Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 28 September 2022
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

We cannot empathize with what we do not recognize: Perceptions of structural versus interpersonal racism in South Africa

  • 1Centre for the Study of the Afterlife of Violence and the Reparative Quest, Stellenbosch University, Stellenbosch, South Africa
  • 2Annenberg School for Communication, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, United States

Recent research suggests holding a structural, rather than interpersonal, understanding of racism is associated with greater impetus to address racial disparities. We believe greater acknowledgment of structural racism also functions to mitigate against empathic failures in response to structural injustices. Given South Africa’s situatedness as a country characterized by historical racialized oppression and continuing unjust legacies, it is appropriate to examine these ideas there. Across three studies, we tested the hypotheses that members of advantaged groups’ perspective taking and empathic concern may be compromised in response to people challenging the unequal status quo, and that a priori perceptions about the impact of structural (vs interpersonal) racism may mitigate or exacerbate such empathic failures. In Study 1, a national sample of White South Africans (n = 195) endorsed perceptions of interpersonal racism more readily than perceptions of structural racism, and expressed high levels of competitive victimhood for perceived anti-White structural racism. Studies 2 (n = 138) and 3 (n = 85) showed that White participants at a historically White university responded with impaired perspective taking and intergroup empathy bias in response to people challenging structural disparities. Finally, reduced recognition of continuing structural racism predicted greater intergroup empathy bias, which, in turn, was associated with reduced willingness to engage in intergroup discussions about past harm (Study 3). We propose that greater acknowledgment of structural racism is necessary not only to surmount intergroup empathic failures, but also to transcend the socioeconomically unequal legacies of apartheid and beyond.

Introduction

Countries around the world have been compelled to acknowledge pervasive systemic and racialized inequality as it was forced into view by the unequal fall-out of the COVID-19 pandemic and the resurgence of the Black Lives Matter movement in response to racialized police brutality. Yet, many do not respond to these inequities with recognition, empathy, and allyship. In fact, some members of advantaged groups respond to people challenging the status quo with condemnation or indifference – a pattern seen in South Africa (Smith, 2017; Claasen, 2020; McMichael, 2020).

While South Africa is known for its peaceful transition from apartheid to democracy (Asmal, 2000), it remains one of the most racially unequal societies worldwide (Chatterjee, 2019). During apartheid, the government imposed socially constructed, hierarchical racial labels on its citizens with the objective to elevate the White minority at the expense of their Black African, Coloured (people of diverse racial origins), and Indian counterparts (Posel, 2001; see also Fourie et al., 2022 for present-day effects). Some argue that in South Africa’s transitional negotiations for peace, unity and reconciliation among citizens took primacy over social justice for the oppressed (Bornman, 2006; Swartz, 2016), so that in time, hopes of broad structural change gave way to disillusionment amongst the economically disenfranchised (Hino et al., 2018). The 2015–2016 #FeesMustFall social justice movement is a powerful example, when Black1 students and allies mobilized nation-wide protests against the institutional forces that perpetuate apartheid-era inequalities (Albertus, 2019). While many White South Africans support social justice, in recent years there has been a surge in denialist right-wing ideologies (Van Zyl-Hermann, 2018). Why then, does empathy break down so readily during pivotal moments when the unequal status quo is being challenged?

Empathy is a key emotional process that can facilitate social change and healing in intergroup contexts (Stephan and Finlay, 1999; Fourie et al., 2013; Cehajic-Clancy et al., 2016). Empathic failures abound, however, driving not only unfavorable intergroup outcomes, such as discrimination and hostility, but also active conflict (Cikara, 2015; Fourie et al., 2017). Today we know that empathy is not automatic, but sensitively deployed based on a priori ideas and expectations as well as situational motivations (Zaki, 2014). Here we focus on empathic concern, which refers to other-oriented feelings of care and compassion when perceiving an individual in distress (Batson, 2009). In intergroup contexts, the difference in empathic concern felt for the ingroup versus the outgroup, referred to as “intergroup empathy bias” or “parochial empathy,” has been found to significantly predict negative intergroup interactions (Cikara et al., 2011; Bruneau et al., 2017).

Perspective taking, a cognitive route to understanding others, refers to the capacity to consciously adopt another’s point of view to imagine what they might be thinking and feeling (Batson et al., 1997; Stietz et al., 2019). It constitutes an effortful process whereby a person actively tries to imagine how another person is thinking and feeling−given the particular situation, how they respond to it, and what we know about their needs and desires (Barrett-Lennard, 1981; Batson and Ahmad, 2009). In this sense, perspective taking involves a sensitive, non-judgmental understanding of the other, whereby one is attuned to the way in which the other is affected by a situation.

Substantial evidence documents the efficacy of perspective taking in eliciting empathic concern, especially in intergroup contexts (Shih et al., 2009; Batson, 2011). Taking an outgroup member’s subjective perspective has the effect of endowing that individual with uniquely human mental qualities (Harris and Fiske, 2006). For example, narrative descriptions of outgroup individuals’ mental states attenuate intergroup empathy bias (Bruneau et al., 2015). By contrast, denying “mind” to outgroup members paves the way for moral disengagement and negative intergroup outcomes, including empathic failures (Čehajić et al., 2009; Kteily et al., 2015).

Of relevance for the present research is the relation between perspective taking and the propensity to recognize racial discrimination. Across a series of studies, adopting the perspective of an outgroup member engendered greater acknowledgment of the persistence of outgroup discrimination and support of actions to combat racial inequality (Todd et al., 2012). Likewise, predetermined political beliefs and ideologies have been shown to impact perspective taking and negative intergroup outcomes (Sparkman and Eidelman, 2016). Here we propose that the tendency of advantaged group members to acknowledge the impact of structural (vs interpersonal) racism is associated with the extent to which they take the perspective and feel empathic concern for those challenging situations of structural racism.

The dominant conceptualization of racism concerns interpersonal moral transgression, when individuals overtly discriminate or exercise prejudice against outgroup members during discrete behaviors (Ansell, 2004; Sommers and Norton, 2006; Unzueta and Lowery, 2008; Verwey and Quayle, 2012; Nelson et al., 2013). A more nuanced and complex conceptualization of racism involves covert institutional practices and structural factors (e.g., laws, policies, inherited wealth) that result in cultural, material, and symbolic advantages for some groups while marginalizing others (Ture and Hamilton, 1992; Lipsitz, 2011). Structural racism therefore concerns historical and ongoing systemic, race-based prejudice, which serves as one of the foundations for interpersonal racism, and perpetuates socioeconomic, health, criminal justice, educational, and ecological inequality (Salter et al., 2017).

Unfortunately, structural understandings of racism tend to be less commonly recognized by advantaged group members and/or believed to be something of the past because of various motivated psychological processes, e.g., the desire to view society as just and merit-based (Bonam et al., 2019; Kraus et al., 2019). In addition, structural factors may play a role in such misperceptions. For example, in South Africa White people may be less likely to hold accurate perceptions of the lived realities of Black (see text footnote 1) people because of less frequent intergroup contact across historically persistent patterns of residential segregation (Durrheim and Dixon, 2010).

Of significance, is that advantaged group members also stand to gain from a more willful denial of the impact of structural racism on disadvantaged groups (Steyn, 2012; Bonam et al., 2019). In fact, denial of discrimination and structural oppression is a worldwide phenomenon amongst advantaged group members despite abundant evidence attesting to its widespread effects (Pager and Shepherd, 2008). Advantaged group members might go as far as to claim that they are victims of structural inequality themselves, thereby maintaining their moral superiority and obstructing social change (Saguy et al., 2013; Young and Sullivan, 2016). This tendency to perceive one’s own group as having suffered more relative to an outgroup is known as competitive victimhood (Noor et al., 2008).

A theory that captures many of the above assumptions is that of system justification (Jost et al., 2004; Jost, 2019). Accordingly, people are motivated to view the social order as fair (to reduce internal conflict) and to maintain and justify existing social, economic, and political positions within society. Of significance to the present work is that system-justifying attitudes are frequently associated with distorted perceptions of the status quo, and situations threatening the system (e.g., protest action) may elicit defensive responses (Jost et al., 2013). Because a structural conception of racism raises awareness of White privilege and one’s implication in unjust systems, denial thereof serves the strategic goal of maintaining the racial status quo and with it access to tangible material advantages (Unzueta and Lowery, 2008; Milazzo, 2016). Indeed, recent research suggests holding a structural (vs. interpersonal) understanding of racism is associated with greater impetus to acknowledge and address existing racial disparities (O’Brien et al., 2009; Rucker et al., 2019; Rucker and Richeson, 2021). We believe that greater perceptions of structural racism also function to mitigate against empathic failures in response to structural injustices.

Current research

Here we examined White South Africans’ perceptions about the prevalence of interpersonal versus structural racism, and whether such perceptions predicted empathic concern toward Black African individuals in multiracial scenarios portraying interpersonal or structural racism. We selected Black Africans as the targets of racism as they represent the largest group who suffered systematic discrimination during apartheid2 (Hino et al., 2018). While we acknowledge that the term racism should be reserved for race-based oppression by historically institutionalized systems, we also examined participants’ beliefs about racism directed against White people (henceforth “anti-White racism”; Saguy et al., 2013), given an increasing sense of perceived discrimination against White people (White “victimhood”) in South Africa (Van Zyl-Hermann, 2018).

We operationalized empathic concern as self-reported compassion for target individuals, and intergroup empathy bias as the difference in empathic concern reported for ingroup versus outgroup individuals. In a novel departure from previous research, we did not manipulate perspective taking as an experimental condition with instructions (e.g., imagine-other, imagine-self, or objective focus; see Stotland, 1969), but assessed whether participants took the perspectives of target individuals spontaneously (e.g., “What do you think about [target] behavior?”). We tested the following three main hypotheses.

First, we predicted that perceptions of racism by a White, national sample would be skewed, such that interpersonal racism would be acknowledged disproportionally more than structural racism (Study 1). The view that racism is lodged within individual minds tends to dominate lay beliefs about racism, is less threatening to group esteem, and might thus be easier to recognize (Sommers and Norton, 2006; Unzueta and Lowery, 2008). By contrast, structural understandings of racism tend to be less prevalent and its impact underestimated (Nelson et al., 2013; Milazzo, 2016; Bonam et al., 2019). Such misperceptions could be attributed to various motivated psychological processes and structural factors (Jost et al., 2013; Kraus et al., 2019). In addition, acknowledging the continuing impact of structural racism threatens White identity and privilege (Steyn and Foster, 2008). Therefore, we also predicted that competitive victimhood would be greater for structural compared to interpersonal racism.

Second, we reasoned that being able to take the perspective of another may not guarantee empathic concern but may be an important determinant of it. Hence, for reasons stated above, we predicted that White participants’ outgroup perspective taking in situations portraying Black individuals’ activism against structural racism will be compromised, and that their responses to such situations will be indicative of intergroup empathy bias (Studies 2 and 3). We did not predict such difficulties in perspective taking and empathic concern for interpersonal situations of racism and injustice. Finally, we hypothesized that perceptions of the prevalence of structural racism would predict empathic concern in situations where the unequal status quo is challenged, such that greater recognition of structural racism will be associated with reduced intergroup empathy bias (Study 3).

Study 1

Here we assessed perceptions about the prevalence of interpersonal versus structural forms of racism in the lives of Black African and White South Africans. We predicted that White participants’ endorsement of interpersonal racism against Black African people would be greater than that of structural racism against Black African people.

Methods

Participants

To determine the number of participants required to detect significant differences in perceptions of interpersonal and structural racism, we conducted an a priori power analysis for paired-samples t-tests using G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2007). This analysis indicated that 199 participants would be required to detect a small within-group effect (d = 0.20) with 80% power. We recruited 200 South African participants identifying as White through a countrywide panel service to participate in an online survey assessing social attitudes (for additional details on sampling, see Supplementary material). We excluded five participants who did not complete all relevant questions, leaving a final sample of 195 (Mage = 39.62, SDage = 12.12, 77% female, Myrs edu = 13.67, SDyrs edu = 3.07). Data were collected as part of a larger study that included additional measures not relevant to the present research question.

Measures

Demographics

Participants reported whether they are South African citizens, fluent in English, and identify as White (inclusion criteria), followed by their gender, age, and years of formal education.

Perceptions about racism

To assess perceptions of interpersonal versus structural racism against Black African and White South Africans, we first defined these terms to ensure that participants understood the distinction between them. Interpersonal racism was defined as “negative attitudes and discriminatory behaviors by individuals toward members of specific racial groups,” whereas structural racism was defined as “institutional practices and structural factors (e.g., laws, policies) that routinely disadvantage specific racial groups.” Participants were then asked the following six questions in counterbalanced order: “How much did [interpersonal/structural] racism affect Black African people during apartheid?” (anti-Black racism apartheid), “How much does [interpersonal/structural] racism affect Black African people today?” (anti-Black racism today), and “How much does [interpersonal/structural] racism affect White people today?” (anti-White racism today).3 These questions were adapted from Rucker et al. (2019), and were collected on continuous sliders ranging from 1 (not at all) to 100 (very much). The difference between participants’ perceptions of anti-Black and anti-White racism today was used as a proxy for competitive victimhood.

Results

As hypothesized, White participants perceived anti-Black structural racism (M = 37.68, SD = 31.03) to have significantly less of an impact than anti-Black interpersonal racism (M = 46.74, SD = 30.52) on Black African people today, t(193) = 5.86, p < 0.001, d = 0.42. This is in contrast to their perceptions of anti-Black racism during apartheid, which was not only much greater than today, but anti-Black structural racism (M = 79.20, SD = 21.92) was also perceived to be more significant than anti-Black interpersonal racism (M = 74.97, SD = 24.62) during that time, t(194) = –3.57, p < 0.001, d = 0.26. By comparison, White participants perceived no significant difference between anti-White structural (M = 75.46, SD = 27.87) and anti-White interpersonal (M = 74.27, SD = 27.04) racism today, t(194) = –0.89, p = 0.376, d = –0.06 (Figure 1A).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Study 1 White participants’ perceptions of anti-Black interpersonal and structural racism during apartheid and today, and anti-White interpersonal and structural racism today. (B) Perceptions of competitive victimhood (anti-White racism today minus anti-Black racism today) were greater for structural than interpersonal racism. Error bars indicate standard error of the mean. ***p < 0.001. Panel (A) is reprinted by permission from Springer Customer Service Centre GmbH: Springer Nature, Affective Science (Fourie and Verwoerd, 2022). ns = not significant.

When comparing perceptions of anti-Black and anti-White racism, it is noteworthy to observe that perceptions of anti-White interpersonal and structural racism today were significantly greater than that experienced by Black African people today (Interpersonal racism: t(193) = 8.93, p < 0.001, d = 0.64; Structural racism: t(193) = 11.98, p < 0.001, d = 0.86). Moreover, perceptions of anti-White interpersonal and structural racism today did not differ significantly from anti-Black interpersonal and structural racism experienced by Black African people under apartheid (Interpersonal racism: t(194) = –0.31, p = 0.757, d = –0.02; Structural racism: t(194) = –1.64, p = 0.102, d = –0.12). Such heightened perceptions of racism experienced by the historically advantaged ingroup (i.e., anti-White racism today minus anti-Black racism today) could be described as perceptions of competitive victimhood. In line with our hypothesis, in the present sample, competitive victimhood for perceived structural racism was significantly greater than competitive victimhood for perceived interpersonal racism, t(193) = –5.15, p < 0.001, d = –0.37 (Figure 1B).

Discussion

As we anticipated, White South Africans in our sample perceived the impact of structural racism to be significantly less severe than interpersonal racism on Black African people’s lives today, despite structural racism’s stronger connection to deepening socioeconomic inequality and low social mobility than interpersonal racism (Day and Fiske, 2017; Salter et al., 2017).

White participants reported significantly inflated perceptions of anti-White racism today—on a par with that experienced by Black African people during apartheid—even though the continuing staying power of White privilege post-apartheid is recognized widely (Steyn and Foster, 2008; Chatterjee, 2019). These results could be explained through the lens of competitive victimhood, with White people not only underestimating the pervasive and continuing impact of structural forms of oppression that Black people experience, but also perceiving themselves to be at the brunt of racial oppression (Noor et al., 2017; Bentrovato et al., 2020).

Power disparities can shape how competitive victimhood processes play out (McNeill and Vollhardt, 2020). Advantaged group members may engage in competitive victimhood as a coping mechanism to deal with accusations of inflicting unjust suffering on disadvantaged groups and to restore their group’s moral integrity (Sullivan et al., 2012). While feelings of competitive victimhood may not be a conscious process, it assuages guilt through retrospective justification. Hence, by claiming relative victim status, people of advantaged groups can reduce feelings of responsibility for previous harmdoing and defend their positive identity (Tov-Nachlieli et al., 2013; Young and Sullivan, 2016). Competitive victimhood also strengthens identification with the ingroup, and has been associated with reduced outgroup empathy (Noor et al., 2008).

Taken together, as in the US (Kraus et al., 2017, 2019), White South Africans in our sample considered the impact of more pervasive (albeit less explicit) structural forms of racism on Black African people to be significantly less than that of interpersonal forms of anti-Black racism today. Such rationalizations are costly as they may serve to justify and stabilize unequal systems (Jost et al., 2013). Notably, distorted perceptions about structural racism, combined with feelings of competitive victimhood, may limit White people’s motivation to recognize and respond with empathic concern to Black people challenging structural oppression. Study 2 tests this hypothesis.

Study 2

Here we examined perspective taking and empathic concern in response to ecological scenarios about structural and interpersonal racism involving Black African and White individuals. We predicted that White participants (i) would not readily take the perspectives of Black individuals who challenge structural racism, and therefore respond with significant intergroup empathy bias, but (ii) would take the perspectives of Black individuals in interpersonal situations of racism or injustice and respond with high empathic concern. We hypothesized that Black African participants would respond with high perspective taking and empathic concern for Black individuals in all situations of racism or injustice.

Methods

Participants

To determine the number of participants required to detect significant differences in ingroup and outgroup empathic concern, we conducted an a priori power analysis for paired- samples t-tests using G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2007). This analysis indicated that 90 participants would be required to detect small-medium within-group effects (d = 0.30) with 80% power. We recruited 247 undergraduate students from lecture halls at a historically White public university to complete study procedures. Because we were interested in White and Black African participant responses, we excluded 80 individuals who identified as either foreign nationals (n = 13), Coloured, Indian or Asian (n = 64), or who did not provide demographic information (n = 3). The final sample consisted of 167 participants (Mage = 19.39, SDage = 1.37, 82% female, Myrs edu = 13.11, SDyrs edu = 0.98, 138 White, 29 Black African). Although the Black African sample was comparatively small, based on our obtained within-group effect sizes (scenario 1: d = 0.49, scenario 2: d = 1.56, scenario 3: d = 0.96), we achieved β > 0.83. We therefore include their responses to the scenarios here as comparison to those of White participants.

Measures

Reflective scenarios

Participants were asked to read and respond to three short multi-racial scenarios including White and Black individuals. To ensure that the scenarios appeared authentic with high ecological validity, they were modeled after real events. Each scenario was furthermore framed to allow perspective taking for both parties involved, thus avoiding an apparent “wrongdoer” and “victim” which would dictate responses. Scenario 1 involved a protest scene at the university that participants attended, where Black workers expressed their frustration toward a statue which symbolizes Black oppression and perpetuation of the outsourcing system, while White students silenced the Black workers’ response. Scenario 2 involved a situation of interpersonal injustice between a Black domestic worker and her White employers. Scenario 3 involved a situation of interpersonal racism common in sports. The three scenarios were similar in length and presented in the following order:

Scenario 1: (opposition to) Structural racism. Last year an incident was reported to the University Transformation Office: During a protest by the university’s outsourced workers, a few workers threw mud on the J.H. Marais statue (a heritage treasure). Some White students then decided to do something about the situation. They went to the statue with cleaning materials and started to clean up the mess. The cleaning of the statue by the students angered the workers, however. Soon a cycle started where workers threw more and more mud on the statue and the students kept cleaning it without backing off.

Scenario 2: Interpersonal injustice. Nomusa is a domestic worker who has worked at the Eksteen household for the past two years. Recently Nomusa suffered a great loss: her house burned down as a result of an accident involving tile glue and she lost most of her possessions. Two months after the incident she asked her well-to-do employers, Mr. and Mrs. Eksteen whether they would help with an expensive dress for her daughter’s matric farewell. The dress cost ZAR800 (approximately US $50). While Mr. and Mrs. Eksteen have been supportive following the accident, they refused to help Nomusa with money for the dress.

Scenario 3: Interpersonal racism. The following scenario was reported at an elite secondary girls’ school in Cape Town. Zoliswa is a talented athlete and recently went to the national hockey championships where she was selected for the National Hockey Team, a very prestigious achievement. Her initial excitement was dampened, however, when some of the White players on the team acted unfriendly/hostile toward her. When she came home, she told her parents, to their utter dismay and disbelief, that she would not take up the position as she did not want to be seen as a “quota player.”

Spontaneous perspective taking was assessed following each scenario through two free-text questions that did not prime participants to imagine each target’s motivation. Participants were asked to indicate in their own words “What do you think about [White target behavior]?” in the scenario and “What do you think about [Black target behavior]?” in the scenario. Responses to each target in each scenario were deductively coded as perspective taking present (perspective taking = 1) or absent (perspective taking = 0). Specifically, a response that suggested “responsively knowing” or a “sensitive understanding” of a party’s internal state was scored positive for perspective taking (Batson and Ahmad, 2009). Imagine-self and imagine-other perspectives were both coded as perspective taking present (Todd et al., 2011). Two independent researchers blind to participants’ identity coded these data. Responses that were unclear/ambiguous in terms of perspective taking were omitted from data analysis (coded as missing data). The intercoder reliability values for scenarios 1 and 2 were moderate to strong (Kappa = 0.75 and Kappa = 0.82, respectively, ps < 0.001), whereas the intercoder reliability value for scenario 3 was somewhat weaker (Kappa = 0.69, p < 0.001; McHugh, 2012; Blick et al., 2018). Instances of intercoder disagreement were resolved through a consensus approach where discrepancies were discussed with the PI and mutual decisions reached (O’Connor and Joffe, 2020).

Self-reported emotion

In response to the Black and White targets in each scenario, participants were asked to indicate “How did [White/Black target behavior] make you feel?” Participants then rated the following emotions on a 1 (not at all) to 9 (very much) Likert-type scale: frustration, compassion, anger, pride, shame, and anxiety. Empathic concern was operationalized as compassion to ensure that participants understood its meaning. We created a composite negative emotion score that consisted of frustration and anger ratings (αs > 0.77).

Results

Perspective taking

White participants’ perspective taking in response to Black target individuals was much less common in response to scenario 1 (structural racism: 27%) than scenario 2 (interpersonal injustice: 84%) or scenario 3 (interpersonal racism: 55%; Table 1). However, most White participants took the perspective of White target individuals in scenario 1 (93%), and less so in scenarios 2 and 3 (57% and 7%, respectively). By comparison, Black African participants readily took the perspectives of Black target individuals in scenarios 1 and 2 (82% and 88%), and to a lesser extent in scenario 3 (54%; Table 1). Examples of perspective taking (present/absent) qualitative responses can be found in Supplementary Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Perspective taking frequency scores: Study 2.

White and Black African participants’ somewhat reduced perspective taking for the Black target in scenario 3, a more obvious situation of racism, was unexpected. Closer inspection of participant responses suggested that perspective taking in some instances were masked by participants’ reactive expression of negative emotion (despair) in response to Zoliswa’s decision to quit the team and “give in” to the White players. In their responses they therefore neglected to responsively consider her feelings and motivations. Indeed, self-reported negative emotion (frustration and anger) did not differ significantly from empathic concern for Zoliswa for either Black African (p = 0.545, d = –0.13) or White (p = 0.224, d = 0.11) participants (see Supplementary Table 2).

Self-reported empathic concern

Self-reported emotion ratings and statistical analyses to determine which emotions were felt most strongly in response to each target can be found in the Supplementary material (Supplementary Table 2). Below we focus on participants’ empathic concern responses.

For scenario 1 (structural racism), a paired-samples t-test indicated that White participants displayed significant intergroup empathy bias: empathic concern ratings were higher in response to White (M = 5.17, SD = 2.26) than Black (M = 2.90, SD = 2.00) target individuals, t(124) = –7.80, p < 0.001, d = 0.70. As anticipated, Black African participants also showed significant intergroup empathy bias, with empathic concern rated higher in response to Black (M = 5.17, SD = 2.62) than White (M = 3.30, SD = 2.49) target individuals, t(22) = 2.36, p = 0.028, d = 0.49 (Figure 2A).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Study 2 self-reported empathic concern ratings for (A) scenario 1 (structural racism), (B) scenario 2 (interpersonal injustice), and (C) scenario 3 (interpersonal racism). Error bars indicate standard error of the mean.

For scenario 2 (interpersonal injustice) and 3 (interpersonal racism), White participants’ responses were reversed: they were more empathetic toward the Black (scenario 2: M = 5.85, SD = 2.32; scenario 3: M = 5.19, SD = 2.56) than White (scenario 2: M = 2.40, SD = 1.96; scenario 3: M = 1.59, SD = 1.36) target individuals, t(123) = 12.59, p < 0.001, d = 1.13 and t(115) = 14.15, p < 0.001, d = 1.31, respectively. Likewise, Black African participants were more empathetic toward the Black (scenario 2: M = 6.28, SD = 2.37; scenario 3: M = 5.35, SD = 2.96) than White (scenario 2: M = 1.83, SD = 1.43; scenario 3: M = 2.13, SD = 2.60) target individuals in these scenarios, t(22) = 7.48, p < 0.001, d = 1.56 and t(21) = 4.50, p < 0.001, d = 0.96, respectively. In fact, White and Black African participants’ empathic concern ratings did not differ significantly from each other for either White or Black target individuals in scenarios 2 and 3 (Mann–Whitney U tests < 1.40, ps > 0.16; Figures 2B,C).

Perspective taking and empathic concern

To determine how well perspective taking predicted empathic concern, we performed a simultaneous regression for each target in each scenario, with perspective taking (absent/present) scores and participant race as predictor variables, and target empathic concern ratings as the dependent variable. We inspected regression coefficients using 95% confidence intervals (CIs) derived through bootstrapping running 1,000 iterations (Efron, 1987; see Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Simultaneous regressions predicting empathic concern as a function of perspective taking and participant race: Study 2.

These analyses indicated that perspective taking significantly predicted empathic concern for both targets in scenario 1 (Black target: β = 0.48, p < 0.001; White target: β = 0.22, p = 0.010), scenario 2 (Black target: β = 0.44, p < 0.001; White target: β = 0.27, p = 0.001), and scenario 3 (Black target: β = 0.23, p = 0.006; White target: β = 0.37, p < 0.001). Participants who took a target’s perspective were thus much more likely to also respond with greater ratings of empathic concern. Of significance, is that participant race predicted empathic concern only for targets in scenario 1 (Black target: β = –0.22, p = 0.005, White target: β = 0.23, p = 0.008), but not those in scenarios 2 or 3. These findings corroborate our hypothesis, namely that White and Black African participants’ empathic concern responses would differ particularly for the scenario characterized by structural racism.

It should be noted that due to the cross-sectional, correlational nature of our study we could not determine causal relations between perspective taking and empathic concern, and that it is also plausible that empathic concern led to greater perspective taking (see Supplementary Table 3).

Discussion

Consistent with our theorizing, taking an outgroup member’s perspective significantly predicted empathic concern for that individual. White participants were unlikely to take the perspective of Black individuals challenging structural racism (scenario 1) and responded with significant intergroup empathy bias, but readily took the perspectives of Black individuals in interpersonal situations of injustice (scenarios 2 and 3) and responded with high empathic concern. By contrast, Black African participants responded with high perspective taking and empathic concern toward Black individuals in each scenario. For scenario 1, in particular, their qualitative responses supported the interpretation that they understood that “the workers’ protest action was directed at University management, not ‘White Students’, who saw it fit to defend a demeaning system” (Black African participant; for more qualitative responses, see Supplementary Results).

Of significance, is that White participants did not fail systematically in all outgroup perspective taking and empathic concern responses. Rather, the context markedly impacted their responses, with a greater likelihood to express empathy for Black individuals when they were in contexts representing interpersonal (scenarios 2 and 3) than structural (scenario 1) racism. Understanding the mechanism(s) that underlie such failures in perspective taking and empathic concern is necessary to address and/or prevent them. Here we theorized that the type of racism (structural vs. interpersonal) plays a key role in motivating outgroup empathy. Below we consider support for this interpretation from the intergroup literature.

First, because structural racism is less about individual behavior and more about collective practices that produce unequal outcomes for different racialized identities, it is more likely to involve groups than interpersonal racism. Intergroup contexts (such as scenario 1), however, may provoke significantly more competition and aggression than interpersonal contexts (such as scenarios 2 and 3), thereby diminishing the motivation to care about an outgroup member’s misfortune (Meier and Hinsz, 2004; Cikara et al., 2011). Explicit competition can also increase the salience of social identity and connection with the ingroup (Hewstone et al., 2002), with potentially damaging effects for socially distant others (Tarrant et al., 2012; Waytz and Epley, 2012). Hence, because situations of structural racism are more likely to involve groups, they may be associated with increased intergroup empathy bias.

Second, structural racism might be threatening to White people’s self-image of being morally “good.” Unlike interpersonal racism, where the “bad” culprit is identifiable, structural racism increases awareness of White privilege and imparts a broader moral implication to the problem of racism (Unzueta and Lowery, 2008; Bonam et al., 2019). Scenario 1 may thus have posed a social identity threat to White participants, which, when disidentification with the ingroup is not possible, may be associated with defensive reactions (Branscombe et al., 1999; Halabi et al., 2008). The threat posed by scenario 1 could also be material, however, because participants’ own institution was at stake. Their empathy failures, therefore, might have been motivated by the desire to avoid loss of valuable resources (Riek et al., 2006; Zaki, 2014), whereas scenarios 2 and 3 did not involve personal costs. To rule out this possibility, we changed the context of scenario 1 to a different institution in Study 3.

Finally, opposition to structural racism is usually set in the public discourse around race and therefore highlights ideological discrepancies between groups, termed symbolic threat (Stephan et al., 2009). The relationship between perceived threat to the dominant (White) cultural worldview and increased racially biased outcomes is well documented (Greenberg and Kosloff, 2008; Craig and Richeson, 2014). In scenario 1, the ideological discrepancy involves beliefs of structural inequity, which was the reason for the workers’ protest, and which the White students silenced with their behavior. White participants might contest beliefs of unfairness and respond with intergroup empathy bias because acknowledging structural racism would mean acknowledging an unjust system and their own complicity in entrenched systems of racial discrimination (Jones, 2004; Steyn, 2012; Jost, 2019).

Taken together, White participants’ outgroup perspective taking and empathic concern were compromised for the scenario portraying opposition to structural racism, potentially because such scenarios typically involve groups, may trigger intergroup threat, and challenge the fairness of the system. We believe prior perceptions about the continued impact of structural racism on Black people’s lives would influence the extent to which advantaged people respond with empathic concern in such situations. Study 3 tests this hypothesis.

Study 3

Study 3 replicated the procedures of Study 2, but with the inclusion of questions to assess perceptions about the impact of interpersonal and structural racism, as in Study 1. We predicted that White participants’ perceptions of racism would again be skewed, with greater acknowledgment of the impact of interpersonal than structural racism in the lives of Black African people. Furthermore, we theorized that such predetermined notions about racism would meaningfully influence participants’ empathy responses (Bar-Tal and Halperin, 2011; Zaki, 2014), such that reduced perceptions of anti-Black structural racism would predict greater intergroup empathy bias in the scenario representing opposition to structural racism. Finally, we hypothesized that intergroup empathy bias would mediate the relationship between perceptions of anti-Black structural racism and willingness to attend an intergroup reconciliatory event.

Methods

Participants

To determine the number of participants required to examine whether perceptions of anti-Black structural racism predict intergroup empathy bias, we conducted an a priori power analysis using G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2007). This analysis indicated that we would need 68 participants for a multiple linear regression model with two predictors to detect a moderate effect (f2 = 0.15) with 80% power. We recruited 149 undergraduate students from lecture halls at the same historically White university as Study 2 to complete study procedures. Because the Black African participant sample was too small for analysis (n = 11), they were excluded, along with individuals self-identifying as Coloured, Indian, or foreign nationals (n = 53). The final sample consisted of 85 White participants (Mage = 19.24, SDage = 0.91, 89% female, Myrs edu = 13.14, SDyrs edu = 0.77).

Measures

Reflective scenarios

The scenarios employed were similar to those in Study 2, with the following two exceptions. First, we changed the context of scenario 1 (structural racism) to that of a different university to determine whether White participants’ responses replicated when personal material costs were not at stake (i.e., their own institution). Second, we omitted scenario 3 (interpersonal racism) for the following reasons: (i) To reduce the amount of time to complete the questions and thus increase attention/interest of respondents. (ii) Because Black African participants reported high negative emotion (anger and frustration), in addition to empathic concern, in response to the Black target individual in this scenario (see Study 2, Supplementary Table 2). Since the scenarios served to examine whether White participants took the perspectives of outgroup individuals and responded with perspective taking and empathic concern in situations where Black African participants typically do, scenario 3 did not prove reliable in this regard. (iii) Scenario 3 was also more leading than scenario 2 in terms of an apparent “wrongdoer” and “victim,” thus requiring less perspective taking from participants. (iv) Finally, the perspective taking intercoder reliability of scenario 3 was the lowest. Scenario 2 (interpersonal injustice) remained unchanged and represented an interpersonal comparative context to scenario 1.

Spontaneous perspective taking for each target individual was assessed, as in Study 2, through two free-text questions following each scenario, i.e., “What do you think about [White/Black target behavior]?” The analyses proceeded in the same way, with perspective taking coded as present (1) or absent (0; Intercoder reliability: KappaScenario 1 = 0.77, KappaScenario 2 = 0.85, ps < 0.001).

Self-reported emotion

Emotions (frustration, compassion, anger, pride, shame, and anxiety) in response to the targets in each scenario were measured as in Study 2, from 1 (not at all) to 9 (very much). Empathic concern in Study 3 was operationalized in the same way as in Study 2 (i.e., compassion), and negative emotion consisted of a composite of frustration and anger ratings (αs > 0.85).

Perceptions about racism

Perceptions about the impact of interpersonal and structural forms of racism against Black African people (anti-Black racism during apartheid and today) and White people (anti-White racism today) were assessed as in Study 1. Responses were collected on Likert scales ranging from 1 (not at all) to 9 (very much). The difference between participants’ perceptions of anti-Black and anti-White racism today was again used as a proxy for perceived victimhood.

Intergroup interaction around historical oppression

A measure assessing participants’ willingness to attend an intergroup reconciliatory event was included. Participants were asked to respond to the question “If you have the opportunity to attend a cross-racial event where people share their experiences of discrimination during and after apartheid, would you be interested in going?” on a scale ranging from 1 (not interested at all) to 9 (extremely interested).

Results

Perspective taking and empathic concern

As in Study 2, White participants’ perspective taking in response to Black target individuals were much less common for scenario 1 (structural racism: 38%) than scenario 2 (interpersonal injustice: 72%). Perspective taking for White target individuals in scenario 1 were again high (70%; Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. White participants’ (N = 85) perspective taking frequency scores for each scenario: Study 3.

Regarding empathic concern, a paired-samples t-test indicated that White participants again displayed significant intergroup empathy bias for scenario 1: empathic concern was higher in response to White (M = 5.13, SD = 2.33) than Black (M = 3.15, SD = 2.30) target individuals, t(78) = –4.90, p < 0.001, d = 0.55 (Figure 3A). For scenario 2, White participants expressed more empathic concern toward the Black (M = 6.25, SD = 2.10) than White (M = 2.39, SD = 1.71) target individual, t(79) = 12.71, p < 0.001, d = 1.43 (Figure 3B). Other self-reported emotion ratings and statistical analyses are reported in the Supplementary material and Supplementary Table 4.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Study 3 White participants’ self-reported empathic concern ratings for (A) scenario 1 (structural racism) and (B) scenario 2 (interpersonal injustice). Error bars indicate standard error of the mean.

Four linear regressions confirmed that perspective taking scores again significantly predicted empathic concern for each target in each scenario (Scenario 1 Black target: β = 0.62, p < 0.001; Scenario 1 White target: β = 0.30, p = 0.008; Scenario 2 Black target: β = 0.24, p = 0.026; Scenario 2 White target: β = 0.56, p < 0.001).

Perceptions about racism

To compare perceptions of interpersonal and structural racism with those of Study 1, recalculated scores ranging from 1 to 100 are reported in Supplementary Table 5.

As in Study 1, participants perceived anti-Black structural racism (M = 3.42, SD = 2.17) to have significantly less of an impact than anti-Black interpersonal racism (M = 5.60, SD = 1.91) on Black African people today, t(84) = 9.94, p < 0.001, d = 1.08. Anti-Black structural racism (M = 8.75, SD = 0.80) was perceived to be of greater significance than anti-Black interpersonal racism (M = 8.26, SD = 1.30) during apartheid, t(83) = –4.06, p < 0.001, d = 0.44.

Regarding anti-White racism, participants perceived anti-White structural racism (M = 4.26, SD = 2.50) to have less impact than anti-White interpersonal racism (M = 4.82, SD = 2.41) on White people today, t(83) = 2.51, p = 0.014, d = 0.27. Importantly, we again observed heightened perceptions of racism experienced by the historically advantaged ingroup (anti-White racism) compared to the historically disadvantaged outgroup (anti-Black racism) for structural racism today, t(83) = 2.18, p = 0.031, d = 0.24. For interpersonal racism, however, Black African people were perceived to be more affected than White people today, t(84) = –2.22, p = 0.030, d = –0.24. Feelings of competitive victimhood for perceived structural racism thus also featured in this student sample.

To examine whether perceived structural racism against Black African people today would predict intergroup empathy bias in scenario 1 (opposition to structural racism), we conducted two simultaneous regressions. The first tested how well perceptions of anti-Black structural versus interpersonal racism today predicted intergroup empathy bias (ingroup empathic concern - outgroup empathic concern) for scenario 1, whereas the second tested how well these perceptions of racism predicted intergroup empathy bias for scenario 2 (see Table 4). As hypothesized, reduced perceptions of anti-Black structural racism significantly predicted greater intergroup empathy bias for scenario 1 (structural racism; B = –0.50, p = 0.016, boot SE = 0.21, 95% CI [–0.89, –0.11]). By comparison, reduced perceptions of anti-Black interpersonal racism significantly predicted intergroup empathy bias for scenario 2 (interpersonal injustice; B = –0.38, p = 0.042, boot SE = 0.21, 95% CI [–0.78, –0.01]).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Simultaneous regressions predicting intergroup empathy bias as a function of perceptions of anti-Black structural and anti-Black interpersonal racism: Study 3.

Intergroup interaction around historical oppression

To examine whether intergroup empathy bias, previously shown to directly motivate intergroup behavior (Bruneau et al., 2017), would mediate the effect of perceived anti-Black structural racism on willingness to engage in intergroup interaction around historical oppression, we conducted an exploratory mediation analysis. We conducted this analysis using Process v.4.1 Model 4 (Hayes, 2018). The model was specified with perceived anti-Black structural racism today as the independent variable, intergroup empathy bias for scenario 1 (structural racism) as the mediator variable, and intergroup interaction (M = 5.32, SD = 2.48) as the dependent variable (Figure 4). Indeed, the positive association between perceived anti-Black structural racism today and intergroup interaction (B = 0.36, p = 0.003, boot SE = 0.12, 95% CI [0.34,0.58]) was rendered non-significant when intergroup empathy bias was added as a mediator (B = –0.28, p < 0.001, boot SE = 0.09, 95% CI [–0.45, –0.10]), indicating full mediation (Indirect effect: B = 0.18, boot SE = 0.08, 95% CI [0.06,0.35]). Therefore, amongst participants with reduced perceptions of anti-Black structural racism today there was greater intergroup empathy bias for a situation portraying opposition to structural racism, which in turn, was associated with reduced willingness to engage in intergroup dialog around people’s lived experiences of historical oppression.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Intergroup empathy bias for situations of structural racism (scenario 1) fully mediated the effect of perceptions of anti-Black structural racism today on willingness to engage in intergroup interaction about historical oppression (R2 = 0.23, p < 0.001). Data presented are standardized β coefficients. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Discussion

Study 3 replicated the effects observed in Studies 1 and 2, showing that the current sample perceived the impact of anti-Black structural racism to be less severe than that of anti-Black interpersonal racism, and that outgroup perspective taking and empathic concern in response to the scenario portraying structural racism was compromised. In addition, our analyses suggest that perceptions of anti-Black structural racism function to predict intergroup empathy bias in response to situations where Black people challenge an unjust system. By contrast, perceptions of anti-Black interpersonal racism predicted intergroup empathy bias in interpersonal contexts.

Because the setting of scenario 1 (opposition to structural racism) changed from Study 2 to 3, the results here suggest that White participant responses were not driven primarily by personal material interests. Rather, it provides support for our interpretation that opposition to structural racism could be symbolically threatening to White identity, because such resistance underscores conflicting beliefs between groups about the prevalence of structural racism (Riek et al., 2006). This interpretation is consistent with previous research suggesting that members of advantaged groups may experience threat when faced with the possibility of change to systems that benefit them, particularly when power relations are unstable (Saguy and Kteily, 2014). Such threat, in turn, often has the effect of raising support for ideologies that legitimize the racial status quo (Knowles et al., 2009; Jost et al., 2013). The present research extends these findings by showing that avoidance, denial, or lack of understanding of such realities of continuing structural racism is associated with reduced outgroup perspective taking and empathic concern when the unequal status quo is being challenged.

As in Study 1, perceptions of anti-Black interpersonal racism far exceeded those of anti-Black structural racism. Perceived anti-White interpersonal and structural racism were less elevated than in Study 1, however. This latter finding might reflect the younger age of the present student sample, as age has also previously been associated with more progressive outcomes in the South African context (Fourie and Verwoerd, 2022). Nevertheless, participants reported significantly greater perceptions of anti-White than anti-Black structural racism today, which suggests that competitive victimhood was also present in this younger sample.

Finally, Study 3 demonstrated that increased intergroup empathy bias in response to structural racism was associated with reduced willingness to engage in potentially challenging, yet important, cross-racial conversations about Black South Africans’ lived experiences of oppression during and after apartheid. Mediation analysis showed that participants with reduced perceptions of anti-Black structural racism responded with greater intergroup empathy bias, which in turn, was associated with reduced willingness to attend a cross-racial event to discuss historical injustices. Such encounters may allow for mutual understanding of the lived realities of people representing different historical perspectives, which has the potential to strengthen empathic connection and restore intergroup relations (Gobodo-Madikizela, 2008; Fourie et al., 2017). Consistent with previous findings, intergroup empathy bias thus served as a direct psychological motivator (barrier) of intergroup behavior (Bruneau et al., 2017).

General discussion

Across three studies our data demonstrated significant discrepancies in White South African participants’ perceptions of anti-Black structural versus interpersonal racism, and that their reduced perceptions of anti-Black structural racism predicted difficulties in recognizing and empathizing with Black African people opposing structural racism. Intergroup empathy bias in response to the scenario depicting opposition to structural racism was furthermore associated with reduced willingness to engage in cross-racial discussions about historical injustices. While various motivated psychological processes and structural factors may contribute to limited recognition and/or insight into structural racism (Kraus et al., 2019), advantaged group members might also experience opposition to existing disparities as threatening to their moral integrity and privilege (Siem et al., 2013; Saguy and Kteily, 2014; Young and Sullivan, 2016). People of historically advantaged groups, such as White South Africans, might therefore be invested in contesting those who challenge current status arrangements, thereby disrupting perspective taking and empathic concern for them.

These findings are relevant because impaired outgroup perspective taking has far-reaching consequences in contexts that require recognition of intergroup inequalities (Todd et al., 2012; Todd and Galinsky, 2014). Appreciating another’s inner state endows them with moral rights and gives meaning to their actions (Waytz et al., 2010; Gray et al., 2012). Without perspective taking, it is a small leap toward perceiving outgroup members who advocate for structural change as a backward, monolithic mass (Kelman, 1973). Our results furthermore support the indelible relationship between recognizing experiences of structural racism and feeling empathic concern toward those who suffer its consequences.

The present research in the South African context corroborates existing accounts showing that the structural dimensions of racism are often minimized or silenced altogether by advantaged group members, despite its pervasive and visible effects (Milazzo, 2016; Cornell and Kessi, 2017; Salter et al., 2017; Albertus, 2019; Rucker and Richeson, 2021). Of significance, is that White participants’ reduced perceptions of anti-Black structural racism in our data were coupled with increased perceptions of anti-White structural racism; this while Whiteness continues to determine and mark privilege and power (Ratele and Laubscher, 2010; Chatterjee, 2019). These findings suggest that competitive victimhood poses a challenge for positive race relations in South Africa.

The needs-based model of reconciliation (Nadler and Shnabel, 2015) postulates that intergroup conflict differentially impacts the identities of victims and perpetrators, such that victims experience a need for empowerment, whereas perpetrators are motivated to restore their moral image. Motivations to restore these identity dimensions are particularly urgent in contexts where group disparity is salient and perceived as resulting from unjust structural violence (long-standing social arrangements that privilege some groups at the expense of others; Fiske et al., 2002; Siem et al., 2013).

Claiming victim status is a powerful strategy to restore a perpetrator group’s moral integrity. Indeed, the threatened identity and reversed moral standing of Whiteness post-apartheid provide ample impetus for White people to engage in competitive victimhood (Steyn, 2001). Various right-wing movements have capitalized on the notion that White South Africans, as a minority, are systematically oppressed and excluded by the ruling government−employing discursive strategies of victimhood and marginalization to bolster support (Van Zyl-Hermann, 2018). Such strategies, however, serve to preserve power and privilege of White people, while denying responsibility for the historical harm suffered by Black people (collectively speaking) and its continuing legacies (Verwey and Quayle, 2012; Noor et al., 2017).

The present work was not intended to reduce the complexities inherent to the racialized realities in South Africa (and beyond) to one underlying fracture, but rather to highlight one mechanism whereby a priori perceptions and motivated processes may give rise to empathic failures. If historically advantaged group members struggle to recognize or apprehend the magnitude of intergenerational structural racism and its impact on Black people’s lives, they are also unlikely to respond with empathic concern and allyship in situations where Black people challenge ongoing structural racism. Recent pushback against the international Black Lives Matter movement with slogans such as “White Lives Matter” or “All Lives Matter,” which devalue the racial injustices of the present political moment, serve as a case in point (Smith, 2017).

Our findings further suggest that intervening at the level of perceptions about structural racism should constitute a key mission for practitioners and policy makers to curb empathic failures or antipathy, and to inspire collective action. This should include instilling an understanding of racism firstly as large-scale structural and cultural forces that maintain racialized patterns of power and privilege, before it becomes interpersonal (Daumeyer et al., 2017; Salter et al., 2017; Kraus et al., 2019). Such messaging may include the continuing asymmetry in Black and White people’s experiences of institutional spaces, their unequal access to wealth and resources (e.g., health, education, justice), and the realities of low social mobility (Cornell and Kessi, 2017; Day and Fiske, 2017; McCall et al., 2017).

Understandings of structural racism should furthermore be embedded within a nuanced context of intergenerational structural violence, and advantaged group members’ role in perpetuating currently unjust realities, to refute assumptions that continuing inequality is a result of legitimate individual-level characteristics (Hetey and Eberhardt, 2018). These insights will additionally contribute to curbing perceptions of White victimhood (Green et al., 2017). Ultimately, the goal of interventions should not be to cultivate empathy in isolation. Rather, it should be to foster recognition of the systemic practices that individually and collectively reproduce structural racism in order to increase empathy-positive norms and garner support for and engagement in social justice (Zaki and Cikara, 2015; Durrheim and Dixon, 2018; Rothberg, 2019).

Limitations

While one strength of the scenarios paradigm we employed is the assessment of spontaneous perspective taking in vivo (i.e., participants responded without overt instructions to take any party’s perspective), we also recognize potential limitations to it. First, because our study design was cross-sectional and correlational in nature, the causal direction of the observed associations between perspective taking and empathic concern in Studies 2 and 3 could not be established. Indeed, it could be that empathic concern predicted perspective taking (rather than the other way around). Nevertheless, we argue that the link from understanding a target’s perspective to empathic concern (compassion) for the target is most consistent with previous research, particularly in the absence of salient visual cues that might trigger concern, as in the present text-driven scenarios (Stietz et al., 2019).

Second, the scenarios were not balanced on all variables, such as the number of people involved, and the target-relevant information participants had (Todd and Galinsky, 2014). Though we have argued that situations of structural racism by nature would be more likely to involve groups than situations of interpersonal racism, scenario 1 (Studies 2 and 3) also dedicated somewhat more time to the White targets’ actions than the other scenarios (equal to that of the Black targets in scenario 1). Given the role of perspective taking in empathy production (Batson et al., 1997), this could have influenced participants’ perspective taking toward those White targets. We do not believe this somewhat unequal distribution of target-relevant information across the scenarios would have had a significant bearing on the principal contribution of this work, however, namely that advantaged group members responded with reduced perspective taking and empathic concern toward disadvantaged outgroup members challenging structural racism. Indeed, Black African participants’ responses showed the opposite effect in scenario 1. Still, future work of this nature should incorporate scenarios where target-relevant information is balanced.

Finally, we also note that a larger sample of Black African participants would give greater credence to our interpretation of their responses in Study 2 and should be pursued in future research. We were reassured by the fact that the present Black African sample was sufficiently powered to draw conclusions from their quantitative data, however. Furthermore, inspection of their qualitative scripts consistently showed Black African participants’ deeply felt concern for and understanding of the outsourced workers’ actions in scenario 1 as a protest against systemic racism and oppressive policies.

Conclusion

Despite growing racialized inequality, White South African participants demonstrated reduced perceptions of anti-Black structural versus interpersonal racism, which not only contributed to reduced empathic concern for those opposing structural racism, but also predicted less willingness to engage in reconciliatory cross-racial dialogs. Such inaccurate perceptions of racism protect the racial status quo while allowing scope for competitive victimhood processes to flourish. We contend that without penetrating lay beliefs about the extent of structural inequities, it will remain hard for some White people to see beyond our privilege to empathize with and address the unequal legacies of apartheid and beyond.

Positionality statement

Mindful that social identities can influence one’s approach to research and interpretation of data (Roberts et al., 2020), the authors wish to provide the reader with potentially relevant information about their backgrounds. With respect to race and gender, both MF and SM-B identify as White and female. With respect to nationality, MF identifies as South African, and SM-B identifies as United States American. While conscious of the intersectional complexity and heterogeneity within socially constructed racial categories, we believe referring to Black African and White people is warranted, without intending to oversimplify, given the legacies of systematic racial discrimination under colonialism and apartheid in South Africa.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Stellenbosch University’s Humanities Research Ethics Committee. The participants provided their written informed consent prior to participating in this study.

Author contributions

MF and SM-B contributed to conceptualizing the research. MF organized the data, performed the statistical analysis, and wrote and prepared the first draft of the manuscript. SM-B contributed to data interpretation and revision of the manuscript. Both authors read and approved the submitted version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by Stellenbosch University through internal research funding.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to our colleagues at the Center for the Study of the Afterlife of Violence and the Reparative Quest for the rich conversations from which this research emerged, to Michaela Hart and Landi-Chantel Meiring, who assisted with data collection and analysis, and to Emile Bruneau, who was integral in conceptualizing this work before he passed away.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.838675/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ Black here is used collectively to refer to all persons of color.
  2. ^ We wish to acknowledge that Coloured and Indian people also suffered systematic racial discrimination under the apartheid state.
  3. ^ We do not uncritically use the potentially problematic term anti-White racism, as racism does not occur in a vacuum separated from its historical context. Rather, our purpose for engaging this construct is to assess perceptions of White victimhood.

References

Albertus, R. W. (2019). Decolonisation of institutional structures in South African universities: A critical perspective. Cogent Soc. Sci. 5:1620403. doi: 10.1080/23311886.2019.1620403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ansell, A. E. (2004). Two nations of discourse: Mapping racial ideologies in post-apartheid South Africa. Politikon 31, 3–26. doi: 10.1080/02589340410001690783

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asmal, K. (2000). Truth, reconciliation and justice: The South African experience in perspective. Modern Law Rev. 63, 1–24.

Google Scholar

Barrett-Lennard, G. T. (1981). The empathy cycle: Refinement of a nuclear concept. J. Couns. Psychol. 28, 91–100. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.28.2.91

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Tal, D., and Halperin, E. (2011). “Socio-psychological barriers to conflict resolution,” in Intergroup Conflicts and Their Resolution: a Social Psychological Perspective, ed. D. Bar-Tal (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 217–239.

Google Scholar

Batson, C. D. (2009). “These things called empathy: Eight related but distinct phenomena,” in The Social Neuroscience of Empathy, eds J. Decety and W. Ickes (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 3–15.

Google Scholar

Batson, C. D. (2011). Altruism in Humans. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Batson, C. D., and Ahmad, N. Y. (2009). Using empathy to improve intergroup attitudes and relations. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 3, 141–177. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-2409.2009.01013.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batson, C. D., Early, S., and Salvarani, G. (1997). Perspective taking: Imagining how another feels versus imaging how you would feel. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 23, 751–758. doi: 10.1177/0146167297237008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentrovato, D., Mari, S., Durante, F., and Wassermann, J. (2020). “Collective victimhood resulting from structural violence,” in The Social Psychology of Collective Victimhood, ed. J. R. Vollhardt (Oxford: University Press), 231–251.

Google Scholar

Blick, B., Nakabugo, S., Garabedian, L. F., Seru, M., and Trap, B. (2018). Evaluating inter-rater reliability of indicators to assess performance of medicines management in health facilities in Uganda. J. Pharm. Policy Pract. 11:11. doi: 10.1186/s40545-018-0137-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonam, C. M., Nair Das, V., Coleman, B. R., and Salter, P. (2019). Ignoring history, denying racism: Mounting evidence for the Marley hypothesis and epistemologies of ignorance. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 10, 257–265. doi: 10.1177/1948550617751583

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bornman, E. (2006). National symbols and nation-building in the post-apartheid South Africa. Int. J. Interc. Relat. 30, 383–399. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2005.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Branscombe, N. R., Ellemers, N., Spears, R., and Doosje, B. (1999). “The context and content of social identity threat,” in Social Identity: Context, Commitment, Content, eds N. Ellemers, R. Spears, and B. Doosje (Oxford: Blackwell Science), 35–58.

Google Scholar

Bruneau, E. G., Cikara, M., and Saxe, R. (2015). Minding the Gap: Narrative descriptions about mental states attenuate parochial empathy. PLoS One 10:e0140838. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0140838

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruneau, E. G., Cikara, M., and Saxe, R. (2017). Parochial empathy predicts reduced altruism and the endorsement of passive harm. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 8, 934–942. doi: 10.1177/1948550617693064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Čehajić, S., Brown, R., and González, R. (2009). What do I care? Perceived ingroup responsibility and dehumanization as predictors of empathy felt for the victim group. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 12, 715–729. doi: 10.1177/1368430209347727

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cehajic-Clancy, S., Goldenberg, A., Gross, J., and Halperin, E. (2016). Social-psychological interventions for intergroup reconciliation: An emotion regulation perspective. Psychol. Inquiry 27:73. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2016.1153945

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chatterjee, A. (2019). Measuring wealth inequality in South Africa: An agenda. Dev. S. Afr. 36, 839–859. doi: 10.1080/0376835X.2019.1690977

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cikara, M. (2015). Intergroup schadenfreude: Motivating participation in collective violence. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 3, 12–17. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.12.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cikara, M., Bruneau, E. G., and Saxe, R. R. (2011). Us and them: Intergroup failures of empathy. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 20, 149–153. doi: 10.1177/0963721411408713

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Claasen, B. (2020). OPINION — Why is it so hard for some white people to understand what Black Lives Matter is about?. News24. Available online at: https://www.news24.com/news24/columnists/guestcolumn/opinion-why-is-it-so -hard-for-some-white-people-to-understand-what-black-lives-matter-is-about-2 0200720

Google Scholar

Cornell, J., and Kessi, S. (2017). Black students’ experiences of transformation at a previously “white only” South African university: A photovoice study. Ethnic Racial Stud. 40, 1882–1899. doi: 10.1080/01419870.2016.1206586

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, M. A., and Richeson, J. A. (2014). On the precipice of a “Majority-Minority” America: Perceived status threat from the racial demographic shift affects White Americans’ political ideology. Psychol. Sci. 25, 1189–1197. doi: 10.1177/0956797614527113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daumeyer, N. M., Rucker, J. M., and Richeson, J. A. (2017). Thinking structurally about implicit bias: Some peril, lots of promise. Psychol. Inquiry 28, 258–261. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2017.1373556

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Day, M. V., and Fiske, S. T. (2017). Movin’ on up? How perceptions of social mobility affect our willingness to defend the system. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 8, 267–274. doi: 10.1177/1948550616678454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durrheim, K., and Dixon, J. (2010). Racial contact and change in South Africa. J. Soc. Issues 66, 273–288. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2010.01645.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durrheim, K., and Dixon, J. (2018). “Intergroup contact and the struggle for social justice,” in Oxford Handbook of Social Psychology and Social Justice, ed. P. L. Hammack (Oxford: Oxford University Press). doi: 10.1177/0146167213477155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Efron, B. (1987). Better bootstrap confidence intervals. J. Am. Statist. Assoc. 82, 171–185. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1987.10478410

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., Glick, P., and Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 82, 878–902. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fourie, M. M., and Verwoerd, W. J. (2022). COVID-19 as natural intervention: Guilt and perceived historical privilege contributes to structural reform under conditions of crisis. Affect. Sci. 3, 34–45. doi: 10.1007/s42761-021-00073-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fourie, M. M., Deist, M., and Moore-Berg, S. L. (2022). Hierarchies of being human: Intergroup dehumanization and its implication in present-day South Africa. Peace Conflict. [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.1037/pac0000616

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fourie, M. M., Gobodo-Madikizela, P., and Stein, D. J. (2013). “Empathy and forgiveness in South Africa: The Truth and Reconciliation Commission,” in Hurting Memories and Beneficial Forgetting, eds M. Linden and K. Rutkowski (London: Elsevier Inc), 227–240.

Google Scholar

Fourie, M. M., Subramoney, S., and Gobodo-Madikizela, P. (2017). “A less attractive feature of empathy: Intergroup empathy bias,” in Empathy: an Evidence-based Interdisciplinary Perspective, ed. M. Kondo. (Norderstedt: Books on Demand), 45–61.

Google Scholar

Gobodo-Madikizela, P. (2008). Trauma, forgiveness and the witnessing dance: Making public spaces intimate. J. Anal. Psychol. 53, 169–188. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5922.2008.00715.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, K., Young, L., and Waytz, A. (2012). Mind perception is the essence of morality. Psychol. Inquiry 23, 101–124. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2012.651387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, E. G. T., Visintin, E. P., Hristova, A., Bozhanova, A., Pereira, A., and Staerklé, C. (2017). Collective victimhood and acknowledgement of outgroup suffering across history: Majority and minority perspectives. Eur. J. Soc Psychol. 47, 228–240. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2237

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, J., and Kosloff, S. (2008). Terror Management Theory: Implications for understanding prejudice, stereotyping, intergroup conflict, and political attitudes. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2, 1881–1894. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00144.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halabi, S., Dovidio, J. F., and Nadler, A. (2008). When and how do high status group members offer help: Effects of social dominance orientation and status threat. Polit. Psychol. 29, 841–858. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00669.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, L. T., and Fiske, S. T. (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the low: Neuroimaging responses to extreme out-groups. Psychol. Sci. 17, 847–853. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01793.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Hetey, R. C., and Eberhardt, J. L. (2018). The numbers don’t speak for themselves: Racial disparities and the persistence of inequality in the criminal justice system. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 27, 183–187. doi: 10.1177/0963721418763931

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hewstone, M., Rubin, M., and Willis, H. (2002). Intergroup bias. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 575–604. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hino, H., Leibbrandt, M., Machema, R., Shifa, M., and Soudien, C. (2018). Identity, Inequality and Social Contestation in the Post-Apartheid South Africa. SALDRU Working Paper No. 233 Cape Town: SALDRU, UCT.

Google Scholar

Jones, J. (2004). “The impairment of empathy in goodwill whites for african americans,” in What White looks like: African-American Philosophers on the Whiteness Question, ed. G. Yancy (Milton Park: Routledge).

Google Scholar

Jost, J. T. (2019). A quarter century of system justification theory: Questions, answers, criticisms, and societal applications. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 58, 263–314. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., and Nosek, B. A. (2004). A decade of system justification theory: Accumulated evidence of conscious and unconscious bolstering of the status quo. Polit. Psychol. 25, 881–919. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00402.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jost, J., Liviatan, I., Toorn, J., Ledgerwood, A., Mandisodza, A., and Nosek, B. (2013). “System justification: A motivational process with implications for social conflict,” in Justice and Conflicts: Theoretical and Empirical Contributions: Theoretical and Empirical Contributions, eds E. Kals and J. Maes (Berlin: Springer-Verlag), 315–327.

Google Scholar

Kelman, H. G. (1973). Violence without moral restraint: Reflections on the dehumanization of victims and victimizers. J. Soc. Issues 29, 25–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1973.tb00102.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knowles, E. D., Lowery, B. S., Hogan, C. M., and Chow, R. M. (2009). On the malleability of ideology: Motivated construals of color blindness. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 96, 857–869. doi: 10.1037/a0013595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraus, M. W., Onyeador, I. N., Daumeyer, N. M., Rucker, J. M., and Richeson, J. A. (2019). The misperception of racial economic inequality. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 14, 899–921. doi: 10.1177/1745691619863049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraus, M. W., Rucker, J. M., and Richeson, J. A. (2017). Americans misperceive racial economic equality. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 10324–10331. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1707719114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kteily, N., Bruneau, E., Waytz, A., and Cotterill, S. (2015). The ascent of man: Theoretical and empirical evidence for blatant dehumanization. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 109, 901–931. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipsitz, G. (2011). How Racism Takes Place. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.

Google Scholar

McCall, L., Burk, D., Laperrière, M., and Richeson, J. A. (2017). Exposure to rising inequality shapes Americans’ opportunity beliefs and policy support. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 9593–9598. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1706253114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McHugh, M. L. (2012). Interrater reliability: The kappa statistic. Biochem. Med. 22, 276–282.

Google Scholar

McMichael, C. (2020). The hallucinatory bunker of the white right: The anti-Black LivesMatter backlash in South Africa highlights the growing ideological convergence between the far right and conservatives. Africa is a country. Available online at: https://africasacountry.com/2020/10/the-hallucinatory-bunker-of-the-white-right

Google Scholar

McNeill, A., and Vollhardt, J. R. (2020). “We All Suffered!” – The role of power in rhetorical strategies of inclusive victimhood and its consequences for intergroup relations,” in the Social Psychology of Collective Victimhood, ed. J. R. Vollhardt (New York, NY: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Meier, B. P., and Hinsz, V. B. (2004). A comparison of human aggression committed by groups and individuals: An interindividual–intergroup discontinuity. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 40, 551–559. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milazzo, M. (2016). On white ignorance, white shame, and other pitfalls in critical philosophy of race. J. Appl. Philos. 34, 557–572. doi: 10.1111/japp.12230

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nadler, A., and Shnabel, N. (2015). Intergroup reconciliation: Instrumental and socio-emotional processes and the needs-based model. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 26, 93–125. doi: 10.1080/10463283.2015.1106712

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, J. C., Adams, G., and Salter, P. S. (2013). The Marley hypothesis: Denial of racism reflects ignorance of history. Psychol. Sci. 24, 213–218. doi: 10.1177/0956797612451466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noor, M., James Brown, R., and Prentice, G. (2008). Precursors and mediators of intergroup reconciliation in Northern Ireland: A new model. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 47, 481–495. doi: 10.1348/014466607x238751

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noor, M., Vollhardt, J. R., Mari, S., and Nadler, A. (2017). The social psychology of collective victimhood. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 47, 121–134. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2300

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connor, C., and Joffe, H. (2020). Intercoder reliability in qualitative research: Debates and practical guidelines. Int. J. Qual. Methods 19:1609406919899220. doi: 10.1177/1609406919899220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Brien, L. T., Blodorn, A., Alsbrooks, A., Dube, R., Adams, G., and Nelson, J. C. (2009). Understanding White Americans’ perceptions of racism in Hurricane Katrina-related events. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 12, 431–444. doi: 10.1177/1368430209105047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pager, D., and Shepherd, H. (2008). The sociology of discrimination: Racial discrimination in employment, housing, credit, and consumer markets. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 34, 181–209. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.33.040406.131740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posel, D. (2001). Race as common sense: Racial classification in twentieth-century South Africa. Afr. Stud. Rev. 44, 87–113. doi: 10.2307/525576

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratele, K., and Laubscher, L. (2010). Making white lives: Neglected meanings of whiteness from apartheid South Africa. Psychol. Soc. 40, 83–99.

Google Scholar

Riek, B. M., Mania, E. W., and Gaertner, S. L. (2006). Intergroup threat and outgroup attitudes: A meta-analytic review. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 10, 336–353. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1004_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, S. O., Bareket-Shavit, C., Dollins, F. A., Goldie, P. D., and Mortenson, E. (2020). Racial inequality in psychological research: Trends of the past and recommendations for the future. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 15, 1295–1309. doi: 10.1177/1745691620927709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothberg, M. (2019). The Implicated Subject: Beyond Victims and Perpetrators. Standford: Standford University Press.

Google Scholar

Rucker, J. M., and Richeson, J. A. (2021). Toward an understanding of structural racism: Implications for criminal justice. Science 374, 286–290. doi: 10.1126/science.abj7779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rucker, J., Duker, A., and Richeson, J. (2019). Structurally unjust: How Lay Beliefs About Racism Relate to Perceptions of and Responses to Racial Inequality in Criminal Justice. Available online at: http://psyarxiv.com/sjkeq (accessed August 26, 2020).

Google Scholar

Saguy, T., and Kteily, N. (2014). Power, negotiations, and the anticipation of intergroup encounters. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 25, 107–141. doi: 10.1080/10463283.2014.957579

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saguy, T., Chernyak-Hai, L., Andrighetto, L., and Bryson, J. (2013). When the powerful feels wronged: The legitimization effects of advantaged group members’ sense of being accused for harboring racial or ethnic biases. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 43, 292–298. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1948

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salter, P. S., Adams, G., and Perez, M. J. (2017). Racism in the structure of everyday worlds: A cultural-psychological perspective. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 27, 150–155. doi: 10.1177/0963721417724239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shih, M., Wang, E., Trahan Bucher, A., and Stotzer, R. (2009). Perspective taking: Reducing prejudice towards general outgroups and specific individuals. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 12, 565–577. doi: 10.1177/1368430209337463

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siem, B., Oettingen, M., Mummendey, A., and Nadler, A. (2013). When status differences are illegitimate, groups’ needs diverge: Testing the needs-based model of reconciliation in contexts of status inequality. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 43, 137–148. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1929

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, D. (2017). The backlash against Black Lives Matter is just more evidence of injustice. The conversation. Available online at: https://theconversation.com/the-backlash-against-black-lives-matter-is-just-more-evidence-of-injustice-85587

Google Scholar

Sommers, S. R., and Norton, M. I. (2006). Lay theories about White racists: What constitutes racism (and what doesn’t). Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 9, 117–138. doi: 10.1177/1368430206059881

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sparkman, D. J., and Eidelman, S. (2016). “Putting myself in their shoes”: Ethnic perspective taking explains liberal–conservative differences in prejudice and stereotyping. Personal. Individ. Diff. 98, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephan, W. G., and Finlay, K. (1999). The role of empathy in improving intergroup relations. J. Soc. Issues 55, 729–743. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephan, W. G., Ybarra, O., and Morrison, K. R. (2009). “Intergroup threat theory,” in Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping, and Discrimination, ed. T. D. Nelson (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 43–59.

Google Scholar

Steyn, M. (2001). ’Whiteness just isn’t what it used to be’: White identity in a changing South Africa. Albany: State University of New York Press.

Google Scholar

Steyn, M. (2012). The ignorance contract: Recollections of apartheid childhoods and the construction of epistemologies of ignorance. Identities 19, 8–25. doi: 10.1080/1070289X.2012.672840

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steyn, M., and Foster, D. (2008). Repertoires for talking white: Resistant whiteness in post-apartheid South Africa. Ethnic Racial Stud. 31, 25–51. doi: 10.1080/01419870701538851

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stietz, J., Jauk, E., Krach, S., and Kanske, P. (2019). Dissociating empathy from perspective-taking: Evidence from intra- and inter-individual differences research. Front. Psychiatry 10:126. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stotland, E. (1969). “Exploratory investigations of empathy,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, ed. L. Berkowitz (New York, NY: Academic Press), 271–314.

Google Scholar

Sullivan, D., Landau, M. J., Branscombe, N. R., and Rothschild, Z. K. (2012). Competitive victimhood as a response to accusations of ingroup harm doing. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 102, 778–795. doi: 10.1037/a0026573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swartz, S. (2016). Another country: Everyday social restitution. Cape Town: HSRC Press.

Google Scholar

Tarrant, M., Calitri, R., and Weston, D. (2012). Social identification structures the effects of perspective taking. Psychol. Sci. 23, 973–978. doi: 10.1177/0956797612441221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todd, A. R., and Galinsky, A. D. (2014). Perspective-taking as a strategy for improving intergroup relations: Evidence, mechanisms, and qualifications. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 8, 374–387. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todd, A. R., Bodenhausen, G. V., Richeson, J. A., and Galinsky, A. D. (2011). Perspective taking combats automatic expressions of racial bias. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 100, 1027–1042. doi: 10.1037/a0022308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todd, A., Bodenhausen, G., and Galinsky, A. (2012). Perspective taking combats the denial of intergroup discrimination. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, 738–745. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.12.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tov-Nachlieli, I. S., Shnabel, N., and Nadler, A. (2013). Individuals’ and groups’ motivation to restore their impaired identity dimensions following conflicts: Evidence and implications. Soc. Psychol. 44, 129–137. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000148

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ture, K., and Hamilton, C. V. (1992). Black Power: The politics of liberation in America. New York, NY: Vintage.

Google Scholar

Unzueta, M. M., and Lowery, B. S. (2008). Defining racism safely: The role of self-image maintenance on white Americans’ conceptions of racism. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 44, 1491–1497. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.07.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Zyl-Hermann, D. (2018). Make Afrikaners great again! National populism, democracy and the new white minority politics in post-apartheid South Africa. Ethnic Racial Stud. 41, 2673–2692. doi: 10.1080/01419870.2017.1413202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verwey, C., and Quayle, M. (2012). Whiteness, racism, and Afrikaner identity in post-apartheid South Africa. Afr. Aff. 111, 551–575. doi: 10.1093/afraf/ads056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waytz, A., and Epley, N. (2012). Social connection enables dehumanization. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, 70–76. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.07.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waytz, A., Gray, K., Epley, N., and Wegner, D. M. (2010). Causes and consequences of mind perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 383–388. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, I. F., and Sullivan, D. L. (2016). Competitive victimhood: A review of the theoretical and empirical literature. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 11, 30–34. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.04.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaki, J. (2014). Empathy: A motivated account. Psychol. Bull. 140, 1608–1647. doi: 10.1037/a0037679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaki, J., and Cikara, M. (2015). Addressing empathic failures. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 24, 471–476. doi: 10.1177/0963721415599978

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: empathy, empathic concern, perspective taking, interpersonal racism, structural racism, intergroup threat, competitive victimhood\keywordbelowspace-30pt

Citation: Fourie MM and Moore-Berg SL (2022) We cannot empathize with what we do not recognize: Perceptions of structural versus interpersonal racism in South Africa. Front. Psychol. 13:838675. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.838675

Received: 18 December 2021; Accepted: 08 September 2022;
Published: 28 September 2022.

Edited by:

Kimberly Rios, Ohio University, United States

Reviewed by:

Jeff Bolles, University of North Carolina at Pembroke, United States
Rebecca Totton, Amherst College, United States

Copyright © 2022 Fourie and Moore-Berg. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Melike M. Fourie, bWFyZXRoZW1AZ21haWwuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.