Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 08 September 2022
Sec. Decision Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Managerial Decision-Making From the Perspectives of Behavioral Science and Neuroscience View all 25 articles

Visual Attention to Novel Products – Cross-Cultural Insights From Physiological Data

\r\nIsabella RinklinIsabella Rinklin1Marco Hubert*Marco Hubert2*Monika KollerMonika Koller3Peter KenningPeter Kenning4
  • 1Department of Corporate Management and Economics, Zeppelin University, Friedrichshafen, Germany
  • 2Department of Management, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
  • 3Department of Marketing, Vienna University of Economics and Business, Vienna, Austria
  • 4Faculty of Business Administration and Economics, University of Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany

The study aims to investigate visual attention and perceived attractiveness to known versus unknown (novel) products above and beyond self-report applying physiological methods. A cross-cultural exploratory approach allows for comparing results gathered in the United States and China. We collected field data on physiological parameters accompanied by behavioral data. Mobile eye-tracking was employed to capture attention by measuring gaze parameters and electrodermal activity serves as indicator for arousal at an unconscious level. A traditional scale approach measuring perceived attractiveness of known versus novel products provide insights at a conscious level. US-American and Chinese consumers in our sample indeed process novel products differently. This can be observed at an unconscious and conscious level. Different gaze movements and arousal levels are observed at an unconscious level. Regarding behavioral data, the level of vertical orientation shapes the perceived attractiveness of novel products at a conscious level. The study showcases how physiological methods complement behavioral ones when investigating visual attention to products. It underlines varying conscious as well as unconscious visual attention and attractiveness ratings comparing known versus novel products, driven by cultural differences. Data from a field setting further enrich the implications derived for new product development and applied marketing.

Introduction

Launching novel products often cuts both ways, it either paves the path for further economic growth or it ends in loss and troubles. Hence, for companies engaging in new product development, it is vital to understand which factors contribute to a new product’s success. In a meta-analysis, Evanschitzky et al. (2012) report product-, strategy-, process-, market- place-, or organizational characteristics to have an effect on new product success. Being innovative positively affects specific economic performance values, like firm value (i.e., market to book ratio), market position (i.e., sales, market share), or financial positions (i.e., return on investment) (Rubera and Kirca, 2012). In turn, this effect can be mainly attributed to the success of novel products. However, products are only successful if they are bought by consumers. Hence, it is all about understanding the consumers’ perceptions and attractiveness ratings which serve as key to new product success.

A substantial percentage of newly developed products fail because consumers do not understand them or do not accept them (Feiereisen et al., 2008; Evanschitzky et al., 2012). Especially the decision-making process of consumers to buy novel products is often characterized by a high degree of complexity (Broniarczyk and Alba, 1994; Mukherjee and Hoyer, 2001). Visual attention plays a crucial role in this context. It serves as gatekeeper for further information processing (Hoyer et al., 2018). Proper processing of information is especially vital when it comes to the understanding and acceptance of novel products. Perceived risk and uncertainty further play a crucial role in this context (Mitchell and Boustani, 1993). In turn, cultural aspects shape the levels of perceived risk and associated levels of arousal (Gierlach et al., 2010). The increased speed at which companies develop novel products has led to a rise in studies on visual attention, associated perceived risk and arousal as well as information processing of consumers regarding novel products (Bettman et al., 1998; Mukherjee and Hoyer, 2001; Arts et al., 2011; Kardes and Wyer, 2013; Hubert et al., 2017). Moreover, in today’s globalized world and with regard to strategies in international marketing, the perception and production of new products needs to be discussed in a multicultural setting (Evanschitzky et al., 2012; Rubera and Kirca, 2012). Having a more comprehensive knowledge of how novel products are perceived by consumers in cross-cultural settings is crucial. Previous studies have already shown that cultural differences drive the perception of products and objects in general (e.g., Ishii et al., 2003; Strombach et al., 2014). As the processes of visual attention and perception are often related to complex unconscious automatic processes (Messner and Wänke, 2011) it is difficult to measure these concepts using self-report data only. Physiological as well as gaze parameters allow for a more comprehensive understanding in this regard (Shimojo et al., 2003). Prior intercultural studies (e.g., Bao et al., 2003; Shavitt et al., 2006) did also not account for socioeconomic changes such as Chinese increasingly focusing on Western behavior (Davis, 2013).

Against this background, the present exploratory study focuses on a cross-cultural comparison of visual attention and attractiveness ratings of already known versus novel products. In particular, the study investigates the visual information processes regarding cars that consumers already are familiar with as well as prototypes of cars that are new to them (novel products). With respect to unconscious processes, eye-tracking was applied to measure consumers’ visual attention and electrodermal activity (EDA) measures were included for investigating their arousal levels, which are according to literature related to psychological correlates such as positive/negative emotion, mental effort, task difficulty, risk perception/uncertainty, and anxiety (Holper et al., 2014). The field setting applied in the United States and China allows for a cross-cultural comparison and increases external validity (Kopton and Kenning, 2014; Gneezy, 2017). With regard to the conscious evaluation of the products under scrutiny, a traditional scale approach featuring measures for attractiveness (Brakus et al., 2014) was employed.

The paper contributes to the understanding of cross-cultural differences in visual information processing of products in general and novel products in particular. It, potentially, provides insights into product evaluation in terms of attractiveness ratings at both, conscious and unconscious levels. This approach enables to understand consumer behavior in a cross-cultural setting above and beyond mere survey data. Implications within the context of new product development can be derived on a more comprehensive level, as physiological data of gaze parameters as well as electrodermal activity provide insights into the unconscious drivers of stated product ratings. Adding this kind of data enables to draw a more holistic picture of what is really going on inside the consumers’ in terms of cognitive and affective information processing and therefore it might help to come up with ideas to enhance the customer experience and consumers’ value perceptions.

Theoretical Frame and Hypotheses

In the following, we provide the conceptual frame for the hypotheses tested along the relevant theoretical concepts investigated in the present study. The study provides insights mainly into the areas of visual attention and perceived attractiveness of novel products. Theoretical underpinnings of how uncertainty and culture relate to those key areas are outlined.

Visual Attention

For human beings vision has been seen as the most dominant sense (Hutmacher, 2019). When it comes to new product evaluation from a consumers’ perspective, visual attention therefore plays a vital role. For the understanding of visual attention and attentional deployment, different antecedents like (1) surrounding context, (2) salience mapping, (3) inhibition of return, (4) eye movement, or (5) scene understanding and object recognition are crucial (Itti and Koch, 2001). Regarding the design aspects in new product development, prior studies have stated that especially visual attention plays an important role in the way that consumers process novel product designs (Johnston et al., 1990; Folkes and Matta, 2004). From a physiological perspective, gaze parameters serve as an indicator for visual inspection (Shimojo et al., 2003). When it comes to the investigation of visual perception of novel products among consumers from different cultures, it is important to note that prior research has shown different gaze parameters comparing Asian and Western cultures. Consumers from Asian nations tend to screen stimuli for short durations, whereas people from Western nations tend to fix their vision on objects for longer durations (Chua et al., 2005; Goh et al., 2009; Peng-Li et al., 2020). Therefore, the following hypothesis is derived:

H1: Chinese consumers in our sample pay shorter visual attention to products than US-American consumers in our sample.

The Role of Arousal in Visual Attention and in Different Cultural Settings

The novelty of products can create uncertainty in decision-making and is related to perceived risk (Cox and Rich, 1964; Bauer, 1967; Taylor, 1974; Shoemaker and Shoaf, 1975). Prior findings showed that the facets of perceived risk are context-dependent (Campbell and Goodstein, 2001; Featherman and Pavlou, 2003). Perceived risk and uncertainty are accompanied by higher EDA levels and activation in the anterior cingulate cortex of the brain, indicating a higher level of arousal (Critchley, 2002). Psychophysiological arousal covary with risk-sensitive decision-making processes (Studer and Clark, 2011). Furthermore, with respect to the interaction of risk-aversion and culture, it has been shown that Asians tend to be more risk-averse than US-Americans (Bao et al., 2003; Sorrentino et al., 2013). Therefore, it is hypothesized that:

H2: While observing novel products, Chinese consumers in our sample show higher levels of arousal (indicating a potentially higher level of perceived risk) than US-American consumers in our sample.

Moreover, prior research has shown that risk-averse consumers tend to delay the adoption of new products (Aggarwal et al., 1998) and relate them to losses (Bao et al., 2003). This connotation of experiencing a loss might in turn have a negative impact on the consumers’ evaluation of the new product. Therefore, it is assumed that:

H3: Perceived attractiveness of the novel car is lower for Chinese consumers compared to US-American consumers in our sample.

The Role of Culture in Visual Attention

The theory of individualism versus collectivism has aimed at explaining cross-cultural differences in consumption (Singelis et al., 1995; Shavitt et al., 2006). Closely related to the theory of individualism/collectivism is the approach of vertical versus horizontal orientation. Vertical orientation emphasizes hierarchy versus horizontal orientation focuses more on equality (Triandis and Gelfand, 1998; Rahman and Luomala, 2020). Early consumer research stated that the Chinese were more vertically oriented and collectivistic than US-Americans (e.g., Bao et al., 2003). However, some studies demonstrate that Chinese society (more specifically, people with high income) are increasingly focusing more on individualistic factors than on collectivistic factors (Yan, 2010). Personal wellbeing is one such individualistic factor (Steele and Lynch, 2013). Recent studies also show acculturation to global consumer culture (Czarnecka et al., 2020) as well as a person’s social value orientation playing a role in this context (Moon et al., 2018). Besides those recent developments, it is assumed that power distance and hierarchy is generally still ranked higher in Asian cultures compared to the US (Li et al., 2014; Goa and Zhang, 2022). The following is hypothesized:

H4a: Chinese and US-American consumers in our sample differ in terms of individualism/collectivism.

H4b: Chinese and US-American consumers in our sample differ in terms of vertical versus horizontal orientation.

More specifically, vertically oriented consumers, such as the Chinese, have a high focus on hierarchy, power distance and structure. These consumers rely less on equality in power/status (higher “power distance”; Hofstede, 1984) and focus more on hierarchical structures. Furthermore, consumers from high power distance cultures show lower impulsive buying tendencies, higher self-control, and more deliberate processing (Zhang and Mittal, 2008; Zhang et al., 2012). The greater degree of deliberate processing and the higher self-control are aligned with higher risk-aversion. The subsequent hypothesis is based on these findings and on the conceptual underpinnings regarding the level of arousal:

H5: The vertical orientation of consumers has a significant effect on their attractiveness ratings of the novel car. This effect is moderated by the level of arousal.

Methodology

Experimental Setting

The present physiological field studies were conducted in the United States (Los Angeles) and China (Beijing). The 42 participants (US: N = 22, Mage = 46.50, SD = 11.97, 14 males; China: N = 20, Mage = 35.95, SD = 9.64, 13 males)1 were randomly recruited from a base of the targeted segment of potential premium car consumers (e.g., participants with high income, generally well-educated). Different cars were chosen from five well-known car brands in the premium class–subsequently referred to as brands C1, C2, C3, C4, and P. The car brands were neither US-American nor Chinese. Thus, “country-of-origin” effects (Michaelis et al., 2008) can be excluded. While, previous studies on novel product designs lack the use of real prototypes (e.g., Kreuzbauer and Malter, 2005), one of the cars with the presented study was a “real” prototype (P). The prototype showed substantial design changes (compared to the existing model) and had been developed exclusively by a well-known automobile company and was not yet available on the market. In consequence, the use of a real prototype offers the advantages of increasing the external validity of the experiment and of ensuring that the unknown car is not processed differently by participants as a result of being perceived as unrealistic or even artificial.

In both cultural groups (China and the United States), a study supervisor led participants through the entire experiment, and for the Chinese group the supervisor was a native speaker of Mandarin. The first sequence of the experiment involved the participants observing all cars from a distant position (capturing their very first impression after opening their eyes from an approximately 5 m distance) (see Figure 1), and the second sequence involved the participants observing each car separately, and from a close-up position (see Figure 2). Each participant was equipped with the mobile neurophysiological technology and ran through the experiment individually. Each participant was instructed by a single experimental supervisor.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. First phase of the experimental setup (own illustration).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Second phase of the experimental setup (own illustration).

Measurements

For capturing the physiological data a mobile eye-tracking tool (SensoMotoric Instruments, Teltow, Germany) and a mobile EDA tool (edaMove by movisens, with Ag/AgCl electrodes) was used. These tools measure, respectively, visual attention (stationary: Rosbergen et al., 1997; Brasel and Gips, 2008; mobile: Clement et al., 2013; Suher et al., 2014) and arousal (stationary: Kopton et al., 2013; mobile: Kuijpers et al., 2012). They were used during the phases of visual information processing and attractiveness ratings of actual novel cars, in an exhibition hall. For eye-tracking data, the dwell times (ms) were measured associated with visual information processing of each car. The participants looked from a fixed place from which the automobiles were positioned in a way so that the participants could observe the front-side perspective of each car (see Figures 1, 2). In contrast to classical computer lab experiments, the field approach featured different time-durations for each participant’s experimental run. Therefore, each EDA integral (μ) was normalized for each stimulus condition concerning time-duration and was furthermore log-transformed (Bach et al., 2010). As relative values (individual baselines) were used for each participant, it is possible to compare the responses of the Chinese and the Americans without any potential bias due to differences in body temperature or respiration levels.

To determine cultural differences, a traditional rating scales approach was applied. The Horizontal (H)/Vertical (V)–Individualism (I)/Collectivism (C) scale (e.g., Triandis and Gelfand, 1998) (with a nine-point Likert scale range from 1 = “strongly disagree” to 9 = “strongly agree”; HI: α = 0.81; HC: α = 0.89; VI: α = 0.72; and VC: α = 0.76) was used. Attractiveness ratings of the cars served as key behavioral dependent variables. Participants were asked to indicate their general liking for the cars’ exterior designs according to a five-point Likert scale (1 = “extremely attractive” and 5 = “not attractive at all”) similar to Brakus et al. (2014). In order to avoid any language induced bias, scales were translated to Mandarin and American English by a native speaker.

Results

Manipulation Check

Responses from all participants measuring attractiveness were generally high for all cars [Median (Mdn)_C1 = 2; Mdn_C2 = 2; Mdn_C3 = 2; Mdn_P = 1.5] with one exception (Mdn C4 = 3). Friedman’s ANOVA for comparison of all cars was significant, χ2(4) = 37.56, p < 0.001. However, Bonferroni-corrected pairwise follow-up tests showed significant differences only for brand C4 (P-C4: T = −1.91, adj. p < 0.001; C1-C4: T = −1.06, adj. p = 0.021; C2-C4: T = −1.16, adj. p = 0.008; C3-C4: T = −1.18, adj. p = 0.006) (see Table 1). The other comparisons were insignificant. Thus, C4 was excluded from analysis.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Manipulation check: Paired comparisons for Friedman’s ANOVA.

Results of Eye-Tracking Measures

Eye-tracking results from independent t-tests (for C2) and from Mann-Whitney tests for non-normally distributed variables (C1, C3, P)2 showed that during the first phase of visual processing of the cars, Chinese consumers significantly showed shorter dwell times for all known cars [car C1: Mdn_US = 1499.50, Mdn_China = 166.40, U = 85.00, z = −2.00, p = 0.024; car C2: M_US = 3685.97, SD = 2359.66; M_China = 1622.05, SD = 1538.38, T(32) = 2.93, p = 0.002; car C3: Mdn_US = 2728.10, Mdn_China = 1131.30; U = 88.00, z = −1.89, p = 0.003] (see Table 2), affirming H1. For the prototype (P), it was found only a marginally significant difference (Mdn_US = 4151.30; Mdn_China = 2446.50, U = 96.00, z = −1.61, p = 0.056).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Statistics of dwell time.

Results of Electrodermal Activity Measures

For data pre-processing prior to the main EDA analyses, each participant’s overall mean arousal level was computed for all cars (with the mean arousal level for each car’s front, sides, and rear considered).3 This allowed to capture the consumers’ complex overall impressions of each car and to account for differences between the front of the car and the other sides of it (see Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Statistics of arousal levels.

The independent t-tests (C1, P) and Mann–Whitney tests (C2, C3) indicated significant differences between American and Chinese participants [car C1: M_US = 0.24, M_China = 0.33, t(38) = −1.67, p = 0.052; car C2: Mdn_US = 0.15, Mdn_China = 0.29, U = 130.00, z = −1.88, p = 0.031; car C3: Mdn_US = 0.15, Mdn_China = 0.33, U = 118.00, z = −2.02, p = 0.022]. Car P showed the highest significant difference [M_US = 0.22, M_China = 0.36, t(38) = −2.60, p = 0.007]. Furthermore, only the arousal levels corresponding to the car fronts, each of which is described as the “car’s face” and which characterize the car’s design (Keaveney et al., 2012), are processed further. These results show no significant differences for the known cars. However, they show significant differences for the front of the prototype P (Mdn_US = 0.17, Mdn_China = 0.39, U = 137.00, z = −1.69, p = 0.047), affirming H2.

Regarding H3, focusing on the conscious level of visual information processing via asking for the attractiveness ratings, it was found that Chinese participants (Mdn_China = 2.00) indicated significantly lower ratings for the prototype than US-American participants (Mdn_US = 1.00, U = 156.00, z = −1.76, p = 0.039), affirming H3. For the three known cars, the differences between Americans and Chinese were not significant (car C1: Mdn_US = 2, Mdn_China = 2.50, U = 185.00, z = −0.92, p = 0.179; car C2: Mdn_US = 2, Mdn_China = 2, U = 193.00, z = −0.72, p = 0.24; car C3: Mdn_US = 2, Mdn_China = 2, U = 206.50, z = −0.36, p = 0.361).

Culture and Arousal as Moderators

Individual cultural differences were captured by using traditional self-report scales for the cultural differences of individualism (vertical/horizontal orientation), collectivism (vertical/horizontal orientation), horizontal orientation (individualism/collectivism), and vertical orientation (individualism/collectivism). There was no significant difference between Chinese and American participants with regard to “individualism” [M_US = 5.97, SD = 1.01, M_China = 6.07, SD = 1.37; t(40) = −0.28, p = 0.391] and “collectivism” [M_US = 6.97, SD = 1.11, M_China = 6.62, SD = 1.44; t(4) = 0.90, p = 0.186]. Therefore, H4a had to be rejected. However, results showed differences between the Chinese and American participants concerning horizontal orientation (Mdn_US = 7.56, Mdn_China = 6.94, U = 132.50, z = −2.21, p = 0.014) and vertical orientation [M_US = 5.36, SD = 1.31, M_China = 6.02, SD = 1.34; t(40) = −1.60, p = 0.059], affirming H4b (see Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Statistics of cultural differences: Combined factors.

Finally, to test H5, stating that the individual vertical orientation has an effect on the attractiveness rating of the novel car, a factorial ANOVA (bootstrapping, within-subject design) with attractiveness of P being the dependent variable (z-transformed) was computed. Moreover, we tested a potential moderating effect of arousal. The independent variables of vertical orientation (mean of factor sum) and arousal relating to prototype P were binary coded by a median split [Mdn (vertical orientation) = 5.78; Mdn (overall arousal level) = 0.28]. The results demonstrate a marginally significant main effect of consumers’ vertical orientation on the attractiveness ratings for the novel car (prototype P) [F(1,15) = 3.09, p = 0.099, η_p^2 = 0.171]. The arousal relating to car P showed no main effect. However, the interaction effect turned out to be highly significant [F(1,15) = 18.74, p = 0.001, η_p^2 = 0.55]. Follow-up tests with confidence intervals based on 1000 bootstrap samples indicated marginally significant differences between consumers with low versus high vertical orientation regarding their attractiveness ratings (M_diff = 0.65, 95% CI [−0.54, 1.22], p = 0.081). However, there is no significant difference regarding the arousal of the novel car (prototype P) (M_diff = 0.26, 95% CI [−1.13, 0.86], p = 0.251) (see Table 5).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Statistics of bootstrapped factorial ANOVA: Cultural differences and arousal level for novel product.

Conclusion, Limitations, and Further Research

Overall, the present small-scale study contributes to a range of areas in consumer research. First, it presents new exploratory findings in the area of cultural differences in consumers’ visual attention of novel products, by providing physiological data from a field study enhancing external validity of the contribution.

Second, the study demonstrates that Chinese and American consumers included in our sample differ in their way, how to visually engage with products. This finding is crucial for any new product development aiming to be launched across different cultures. The cultural differences in visual attention are specifically based on unconscious processes (such as differences in gaze parameters as well as in arousal levels). These findings underline the relevance of investigating cultural differences with physiological measures in addition to behavioral ones.

Third, the analyses reveal that Chinese and US-American consumers included in our sample approach novel products differently. Vertical orientation in particular seems to shape the perceived attractiveness of the novel product. The level of arousal (measured at an implicit level), turns out to be the driving force in this context. Although these findings are already intriguing, further studies would be valuable to test the robustness of the differences found. Further analyses could also focus on a potentially moderating role of perceived uncertainty, thereby strengthening the findings with regard to the differences in arousal levels. Moreover, prior studies have revealed that Chinese participants show lower impulsive buying tendencies due to triggered self-control (Zhang et al., 2012). Because uncertainty could also be managed with higher self-control, it would be useful to observe whether the reaction to uncertainty resulting from novel product changes over time (for instance, after self-control exhaustion for the different cultures). Additionally, the findings provide an indication that Chinese consumers who are targeted for premium products like cars also tend to be more individualistic, like Western consumers. This cultural finding, potentially, demonstrates that socioeconomic change in China is shaping new cultural approaches, which is an area of inquiry that should deserve substantial attention from researchers in the near future. The present study shows that among present-day consumers, US-Americans and Chinese differ predominantly on the basis of divergent horizontal and vertical approaches.

Due to the small sample size, which is often the case in psychophysiological studies (Riedl et al., 2020), the results of the present study are exploratory in nature. Future research should build upon our findings and test whether the effects found also hold for a broader sample.

Moreover, we also suggest including a more diverse set of stimuli as well as further features of the cars tested. Regarding a potential psychological explanation, further research in this context should also account for further socio-demographic variables of the respondents, including individual personality characteristics beyond variables focusing on cultural difference. Doing so would enable to get a more comprehensive picture on different market segments. Focusing not only on evaluations on an attitude-level but also on intentions or actual purchase behavior would further enhance the practical implications of the topic.

Further research could include studies manipulating particular features and element of the cars as well as to include overall evaluations of those features beyond mere visual aspects or where subject asked to mentally compare the cars. This could include qualitative investigations focusing on the mental representations of the features of the cars.

A valuable extension of this research would result from measuring not only the arousal level as such but also including other methods, which are capable of capturing the associated valence component. In this regard, methods like startle reflex modulation (e.g., Koller and Walla, 2012) or fMRI would support the further exploration of the valence changes in EDA levels of consumers with low or high vertical orientations. This would also allow gaining a better understanding of the role of arousal within the context of novel products in a qualitative manner. It would help to develop further insights into the role of positively toned (e.g., excitement or curiosity) or rather negatively toned (e.g., risk and uncertainty) facets within the evaluation and decision-making processes of novel products. Furthermore, moving into the area of brain activation research offers potential for providing information about underlying brain mechanisms that can reveal important insights into the influence that cultural patterns and ongoing cultural changes impose on consumer decision-making. The significance of this line of research extends beyond Chinese and US-American cultures, and beyond the valuable knowledge of responses to novel products. In this way, the present research has potentially opened an avenue of investigation that both needs and deserves an energetic research response.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the first and corresponding author upon request, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by IRB, Zeppelin University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

IR was the leading author responsible for data collection, data analysis, and the first draft of the manuscript. This manuscript was one part of her Ph.D. thesis “Beiträge zur Integration neurophysiologischer Methoden in das Innovationsmanagement.” MH, MK, and PK contributed equally to the manuscript, revisions, and writing on the provided draft. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

IR received funding of personnel and travel costs for her Ph.D. from AUDI AG. The authors declare that for this study, AUDI AG. did not influence the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article or the decision to submit it for publication. Studies were planned and applied according to standard rules of freedom in research.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Bruno Preilowski, former head of Hugo-Eckener Laboratory for Experimental Psychology and Brain Research at Zeppelin University for supporting with the experimental set-up.

Footnotes

  1. ^ Age differences between samples are due to the possible pool of participants in the targeted consumer segment. We assume no bias here, because studies on age differences and visual attention are mainly focused on performance and age ranges are much higher between young (around 20 years) and older (around 60 years) participants (see for example Madden et al., 2007).
  2. ^ It needs to be mentioned that three US-American and four Chinese participants had to be excluded due to inaccurate eye tracking after re-calibration.
  3. ^ It needs to be mentioned that one US-American and one Chinese participants had to be excluded due to inaccurate EDA measurements.

References

Aggarwal, P., Caha, T., and Wilemon, D. (1998). Barriers to the adoption of really-new products and the role of surrogate buyers. J. Consum. Mark. 15, 358–371. doi: 10.1108/07363769810226000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arts, J. W., Frambach, R. T., and Bijmolt, T. H. (2011). Generalizations on consumer innovation adoption: a meta-analysis on drivers of intention and behavior. Int. J. Res. Mark. 28, 134–144. doi: 10.1016/j.ijresmar.2010.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bach, D. R., Friston, K. J., and Dolan, R. J. (2010). Analytic measures for quantification of arousal from spontaneous skin conductance fluctuations. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 76, 52–55. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bao, Y., Zhou, K. Z., and Su, C. (2003). Face consciousness and risk aversion: do they affect consumer decision-making? Psychol. Mark. 20, 733–755. doi: 10.1002/mar.10094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauer, R. (1967). “Consumer behavior as risk taking,” in Risk Taking and Information Handling in Consumer Behavior, ed. D. Cox (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press), 23–33.

Google Scholar

Bettman, J. R., Luce, M. F., and Payne, J. W. (1998). Constructive consumer choice processes. J. Consum. Res. 25, 187–217. doi: 10.1086/209535

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brakus, J. J., Schmitt, B. H., and Zhang, S. (2014). Experiential product attributes and preferences for new products: the role of processing fluency. J. Bus. Res. 67, 2291–2298. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.06.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brasel, S. A., and Gips, J. (2008). Breaking through fast-forwarding: brand information and visual attention. J. Mark. 72, 31–48. doi: 10.1509/jmkg.72.6.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broniarczyk, S. M., and Alba, J. W. (1994). The role of consumers’ intuitions in inference making. J. Consum. Res. 21, 393–407. doi: 10.1086/209406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, M. C., and Goodstein, R. C. (2001). The moderating effect of perceived risk on consumers’ evaluations of product incongruity: preference for the norm. J. Consum. Res. 28, 439–449. doi: 10.1086/323731

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chua, H. F., Boland, J. E., and Nisbett, R. E. (2005). Cultural variation in eye movements during scene perception. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 12629–12633. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506162102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clement, J., Kristensen, T., and Grønhaug, K. (2013). Understanding consumers’ in-store visual perception: the influence of package design features on visual attention. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 20, 234–239. doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2013.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, D. F., and Rich, S. U. (1964). Perceived risk and consumer decision-making: the case of telephone shopping. J. Mark. Res. 1, 32–39. doi: 10.1177/002224376400100405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Critchley, H. D. (2002). Electrodermal responses: what happens in the brain. Neuroscientist 8, 132–142. doi: 10.1177/107385840200800209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czarnecka, B., Schivinski, B., and Keles, S. (2020). How values of individualism and collectivism influence impulsive buying and money budgeting: the mediating role of acculturation to global consumer culture. J. Consum. Behav. 19, 505–522. doi: 10.1002/cb.1833

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, L. Y. (2013). Let us go shopping: exploring Northwest Chinese consumers’ shopping experiences. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 37, 353–359. doi: 10.1111/j.1470-6431.2012.01095.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evanschitzky, H., Eisend, M., Calantone, R. J., and Jiang, Y. (2012). Success factors of product innovation: an updated meta-analysis. J. Prod. Innov. Manag. 29, 21–37.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Featherman, M. S., and Pavlou, P. A. (2003). Predicting e-services adoption: a perceived risk facets perspective. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 59, 451–474. doi: 10.1016/S1071-5819(03)00111-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feiereisen, S., Wong, V., and Broderick, A. (2008). Analogies and mental simulations in learning for really new products: the role of visual attention. J. Prod. Innov. Manag. 25, 593–607. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5885.2008.00324.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folkes, V., and Matta, S. (2004). The effect of package shape on consumers’ judgements of product volume: attention as a mental contaminant. J. Consum. Res. 31, 390–401. doi: 10.1086/422117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gierlach, E., Belsher, B. E., and Beutler, L. E. (2010). Cross-cultural differences in risk perceptions of disasters. Risk Anal. 30, 1539–1549. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2010.01451.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gneezy, A. (2017). Field experimentation in marketing research. J. Mark. Res. 54, 140–143. doi: 10.1509/jmr.16.0225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goa, H., and Zhang, Y. (2022). How does power distance belief impact consumers’ responses to demotion in hierarchical loyalty programs? The dual processes of monetary and psychological losses. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 50, 822–840. doi: 10.1007/s11747-021-00825-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goh, J. O., Tan, J. C., and Park, D. C. (2009). Culture modulates eye-movements to visual novelty. PLoS One 12:e8238. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofstede, G. (1984). Culture’s Consequences: International Differences in Work-Related Values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.

Google Scholar

Holper, L., Wolf, M., and Tobler, P. N. (2014). Comparison of functional near-infrared spectroscopy and electrodermal activity in assessing objective versus subjective risk during risky financial decisions. Neuroimage 84, 833–842. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoyer, W. D., MacInnis, D. J., and Pieters, R. (2018). Consumer Behavior. Boston, MA: Cengage Learning.

Google Scholar

Hubert, M., Florack, A., Gattringer, R., Eberhardt, T., Enkel, E., and Kenning, P. (2017). Flag up!–Flagship products as important drivers of perceived brand innovativeness. J. Bus. Res. 71, 154–163. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2016.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutmacher, F. (2019). Why is there so much more research on vision than on any other sensory modality? Front. Psychol. 10:2246. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishii, K., Reyes, A., and Kitayama, S. (2003). Spontaneous attention to word content versus emotional tone: differences among three cultures. Psychol. Sci. 14, 39–46. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Itti, L., and Koch, C. (2001). Computational modelling of visual attention. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 194–203. doi: 10.1038/35058500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, W. A., Hawley, K. J., Plewe, S. H., Elliott, J. M., and DeWitt, M. J. (1990). Attention capture by novel stimuli. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 119, 397–411. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.119.4.397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kardes, F. R., and Wyer, R. S. Jr. (2013). “Consumer information processing,” in Oxford Library of Psychology. The Oxford Handbook of Social Cognition, ed. D. E. Carlston (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 806–828. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199730018.013.0039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keaveney, S. M., Herrmann, A., Befurt, R., and Landwehr, J. R. (2012). The eyes have it: how a car’s face influences consumer categorization and evaluation of product line extensions. Psychol. Mark. 29, 36–51. doi: 10.1002/mar.20501

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koller, M., and Walla, P. (2012). “Measuring affective information processing in information systems and consumer research - Introducing startle reflex modulation,” in Proceedings of the 23th International Conference on Information Systems (ICIS), Orlando, FL (Atlanta, GA: Association for Information Systems (AIS)), 1–16.

Google Scholar

Kopton, I., and Kenning, P. (2014). Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (NIRS) as a new tool for neuroeconomic research. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:549. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopton, I., Preilowski, B., and Kenning, P. (2013). Implementation intentions as self-regulation tool for low- and high-level impulsive buyers: a behavioral and neurophysiological investigation. Adv. Consum. Res. 40:1106.

Google Scholar

Kreuzbauer, R., and Malter, A. J. (2005). Embodied cognition and new product design: changing product form to influence brand categorization. J. Prod. Innov. Manag. 22, 165–176. doi: 10.1111/j.0737-6782.2005.00112.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuijpers, E., Nijman, H., Bongers, I., Lubberding, M., and Ouwerkerk, M. (2012). Can mobile skin conductance assessments be helpful in signaling imminent inpatient aggression? Acta Neuropsychiatr. 24, 56–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-5215.2011.00582.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, C., Zhao, H., and Begley, T. M. (2014). Transformational leadership dimensions and employee creativity in China: a cross-level analysis. J. Bus. Res. 68, 1149–1156. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.11.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madden, D. J., Spaniol, J., Whiting, W. L., Bucur, B., Provenzale, J. M., Cabeza, R., et al. (2007). Adult age differences in the functional neuroanatomy of visual attention: a combined fMRI and DTI study. Neurobiol. Aging 28, 459–476. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Messner, C., and Wänke, M. (2011). Unconscious information processing reduces information overload and increases product satisfaction. J. Consum. Psychol. 21, 9–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2010.09.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michaelis, M., Woisetschläger, D. M., Backhaus, C., and Ahlert, D. (2008). The effects of country of origin and corporate reputation on initial trust. An experimental evaluation of the perception of Polish consumers. Int. Mark. Rev. 25, 404–422. doi: 10.1108/02651330810887468

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, V.-W., and Boustani, P. (1993). Market development using new products and new customers: a role for perceived risk. Eur. J. Mark. 27, 17–32. doi: 10.1186/s13054-016-1208-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moon, C., Travaglino, G. A., and Uskul, A. K. (2018). Social value orientation and endorsement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism: an exploratory study comparing individuals from North America and South Korea. Front. Psychol. 9:2262. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mukherjee, A., and Hoyer, W. D. (2001). The effect of novel attributes on product evaluation. J. Consum. Res. 28, 462–472. doi: 10.1086/323733

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng-Li, D., Byrne, D. V., Chan, R. C. K., and Wand, Q. J. (2020). The influence of taste-congruent soundtracks on visual attention and food choice: a cross-cultural eye-tracking study in Chinese and Danish consumers. Food Qual. Preference 85:103962. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.103962

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahman, S., and Luomala, H. (2020). Demystifying horizontal/vertical cultural difference in green consumption: a cross-cultural comparative study. J. Int. Consum. Mark. 33, 543–558. doi: 10.1080/08961530.2020.1857669

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riedl, R., Fischer, T., Léger, P.-M., and Davis, F. D. (2020). A decade of NeuroIS research: progress, challenges, and future directions. Data Base Adv. Inf. Syst. 51, 13–54. doi: 10.1145/3410977.3410980

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosbergen, E., Pieters, R., and Wedel, M. (1997). Visual attention to advertising: a segment-level analysis. J. Consum. Res. 24, 305–314. doi: 10.1086/209512

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubera, G., and Kirca, A. H. (2012). Firm innovativeness and its performance outcomes: a meta-analytic review and theoretical integration. J. Mark. 76, 130–147. doi: 10.1509/jm.10.0494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shavitt, S., Lalwani, A. K., Zhang, J., and Torelli, C. J. (2006). The horizontal/vertical distinction in cross-cultural consumer research. J. Consum. Psychol. 16, 325–356. doi: 10.1207/s15327663jcp1604_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimojo, S., Simion, C., Shimojo, E., and Scheier, C. (2003). Gaze bias both reflects and influences preference. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 1317–1322. doi: 10.1038/nn1150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shoemaker, R. W., and Shoaf, F. R. (1975). Behavioral changes in the trial of new products. J. Consum. Res. 2, 104–109. doi: 10.1086/208621

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singelis, T. M., Triandis, H. C., Bhawuk, D. P. S., and Gelfand, M. J. (1995). Horizontal and vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism: a theoretical and measurement refinement. Cross Cult. Res. 29, 240–275. doi: 10.1177/106939719502900302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorrentino, R. M., Szeto, A. C. H., Chen, X., and Wang, Z. (2013). Uncertainty regulation across cultures: an exploration of individual differences in Chinese and Canadian children in the classroom. Pers. Individ. Differ. 54, 378–382. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steele, L. G., and Lynch, S. M. (2013). The pursuit of happiness in China: individualism, collectivism, and subjective well-being during China’s economic and social transformation. Soc. Indic. Res. 114, 1–13. doi: 10.1007/s11205-012-0154-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strombach, T., Jin, J., Weber, B., Kenning, P., Shen, Q., Ma, Q., et al. (2014). Charity begins at home: cultural differences in social discounting and generosity. J. Behav. Decis. Making 27, 235–245. doi: 10.1002/bdm.1802

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Studer, B., and Clark, L. (2011). Place your bets: psychophysiological correlates of decision-making under risk. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 11, 144–158. doi: 10.3758/s13415-011-0025-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suher, J., Hutchinson, J. W., and Sorensen, H. (2014). Windows to the sale: mobile eye-tracking and in-store decision making. Adv. Consum. Res. 41:828. doi: 10.1037/e509992015-245

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, J. W. (1974). The role of risk in consumer behavior. J. Mark. 38, 54–62. doi: 10.2307/1250198

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Triandis, H. C., and Gelfand, M. J. (1998). Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71, 118–128. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e05871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, Y. (2010). The Chinese path to individualization. Br. J. Sociol. 61, 489–512. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-4446.2010.01323.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., and Mittal, V. (2008). Culture matters: the impact of power-distance belief on consumers’ impulsive buying tendency. Adv. Consum. Res. 35:643.

Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., Winterich, K. P., and Mittal, V. (2012). Power distance belief and impulsive buying. J. Mark. Res. 47, 945–954. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.47.5.945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: visual attention, attractiveness of novel products, mobile eye-tracking, electrodermal activity, cross-cultural comparison, consumer neuroscience

Citation: Rinklin I, Hubert M, Koller M and Kenning P (2022) Visual Attention to Novel Products – Cross-Cultural Insights From Physiological Data. Front. Psychol. 13:840862. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.840862

Received: 21 December 2021; Accepted: 18 May 2022;
Published: 08 September 2022.

Edited by:

Wuke Zhang, Ningbo University, China

Reviewed by:

Debora Bettiga, Politecnico di Milano, Italy
Naveen Kashyap, Indian Institute of Technology Guwahati, India

Copyright © 2022 Rinklin, Hubert, Koller and Kenning. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marco Hubert, mah@mgmt.au.dk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.