates a penalized regression model and computes the precision and stability of the selected biomarkers. The procedure uses the glmnet package of MATLAB for estimating the penalized regression (Hastie et al., 2016). It then applies nested cross-validation to the data. Glmnet has been previously used for the development of a graphical representation of a mathematical function, called a nomogram, that predicts cognitive impairment in healthy older adults from different risk factors (including mobility) (Liu et al., 2021). SSC helps to deal with the fact of having similar numbers of features and subjects (Bosch-Bayard et al., 2018), and thus it is an ideal candidate to apply to our study.

Here we tested if SSC based on STGF could discriminate between young adults and healthy elderly adults (as a proof of concept) and more importantly if this approach could discriminate between healthy and minimally cognitively impaired older adults. We recorded gait patterns using IMU with a moderate-sized sample of participants. Participants were assessed with five tests from different cognitive domains. A summarized cognitive index (MDCog), based on the Mahalanobis distance from normative data, was calculated. In another study (Aznielle-Rodríguez et al., 2022), we have demonstrated that MDCog has a closer relationship with STGF than other cognitive measures. An additional goal was to use SSC to identify the best combination of walking tasks and associated STGF to predict cognitive status.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants were selected from different places of Havana city: the Cuban Center for Neuroscience (CNEURO), three nursing homes, several grandparent homes, and primary health care areas. The inclusion criteria were the participant's agreement, with ages between 20 and 40 years for young adults and above 60 years for older adults, and a Katz Index of independence ≥4 (Shelkey and Wallace, 2001). This index measures functional independence, based on the need for supervision or external help in performing basic activities of daily life (Katz et al., 1963). Participants were excluded if they could not walk or had to use walking aids, presented major neurological disorders, diseases of the musculoskeletal system, or severe cognitive impairment that prevented complying with instructions. Before gait evaluation, all participants underwent a neurological examination, filled out a questionnaire designed to explore the participant's habits, and underwent a cognitive assessment explained in the following section. Ultimately, 125 participants completed all the requirements and participated in the study, divided into 40 young adults (mean age 27.65 ± 4.14, 50% women) and 85 older adults (mean age: 73.25 ± 6.99, 62.3% women). Written informed consent was obtained from the participants or caregivers and the study was approved by the ethics committee of CNEURO, which verified compliance with the Helsinki declaration.

Cognitive assessment

Five neuropsychological tests were applied for assessing the participant's cognitive trait: Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Attentional Span or Brief Test of Attention (BTA), Trail Making Test (TMT), Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (HLVT), and Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DS). Global cognition was assessed by using the MMSE (Folstein et al., 1975). BTA is a measure of auditory divided attention (Schretlen et al., 1996). Attention, visuospatial abilities, mental flexibility, and executive functions were assessed by the TMT, parts A and B (Rabin et al., 2005). Memory was evaluated by HLVT, including immediate recognition and delayed recall (Brandt, 1991). The DS evaluated the focused, selective, and sustained attention as well as visual perception (Shum et al., 1990). Twelve cognitive measures were extracted. Normative data for these tests had been obtained for the Cuban adult population in an international collaborative study (Arango-Lasprilla et al., 2015a,b,c; Rivera et al., 2015).

Group definition

The cognitive index MDCog was calculated for each participant (Aznielle-Rodríguez et al., 2022). This cognitive index is an objective and continuous measure of the subject's cognitive status. It takes into account the results in each neuropsychological test, adjusted by age and educational level, and compensated for the correlation between them. It summarizes the overall deviation of the subject's cognitive performance from the normative data. The participants were classified into two groups (healthy and impaired), using k-means clustering of MDCog (1,000 repeats). The healthy group was split based on age. Thus three groups were defined: young adults (YA), healthy older adults (HE), and cognitively impaired older adults (MCI-E).

Walking tasks

Participants executed four walking tasks in an obstacle-free and flat environment. They covered 40 m (20 m in one direction and 20 m in the opposite direction). The walking tasks were: (1) walking freely at a comfortable self-chosen speed (NormalW); (2) walking at a comfortable self-chosen speed while simultaneously counting their steps, an easy cognitive task (EasyD); (3) walking at a comfortable self-chosen speed while simultaneously counting backward from 100, a hard cognitive task (HardD); and (4) walking as fast as possible without running (FastW). Counting backward is hard because it requires both working memory and attention (Montero-Odasso et al., 2009). Participants were not instructed to prioritize walking or counting.

Gait assessment

Data were collected with the Bitalino RIoT (Plux Wireless Biosignals, Portugal) IMU, attached to a velcro band, and worn firmly near the body's center of mass, on the lower back at L3 spinal level. The software OpenSignals (Plux Wireless Biosignals, Portugal) was used to record the data. The IMU data axes were transformed to comply with the International Society of Biomechanics (ISB) recommendations (Wu and Cavanagh, 1995).

The recorded gait patterns were split into two segments, one for each direction of the walk. The strides corresponding to the first and last 3 s of each segment were removed. Dynamic tilt correction (Moe-Nilssen, 1998; Millecamps et al., 2015) was applied to compensate for the imprecise position of the IMU and the effects of gravity on the measured accelerations. Acceleration signals were low-pass filtered (4th order zero-lag Butterworth filter at 3 Hz). The vertical acceleration signal was used since it is more robust and greater reliable than the two other axes (Alvarez et al., 2006; Kavanagh et al., 2006; Maquet et al., 2010).

The initial contacts (IC) and final contacts (FC) instants of the gait cycle were estimated with the method proposed by McCamley et al. (2012). This method smooths the vertical acceleration signal by integration (Zok et al., 2004), and then obtain two derivatives using a Gaussian continuous wavelet transform (Luo et al., 2006). The IC corresponded to the minima of the first derivative. The FC correspond to peaks in the second derivative. Then sixteen STGF were estimated: (1) step time (StpT); (2) step time variability or step time coefficient of variation (StpTCoV); (3) stride time (StrT); (4) stride time variability or stride time coefficient of variation (StrTCoV); (5) cadence (Cd); (6) root mean square amplitude of the vertical acceleration (RMS); (7) double support duration or double support time (DSD); (8) single support duration or single support time (SSD); (9) swing duration feed 1 (SwDurF1); (10) swing duration feed 2 (SwDurF2); (11) stance duration feed 1 (StDurF1); (12) stance duration feed 2 (StDurF2); (13) step duration feed 1 (StepDurF1); (14) step duration feed 2 (StepDurF2); (15) step length (StepLg); and (16) speed (GS). Almost all STGF are expressed in seconds (s), except Cd (steps/min), RMS (g), StepLg (m) and GS (m/s). STGF were computed using algorithms described in literature (Zijlstra and Hof, 2003; Zijlstra, 2004; Montero-Odasso et al., 2011; Bugané et al., 2012; Yang et al., 2012; Del Din et al., 2016a,b; Jarchi et al., 2018).

Thus 64 STGF were computed. Dual-task costs (DTC) for the STGF in the two dual-tasks were also calculated (32 costs). These costs (expressed as percentages) were calculated using as follows (Montero-Odasso et al., 2017):

DTC=(single_task_valuedual_task_valuesingle_task_value)100    (1)

Finally, 96 measures (STGF and DTC) were obtained for each walking direction.

Stable Sparse Classifiers procedure

SSC consists of the following steps (Bosch-Bayard et al., 2018):

1. Biomarkers selection: Initially data is perturbed (70% of subjects and 70% of variables are randomly selected by resampling method), followed by a screening step for eliminating variables with a minimal contribution. Later, a smaller set of biomarkers is selected using glmnet (see Model formulation below) with cross-validation. These steps are repeated iteratively (n = 500) to identify the variables that have a frequency of selection above a consistency threshold (50%). The parameters of the models were calculated intrinsically by the procedure.

2. Model validation (using the variables selected previously) based on ROC and stability assessments: A random subsample with the 70% of the subjects is used to classify using glmnet, and the rest of the sample is used to calculate the ROC values. This procedure is executed in several iterations (n = 500) to estimate the distribution of ROC values. The AUC at 50 percentile of the distribution is used to measure classification accuracy, and the model with highest accuracy is then selected as the best model.

Model formulation: Glmnet

The elastic-net model, used to select the biomarkers, is formulated as a weighted multivariate linear regression model, described by the equation (Zou and Hastie, 2005; Friedman et al., 2010):

β0,βminp[12Ni=1N(yiφ0xiTφ)2+λ Pγ(φ)]    (2)

Where: N is the number of subjects, xi p observations of subject i, and yi ∈ ℝ is the label group of subject i; φ0 ∈ ℝ, φ ∈ ℝp are the model parameters; γ is the regularization parameter; p is the number of variables in the model; and:

Py(φ)=(1γ)12 φl22+ γ φl1    (3)

is the penalty equation known as elastic-net norm (Zou and Hastie, 2005). The use of L2 norm ||φ||l22 induces a regression (known as ridge regression) that behaves well for high dimensional data but tends to spread out coefficient weights among highly correlated variables, and L1 norm ||φ||l1 produces the “lasso regression”, indifferent to highly-correlated predictors, which tries to estimate only a few non-zero coefficients (i.e., performs variable selection), thus inducing sparsity in the vector of coefficients. γ and λ are the parameters of the relative contributions of the ridge and the lasso to the elastic net.

The characteristics of the elastic net allow SSC to deal with highly correlated features. In addition, the glmnet classification algorithm can solve the problems related to the high number of features and the low number of subjects. Both advantages are useful for this study.

Analysis

The analysis flowchart of this work is shown in Figure 1. The goals of this work are presented in section A of the Figure 1, and the corresponding analyses to achieve them are shown in section B. Since the 96 STGF from the two walking directions were highly correlated only their average was used.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Analysis flow aimed to address the objectives of the work. (A) Questions derived from the goals of the study: Q1. Can SSC on STGF predict the mild cognitive changes present in healthy older adults compared to young adults? Q2. Can SSC on STGF predict the cognitive changes present in cognitive impaired older adults compared to healthy older adults? and Q3. Can SSC on STGF identify the best combination of walking tasks and associated STGF to predict cognitive status? (B) General description of the methods used to answer the questions and their interrelations.

SSC was applied for discriminating between: (a) YA and HE (used as a sanity check), and (b) HE and MCI-E (the main purpose of this study). In both cases, the following subsets of features (models) were explored: (i) the STGF from all walking tasks lumped together (All-tasks); (ii) the STFG for each walking task separately (e.g., NormalW); (iii) the six combinations of STFG from pairs of walking tasks (e.g., NormalW + EasyD).

After applying SSC for the discrimination between HE vs. MCI-E, the Bonferroni pairwise comparisons test were performed to carry out post-hoc comparisons between the AUC of all models. Accuracy, sensitivity and specificity were calculated such that we do not fail in detecting more than 10% of MCI subjects (i.e., sensitivity was fixed at 90%). SSC was applied also to identify the most stable variables in the two models with the highest accuracies.

The prediction accuracy of SSC was compared with two other classifiers: SVM and a regularized linear discriminant analysis (LDA). The same training and test sets, selected through 10-fold cross-validation with 500 repetitions, were used for these two classifiers.

Results

Clustering of subjects

The cognitive index MDCog ranged from 1.57 (the best performance) to 12.15 (the worst performance). The three clusters or groups obtained are presented in Figure 2 and their demographic characterization is presented in Table 1. Note in the figure a slight overlap between the values of MDCog between the HE and MCI-E, with the cut-off at about six. No differences in weight and height (which are known to affect gait) were found between the two elderly groups in a Kruskal-Wallis test. The elderly groups differed significantly from the YA group in these variables.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Division by groups using age variable and k-means clustering based on MDCog.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characterization of the groups obtained by k-means clustering based on MDCog.

Differences in STGF between groups

Supplementary Figures 14 show the normalized values in regarding to mean and standard deviation (Z-scores) for each walking task in the three groups.

The features related to the gait cycle phases and periods (i.e., gait variability and gait symmetry) were larger in the MCI-E group, somewhat smaller in the HE group, and even smaller in the YA. On the contrary, Cd, RMS, StepLg, and GS had larger values in the YA than in HE and MCI-E.

A non-parametric permutation two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) test, with GROUP and TASK as main effects, was performed individually on each STGF. The interaction between these main effects was also tested. The results from this ANOVA are shown in Supplementary Table 1. For all STGF and all analyses, there were significant effects of GROUP and TASK (p < 0.001) in almost all cases. The only significant interaction effect found was for StepTimeVar (p < 0.02).

Discrimination between YA and HE

This comparison was included as a basic and trivial test of our analyses. Figure 3 shows the results of classification procedure for all defined models. The ROC curves on the individual walking tasks models and the All-tasks are shown in Figure 3A. The same curves for walking task pairs are shown in Figure 3B. The AUC values ranged between 0.77 and 0.64. The models with the largest AUC were EasyD + FastW and All-tasks (both with 0.77), while the model with the smallest one was NormalW.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. ROC curves with 95% confidence bands and model AUC values for predicting the cognitive status between YA and HE groups using (A) the individual walking tasks and the all-tasks, and (B) the walking task pairs.

Discrimination between HE and MCI-E

The ROC curves for SSC between HE and MCI-E based on the individual walking tasks and the All-tasks are shown in Figure 4A. The largest AUC was obtained with All-tasks, followed by FastW and EasyD tasks. Interestingly, the lowest AUC was obtained for NormalW and HardD tasks. Figure 4B shows the ROC curves for SSC between HE and MCI-E based on pairs of tasks. The task-pairs including FastW yielded the largest AUCs. The most accurate combination was SSC based on the EasyD + FastW combination (AUC = 0.86, sensitivity: 90.1%, specificity: 96.9%, accuracy: 95.8%), which was close to that obtained with all the features together (All-tasks).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. ROC curves with 95% confidence bands and model AUC values for predicting the cognitive status between HE and MCI-E groups using (A) the individual tasks and the complete set, and (B) the combined tasks sets.

Figure 5 shows the results of a multiple-comparison procedure for discriminating between HE and MCI-E, using NormalW model as a reference. The AUC mean ranks and the confidence interval for each model are presented. The AUC mean ranks differences between NormalW and the rest of the individual tasks and combinations were assessed The differences for NormalW + HardD and EasyD + HardD models were not significantly different from NormalW (est = 89.58, p < 1.000 and est = −107.79, p < 0.904, respectively). The rest of the combinations were significantly different. The models with larger performance were NormalW + FastW and EasyD + FastW (est = −658.25, p < 0.0001 and est = −771.32, p < 0.0001, respectively). The only model that underperformed with respect to the reference was HardD (est = 208.00, p < 0.0002).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. AUC mean ranks and 95% confidence intervals (using Bonferroni) for individual and combined by pair tasks for discriminating between HE and MCI-E groups, calculated by the multcompare function of Matlab. The blue marker is the reference task, black dot indicates no significant differences regarding to the reference, red dots mean more significant differences from the reference, and green ones, less significant differences.

Contribution of selected stable biomarkers to the two best models

The selected biomarkers in the best models for discriminating between HE and MCI-E (NormalW + FastW and EasyD + FastW tasks) were analyzed. Figure 6 shows a plot with the proportions of time that each STGF was retained in SSC.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. The proportion of time (%) each measure appeared as significantly for two estimated models across iterations: (A) NormalW + FastW and (B) EasyD + FastW.

The best predictors in both models belonged to the FastW task. Specifically, they were the gait variability in step and stride times (StpTCoV and StrTCoV) and the RMS value. Also, the same features in NormalW and EasyD tasks were selected in each model, respectively.

Other features related to the description of the gait cycle (NormalW: StDurF2 and StepDurF1; EasyD: SSD and SwDurF2; HardD: SwDurF1) were also selected with high predictor power. In correspondence to what was explained in section Differences in STGF between groups, the features related to gait variability and gait cycle had higher means in the MCI-E group than in HE group. Contrary, the RMS values were higher in the HE group as expected result (see Supplementary Figures 14).

Comparison of SSC performance with other classifiers for discriminating HE and MCI-E

The means and standard deviations of AUC values for classifications between HE and MCI-E with SSC, LDA and SVM are presented in Table 2. As shown in this table, the best results were achieved with SSC. The other two classifiers had lower performances in all models. There were significant differences between the AUC of the three classifiers. Interestingly, the EasyD + FastW SCC with AUC of 0.86 was more accurate than SVM (AUC = 0.76) and LDA (AUC = 0.83).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. AUC values for the three classifiers using the eleven models for HE and MCI-E groups.

Discussion

The relationship between STGF (measured in four walking tasks with IMU sensors) and the cognitive status of elderly and young participants was studied here. All the STGF differed significantly between groups and walking tasks, with no interactions between these effects. Accurate classification of the participants into our experimental groups was found with SSC. The best SSCs discriminating between healthy and cognitively-impaired adults were obtained with a combination of EasyD and FastW tasks. Contrary to our expectations, classification based on the HardD task was poor. Variability of step and stride time, and the RMS value of vertical acceleration were the features that contributed most to classification power. Compared to SVM and LDA, the SSC was more accurate in classifying the participants into their groups.

We do not need sophisticated gait measurements to distinguish YA from HE. However this comparison serves as a proof of concept for our methods. Similar to many other studies we found statistically significant differences in STGF between YA and HE results (Menz et al., 2003; Zijlstra, 2004; Kobsar et al., 2014). Part of these effects related to aging could be due to impairments in skeletal-muscular function, but small alterations in brain function could also be playing a role. Previous work has been able to adequately discriminate YA and HE with other classifier algorithms. Previously, Begg and Kamruzzaman (2005) were able to discriminate between YA and HE, based on STGF extracted from normal walk, using SVM with different kernel functions. The got accuracies between 0.58 and 0.62 for the different kernels, similar values to our accuracy with the STGF from NormalW (0.61).

More relevant was the comparison between HE and MCI-E since the latter condition may not be so obvious to detect. We found statistically significant differences in all STGF between the HE and MCI-E groups. Previous articles (using smaller sets of STGF than ours) describe similar results to this when comparing older adults that are healthy and those with different degrees of cognitive impairment (Zijlstra, 2004; Buracchio et al., 2010; Beauchet et al., 2017; Kikkert et al., 2017; MacAulay et al., 2017; Montero-Odasso et al., 2017) or with neurological pathologies (Fitzpatrick et al., 2007; Ren et al., 2017). The fact we did not find interactions between GROUP and TASK in our ANOVAs indicates that alterations in gait patterns were pervasive and showed up in all tasks. Thus, we replicate previous findings while extending them to a much larger number of STGF.

In partial agreement with previous studies, we found that STGF from dual walking tasks improved discrimination of SSC. The FastW and EasyD tasks, alone and in combination, predicted cognitive status very well (although the HardD did not). To our knowledge, the use of counting steps aloud has not been previously reported as a walking dual-task. Fast walking has been previously described as more demanding than normal walking, and consistent with this, subjects with cognitive impairment (even in its early stages) perform it worse (Fitzpatrick et al., 2007; Montero-Odasso et al., 2011; Bahureksa et al., 2016). Reduced speed for fast walking is a sensitive predictor of impaired cognitive function (Beauchet et al., 2013; Bahureksa et al., 2016; Callisaya et al., 2017). Here we found that combining both EasyD + FastW enhanced prediction of cognitive impairments.

Gait quality should deteriorate with increased dual-tasks difficulty, especially when there is cognitive impairment (Montero-Odasso et al., 2009; MacAulay et al., 2017). However, here STGF from the HardD task (count backward from 100) did not enable better discrimination between HE and MCI-E compared to STFG from the EasyD task (count steps). The opposite result was found. One explanation for this is that perhaps not all of our MCI-E subjects understood the HardD task well, or were unable to perform it consistently. This could have increased the variability in STGF within the same subject, or between subjects, in the MCI-E group. For example, gait could momentarily improve if subjects abandon the HardD task for short periods of time, something we informally observed in many cases. Thus making the dual-task too hard may impair its diagnostic utility.

Interestingly, other authors have also reported low correlations between dual-task STGF (or dual-task costs) and cognitive performance, both in older adults (Lønseth, 2016; Kikkert et al., 2017) and in subjects with Parkinson's disease (Gaßner et al., 2017). The diversity in sample selection, the lack of standard protocols for the dual-tasks (Montero-Odasso et al., 2012), and the peculiarities of task structure (Halvorson, 2013) have advanced as explanations for these negative results. The specification of the dual-task paradigm seems to be the most important issue for detecting cognitive impairment with gait measurements (Montero-Odasso et al., 2017). Thus failure in task compliance due to an excessively high cognitive load is the most probable explanation for the low classification accuracy we found using HardD STGF. More naturalistic tasks that would be easier to perform, such as listening to conversations of increasing complexity, should be explored to solve this problem.

Higher accuracies were found here for SSC compared to SVM and LDA classifiers. However, this may not only be due to the advantage of the glmnet over these other algorithms in handling many features with few subjects. In SSC, identification of stable sparse biomarkers is executed prior to building the classifier in the prediction stage. In addition, the methodology uses different cross-validation algorithms which ensures prediction stability and techniques to avoid overfitting (Bosch-Bayard et al., 2018). These additional steps should also be combined with other algorithms (e.g., SVM) for a fairer comparison. Also, only the default kernel was used with SVM. Other kernels could improve its performance.

Similar to previous studies, we found that features related to gait variability (StpTCoV and StrdTCov), and RMS, had the best predictive power. Previous work has reported that gait variability is larger in healthy older adults than young adults at a normal pace (Kobsar et al., 2014). Increased gait variability in the fast walk of older adults with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease, compared to healthy older adults, has also been reported (Beauchet et al., 2013). The reduction of walking speed in older adults has been associated with the reduced magnitude of their RMS (Menz et al., 2003; Kikkert et al., 2017; Zhong et al., 2018).

This study has several limitations. One is that the sample of older adults with cognitive impairment is smaller than the sample of healthy older adults, which could affect the reliability of our findings. Moreover, the sample of healthy and cognitively impaired older adults was not completely paired by sex and age. This disbalance in the sample could affect any classifier used, not only SSC. Further work should examine the possibility of predicting cognitive scores (not only classifying into groups) through regression analysis, a goal for which preliminary results are presented in Aznielle-Rodríguez et al. (2022). Also, SSC should be compared with other classifiers, not only SVM and LDA.

Conclusions

This study confirms that the changes in gait patterns can be used to discriminate between healthy and cognitively impaired adults. The use of SSC improves classification accuracy over traditional approaches with classifiers like SVM and LDA. The SSCs using STGF from combinations of EasyD and FastW tasks produced the most accurate results, and were equivalent to using all the features. Hence, it is possible to use a reduced battery of walking tasks resulting in lower testing times (which is important with elderly patients). Since this study was carried out with IMU in naturalistic environments, our results suggest that applications of the methods in clinical settings should be explored.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee of Cuban Center for Neuroscience. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

TA-R: gait recordings, data curation, software, and writing. MO-O: methodology, formal analysis, reviewing, and editing. LG-G: methodology, formal analysis, and reviewing. HS and MV-S: conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, reviewing, and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was funded by the Flemish Interuniversity Council (VLIR-UOS) through the CU2017TEA436A103 project, awarded to Ghent University, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, and Cuban Center for Neuroscience. Also, it was supported by the PN305LH13-050 project funded by the Oficina de Gestión de Fondos y Proyectos Internacionales, CITMA, Cuba.

Acknowledgments

We thank the participants in this study, as well as the older adult's caregivers who gave their consent. To Daysi García Agustín, Ana Fernández Nin, and Yoelvis Pozo Burgos for the design and execution of the neurological examination. To Karen Aguilar Mateu for the design of the neuropsychological test battery, and to Ana Castro Laguardia, Neisbet Blasco Fanego, Brenda Peón, and José Aníbal Ojeda Núñez for the application and evaluation of the tests. To Jaime Menéndez Álvarez, Leisy Serrano Blanco, and Gianna Arencibia for recording the gait patterns of the study participants. Lastly, we also acknowledge the technical assistance of Agustín Lage, Eduardo Martínez, and Eduardo Garea.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.894576/full#supplementary-material

References

Alvarez, D., González, R., López, A., and Alvarez, J. C. (2006). “Comparison of step length estimators from weareable accelerometer devices,” in Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology—Proceedings, 5964–5967. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2006.259593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arango-Lasprilla, J. C., Rivera, D., Aguayo, A., Rodríguez, W., Garza, M. T., Saracho, C. P., et al. (2015a). Trail making test: normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation 37, 639–661. doi: 10.3233/NRE-151284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arango-Lasprilla, J. C., Rivera, D., Garza, M. T., Saracho, C. P., Rodríguez, W., Rodríguez-Agudelo, Y., et al. (2015b). Hopkins verbal learning test-revised: normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation 37, 699–718. doi: 10.3233/NRE-151286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arango-Lasprilla, J. C., Rivera, D., Rodríguez, G., Garza, M. T., Galarza-del-Angele, J., Rodríguez, W., et al. (2015c). Symbol digit modalities test: normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation 37, 625–638. doi: 10.3233/NRE-151282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aznielle-Rodríguez, T., Galán-García, L., Ontivero-Ortega, M., Aguilar-Mateu, K., Castro-Laguardia, A. M., Fernández-Nin, A., et al. (2022). Association Between Cognitive Indexes and Gait Patterns. In preparation.

Google Scholar

Bahureksa, L., Najafi, B., Saleh, A., Sabbagh, M., Coon, D., Mohler, J., et al. (2016). The impact of mild cognitive impairment on gait and balance: a systematic review and meta-analysis of studies using instrumented assessment. Gerontology 63, 67–83. doi: 10.1159/000445831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauchet, O., Allali, G., Launay, C. P., Herrmann, F. R., and Annweiler, C. (2013). Gait variability at fast-pace walking speed: a biomarker of mild cognitive impairment? J. Nutr. Health Aging 17, 235–239. doi: 10.1007/s12603-012-0394-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauchet, O., Launay, C. P., Sekhon, H., Barthelemy, J. C., Roche, F., Chabot, J., et al. (2017). Association of increased gait variability while dual tasking and cognitive decline: results from a prospective longitudinal cohort pilot study. Geroscience 39, 439–445. doi: 10.1007/s11357-017-9992-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Begg, R., and Kamruzzaman, J. (2005). A machine learning approach for automated recognition of movement patterns using basic, kinetic and kinematic gait data. J. Biomech. 38, 401–408. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2004.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benson, L. C., Clermont, C. A., Bošnjak, E., and Ferber, R. (2018). The use of wearable devices for walking and running gait analysis outside of the lab: a systematic review. Gait Post. 63, 124–138. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.04.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosch-Bayard, J., Galán-García, L., Fernandez, T., Lirio, R. B., Bringas-Vega, M. L., Roca-Stappung, M., et al. (2018). Stable sparse classifiers identify qEEG signatures that predict learning disabilities (NOS) severity. Front. Neurosci. 11, 1–13. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandt, J. (1991). The Hopkins verbal learning test: development of a new memory test with six equivalent forms. Clin. Neuropsychol. 5, 125–142. doi: 10.1080/13854049108403297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bugané, F., Benedetti, M. G., Casadio, G., Attala, S., Biagi, F., Manca, M., et al. (2012). Estimation of spatial-temporal gait parameters in level walking based on a single accelerometer: validation on normal subjects by standard gait analysis. Comput. Methods Prog. Biomed. 108, 129–137. doi: 10.1016/j.cmpb.2012.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buracchio, T., Dodge, H. H., Howieson, D., Wasserman, D., and Kaye, J. (2010). The trajectory of gait speed preceding mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol. 67, 980–986. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byun, S., Han, J. W., Kim, T. H., Kim, K., Kim, T. H., Park, J. Y., et al. (2018). Gait variability can predict the risk of cognitive decline in cognitively normal older people. Demen. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 45, 251–261. doi: 10.1159/000489927

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Callisaya, M. L., Launay, C. P., Srikanth, V. K., Verghese, J., Allali, G., and Beauchet, O. (2017). Cognitive status, fast walking speed and walking speed reserve—the gait and Alzheimer interactions tracking (GAIT) study. Geroscience 39, 231–239. doi: 10.1007/s11357-017-9973-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, P. H., Lien, C. W., Wu, W. C., Lee, L. S., and Shaw, J. S. (2020). Gait-based machine learning for classifying patients with different types of mild cognitive impairment. J. Med. Syst. 44, 1–6. doi: 10.1007/s10916-020-01578-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dasgupta, P., Vanswearingen, J., and Sejdic, E. (2018). “You can tell by the way i use my walk.” Predicting the presence of cognitive load with gait measurements. Biomed. Eng. Online 17, 1–18. doi: 10.1186/s12938-018-0555-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Din, S., Godfrey, A., and Rochester, L. (2016a). Validation of an accelerometer to quantify a comprehensive battery of gait characteristics in healthy older adults and Parkinson's disease: toward clinical and at home use. IEEE J. Biomed. Health Inf. 20, 838–847. doi: 10.1109/JBHI.2015.2419317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Din, S., Hickey, A., Ladha, C., Stuart, S., Bourke, A. K., Esser, P., et al. (2016b). Instrumented gait assessment with a single wearable: an introductory tutorial. F1000Res. 5, 1–17. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.9591.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, A. L., Buchanan, C. K., Nahin, R. L., DeKosky, S. T., Atkinson, H. H., Carlson, M. C., et al. (2007). Associations of gait speed and other measures of physical function with cognition in a healthy cohort of elderly persons. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 62, 1244–1251. doi: 10.1093/gerona/62.11.1244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “MINI-MENTAL STATE” a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiat. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, J., Hastie, T., and Tibshirani, R. (2010). Regularization paths for generalized linear models via coordinate descent. J. Stat. Softw. 33, 1–22. doi: 10.18637/jss.v033.i01

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaßner, H., Marxreiter, F., Steib, S., Kohl, Z., Schlachetzki, J. C. M., Adler, W., et al. (2017). Gait and cognition in parkinson's disease: Cognitive impairment is inadequately reflected by gait performance during dual task. Front. Neurol. 8, 550. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghoraani, B., Boettcher, L. N., Hssayeni, M. D., Rosenfeld, A., Tolea, M. I., and Galvin, J. E. (2021). Detection of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease using dual-task gait assessments and machine learning. Biomed. Signal Process. Control 64, 102249. doi: 10.1016/j.bspc.2020.102249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, S. A., Lee, E. E., Jeste, D. V., Van Patten, R., Twamley, E. W., Nebeker, C., et al. (2020). Artificial intelligence approaches to predicting and detecting cognitive decline in older adults: a conceptual review. Psychiatry Res. 284, 112732. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2019.112732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gwak, M., Sarrafzadeh, M., and Woo, E. (2018). “Support for a clinical diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment using photoplethysmography and gait sensors,” in Proceedings, APSIPA Annual Summit and Conference 2018, (November), 671–678.

Google Scholar

Halvorson, K. M. (2013). What causes dual-task costs? (Ph.D. thesis). University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, United States. doi: 10.17077/etd.1dpxsatz

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hastie, T., Qian, J., and Tay, K. (2016). An Introduction to Glmnet. CRAN R Repositary.

Google Scholar

Jarchi, D., Pope, J., Lee, T. K. M., Tamjidi, L., Mirzaei, A., and Sanei, S. (2018). A review on accelerometry-based gait analysis and emerging clinical applications. IEEE Rev. Biomed. Eng. 11, 177–194. doi: 10.1109/RBME.2018.2807182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katz, S., Ford, A. B., Moskowitz, R. W., Jackson, B. A., and Jaffe, M. W. (1963). Studies of illness in the aged the index of ADL: a standardized measure of biological and psychosocial function. JAMA 185, 94–99. doi: 10.1001/jama.1963.03060120024016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kavanagh, J. J., Morrison, S., James, D. A., and Barretta, R. (2006). Reliability of segmental accelerations measured using a new wireless gait analysis system. J. Biomech. 39, 2863–2872. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2005.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kikkert, L. H. J., Vuillerme, N., van Campen, J. P., Appels, B. A., Hortobágyi, T., and Lamoth, C. J. C. (2017). Gait characteristics and their discriminative power in geriatric patients with and without cognitive impairment. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 14, 1–10. doi: 10.1186/s12984-017-0297-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kobsar, D., Olson, C., Paranjape, R., Hadjistavropoulos, T., and Barden, J. M. (2014). Evaluation of age-related differences in the stride-to-stride fluctuations, regularity and symmetry of gait using a waist-mounted tri-axial accelerometer. Gait Post. 39, 553–557. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Wei, K., Cao, X., Jiang, L., Gu, N., Feng, L., et al. (2021). Development and validation of a nomogram based on motoric cognitive risk syndrome for cognitive impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 13, 1–9. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.618833

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lønseth, A. (2016). The Association Between Gait and Cognitive Function in Relatively Healthy Elderly People. Master's thesis, NTNU.

Google Scholar

Lundin-Olsson, L., Nyberg, L., and Gustafson, Y. (1997). “Stops walking when talking” as a predictor of falls in elderly people. Lancet 349, 617. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)24009-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, J., Bai, J., and Shao, J. (2006). Application of the wavelet transforms on axial strain calculation in ultrasound elastography. Prog. Nat. Sci. Mater. Int. 16, 942–947. doi: 10.1080/10020070612330093

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacAulay, R. K., Wagner, M. T., Szeles, D., and Milano, N. J. (2017). Improving sensitivity to detect mild cognitive impairment: cognitive load dual-task gait speed assessment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 23, 493–501. doi: 10.1017/S1355617717000261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahlknecht, P., Kiechl, S., Bloem, B. R., Willeit, J., Scherfler, C., Gasperi, A., et al. (2013). Prevalence and burden of gait disorders in elderly men and women aged 60–97 years: a population-based study. PLoS ONE 8, 1–7. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mannini, A., Trojaniello, D., and Della Croce, U. (2015). “Hidden Markov model-based strategy for gait segmentation using inertial sensors: application to elderly, hemiparetic patients and Huntington's disease patients,” in Proceedings of the Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, EMBS, 5179–5182. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2015.7319558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maquet, D., Lekeu, F., Warzee, E., Gillain, S., Wojtasik, V., Salmon, E., et al. (2010). Gait analysis in elderly adult patients with mild cognitive impairment and patients with mild Alzheimer's disease: simple versus dual task: a preliminary report. Clin. Physiol. Funct. Imaging 30, 51–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-097X.2009.00903.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCamley, J., Donati, M., Grimpampi, E., and Mazzà, C. (2012). An enhanced estimate of initial contact and final contact instants of time using lower trunk inertial sensor data. Gait Post. 36, 316–318. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.02.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menz, H. B., Lord, S. R., and Fitzpatrick, R. C. (2003). Age-related differences in walking stability. Age Ageing 32, 137–142. doi: 10.1093/ageing/32.2.137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millecamps, A., Lowry, K. A., Brach, J. S., Perera, S., Redfern, M. S., and Sejdić, E. (2015). Understanding the effects of pre-processing on extracted signal features from gait accelerometry signals. Comput. Biol. Med. 62, 164–174. doi: 10.1016/j.compbiomed.2015.03.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moe-Nilssen, R. (1998). A new method for evaluating motor control in gait under real-life environmental conditions. Part 1: the instrument. Clin. Biomech. 13, 320–327. doi: 10.1016/S0268-0033(98)00089-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero-Odasso, M., Casas, A., Hansen, K. T., Bilski, P., Gutmanis, I., Wells, J. L., et al. (2009). Quantitative gait analysis under dual-task in older people with mild cognitive impairment: a reliability study. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 6, 1–6. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-6-35

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero-Odasso, M., Muir, S. W., Hall, M., Doherty, T. J., Kloseck, M., Beauchet, O., et al. (2011). Gait variability is associated with frailty in community-dwelling older adults. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 66, 568–576. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero-Odasso, M., Verghese, J., Beauchet, O., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2012). Gait and cognition: a complementary approach to understanding brain function and the risk of falling. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 60, 2127–2136. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04209.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montero-Odasso, M. M., Sarquis-Adamson, Y., Speechley, M., Borrie, M. J., Hachinski, V. C., Wells, J., et al. (2017). Association of dual-task gait with incident dementia in mild cognitive impairment: results from the gait and brain study. JAMA Neurol. 74, 857–865. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.0643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychol. Bull. 116, 220–244. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pirker, W., and Katzenschlager, R. (2017). Gait disorders in adults and the elderly: a clinical guide. Wien. Klin. Wochenschr. 129, 81–95. doi: 10.1007/s00508-016-1096-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabin, L. A., Barr, W. B., and Burton, L. A. (2005). Assessment practices of clinical neuropsychologists in the United States and Canada: a survey of INS, NAN, and APA Division 40 members. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 20, 33–65. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2004.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ren, P., Tang, S., Fang, F., Luo, L., Xu, L., Bringas-Vega, M. L., et al. (2017). Gait rhythm fluctuation analysis for neurodegenerative diseases by empirical mode decomposition. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 64, 52–60. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2016.2536438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ricciardi, C., Amboni, M., De Santis, C., Ricciardelli, G., Improta, G., D'Addio, G., et al. (2020). “Machine learning can detect the presence of mild cognitive impairment in patients affected by Parkinson's disease,” in IEEE Medical Measurements and Applications, MeMeA 2020—Conference Proceedings. doi: 10.1109/MeMeA49120.2020.9137301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivera, D., Perrin, P. B., Aliaga, A., Garza, M. T., Saracho, C. P., Rodríguez, W., et al. (2015). Brief test of attention: normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation 37, 663–676. doi: 10.3233/NRE-151283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schretlen, D., Bobholz, J. H., and Brandt, J. (1996). Development and psychometric properties of the brief test of attention. Clin. Neuropsychol. 10, 80–89. doi: 10.1080/13854049608406666

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shelkey, M., and Wallace, M. (2001). Katz index of independence in activities of daily living. Home Healthcare Nurse. 19, 323–324. doi: 10.1097/00004045-200105000-00020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shum, D. H. K., McFarland, K. A., and Bain, J. D. (1990). Construct validity of eight tests of attention: comparison of normal and closed head injured samples. Clin. Neuropsychol. 4, 151–162. doi: 10.1080/13854049008401508

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, G., and Cavanagh, P. R. (1995). ISB recommendations for standardization in the reporting of kinematic data. J. Biomech. 28, 1257–1261. doi: 10.1016/0021-9290(95)00017-C

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, M., Zheng, H., Wang, H., McClean, S., and Newel, D. (2012). IGAIT: an interactive accelerometer based gait analysis system. Comput. Methods Prog. Biomed. 108, 715–723. doi: 10.1016/j.cmpb.2012.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yogev-Seligmann, G., Hausdorff, J. M., and Giladi, N. (2008). The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov. Disord. 23, 329–342. doi: 10.1002/mds.21720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhong, R., Rau, P. L. P., and Yan, X. (2018). Application of smart bracelet to monitor frailty-related gait parameters of older Chinese adults: a preliminary study. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 18, 1366–1371. doi: 10.1111/ggi.13492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Y., Romijnders, R., Hansen, C., van Campen, J., Maetzler, W., Hortobágyi, T., et al. (2020). The detection of age groups by dynamic gait outcomes using machine learning approaches. Sci. Rep. 10, 1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-61423-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zijlstra, W. (2004). Assessment of spatio-temporal parameters during unconstrained walking. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 92, 39–44. doi: 10.1007/s00421-004-1041-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zijlstra, W., and Hof, A. L. (2003). Assessment of spatio-temporal gait parameters from trunk accelerations during human walking. Gait Post. 18, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(02)00190-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zok, M., Mazzà, C., and Della Croce, U. (2004). Total body centre of mass displacement estimated using ground reactions during transitory motor tasks: application to step ascent. Med. Eng. Phys. 26, 791–798. doi: 10.1016/j.medengphy.2004.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zou, H., and Hastie, T. (2005). Regularization and variable selection via the elastic net. J. Roy. Stat. Soc. Ser. Stat. Methodol. 67, 301–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9868.2005.00503.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: gait analysis, cognitive impairment, classification, glmnet, stability ROC, biomarkers

Citation: Aznielle-Rodríguez T, Ontivero-Ortega M, Galán-García L, Sahli H and Valdés-Sosa M (2022) Stable Sparse Classifiers predict cognitive impairment from gait patterns. Front. Psychol. 13:894576. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.894576

Received: 12 April 2022; Accepted: 06 July 2022;
Published: 16 August 2022.

Edited by:

John B. Shea, Indiana University Bloomington, United States

Reviewed by:

Andrea Tacchino, Associazione Italiana Sclerosi Multipla, Italy
Elaheh Moradi, University of Tampere, Finland

Copyright © 2022 Aznielle-Rodríguez, Ontivero-Ortega, Galán-García, Sahli and Valdés-Sosa. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mitchell Valdés-Sosa, mitchell@cneuro.edu.cu

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share senior authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.