Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 30 January 2023
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Methods and Applications in Personality and Social Psychology: The Person - Environment Interaction: New Instruments and their First Applications View all 9 articles

Revised environmental identity scale: Adaptation and preliminary examination on a sample of Italian pet owners

  • 1Department of Psychology of Developmental and Socialization Processes, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 2Department of Education, Psychology, Philosophy, University of Cagliari, Cagliari, Italy

The Revised Environmental Identity (EID) Scale is a tool proposed by Clayton in 2021 to replace her 2003’s EID Scale and aims to measure individual differences in a stable sense of interdependence and connectedness with nature. Since an Italian version of this scale was still missing, the present study presents an adaptation of the Revised EID Scale in Italian. The scale has been translated, back-translated, and administered online to 163 pet owners living in Italy in the context of a study about pet attachment. A parallel analysis suggested the existence of two factors. The exploratory factor analysis (EFA) identified the same number of factors: “Connectedness to nature” (nine items) and “Protection of nature” (five items); the two subscales were found to be consistent. This structure explains more variance compared with the traditional one-factor solution. Sociodemographic variables do not seem to affect the scores of the two EID factors. This adaptation and preliminary validation of the EID scale have relevant implications for studies in the Italian context as well as on specific population groups such as pet owners, and more generally, for international studies on EID.

Introduction

Environmental identity and its relationship with similar constructs

As a facet of self-identity, environmental identity (EID) is considered an aspect of one’s identity describing individuals’ identification with the physical and natural world, and it is thus viewed as a sub-identity similar to other sub-levels, such as personal identity, social identity, and place identity (Proshansky, 1978; Proshansky and Fabian, 1987; Twigger-Ross et al., 2003). More specifically, EID is defined as “a sense of connection to some part of the non-human natural environment (…) that affects the way in which we perceive and act toward the world; a belief that the environment is important to us and an important part of who we are” (Clayton, 2003 p. 45–46).

Similar to an identity, EID is also defined both as a product and a force (Rosenberg, 1981; Clayton, 2003). It is considered a product because it is the result of personal history and the emotional connection the individual has developed toward the environment. As for the role of EID as a force, Clayton’s and several consequent studies highlight how EID is associated with environmental concern (Clayton and Kilinç, 2013; Clayton et al., 2019) and pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors (Clayton, 2003; Tam, 2013; Dresner et al., 2015; Scopelliti et al., 2018; Clayton et al., 2021). Identifying with nature also implies the perception of similarities between the self and nature, and a sense of connection or emotional attachment to it (Clayton and Myers, 2009; Schultz, 2001). This connection is possibly explained by the biophilia hypothesis, which asserts that the human dependence on nature “extends far beyond the simple issues of material and physical sustenance to encompass as well the human craving for aesthetic, intellectual, cognitive, and even spiritual meaning and satisfaction” (Kellert, 1993, p. 21), and therefore, the self-knowledge the environment provides, satisfies basic psychological needs (Clayton, 2003). Identification with nature is also relevant to the pets as ambassadors hypothesis, initially proposed by Serpell and Paul (1994), which states that owning pets during childhood may lead to positive attitudes toward animals in general, later in life, and to related behaviors, such as becoming members of animal welfare charities, and sometimes, by spill-over effects, even environmental and conservation organizations (Serpell and Paul, 1994; Miura et al., 2002; Auger and Amiot, 2017; Possidónio et al., 2021). Concretely, our connection with nature is in part built on our interactions with animals and the natural world (Clayton, 2003), and animals, in this view, can be considered as a bridge to the consideration and care of the natural world. Contact with animals, both domestic and wild, can promote contact with nature through everyday activities, for example, walking one’s dog or observing wild animals in green areas, urban and/or rural areas (Podberscek et al., 2000).

Different studies have investigated the links between the relationship individuals have with animals and the one they have with nature. For example, Nisbet et al. (2009) reported that feeling to be close to nature is associated with a greater love of animals, and Gosling and Williams (2010) confirmed that assigning greater value to animals is associated with the feelings of an emotional connection with nature. These results are coherent with animals’ conceptualizations as an inherent part of nature (Kellert, 1993; Schultz, 2000; Clayton, 2003).

Auger and Amiot (2017), following and extending the pets as ambassadors hypothesis, proposed that the same mechanisms through which pets act as ambassadors for animals should also apply between animals and nature. It should be noted that the relationship between various theoretically distinct constructs representing EID and related concepts, such as connectedness to nature, the inclusion of nature in the self, and environmental self-identity, is unclear. Recently, Balundé et al. (2019), in a meta-analysis confirmed the strong links (i.e., a great amount of shared variance) between concepts such as identification with the natural environment, (Clayton, 2003) and the emotional aspect of connection with nature (Schultz, 2001). According to the classical test theory (Cronbach and Meehl, 1955; Campbell and Fiske, 1959), this high shared variance could potentially point to these measures being indistinguishable in some cases. In the next paragraph, the issue of the dimensionality of existing scales will be addressed.

The environmental identity scale

Since theoretical complexity has often emerged in the history of this scale and its structural properties, the following paragraphs will briefly present the psychometric issues that guided the present work.

The EID Scale was developed to measure individual differences in a stable sense of interdependence and connectedness with nature, considering both the cognitive and the emotional facets of people’s relationship with the natural world (Clayton, 2003). The first EID Scale was composed of 24 items with a seven-point Likert scale as an answer format and measured five different aspects of EID: (1) the salience of identity, indicating the degree and significance of an individual’s relationship with nature; (2) the feeling of membership referring to the mode in which nature has a connection to the community with which one identifies oneself; (3) the acceptance of an ideology shared by the group reflected by the endorsement of environmental education and a sustainable lifestyle; (4) positive emotions related to the group identified by the pleasure felt in nature through fulfillment and esthetic enjoyment; (5) an autobiographical component comprising memories of interactions with nature. Although EID is theoretically multidimensional, Clayton (2003) stated that “factor analyses suggest that a single factor accounts for most of the variance” (p. 53), which is why the EID Scale has always been proposed as one-dimensional. However, some studies in the past have proposed different factorial solutions, e.g., Olivos and Aragonés (2011) found four factors plus a one-item factor. Occasionally, a complex structure emerged also out of the borders of Spain, for example, Fritsche and Häfner (2012), employing a set of 12 items issued from the 2003’s EID Scale, found two factors among German students, one interpreted as “Reflecting Contact with Nature” and the other as “Reflecting Self-definition.”

A second, shorter scale was later proposed, which is composed of 11 items (Clayton, 2012; Prévot et al., 2018), for example, “In general, being part of the natural world is an important part of my self-image” and “When I am upset or stressed, I can feel better by spending some time outdoors “communing with nature”. This scale has been used in a study conducted by Scopelliti et al. (2022), which is aimed at gaining a better understanding of the psychosocial determinants of young adults’ pro-environmental behaviors (PEBs), adding the contribution of affect and identity processes to well-established cognitive factors, and the role of intergenerational transmission of ecological values in these mechanisms. Concerning the dimensionality of the scales, most studies are consistent in finding a one-factor solution (e.g., Chew, 2019). Moreira et al. (2021) directly addressed this issue, testing different models through Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA), and bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling, finding that EID is an essentially one-dimensional construct.

Literature reports high internal reliability and validity of Clayton’s EID Scale, both in its original 24-item form and in its 11-item version (Clayton et al., 2021).

In 2021 Clayton proposed the Revised EID Scale, an update of the EID scale aiming to have items that are more inclusive of a broad variety of populations and nature experiences, especially to make the scale more applicable to urban and cross-cultural populations (Clayton et al., 2021). For the validation process, the reliability and the validity of the new scale was tested in seven diverse, international samples and reported to be satisfactory. The paper presents the Revised EID scale as composed by one factor, however, in a personal communication, Clayton reported that this solution was chosen for the sake of parsimony and that more factors emerged in preliminary analysis. “In each case the first factor was dominant, explaining from 32% (in the smallest sample, the Chicago students) to 58% (in the MTurk sample) of the total variance. In each sample, all items loaded positively on this first factor. The composition of subsequent factors varied between samples. A factor analysis combining all samples (N = 1650) had similar results, with a single factor that explained 48% of variance. A second factor had an eigenvalue just barely over 1.0 (1.05). We concluded that it is most parsimonious to interpret the scale in terms of a single factor” (Clayton, 2022, Personal Communication, March 2).

An adaptation among Italian pet owners

Even though environmental psychology studies are abundant in Italy, there does not seem to be any available Italian adaptation of the EID Scale, while some translations already exist (see Scopelliti et al., 2022 for the 11-item version). In the framework of an international study on pet owners, involving an Italian sample, it was thus decided to work on an Italian translation and adaptation of the Revised EID Scale.

Even if according to the “pet as ambassador” hypothesis, individuals that owned pets during their childhood are known to have a greater concern for non-pet animals than adults (Paul and Serpell, 1993), studies about EID among pet owners are scarce. More generally, existing evidence shows how early childhood and upbringing experiences can be capital for people’s EID, as well as pro-environmental attitudes (Green et al., 2016; Prévot et al., 2018; Molinario et al., 2020). In this sense, adult pet owners are likely to have a high level of EID and are thus a relevant public to address for studying the scale’s statistical properties.

This report thus intends to present the adaptation of the Revised EID Scale in an Italian sample of pet owners and its preliminary statistical properties. The scale’s structure is going to be explored and compared with the structure proposed for other versions of the same scale.

According to the previous published factorial solutions of the EID, two hypotheses are formulated:

H1: The reliability of the EID Scale is more than 0.70.

H2: The one-factor solution of the EID Scale outperforms the multi-factorial solution.

Materials and methods

ITC guidelines were followed for developing the adaptation of the scale (Hernández et al., 2020). Most criteria were satisfied with acceptable or excellent criteria, some criteria were not applicable since they refer to psychological tests and some are still to be implemented by future studies (see Section “Discussion”).

More specifically, as a precondition for the adaptation, as explained in the introduction, we evaluated the feasibility and the opportunity of the translation, and we informed the author of the scale (i.e., Susan Clayton) by email about the adaptation project.

As for the development phase, first of all, the study was approved by the faculty’s ethical committee (prot. N. 1214/2021). The adaptation team included three Italian experts in social psychology with professional knowledge of English. Of the three, one was solely involved in the translation process and provided the English-to-Italian translation. The second, who is an expert in environmental psychology, conducted the back translation from Italian to English. The third, which has specific expertise in test construction and validation, was involved with the other two to solve final translation discrepancies and doubts. The team of experts has reviewed, approved, and documented the adequacy and comparability of the instructions, and item content. As in its validated version, the Revised EID Scale was presented as a seven-point Likert scale. A preliminary online pilot study with 88 participants showed that individuals from the target population (i.e., Italian pet owners) do not have difficulties using the proposed procedures.

As for the confirmation step, the scale was included in an online questionnaire on pet attachment. To specifically target pet owners, the questionnaire, hosted on Qualtrics, was distributed through Prolific only to Italian residents owning at least one pet. A question asked participants how many pets they owned (in the present or the past) and how many different types of pets they owned (cats, dogs, fishes, reptiles, small mammals, and birds). Each participant was rewarded with 1.25 £. The sample size was sufficient to carry out exploratory factor analyses. Cronbach’s alpha was employed to test for construct equivalence.

Even though this is not a psychological test expected to have comparable scores and norms among different countries and populations, the present study reports sociodemographic characteristics and their relationships with the adapted scale, aiming to ease comparisons with the samples on which this scale has already been validated.

Participants

Participants were 163 Italian residents of young age (M = 26.61 years, SD = 7.05). Male participants were slightly over-represented (90, 55.22%) in comparison to female participants (69, 42.33%), and participants identifying themselves as the third gender were 2.45%. The median completion time of the whole questionnaire was 8.32 min, and participants employing less than 5 min or more than 25 min to complete the whole questionnaire were excluded from the analysis. The sample has been collected within a project on pet owners, so all the participants own one or more pets. Participants reported owning on average nine pets (SD = 7.08) of three different types (SD = 1.21), such as cats, dogs, and fish.

The sample was very highly educated (47.24% have at least a bachelor’s degree). Six participants report their mother language to be different from Italian. Since answers of non-native Italian speakers were consistent with the other ones for completion time and quality, it was chosen to keep them, in order to account for the wide community of Italian residents whose native language is not Italian.

Analyses

Analyses were run on RStudio software (version 1.2.1578).

To establish whether the dataset was suitable for exploratory factor analysis (EFA), the sample adequacy, and strength of the intercorrelation of items were examined. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) measure was used to test sample adequacy. KMO values between 0.7 and 1 indicate adequate sampling. Bartlett’s test of sphericity was used to test the hypothesis that the correlation matrix is an identity matrix, which would indicate that variables are unrelated and therefore unsuitable for structure detection. A parallel analysis and then an EFA (with the minimum residual method and Oblimin rotation) were run to identify the number of factors suggested by the parallel analysis (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2013). A second EFA, set to extract only one factor, was run to test the mono-factorial solution since this was the structure most commonly proposed for this scale. At last, Cronbach’s alpha was used to examine reliability. Cronbach’s alpha higher than 0.70 indicates that the dataset was reliable and acceptable (Nunnally, 1978).

Finally, the present study reports sociodemographic characteristics and their relationship with the adapted scale, aiming to ease comparisons with other samples on which this scale has already been validated. This type of information will enable future systematic reviews and meta-analyses dealing with this scale to compare samples’ characteristics (Shamseer et al., 2015).

Results

Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin was found to be optimal (KMO = 0.93), and Bartlett’s index [K2(13) = 56.216, p < 0.001] was satisfactory. As shown in Figure 1, the parallel analysis (5,000 iterations) suggests the existence of two factors.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scree plot of the parallel analysis.

As shown in Table 1, the two-factor solution consists of a factor labeled “Connectedness with nature” (nine items) and a factor labeled “Protection of nature” (five items), after examining their content. The two factors are correlated r = 0.58 and together explain 52% of the total variance. The one-factor solution, on the other hand, explains 46% of the total variance.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Two-factor and one-factor exploratory factor analysis (EFA) solutions for the Italian revised environmental identity (EID) scale.

It should be noted that, in the two-factor solution, item 8 has a low loading (only slightly higher than the 0.32 threshold) (Hair et al., 2006; Tabachnick and Fidell, 2013) on both factors. However, if, consistently with its content, it is included in the “Connectedness with nature” factor, it does not seem to negatively affect its Cronbach’s alpha. Moreover, removing it does not change the overall factors’ structure.

Cronbach’s alpha has satisfactory values both for the one-factor and the two-factor solutions, confirming H1.

The only way to compare EFA solutions is by comparing explained variance percentages. Since the two-factor solution explains a higher percentage of variance, it seems like a better solution. The two-factor solution was also the one suggested by the parallel analysis as well as by eigenvalues (the first two factors were the only ones with an eigenvalue above 1, i.e., 6.87 and 1.40). Thus, H2 was disconfirmed.

Here, the descriptive characteristics of the two-factor solution and the correlations between the factors and the sociodemographic variables are presented.

As shown in Table 2, the distribution of both factors was moderately skewed with participants giving in general high-value answers.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics of the two factors of the scale.

As shown in Table 3, no differences between age and education were found. Descriptive results were also independent of the number and variety of pets owned. Student’s t-tests showed no differences between men and women in both factors.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlations between the two environmental identity (EID) scale factors, age, education, number of pet types, and number of pets (Spearman’s ρ).

Discussion

The adaptation procedure allowed the production of a reliable and acceptable version of the Revised EID Scale. Moreover, the EFA provided an adequate, albeit preliminary factor solution. The solution has good internal reliability (confirming H1) but is different from the only study currently presenting factor analyses about this scale (i.e., Clayton et al., 2021). Here, the scale’s structure is better described by a two-factor solution, disconfirming H2. Since the original validation study presents only a CFA and no EFA, it is impossible to know if a bi-factor solution would have been appropriate in that case.

However, the two factors proposed in the present study are consistent with other solutions that emerged for the previous versions of the EID Scale, especially with the four-factor solution proposed by Olivos and Aragonés (2011; see also Larionov, 2020). Even though that solution had more factors than the present one, it can be noted how the “Connectedness to nature” factor emerging here is close to the factors “Enjoying Nature” and “Appreciation of Nature,” while the “Protection of nature” factor is linked to the “Environmentalism” factor, and also, partially to the “Environmental Identity” factor emerged in Spain.

The skewed distribution of both factors is possibly determined by the present sample consisting of pet owners since they are known to have stronger environmental concerns than the average (Auger and Amiot, 2017). At the same time, Clayton already reported that in the original samples “all item means were above the neutral midpoint” (Clayton et al., 2021 p. 8), so the issue might not only be related to the specificities of this sample.

Similar to the cross-cultural validation (2021), no relevant associations between gender, age, and/or education, and EID emerged in this study on Italian pet owners, suggesting that EID might be independent of the main sociodemographic variables also in this specific sample. Moreover, this study shows that the number and the variety of pets do not seem to affect the EID scores. At first sight, this result seems to be inconsistent with the “pet as ambassador” hypothesis (Serpell and Paul, 1994); however, it should be considered that all the participants in this study are pet owners. In this sense, it is possible that including non-pet owners would show that owning even just one pet is associated with a higher EID. Thus, it might not be a matter of how many pets one has but simply being or not being a pet owner might make a difference. This hints that future studies should consider populations with different characteristics (e.g., lack of pets, specific marital status, and presence of children), as well as representative samples of the general population. It should be noted that the ITC guidelines suggest following some extra steps that are not presented in this preliminary report (Hernández et al., 2020). Other validation tests should be run, especially for what concerns discriminant validity, convergent validity, and test–retest validity. For better comparisons with other adaptations of this scale, construct equivalence, method equivalence, and measurement equivalence (item functioning) should be tested more thoroughly.

More generally, results should be considered with caution, and the structure should be tested again on a bigger and more representative sample of the Italian population. Moreover, future studies should test this structure with more and more complex analyses such as CFA and exploratory structural equation modeling that were not run here because of the relatively small sample size.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Department of Psychology of Developmental and Socialization Processes, Sapienza University of Rome–prot. N. 1214/2021. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

SA was responsible for conception, design of the study, and the organization of the database. SA and OM performed the statistical analysis and wrote the manuscript together. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The publication of this article was funded by the Department of Education, Psychology, Philosophy, University of Cagliari (Italy).

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Roberta Benzan for her collaboration in the translation and adaptation of the scale to Italian and Prof. Susan Clayton for her support and her communication about statistical results in previous analyses.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Auger, B., and Amiot, C. (2017). Testing and extending the pets as ambassadors hypothesis: The role of contact with pets and recategorization processes in predicting positive attitudes toward animals. Hum. Anim. Interact. Bull. 5, 1–25.

Google Scholar

Balundé, A., Perlaviciute, G., and Steg, L. (2019). The relationship between people’s environmental considerations and pro-environmental behavior in Lithuania. Front. Psychol. 10:2319. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, D. T., and Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychol. Bull. 56:81. doi: 10.1037/h0046016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chew, P. K. H. (2019). Psychometric evaluation of two instruments to assess connection to nature/Evaluación psicométrica de dos instrumentos que miden la conexión con la naturaleza. Psyecology 10, 313–343. doi: 10.1080/21711976.2019.1607470

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clayton, S. (2003). “Environmental identity: A conceptual and an operational definition,” in Identity and the natural environment: The psychological significance of nature, eds S. Clayton and S. Opotow (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press), 45–66.

Google Scholar

Clayton, S., Czellar, S., Nartova-Bochaver, S., Skibins, J., Salazar, G., Tseng, Y.-C., et al. (2021). Cross-cultural validation of a revised Environmental Identity Scale. Sustainability 13:2387. doi: 10.3390/su13042387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clayton, S., Irkhin, B. D., and Nartova-Bochaver, S. K. (2019). Environmental identity in Russia: Validation and relationship to the concern for people and plants. Psikhologiya 16, 85–107. doi: 10.17323/1813-8918-2019-1-85-107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clayton, S., and Kilinç, A. (2013). Proenvironmental concern and behavior in Turkey: The role of national and environmental identity. Psyecology 4, 311–330. doi: 10.1174/217119713807749850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clayton, S. D. (2012). “Environment and identity,” in The Oxford handbook of environmental and conservation psychology, ed. S. D. Clayton (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 164–180.

Google Scholar

Clayton, S., and Myers, G. (2009). Conservation psychology: Understanding and promoting human care for nature. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.

Google Scholar

Cronbach, L. J., and Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychol. Bull. 52:281. doi: 10.1037/h0040957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dresner, M., Handelman, C., Braun, S., and Rollwagen-Bollens, G. (2015). Environmental identity, pro-environmental behaviors, and civic engagement of volunteer stewards in Portland area parks. Environ. Educ. Res. 21, 991–1010. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2014.964188

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritsche, I., and Häfner, K. (2012). The malicious effects of existential threat on motivation to protect the natural environment and the role of environmental identity as a moderator. Environ. Behav. 44, 570–590. doi: 10.1177/0013916510397759

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gosling, E., and Williams, K. J. (2010). Connectedness to nature, place attachment and conservation behaviour: Testing connectedness theory among farmers. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 298–304. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, C., Kalvaitis, D., and Worster, A. (2016). Recontextualizing psychosocial development in young children: A model of environmental identity development. Environ. Educ. Res. 22, 1025–1048. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2015.1072136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J. F. J., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., Anderson, R. E., and Tatham, R. L. (2006). Multivariate Data Analysis, 6th Edn. Hoboken, NJ: Pearson Education, Inc.

Google Scholar

Hernández, A., Hidalgo, M. D., Hambleton, R. K., and Gómez-Benito, J. (2020). International Test Commission guidelines for test adaptation: A criterion checklist. Psicothema 32, 390–398. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2019.306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kellert, S. R. (1993). “Introduction,” in The Biophilia hypothesis, eds S. R. Kellert and E. O. Wilson (Washington, DC: Island Press), 20–27.

Google Scholar

Larionov, P. (2020). W stronê psychologii środowiskowej: Właściwości psychometryczne polskiej wersji Skali Tożsamości Środowiskowej [Toward an Environmental Psychology: Psychometric properties of the Polish version of the Environmental Identity Scale]. Kwartalnik Naukowy Fides Ratio 44, 190–208. doi: 10.34766/fetr.v44i4.452

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miura, A., Bradshaw, J. W. S., and Tanida, H. (2002). Childhood experiences and attitudes towards animal issues: A comparison of young adults in Japan and the UK. Anim. Welfare 11, 437–448.

Google Scholar

Molinario, E., Kruglanski, A. W., Bonaiuto, F., Bonnes, M., Cicero, L., Fornara, F., et al. (2020). Motivations to act for the protection of nature biodiversity and the environment: A matter of “Significance”. Environ. Behav. 52, 1133–1163. doi: 10.1177/0013916518824376

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreira, P., Loureiro, A., Inman, R., and Olivos-Jara, P. (2021). Assessing the one-dimensionality of Clayton’s Environmental Identity Scale using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling (bifactor-ESEM). Collabra Psychol. 7:28103. doi: 10.1525/collabra.28103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbet, E. K., Zelenski, J. M., and Murphy, S. A. (2009). The nature relatedness scale: Linking individuals’ connection with nature to environmental concern and behavior. Environ. Behav. 41, 715–740. doi: 10.1177/0013916508318748

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Olivos, P., and Aragonés, J. I. (2011). Psychometric properties of the Environmental Identity Scale (EID). Psyecology 2, 65–74. doi: 10.1174/217119711794394653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paul, E. S., and Serpell, J. A. (1993). Childhood Pet keeping and Humane Attitudes in Young Adulthood. Anim. Welfare 2, 321–337.

Google Scholar

Podberscek, A. L., Paul, E. S., and Serpell, J. A. (Eds) (2000). Companion animals and us: Exploring the relationships between people and pets. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Possidónio, C., Piazza, J., Graça, J., and Prada, M. (2021). From pets to pests: Testing the scope of the “pets as ambassadors” hypothesis. Anthrozoos 34, 707–722. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2021.1926708

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prévot, A.-C., Clayton, S., and Mathevet, R. (2018). The relationship of childhood upbringing and university degree program to environmental identity: Experience in nature matters. Environ. Educ. Res. 24, 263–279. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2016.1249456

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proshansky, H. M. (1978). The city and self-identity. Environ. Behav. 10, 147–169. doi: 10.1177/0013916578102002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proshansky, H. M., and Fabian, A. K. (1987). “The development of place identity in the child,” in Spaces for children, eds C. S. Weinstein and T. G. David (Boston, MA: Springer), 21–40. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4684-5227-3_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, M. (1981). “The self-concept: Social product and social force,” in Social psychology: Sociological perspectives, eds M. Rosenberg and R. H. Turner (New York, NY: Basic Books), 593–624. doi: 10.4324/9781315129723-19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, P. W. (2000). New environmental theories: Empathizing with nature: The effects of perspective taking on concern for environmental issues. J. Soc. Issues 56, 391–406.

Google Scholar

Schultz, P. W. (2001). The structure of environmental concern: Concern for self, other people, and the biosphere. J. Environ. Psychol. 21, 327–339.

Google Scholar

Scopelliti, M., Barni, D., and Rinallo, E. (2022). My Parents Taught…Green Was My Growth! The Role of Intergenerational Transmission of Ecological Values in Young Adults’ Pro-Environmental Behaviors and Their Psychosocial Mechanisms. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:1670. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19031670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scopelliti, M., Molinario, E., Bonaiuto, F., Bonnes, M., Cicero, L., De Dominicis, S., et al. (2018). What makes you a ‘hero’ for nature? Socio-psychological profiling of leaders committed to nature and biodiversity protection across seven EU countries. J. Environ. Plan. Manag. 61, 970–993. doi: 10.1080/09640568.2017.1421526

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serpell, J., and Paul, E. (1994). Pets and the development of positive attitudes to animals. Animals and human society: Changing perspectives. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Shamseer, L., Moher, D., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., Petticrew, M., et al. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015: Elaboration and explanation. BMJ 349:g7647. doi: 10.1136/bmj.g7647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabachnick, B., and Fidell, L. (2013). Using multivariate statistics, 6th Edn. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Google Scholar

Tam, K. P. (2013). Concepts and measures related to connection to nature: Similarities and differences. J. Environ. Psychol. 34, 64–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Twigger-Ross, C. T., Bonaiuto, M., and Breakwell, G. M. (2003). “Identity theories and environmental psychology,” in Psychological theories for environmental issues, eds M. Bonnes and T. R. Lee (Farnham: Ashgate), 203–234.

Google Scholar

Keywords: environmental identity, Italy, translation, adaptation, pet owners, EFA, Revised EID, scale

Citation: Ariccio S and Mosca O (2023) Revised environmental identity scale: Adaptation and preliminary examination on a sample of Italian pet owners. Front. Psychol. 13:892841. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.892841

Received: 09 March 2022; Accepted: 12 December 2022;
Published: 30 January 2023.

Edited by:

Elisa Delvecchio, University of Perugia, Italy

Reviewed by:

Sofya K. Nartova-Bochaver, HSE University, Russia
Luis Carlos Jaume, University of Buenos Aires, Argentina

Copyright © 2023 Ariccio and Mosca. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: S. Ariccio, www.frontiersin.org silvia.ariccio@uniroma1.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.