Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 04 May 2022
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Rhythm in Human Cognition and Action: Health and Pathology View all 11 articles

Temporal Predictions in Space: Isochronous Rhythms Promote Forward Projections of the Body

  • Laboratoire d’Étude des Mécanismes Cognitifs, Université Lumière Lyon 2, Lyon, France

A regular rhythmic stimulation increases people’s ability to anticipate future events in time and to move their body in space. Temporal concepts are usually prescribed to spatial locations through a past-behind and future-ahead mapping. In this study, we tested the hypothesis that a regular rhythmic stimulation could promote the forward-body (i.e., toward the future) projections in the peri-personal space. In a Visual Approach/Avoidance by the Self Task (VAAST), participants (N = 24) observed a visual scene on the screen (i.e., a music studio with a metronome in the middle). They were exposed to 3 s of auditory isochronous or non-isochronous rhythms, after which they were asked to make as quickly as possible a perceptual judgment on the visual scene (i.e., whether the metronome pendulum was pointing to the right or left). The responses could trigger a forward or backward visual flow, i.e., approaching or moving them away from the scene. Results showed a significant interaction between the rhythmic stimulation and the movement projections (p < 0.001): participants were faster for responses triggering forward-body projections (but not backward-body projections) after the exposure to isochronous (but not non-isochronous) rhythm. By highlighting the strong link between isochronous rhythms and forward-body projections, these findings support the idea that temporal predictions driven by a regular auditory stimulation are grounded in a perception-action system integrating temporal and spatial information.

Introduction

Music is a dynamic process occurring over time. During music listening, we constantly generate hypotheses about what could happen next in terms of time (i.e., when expectations) and content (i.e., what expectations; Tillmann et al., 2014). While what expectations are usually manipulated through changes in melodic or harmonic sequences (i.e., the spectral structure), when predictions are mainly related to the temporal structure of music, based on repeated beats (Koelsch et al., 2019). When exposed to isochronous rhythms (i.e., auditory stimulations with temporally equidistant beats), listeners are able to implicitly predict where each beat will fall in time (Cannon and Patel, 2021). At a behavioral level, temporal predictions driven by periodic stimulation have been shown to improve performance by increasing perceptual sensitivity and reducing response latencies (e.g., Jones et al., 2002; Stefanics et al., 2010; Lawrance et al., 2014; Morillon et al., 2016).

Human ability to perceive beat is closely related to movement on both behavioral and neural levels. People show a spontaneous tendency to move (from finger tapping to entire body movement) in response to the beat of rhythmic sound (Repp, 2005; Damm et al., 2020). In particular, successful motor synchronization seems to be driven by the more regular and predictable musical sequences (Dalla Bella et al., 2013). Furthermore, brain studies highlighted that mere listening to musical beat (i.e., in the absence of movement) can activate regions of the motor system (Zatorre et al., 2007; Grahn and Rowe, 2009). Even simple human actions such as walking are coordinated and rhythmic and have been shown to operate at a spontaneous preferred tempo of ∼1.5–2 Hz (see e.g., MacDougall and Moore, 2005; Zalta et al., 2020). Moving to the beat has been shown to benefit auditory perception tasks as well (Manning and Schutz, 2013; Morillon et al., 2014). In this case, the motor system, by simulating the rhythmic actions, might provide for temporal predictions about beat times in the auditory regions (Patel and Iversen, 2014; see also Gatti et al., 2021).

Taken together, these studies suggest that a regular rhythmic stimulation, by tapping on perceptual and motor processes, promotes people’s body movement in space. A regular auditory stimulation is likely to stimulate people’s ability to anticipate future auditory events in time. Indeed, according to the dynamic attending theory (Jones, 1976; Jones and Boltz, 1989; Large and Jones, 1999), the attentional resources are not distributed continuously and equally but cyclically develop over time. By entraining internal oscillations to an external regular rhythm, temporal regularities can then guide attention. Consequently, perceivers can develop predictions about the temporal occurrence of a future event and allocate more attentional resources at the expected moment, and this in turn results in enhanced cognitive processing of an event occurring at this moment (see e.g., Jones et al., 2002; Escoffier et al., 2010; Fanuel et al., 2018).

Such time–space link becomes particularly interesting within the embodied cognition framework (Glenberg, 1997; Wilson, 2002; Barsalou, 2008; Versace et al., 2014), which considers the abstract concept of time as embedded in a perception-action system and integrated with spatial information (Lakoff and Johnson, 1980; Núñez and Cooperrider, 2013). In daily life, we tend to prescribe temporal concepts to spatial locations through a past-behind and future-ahead mapping (e.g., we move back to the past, and look forward to the future; Boroditsky, 2000). Several studies highlighted that the ability to anticipate future events or mentally travel through the past is consistently related to physically moving in space forward or backward, respectively (i.e., a front-back representation; Torralbo et al., 2006; Miles et al., 2010; Rinaldi et al., 2016; Aksentijevic et al., 2019). For example, in a word categorization task, Rinaldi et al. (2016) showed that participants were faster to step forward in response to future-than past-related words, whereas they stepped backward faster in response to past-than to future-related words.

By showing a link between temporal processes and space-motor programming employing mental time traveling paradigms (i.e., related to remembering past events or thinking about the future; Tulving, 2002), these studies support the idea that cognition is dynamically rooted in the interactions that the body maintains with the environment over time (Barsalou, 2008; Bender and Beller, 2014; Versace et al., 2014). Although the tight coupling between the temporal qualities of music and movement is well stated in literature (see Levitin et al., 2018), to the very best of our knowledge, no study so far manipulated the predictability of a rhythmic stimulation to study the link between the abstract concept of time, spatial references frames, and body movements. Therefore, we aim here at investigating whether the ability to predict future events driven by a regular auditory stimulation can specifically influence movements in space through a backward–forward mapping.

To this aim, we employed an adapted version of the Visual Approach/Avoidance by the Self Task (VAAST, Rougier et al., 2018) to test participants’ implicit forward and backward body movements following regular (i.e., isochronous) or irregular (non-isochronous) auditory rhythmic stimulations presented at different tempi. This task has been effectively used in previous studies to show the approach/avoidance compatibility effect (Solarz, 1960). Typically, participants provide faster responses to approach positive (vs. negative) stimuli and to avoid negative (vs. positive) stimuli presented in a virtual scene. Here, we used it to study whether the temporal regularity of auditory stimuli could impact the time taken to approach a visual scene or avoid it. We asked participants to make as quickly as possible a perceptual judgment on the visual scene (i.e., whether the metronome pendulum represented on the scene was pointing to the right or left). The responses could trigger a forward or backward visual flow, respectively approaching or moving them away (i.e., avoidance) from the scene.

We made the hypothesis that if temporal predictions promoted by a regular (but not irregular) rhythmic stimulations are grounded in a perception-action system integrating temporal and spatial information, and therefore are tightly related to forward body movements, then an isochronous rhythm should lead to faster responses associated to approach (i.e., forward) behavior than a less predictable, non-isochronous rhythms.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Twenty-four non-musician participants (17f, mean age = 22.08, SD = 3.19), with normal or corrected-to-normal vision and reported normal audition were included in the study. Statistical sample size analysis run on G*power, based on an effect size of 0.25 and 95% confidence interval for the effect for a 2 × 2 × 2 within subject design (see Analysis section) indicated a total sample size of 23. All participants signed a written informed consent before the participation. The study was conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration, seventh revision. Ethical review and approval were not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements.

Auditory Stimuli

Auditory stimuli consisted of 3-s (enough to entrain synchronization, Trapp et al., 2020) isochronous and non-isochronous bass drum-like rhythmic tone sequences. To control whether an isochronous-effect over body movements could be generalized to different frequencies rather than being limited to preferred natural cadences (see Zalta et al., 2020), the auditory stimuli were presented at two different tempi, namely 120 (i.e., 2 Hz, similar to the preferred natural frequency) and 240 BPM (i.e., 4 Hz). In particular, for the isochronous rhythms (2 in total), the tone was regularly presented at a tempo of 120 or 240 BPM. The non-isochronous rhythms sequences (12 in total) consisted of the same tone presented for 6 or 12 subsequent pulsations with random intervals. The onsets of the first and the last tones remained constant across sequences (i.e., 0 and 3,000 ms, respectively; Figure 1 and Table 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic representations of one isochronous (iso) and one non-isochronous (non-iso) auditory sequence, for each tempo (i.e., 120 and 240 BPM). Each vertical line represents a tone (see Table 1 for a complete description).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Description of onsets (in ms) in isochronous (iso) and non-isochronous (non-iso) tones sequences, for the 120 (a., i.e., a total of 7 tones) and 240 BPM tempi conditions (b., i.e., a total of 13 tones).

In order to control for possible affective responses associated to the auditory stimuli, participants’ ratings of arousal and valence/pleasantness were collected after the main experimental task (see section below). After each auditory sequence, participants had to indicate, by moving a cursor on a bar on the screen (values ranging from 0 to 100), how much they rated the stimulus arousing (from very relaxing to very exciting) and enjoyable (i.e., valence, from lowly enjoyable to highly enjoyable).

Visual Approach/Avoidance by the Self-Task Task

The Visual Approach/Avoidance by the Self-Task (VAAST) developed by Rougier et al. (2018) relied on sensorimotor indices provided by a realistic visual flow on the screen stimulating whole-body movements in relation to an emotional stimulus (i.e., target words). In a series of six experiments, the authors showed that participants provided faster response times to approach (via a zoom-in on the screen approaching the subject to the scene) positive stimuli and avoid (via a zoom-out moving the subject away from the scene) negative stimuli, than the reverse (also known as the approach/avoidance compatibility effect, Solarz, 1960).

While the procedure originally investigated virtual body movements in relation to emotional stimuli, here we employed it to study whether the temporal regularity (or irregularity) of auditory stimuli could impact the time taken to produce a motor response generating forward (i.e., approaching) or backward (i.e., avoiding) visual flow. In our study, participants were seated in a comfortable chair in a soundproof experimental room and wore headphones. During a training phase, they were instructed to use with the index finger of the dominant hand three adjacent buttons disposed vertically on the keyboard (see Figure 2): a middle “start” button to start each trial, and two external buttons to perform the categorization task labeled “forward” (triggering the approach) and “backward” (triggering the avoidance).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Schematic representation of the experimental procedure.

The VAAST task consisted of 120 trials: 60 presented in the isochronous and 60 in the non-isochronous condition (i.e., 30 for the 120 BPM condition, and 30 for the 240 BPM one). The trials were divided into 5 blocks of 24 trials each (i.e., 6 trials for each experimental condition for each block). For each trial, a visual scene was presented on the screen. The visual scene represented a music studio with a metronome in the middle (see Figure 2). This scene was constructed in order to be realistic and coherent with the presented auditory stimulation. Participants were instructed to fixate a white circle placed in the middle of the screen, below the needle of the metronome. As soon as participants pressed the “start” button, the 3-s auditory stimulation (i.e., isochronous or non-isochronous, in a random order) started. During the auditory stimulation, the needle of the metronome was stationary. Immediately after, the needle of the pendulum in the middle of the visual scene randomly pointed to the left or to the right. Therefore, there was no pause between the end of the auditory stimulation and the moving needle on the screen. Participants were asked to indicate as fast and as correctly as possible whether the pendulum of the metronome was pointing to the right or left by pressing on the “forward” or “backward” button. The association between forward/backward button and left/right response was counterbalanced across participants. For correct responses, according to the participants’ approach/avoidance action, the visual environment was zoomed in (i.e., approach, “forward” button) or zoomed out (i.e., avoidance, “backward” button) by 30% after each button press, thus giving the visual impression to move forward or backward as a consequence of these actions. After the zoom, the initial scene was reset on the screen for the next trial. The incorrect answers did not trigger any visual flow and were accompanied by a visual feedback of error (i.e., a red cross on black background).

Analysis

Reaction times (RTs) of correct responses to the perceptual categorization task were first computed for each participant. Outlier RT values (i.e., ±2.5 standard deviations from the mean) within each experimental condition, corresponding to the 5.49% of the data, have been excluded from the analyses. In order to test the results of the VAAST task, we run a repeated-measures ANOVA (JASP 0.11.1.0) with correct responses RTs as a dependent variable and body movement (approach vs. avoidance), rhythm (i.e., isochronous vs. non-isochronous) and tempo (i.e., 120 vs. 240 BMP) as within-subject factors. In an additional analysis, in order to test for an eventual congruency effect between left-right responses and past-future representations, we added the left/right counterbalancing (i.e., the association between forward/backward button and left/right response across participants) as between-subject factor.

In order to investigate possible difference in subjective ratings of auditory stimulation, we run repeated-measures ANOVAs with rhythm (i.e., isochronous vs. non-isochronous) and tempo (i.e., 120 vs. 240 BMP) as within-subject factors on both arousal and valence subjective ratings.

Results

RTs analysis showed a good overall performance of participants, with the 94.51% of correct responses in the left-right categorization task. Repeated-measures ANOVA on VAAST task revealed an effect of body movement, indicating that participants were faster in providing responses associated to approach rather than avoidance movements [F(1, 23) = 16.53 p < 0.001, η2 = 0.135; Figure 3]. A main effect of rhythm was also observed, with faster responses following isochronous than non-isochronous auditory stimulation [F(1, 23) = 25.77, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.145], and independently from the tempo variations (F < 1). Crucially, results showed a significant interaction between body movements and rhythm [F(1, 23) = 57.79, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.114], with participants providing faster approaching responses after isochronous than after non-isochronous auditory stimulations (t = –8.139, p < 0.001, Bonferroni-corrected), and without significant difference between rhythmic conditions in the avoidance body movement responses (t = –0.49, p = 1.000, Bonferroni-corrected). We found no significant effect of counterbalancing of forward/backward movement and right/left responses [F(1, 22) = 1.71, p = 0.204, η2 = 0.034], nor any interactions involving this factor.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Results of the VAAST task showing RTs for correct responses according to the body movement (approach/avoidance), auditory rhythmic stimulation (isochronous/non-isocrhonous) and tempo (120/240 BPM). Analysis revealed significant main effects of body movement and rhythm, as well as a significant interaction body movement × rhythm, thus indicating that participants were faster in approach (vs. avoidance) response following an isochronous (vs. non-isochronous) auditory stimulation.

The repeated-measures ANOVAs on affective ratings did not show any significant effect of rhythm and tempo on arousal (F < 1 for both rhythm and tempo) nor valence subjective ratings [for rhythm, F < 1; for tempo, F(1, 23) = 1.140, p = 0.297; see Table 2].

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Means and standard deviations (SD) of mean arousal and valence subjective ratings according to auditory stimulation (Iso, isochronous; Non-iso, non-isochronous) and tempo (120 and 240 bpm).

Discussion

In this study, we aimed at investigating whether a regular rhythmic auditory stimulation, by allowing anticipation in time, could promote forward body projections in the peri-personal space as compared to an irregular, non-isochronous rhythm. Using an approach/avoidance task in a visual virtual environment, we found that participants provided faster forward-approach (but not backward-avoidance) responses following an isochronous (but not non-isochronous) auditory stimulation. Importantly, temporal regularities promoted forward projections of body movements no matter the tempo of the auditory stimuli, thus suggesting a generalizable effect relying on the isochronous nature of the stimulation rather than on its preferred spontaneous frequency (MacDougall and Moore, 2005; Zalta et al., 2020).

Research on temporal dynamics has shown that a constant duration of intervals delineating an isochronous (rhythmic) stimulation promotes an accurate prediction about when event is likely to occur, and therefore allows attention resources to be oriented toward that moment in time (Jones and Boltz, 1989; Large and Jones, 1999; Jones et al., 2002). Such temporal predictability of isochronous sequences has been shown to consistently improve sensorimotor processing of events occurring in phase with the rhythm, thus enhancing not only perceptual sensitivity (Morillon et al., 2014) and target detection speed (Bolger et al., 2014), but also working- (Fanuel et al., 2018, 2020; Plancher et al., 2018) and long-term memory (Thavabalasingam et al., 2016) processes. Here, we considered the tight time–space relationship, according to which temporal concepts are usually prescribed to spatial locations through a past-behind and future-ahead mapping (Lakoff and Johnson, 1980; Núñez and Cooperrider, 2013). As humans, we tend to conceptualize time along the sagittal space (Bender and Beller, 2014), thus representing the past as “back” and the future as “forward” (Núñez and Cooperrider, 2013). Accordingly, many studies have shown that motor responses to past- and future-related information are significantly faster when the response direction is compatible with such sagittal mental time line (Sell and Kaschak, 2011; Rinaldi et al., 2016, 2018; Walker et al., 2017). Our findings suggest that a close link exists between temporal dynamics triggered by regular rhythms and body projections in space over such sagittal representation.

In particular, by showing for the first time that regular auditory stimulations are related to forward, but not backward body movements, our results support the idea that cognition is rooted in the interactions between the body and the environment and projected onto a temporal dimension (see Barsalou, 2008; Versace et al., 2014). From a more speculative perspective, our findings would suggest that isochronous-related benefits on perception and cognition could be based on, or at least intrinsically related to the concept of projections in space. Indeed, temporal regularities might promote the sensorimotor anticipations in space likely to benefit perceptual and cognitive processes (Barsalou, 2008; Pezzulo, 2011; Koziol et al., 2012).

In order to provide an ecological, realistic, and immersive virtual environment likely to promote participants body projections in space, we decided to employ the VAAST task (Rougier et al., 2018; see also Cereghetti et al., 2021 for an adaptation in another sensory modality). However, it is worth mentioning that the nature of the paradigm itself could suggest possible alternative interpretations of results. First, the procedure originally relied on verbal emotional stimuli and showed that participants’ responses were faster in approaching positive and avoiding negative stimuli (i.e., approach/avoidance compatibility effect, Solarz, 1960). In our study, we were interested in the impact of the temporal regularities on the time taken to produce a motor response generating a forward or backward visual flow. One might therefore argue that the faster responses following isochronous auditory stimulation are related to a more positive emotional valence associated with temporal regularities (vs. irregularities; Rougier et al., 2018). However, the affective (i.e., arousal and valence) subjective ratings provided by our participants did not show any significant difference between isochronous and non-isochronous auditory stimulations, thus excluding an approach/avoidance compatibility effect driven by hedonic-emotional responses.

Another possible interpretation of our findings relies on the fact that participants were asked to provide responses associated to approach/avoidance body movement toward the center of the screen, represented by a metronome. As the metronome is usually associated to regular rhythmic stimulation, faster approach responses following isochronous rhythms could reflect congruency effect. Further studies employing more neutral stimuli would help to disentangle the contribution of stimuli congruency in the observed findings. However and crucially, we showed that our participants were faster in approaching the metronome after an isochronous rhythm, but not in avoiding it after a non-isochronous auditory stimulation. Therefore, our results would point to a specific effect of isochronous-driven temporal prediction over body movements in space rather than a more general priming-congruency effect.

Such isochronous-driven facilitation over forward movements can also be explained by the fact that our daily body activities in space, such as walking or running, usually follow a regular rhythm and are associated with forward body movements. Many studies have shown the deep relationship between isochronous auditory sequences and ambulation. For example, convergent evidence has shown that regular beats are able to enhance perceptual and motor timings in Parkinson’s patients, in turn improving their gait (e.g., Benoit et al., 2014; Dalla Bella et al., 2017; Koshimori and Thaut, 2018). However, in our study we observed no specific facilitation of preferred walking tempo (i.e., our 120 BPM condition; see MacDougall and Moore, 2005; Zalta et al., 2020) on participants’ performance. Furthermore, the influence of time representation (i.e., backward-past, forward-future) on whole-body movements has been well documented (e.g., Rinaldi et al., 2016). It appears therefore difficult to disentangle the contribution of the motor (i.e., ambulation) and time representation (i.e., backward-past and forward-future) components in the observed findings. An interesting perspective would be to investigate, in future research, cultures in which the time representation is inverted, with a timeline representing past forward and future backward (Núñez and Sweetser, 2006; Boroditsky et al., 2011). If our results could be entirely explained by the motor-ambulation component, then this should act independently from the time–space representation, and we should observe in these cultures the same effect as the one observed in the present work. Otherwise, if the motor component works intertwined with time–space representation, then the isochronous facilitation over forward body movements should disappear or be significantly reduced.

In sum, this study shows for the first time that a tight link exists between isochronous auditory stimulations, likely to promote the anticipation of events in time, and forward-body projections in the peri-personal space. Taken together, our findings support the idea that temporal predictions driven by a regular auditory stimulation are grounded in a perception-action system integrating temporal and spatial information.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval were not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

LF, RV, and CV made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the work and the acquisition, analysis, and interpretation of data for the work. LF, RV, and GP drafted the work and revised it critically for important intellectual content. LF, RV, CV, and GP gave final approval of the version to be published and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy and integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aksentijevic, A., Brandt, K. R., Tsakanikos, E., and Thorpe, M. J. (2019). It takes me back: the mnemonic time-travel effect. Cognition 182, 242–250. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 617–645.

Google Scholar

Bender, A., and Beller, S. (2014). Mapping spatial frames of reference onto time: a review of theoretical accounts and empirical findings. Cognition 132, 342–382. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benoit, C. E., Dalla Bella, S., Farrugia, N., Obrig, H., Mainka, S., and Kotz, S. A. (2014). Musically cued gait-training improves both perceptual and motor timing in Parkinson’s disease. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:494. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolger, D., Coull, J. T., and Schön, D. (2014). Metrical rhythm implicitly orients attention in time as indexed by improved target detection and left inferior parietal activation. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 26, 593–605. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition 75, 1–28. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00073-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boroditsky, L., Fuhrman, O., and McCormick, K. (2011). Do English and Mandarin speakers think about time differently? Cognition 118, 123–129. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cannon, J. J., and Patel, A. D. (2021). How beat perception co-opts motor neurophysiology. Trends Cognit. Sci. 25, 137–150. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cereghetti, D., Faye, P., Gros, L., Mahé, L., Diaz, E., Cayeux, I., et al. (2021). Validation of new methods of using simulated whole-body movements as implicit indicators of sound and odor preferences. Front. Psychol. 12:659269. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.659269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalla Bella, S., Benoit, C. E., Farrugia, N., Keller, P. E., Obrig, H., Mainka, S., et al. (2017). Gait improvement via rhythmic stimulation in Parkinson’s disease is linked to rhythmic skills. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/srep42005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalla Bella, S., Białuńska, A., and Sowiński, J. (2013). Why movement is captured by music, but less by speech: role of temporal regularity. PLoS One 8:e71945. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damm, L., Varoqui, D., De Cock, V. C., Dalla Bella, S., and Bardy, B. (2020). Why do we move to the beat? A multi-scale approach, from physical principles to brain dynamics. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 112, 553–584. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.12.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Escoffier, N., Sheng, D. Y. J., and Schirmer, A. (2010). Unattended musical beats enhance visual processing. Acta Psychol. 135, 12–16. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanuel, L., Portrat, S., Dalla Bella, S., Tillmann, B., and Plancher, G. (2020). Temporal regularities during maintenance benefit short-term memory in the elderly. Exp. Aging Res. 46, 396–415. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2020.1776572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanuel, L., Portrat, S., Tillmann, B., and Plancher, G. (2018). Temporal regularities allow saving time for maintenance in working memory. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1424, 202–211. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gatti, D., Rinaldi, L., Ferreri, L., and Vecchi, T. (2021). The human cerebellum as a hub of the predictive brain. Brain Sci. 11:1492. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11111492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for? Behav. Brain Sci. 20, 1–55. doi: 10.1037/13002-001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grahn, J. A., and Rowe, J. B. (2009). Feeling the beat: premotor and striatal interactions in musician and nonmusicians during beat perception. J. Neurosci. 29, 7540–7548. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2018-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, M. R. (1976). Time, our lost dimension: toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychol. Rev. 83, 323–355. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.83.5.323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, M. R., and Boltz, M. (1989). Dynamic attending and responses to time. Psychol. Rev. 96, 459–491. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.96.3.459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, M. R., Moynihan, H., MacKenzie, N., and Puente, J. (2002). Temporal aspects of stimulus-driven attending in dynamic arrays. Psychol. Sci. 13, 313–319. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S., Vuust, P., and Friston, K. (2019). Predictive processes and the peculiar case of music. Trends Cognit. Sci. 23, 63–77. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koshimori, Y., and Thaut, M. H. (2018). Future perspectives on neural mechanisms underlying rhythm and music based neurorehabilitation in Parkinson’s disease. Ageing Res. Rev. 47, 133–139. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2018.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koziol, L. F., Budding, D. E., and Chidekel, D. (2012). From movement to thought: executive function, embodied cognition, and the cerebellum. Cerebellum 11, 505–525. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0321-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M. (1980). Conceptual metaphor in everyday language. J. Philos. 77, 453–486. doi: 10.2307/2025464

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Large, E. W., and Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How people track time-varying events. Psychol. Rev. 106, 119–159. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.106.1.119

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrance, E. L., Harper, N. S., Cooke, J. E., and Schnupp, J. W. (2014). Temporal predictability enhances auditory detection. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 135, EL357–EL363. doi: 10.1121/1.4879667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levitin, D. J., Grahn, J. A., and London, J. (2018). The psychology of music: rhythm and movement. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 69, 51–75. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDougall, H. G., and Moore, S. T. (2005). Marching to the beat of the same drummer: the spontaneous tempo of human locomotion. J. Appl. Physiol. 99, 1164–1173. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00138.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manning, F., and Schutz, M. (2013). “Moving to the beat” improves timing perception. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 1133–1139. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0439-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miles, L., Nind, L., and Macrae, C. (2010). Moving through time. Psychol. Sci. 21, 222–223.

Google Scholar

Morillon, B., Schroeder, C. E., and Wyart, V. (2014). Motor contributions to the temporal precision of auditory attention. Nat. Commun. 5, 1–9. doi: 10.1038/ncomms6255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morillon, B., Schroeder, C. E., Wyart, V., and Arnal, L. H. (2016). Temporal prediction in lieu of periodic stimulation. J. Neurosci. 36, 2342–2347. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0836-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Núñez, R., and Cooperrider, K. (2013). The tangle of space and time in human cognition. Trends Cognit. Sci. 17, 220–229. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Núñez, R. E., and Sweetser, E. (2006). With the future behind them: convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognit. Sci. 30, 401–450. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_62

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, A. D., and Iversen, J. R. (2014). The evolutionary neuroscience of musical beat perception: the Action Simulation for Auditory Prediction (ASAP) hypothesis. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:57. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pezzulo, G. (2011). Grounding procedural and declarative knowledge in sensorimotor anticipation. Mind Lang. 26, 78–114.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Plancher, G., Lévêque, Y., Fanuel, L., Piquandet, G., and Tillmann, B. (2018). Boosting maintenance in working memory with temporal regularities. J. Exp. Psychol. 44, 812–818. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Repp, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: a review of the tapping literature. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12, 969–992. doi: 10.3758/bf03206433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rinaldi, L., Locati, F., Parolin, L., Bernardi, N. F., and Girelli, L. (2016). Walking on a mental time line: temporal processing affects step movements along the sagittal space. Cortex 78, 170–173. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rinaldi, L., Vecchi, T., Fantino, M., Merabet, L. B., and Cattaneo, Z. (2018). The ego-moving metaphor of time relies on visual experience: no representation of time along the sagittal space in the blind. J. Exp. Psychol. 147, 444–450. doi: 10.1037/xge0000373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rougier, M., Muller, D., Ric, F., Alexopoulos, T., Batailler, C., Smeding, A., et al. (2018). A new look at sensorimotor aspects in approach/avoidance tendencies: the role of visual whole-body movement information. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 76, 42–53. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.12.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sell, A. J., and Kaschak, M. P. (2011). Processing time shifts affects the execution of motor responses. Brain Lang. 117, 39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solarz, A. K. (1960). Latency of instrumental responses as a function of compatibility with the meaning of eliciting verbal signs. J. Exp. Psychol. 59, 239–245. doi: 10.1037/h0047274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stefanics, G., Hangya, B., Hernadi, I., Winkler, I., Lakatos, P., and Ulbert, I. (2010). Phase entrainment of human delta oscillations can mediate the effects of expectation on reaction speed. J. Neurosci. 30, 13578–13585. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0703-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thavabalasingam, S., O’Neil, E. B., Zeng, Z., and Lee, A. C. (2016). Recognition memory is improved by a structured temporal framework during encoding. Front. Psychol. 6:2062. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tillmann, B., Poulin-Charronnat, B., and Bigand, E. (2014). The role of expectation in music: from the score to emotions and the brain. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. 5, 105–113. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torralbo, A., Santiago, J., and Lupiáñez, J. (2006). Flexible conceptual projection of time onto spatial frames of reference. Cognit. Sci. 30, 745–757. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_67

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trapp, S., Havlicek, O., Schirmer, A., and Keller, P. E. (2020). When the rhythm disappears and the mind keeps dancing: sustained effects of attentional entrainment. Psychol. Res. 84, 81–87. doi: 10.1007/s00426-018-0983-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 1–25. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Versace, R., Vallet, G. T., Riou, B., Lesourd, M., Labeye, E., and Brunel, L. (2014). Act-In: an integrated view of memory mechanisms. J. Cognit. Psychol. 26, 280–306. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2017.1387101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, E. J., Bergen, B. K., and Núñez, R. (2017). The spatial alignment of time: differences in alignment of deictic and sequence time along the sagittal and lateral axes. Acta Psychol. 175, 13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 9, 625–636. doi: 10.3758/bf03196322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zalta, A., Petkoski, S., and Morillon, B. (2020). Natural rhythms of periodic temporal attention. Nat. Commun. 11:1051. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-14888-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zatorre, R. J., Chen, J. L., and Penhune, V. B. (2007). When the brain plays music: auditory–motor interactions in music perception and production. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 547–558. doi: 10.1038/nrn2152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: temporal prediction, isochronous rhythm, VAAST, body projection, embodied cognition

Citation: Ferreri L, Versace R, Victor C and Plancher G (2022) Temporal Predictions in Space: Isochronous Rhythms Promote Forward Projections of the Body. Front. Psychol. 13:832322. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.832322

Received: 09 December 2021; Accepted: 17 March 2022;
Published: 04 May 2022.

Edited by:

Benedikt Zoefel, UMR 5549 Centre de Recherche Cerveau et Cognition (CerCo), France

Reviewed by:

Anna Fiveash, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), France
Jonathan Cannon, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, United States

Copyright © 2022 Ferreri, Versace, Victor and Plancher. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gaën Plancher, gaen.plancher@univ-lyon2.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.