Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Psychol., 08 February 2022
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Obsessive-Compulsive Related Disorders: Towards an Advancement of the Knowledge of These Internalizing Disorders View all 7 articles

Editorial: Obsessive-Compulsive Related Disorders: Towards an Advancement of the Knowledge of These Internalizing Disorders

  • 1Department of Health Sciences, School of Psychology, University of Florence, Florence, Italy
  • 2Center for the Study of Stress, Trauma, and Resilience, Department of Counseling and Psychological Services, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, United States

This Research Topic arose from the editors' interest in Obsessive-Compulsive related Disorders and a new potential clinical disorder associated with problematic overstudying, which has been conceptualized as a behavioral addiction (Atroszko et al., 2015) or an OCD-related disorder (Loscalzo and Giannini, 2017, 2018). Loscalzo and Giannini (2017), in line with Kardefelt-Winther's (2015) recommendations, considered alternative criteria to addiction to identify the authentic manifestation of problematic overstudying. This seemed critical considering the increasing tendency to introduce new behavioral addictions. The year when the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) was published saw a peak of 2,563 papers about behavioral addictions (Billieux et al., 2015). The DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) included Gambling Disorder in the addictive behaviors section and Internet Gaming Disorder in the section related to Emerging Measure and Models. This may have legitimized the study of new behavioral addictions. However, we reached a point where almost every common behavior has been labeled as a potential behavioral addiction, such as tanning (Kourosh et al., 2010) and fortune-telling (Grall-Bronnec et al., 2015). Therefore, Reinarman and Granfield (2014) suggested that scholars have become “addicted to addiction” (p. 1). More importantly, as denounced by Billieux et al. (2015), creating these diagnoses generally follows a three-step approach that is atheoretical and confirmatory. First, following an anecdotal observation, researchers consider the excessive behavior as an addictive behavior (a priori approach). Next, they develop a screening instrument referring to the core symptoms of substance addictions (confirmatory approach). Finally, they carry on studies to unearth those risk factors that are usually associated with substance addictions and that, by analogy, they suppose characterize the new behavioral addiction (confirmatory approach). However, there are some critical problems and issues with these steps. First, an anecdotal observation should not be sufficient for proposing a new behavioral addiction. Second, Billieux et al. (2015) observed that developing items based on the criteria of addictive disorders is not simple for all the addictions components, especially as concerns tolerance, which is one of the key addiction features (Shaffer et al., 2004). Moreover, there are several instruments proposed to evaluate these new excessive behaviors. Besides being based on the core components of addiction, these instruments, in some cases, come dangerously close to duplicating previous instruments (Griffiths et al., 2016). Finally, Billieux et al. (2015) pointed out that, by using instruments based on the addiction framework, it is obvious that the researchers will find the expected relationships with the risk factors typical of addictions. Moreover, they underscored that the addiction model leads to a lack of specificity and theoretically sound models that are able to specify the particular factors involved in each problematic and excessive behavior.

In light of these issues, Starcevic (2016) suggested taking a step back to recall that a few years ago, there was a similar excess regarding obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders; it seems prudent to pay attention to history and avoid repeating the same mistakes. He believes that disorders characterized by repetitive and problematic behaviors and poor impulse control might be conceptualized using the addiction framework, but there is no evidence that other theoretical approaches are inferior. Hence, he stressed that we still have to understand whether specific behaviors are better conceptualized as impulse control disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, addiction, or other. Surprisingly, the situation has not much improved, despite Starcevic's (2016) cautions regarding a prior excess of diagnoses concerning OCD-related disorders. Separate literature searches (using the OneSearch database) conducted at the beginning of January 2022, restricted to peer-reviewed papers, and using the keywords “behavioral addiction” and “Obsessive Compulsive related Disorder” yielded, respectively, 151,070 and 60,536 papers. The number of publications about behavioral addictions was more than double the number of OCD-related papers. Hence, the present Research Topic aimed to prompt scholars contributing to the advancement of knowledge about OCD-related disorders, pointing to balance the wide attention currently given to the addiction/externalizing framework, and hence trying to retrieve the main essence of scientific research: looking for the real nature of phenomena while avoiding aprioristic and confirmatory approaches.

Besides Loscalzo and Giannini's paper about Studyholism (or obsession toward study), which provided some evidence about its conceptualization as an OCD-related disorder (or, more generally, as an internalizing disorder), there are other interesting studies about hoarding, trichotillomania, and skin picking disorder. He et al. investigated the relationship between hoarding and compulsive buying, highlighting a positive correlation between the two conditions. Grant et al., analyzing both trichotillomania and skin picking, showed a lack of relationship between past-year fat/sugar dietary intake and these disorders' severity, while they found an association between nutrition and impulsivity and compulsivity (as transdiagnostic factors). Next, Kłosowska et al. showed that traumatic life events (e.g., emotional neglect, sexual abuse, and bodily threat) might contribute to the development of skin picking as a regulation strategy and that dissociative symptoms partially explained the relationship, for both the automatic/unconscious and focused skin picking style. In line with these findings, Gallinat et al. highlighted that most subjects experience a loss of control, trance, and positive feelings during pathological skin-picking episodes. However, these episodes are often preceded by boredom, bodily tension, and strong negative feelings, and they are usually followed by shame, guilt, and anger. Among the main findings, they also showed that skin picking severity is positively associated with body image disturbances and low self-esteem. Finally, Lochner et al. suggested that factors other than emotion dysregulation should be analyzed as potential contributors to trichotillomania, based on their findings that patients have higher rates of childhood trauma, perceived stress, and emotion dysregulation than healthy controls. However, they found no association between emotion dysregulation and the severity of hair-pulling. Moreover, the association between perceived stress and emotion dysregulation was not specific to trichotillomania; it was also evident in the healthy group.

In conclusion, the interesting findings included in the current Research Topic provide ample justification for further, and similarly creative, attention on OCD-related disorders. Moreover, analyzing excessive behaviors using an OCD-related perspective—and not only an addiction one—might help reveal the real nature of understudied (or new potential) clinical diagnoses.

Author Contributions

YL wrote the draft of the manuscript. MG and KGR critically revised its content. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). DSM-5. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atroszko, P. A., Andreassen, C. L., Griffiths, M. D., and Pallesen, S. (2015). Study addiction – a new area of psychological study: conceptualization, assessment, and preliminary empirical findings. J. Behav. Addict. 4, 75–84. doi: 10.1556/2006.4.2015.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Billieux, J., Schimmenti, A., Khazaal, Y., Maurage, P., and Heernen, A. (2015). Are we overpathologizing everyday life? A tenable blueprint for behavioral addiction research. J. Behav. Addict. 4, 119–123. doi: 10.1556/2006.4.2015.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grall-Bronnec, M., Bulteau, S., Victorri-Vigneau, C., Bouju, G., and Sauvaget, A. (2015). Fortune telling addiction: unfortunately a serious topic. About a case report. J. Behav. Addict. 4, 27–31. doi: 10.1556/JBA.4.2015.1.7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, M. D., Andreassen, C. S., Pallesen, S., Bilder, R. M., Torsheim, T., and Aboujaoude, E. (2016). When is a new scale not a new scale? The case of the Bergen Shopping Addiction Scale and the Compulsive Online Shopping Scale. Int. J. Mental Health Addict. 14, 1107–1110. doi: 10.1007/s11469-016-9711-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2015). Commentary on: are we overpathologizing everyday life? A tenable blueprint for behavioral addiction research. Problems with atheoretical and confirmatory research approaches in the study of behavioral addictions. J. Behav. Addict. 4, 126–129. doi: 10.1556/2006.4.2015.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kourosh, A. S., Harrington, C. R., and Adinoff, B. (2010). Tanning as a behavioral addiction. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse 36, 284–290. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2010.491883

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loscalzo, Y., and Giannini, M. (2017). “Studyholism or study addiction? A comprehensive model for a new clinical condition,” in Advances in Psychology Research, ed A. M. Columbus (New York, NY: Nova Publisher), 19–37.

Google Scholar

Loscalzo, Y., and Giannini, M. (2018). Problematic overstudying: studyholism or study addiction? Commentary on: ten myths about work addiction. J. Behav. Addict. 7, 867–870. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinarman, C., and Granfield, R. (2014). “Addiction is not just a brain disease: critical studies of addiction,” in Expanding Addictions. Critical Essays, eds R. Granfield and C. Reinarman (New York, NY: Routledge), 22.

Google Scholar

Shaffer, H. J., LaPlante, D. A., LaBrie, R. A., Kidman, R. C., Donato, A. N., and Stanton, M. V. (2004). Toward a syndrome model of addiction: multiple expressions, common etiology. Harv. Rev. Psychiatr. 12, 367–374. doi: 10.1080/10673220490905705

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Starcevic, V. (2016). Behavioural addictions: a challenge for psychopathology and psychiatric nosology. Austr. N. Zeal. J. Psychiatr. 50, 721–725. doi: 10.1177/0004867416654009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: OCD, Studyholism, body dysmorphic disorder, excoriation, trichotillomania, hoarding disorder, study addiction, behavioral addiction

Citation: Loscalzo Y, Giannini M and Rice KG (2022) Editorial: Obsessive-Compulsive Related Disorders: Towards an Advancement of the Knowledge of These Internalizing Disorders. Front. Psychol. 13:849610. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.849610

Received: 06 January 2022; Accepted: 13 January 2022;
Published: 08 February 2022.

Edited and reviewed by:

Antoine Bechara, University of Southern California, United States

Copyright © 2022 Loscalzo, Giannini and Rice. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yura Loscalzo, eXVyYS5sb3NjYWx6byYjeDAwMDQwO2dtYWlsLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.