Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 25 February 2022
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Motivational Development in Current Educational Contexts View all 28 articles

Exploring Gameful Motivation of Autonomous Learners

  • 1Department of Music, Art and Culture Studies, University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylä, Finland
  • 2School of Economics, Centre for Collaborative Research, University of Turku, Turku, Finland

In this explorative study, we investigated motives of autonomous learners to participate in an online course, and how these motives are related to gameplay motivations, engagement in the course experience, and learning outcomes. The guiding premise for the study has been the idea that learning and game playing carry phenomenal similarities that could be revealed by scrutinizing motives for participating in a massive open online course that does not involve any intentionally game-like features. The research was conducted by analyzing survey data (N = 705) collected from individuals who had voluntarily participated in an open online course about artificial intelligence and its societal impact. The survey included an explorative Motives for Autonomous Learning (MAL) inventory. Exploratory factor analysis suggested that the MAL inventory consisted of six dimensions out of which four were consistent with factors that earlier research has associated with motives to engage with video games. Of the identified factors, the dimension that most clearly described autonomous and playful predispositions was found to be a main precedent for both experienced gamefulness of the learning experience and positive learning outcomes. In all, the results of this study demonstrated that playfulness and autonomy were both prominent and significant factors across the whole learning process.

Introduction

People all around the world spend an astonishing number of hours playing video games each day. Today, this is not only true for young men but rather for people representing all genders and ages as players’ mean age is already close to 40 years (Vahlo et al., 2018; Kinnunen et al., 2020). Consequently, both researchers and game industry representatives are keen to understand what motivates players to begin to play and what keeps them engaged with the same application often for hundreds of hours or even more (e.g., Ryan et al., 2006; Sherry et al., 2006; Yee, 2006; Przybylski et al., 2010; Yee et al., 2012; Kahn et al., 2015; De Grove et al., 2017; Vahlo and Hamari, 2019). Indeed, the literature on player motives is a rapidly growing area of research and many models have been published and empirically validated during the past two decades. In the field of education research, the tradition of studying learners’ motivation extends back even further in history. The studies of learning motivation revolve around questions very similar to the ones in game motivation research, that is, the general aim is to understand what ignites the interest for learning and what keeps learners engaged with their studies (e.g., Skinner and Belmont, 1993; Appleton et al., 2006; Zepke and Leach, 2010; Christenson et al., 2012; Sinatra et al., 2015).

The relationship between play and learning has been acknowledged and also studied extensively since the early 20th century (Curtis, 1915; Coleman, 1971; Bruner et al., 1976; Vygotsky, 2004). The emergence of digital gaming further increased researchers’ interest in games for learning (e.g., Gee, 2003; Squire, 2003). As a consequence, serious game (i.e., edutainment) applications (e.g., Charsky, 2010) and learning applications with gamified elements (e.g., Sailer and Homner, 2020; Zainuddin et al., 2020) have established themselves as common practices in their respective fields. Yet, there is much that is still not understood about the relationship between learning motivation and motivation to play video games. This area of investigation has a potentially high research impact and societal implications, as being motivated is known to be associated with higher levels of engagement with learning activities and positive learning outcomes (e.g., Janosz, 2012).

While there is a long tradition in investigating learning motivations and while literature on game playing motivations is expanding rapidly, there exists a surprisingly limited number of studies on how gameplay motivation might be interwoven with learning motives. There is an evident need for understanding these interconnections as many learning experiences of today take place in digital environments that could make use of game-based solutions in a profound and versatile fashion. And there is even more prominent justification for setting the focus specifically on learning motives (e.g., different reasons and other predispositions for learning). The reason is that motives for learning are not that often made explicitly visible in the motivation research models used in the field of education, apart from more informal learning perspectives concerning professional or higher education (see, e.g., Bahn, 2007; Diseth et al., 2010). Within the contexts of more traditional or formal education, the models utilized for studying learning motivation (see, e.g., Skinner and Pitzer, 2012; Virtanen et al., 2014) tend to underline the teachers’ and school’s ability to support engagement with learning activities, teachers and peers rather than individual predispositions (i.e., motives) for voluntarily choosing what to learn.

Engagement with game activities is a central subject for game motivation research as well, and even the concept of playfulness (and playful motivation) is considered as an experiential property that gets (re)organized in engaging with contextual activities (Masek and Stenros, 2021). The viewpoint of game research, however, appears to have a somewhat different basis in approaching the voluntary nature of the activity. Throughout the existence of game research as a field of academic inquiry, it has emphasized that player participation in game experiences is characteristically voluntary and autonomous (Huizinga, 1949; Goffman, 1959; Goffman, 1961; Suits, 1978; Caillois, 2001). The models of game motivation research typically build upon this premise and often emphasize the individual’s motives for playing (e.g., Sherry et al., 2006; Yee, 2006; Vahlo and Hamari, 2019). In exploring linkages between gameplay and learning motives, we consider it necessary to apply a similar framework of autonomous and voluntary participation as a basis of our inquiry. It is crucial to note that much of the learning motivation research assumes the existence of a formal institutional context (e.g., a classroom setting in a school) and a generalized achievement structure (e.g., curriculum and the related degrees) within which the motivations for learning activities are developed. With respect to a growing number of today’s online learning services and online courses, however, it is evidently not valid to assume such circumstances that contribute to the learner’s motivational disposition. Furthermore, also in formal institutional contexts, more consideration should be given to understanding how learners’ motives to participate may impact their learning experience and also learning outcomes. In this paper, we have chosen to emphasize the viewpoint of an autonomous learner, which embraces the free will of a learner to reflect on the reasons for their participation and even choose whether or not to participate in a course in the first place. This approach puts motives into the spotlight, and it is especially relevant to the different forms of adult education and massive open online courses (MOOCs), where participation in learning activities is usually more or less voluntarily initiated.

In this explorative study, we empirically investigate the motives of autonomous learners to participate in an online course and how these motives are related to gaming motivations, course experience and engagement, and learning outcomes. This will be done by analyzing survey data (N = 705) collected from individuals who voluntarily participated in an online course, “Elements of AI,” about artificial intelligence and its societal impact. By studying course participants’ experiences, we explore how playfulness should be understood both from the perspectives of an autonomous learner (i.e., playful motives) and the learning situation (i.e., playful engagement with learning). In general, our aim is to unveil the playfulness inherent in learning experiences, which do not include any intentionally game-like design solutions, and how this playfulness associates with learning motivation and learning outcomes.

Theoretical Background

Combining Educational and Gaming Contexts

Utilization of games as technologies of learning and pedagogical tools has a long history that precedes the current era of digital gaming (Coleman, 1971). Likewise, the study of play in education and human development has been established decades ago (e.g., Groos, 1908; Piaget, 1952; Bruner et al., 1976; Vygotsky, 2004). Being playful is associated with spontaneous learning in seemingly aimless activities (Lieberman, 1977). It is widely acknowledged, also within the field of game design (e.g., Crawford, 1984), that gameplay in itself incorporates learning processes, even when games are not deliberately meant to teach us anything outside of how to play the game itself. Games have also been criticized for teaching the subject matter through an inherently mechanical and reductionist simulation (Kraft, 1971), but on the other hand, the very ability of providing predictable simulated realities, in which a learner can experience agency while engaging with the playing, is considered to be an intrinsic virtue of games in education (Coleman, 1971). In this paper, the main rationale for combining educational and gaming contexts in studying motivation lies in two premises: (1) motivational factors for playing games potentially overlap with the motivational factors for engaging oneself voluntarily and autonomously in learning, and (2) all learning experiences necessarily include elements that can be considered to be gameful as these elements are necessary for all game playing experiences.

The modern conceptualization of digital games tends to separate the domains of entertainment and serious gaming. The educational functions of digital games are thus often seen only in the light of the serious and purposeful activities that aim for learning outcomes, in contrast with “just playing for fun.” Such a distinction between “fun” and “serious” games is problematic in many respects, and it is not necessarily justified when playful learning processes and gameful student engagement are concerned. From the perspectives of evolution and human development, play literally is a “serious” practice and an important adaptation method as well as a method for learning skills that are ultimately essential for survival. On the other hand, gameplay could be characterized as a safe constitution of “reality” (e.g., Piaget, 1952; Stenros, 2014) and as an activity that captivates its actors, induces passion, and facilitates social bonding (e.g., Whitton and Moseley, 2014). In all its seriousness, gameplay thus may be absorbing, engaging, and, in all, an entertaining experience regardless of whether the game has been designed with a particular learning purpose in mind.

The most prominent justification for game-based learning (Prensky, 2001) or gamification of learning (Deterding et al., 2011; Faiella and Ricciardi, 2015) has been that games are effective in motivating and engaging students in learning activities. This effect has been found to be similarly important for other kinds of serious games, such as games for health (Kowert and Quandt, 2016). By definition, games include conditions that players need to meet in order to progress in the game. These conditions are environmental demands that impose challenges for the player’s performance. An optimal gameplay experience is often depicted as the player acting in a state of flow, where there is a balance between the challenges of the game and the player’s abilities. It has been shown that, within such approaches, this type of engagement indeed has a positive effect on learning (Hamari et al., 2016). According to Mayer’s (2019) review, previous research on game-based learning has focused on (1) the important features in games that support learning, (2) the potential learning effects of commercial video games, and (3) the benefits for learning through games when compared to traditional teaching methods. Although there is increasing knowledge on the educational benefits of computer games (Mayer, 2019), the gameful features, such as challenge and flow (Hamari et al., 2016), that work for learning, as well as instructional techniques of serious games, such as context integration, narration-based techniques, feedback, and adaptivity (Wouters and Van Oostendorp, 2017), it is still not well understood in which situations these features work the best, and for who, what are the underlying motivational cognitive, affective and social mechanisms explaining these effects (Faiella and Ricciardi, 2015; Mayer, 2019).

Instead of focusing on the ways games work as motivators for learning, our focus is on discovering how motives to participate in an online course are potentially associated with the perceived playfulness/gamefulness of a learning experience which does not include a standalone game and which has not even been purposefully gamified. In addition, we are also interested in understanding how both motives to participate and gameful qualities inherent to the learning experience (as a part of the given motivational climate) are associated with learning outcomes.

Our scrutiny aims to treat both contexts on an equal basis: play is viewed as a natural part of knowledge and skills creation, and playfulness as a general motivational foundation in humans (e.g., Masek and Stenros, 2021). At the same time, we presume that learning inevitably incorporates aspects of play and game-like activities. Therefore, for game players and online learners alike, it makes sense to equally ask questions related to their “gameful” and “learnful” motivations. In the present study, our focus is set on online learners but the integrative strategy with respect to the two motivational contexts remains the same.

Autonomy of a Player and a Learner

Motivation to play games is often conceived through intrinsic motivation, underlining the spontaneous curiosity, interest, and free will of a player (Ryan and Deci, 2000; Sansone and Harackiewicz, 2000; Rheinberg and Engeser, 2018). Unlike the purposeful activities that we do in order to provide sustenance for ourselves and our community, play does not seem to have any obvious goal. Therefore it seems to epitomize humans’ desire to act on a purely voluntary basis—basically just for fun. This is not to say, however, that play would not have any benefits. Human play is associated with physical and psychological wellbeing (Proyer, 2013; Kowert and Quandt, 2016) and has various functions that relate to physical activity, exploration with objects, and socially valuable pretense behavior (Sutton-Smith, 1983; Smith, 2005). From the developmental and evolutionary perspective, play and games have functioned as pivotal ways of learning (e.g., Bruner et al., 1976). For a child, playing might be just a fun activity, but implicitly children also develop their physical or social skills that are relevant and useful in their future life (Piaget, 1952). It has even been argued that abilities to play and imagine have been key preconditions in the survival and development of the human species (Huizinga, 1949). From this perspective, it is intriguing to consider play as a human need for actualizing one’s playful self (i.e., homo ludens). Presumably such a need is strongly tied to autonomy—a “capacity for and desire to experience self-regulation and integrity” (Deci and Ryan, 2012, p. 85). Because of the close relationship of play and learning, it is equally tempting to consider that there could be some profound similarities between an autonomous player and an autonomous learner.

Nevertheless, games, as a form of organized play, can be approached from a seemingly contradictory direction, as the rules and goals they impose on a player can also be described as external control systems (Avedon and Sutton-Smith, 1971, p. 7), which effectively regulate the player’s motivation and action. The procedural control is maintained through either persuasive or intrusive rhetorics and contribution to the player’s motivational self-regulation (Bogost, 2007). This kind of understanding of player participation assumes that a player’s desire to play is given and that the player is not a fully free subject who can make decisions as she chooses (Aarseth, 2007). Instead of being a free subject, the player is subjected to the gameplay condition, that is, she is a player only because she agrees to fulfill the requirements the game sets for their interaction (Leino, 2010). However, in the enactive approach to human cognition it is conversely argued that an environment does not impose any dictating condition over an autonomous subject (Weber and Varela, 2002; Thompson, 2007). Instead, it is the individual, and in this context a player, who voluntarily meta-regulates her ongoing playful player–game interaction (Vahlo, 2017). Games represent a type of environment, which may obviously incorporate persuasive design features, such as rewarding possibilities for action (Hamari et al., 2014), but that does not mean that a player necessarily experiences any external control as long as it is coherent with her self-regulative orientation in engaging with the play-world (see Tuuri et al., 2017). A person’s intention to play, her motives, and her playful dispositions are all essential constituents of the game experience (Sutton-Smith, 2009; Karhulahti, 2015). In terms of comparing the autonomies of a learner and a player, learning activities too are often externally (i.e., pedagogically) organized, in one form or another. In the present study, our focus is on an open online course, which is voluntarily chosen by its participants—just as games are voluntarily chosen by their players.

Ecological Approach to Motivational Development: Taking Contextual Engagement Into Account

Our understanding of motives and motivations is based on how the human mind is conceptualized in ecological and enactive approaches to psychology. The basic premise in these approaches holds that the human mind is not just inside the head but extends to everything that the head and the body are inside of (Gibson, 1977; Mace, 1977; Noë, 2009). This demarcation states that the organization of our perception, action, and knowing—basically also the substrate of motivational development—relies on embodied interactions that couple our cognition with the environment (see also Varela et al., 1992). Autonomy, and especially biological autonomy, has been described as a core tenet of enactive approaches to cognition (Di Paolo et al., 2010; Froese and Stewart, 2012). The concept of autonomy is defined as a core organizing principle for all living systems, and it is directly associated with the identity and survival of an individual who is required, hence motivated, to interact with the world to maintain its autonomy (Thompson and Stapleton, 2009; Di Paolo and Thompson, 2014).

Of the broader field of research on human motivations, a specific interest of this study concerns explicit motives, which denote certain “predispositions to approach a particular class of incentives or to avoid a particular class of threats” (Thrash et al., 2012, p. 141). Differently put, explicit motives are reasons individuals hold for their involvement in particular activities, and therefore, these motives are also self-attributed conscious expressions of individuals’ values, goals, and self-identities. Explicit motives have a directive function in our decision-making and reasoning whereas unconscious implicit motives serve as an energizing function in our actions (McClelland et al., 1989; Thrash et al., 2012; Brunstein, 2018). The approach of explicit motives is widely utilized in self-report surveys and questionnaires in which respondents operate with verbally encoded explicit motives (Thrash et al., 2012). In these studies, explicit motives can be treated as verbalized results of a respondent’s self-reflective cognitive appraisal of the mentioned predispositions to act on the environment.

Explicit and implicit motives are useful concepts in studies which aim to understand how individuals make sense of their own decision-making and how motives which they consider to be important predict patterns of their behavior as well as outcomes of their actions. However, within a broader theoretical model, motives may appear as static reflections of an essentially dynamic motivational development. Behind motives, we can outline several different motivational dispositions that are potentially involved and projected in the constitution of a motive. Such motivation background can consist of, for example, personal goal pursuit and the related commitment (Gollwitzer and Oettingen, 2012), achievement goal orientation (Elliot and Thrash, 2001), social self-regulation (Schunk and Usher, 2012), playful attitude/intentions (Masek and Stenros, 2021), culture and personality (Higgins, 2008), or self-regulation in terms of basic need satisfaction and one’s autonomic “true self” (Deci and Ryan, 1991). Explicit motives to participate, for instance, in an online course, potentially incorporate a contextual and situated foundation of different background elements. While the motivational background arguably contributes to the motive generation, in a simple verbalization of an explicit motive, it may remain only implied.

Explicit motives are products of making sense of a person’s motivational predisposition for activity. But they should not be taken merely as propositional reasonings or planning that precedes action. According to the framework of the embodied and ecological mind, human motivation and the appraisal of explicit motives are necessarily co-constituted in engagement with a given environment. Similarly to Gibson’s (1977) concept of action affordances, motivational cognition is neither a property of a living entity nor a property of an environment, but rather both of them, being enactively structured (Varela et al., 1992) through interactions with the environment. The essence of motives (as predispositions to approach or avoid) are thus not detached from a person’s experiential history of activities and contexts they relate to, including what kind of outcome expectations an individual has for participating in these contextual activities (Schunk and Usher, 2012).

All this is to say that motives are best scrutinized in dynamic connection with the contextual processes of motivational development and engagement. Similarly to Skinner and Pitzer’s (2012) model of motivational dynamics in learning, we see contextual engagement with (learning/gaming) activities pivotal both in understanding the adaptive and self-regulative processes of motivation, being tied in situational engagement, and embedded in the motivational climate that is afforded to the actor by the context (see Figure 1). Motivational climate is here broadly understood as the overall motivational “environment” that contributes to the experiential organization of contextual action affordances and incentives, instructional elements, autonomy and control, communal support, and consequently to the ways how engagement with activities is structured. As contextual engagement and motivation background are interconnected, the climate arguably also contributes to the development of individuals’ motives and motivational orientations (such as mastery or performance approaches, see Ames, 1992, p. 262). Unlike the model of Skinner and Pitzer, however, the model of motivational development constructed for the use of this study does not presuppose any context of activity, such as classroom, school, or any other institutional establishment.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The model of motivational development consisting of three domains (motivation background, contextual engagement, learning outcomes) and their interrelations.

Masek and Stenros (2021) have recently conducted a systematic, integrative literature review on the concept of playfulness across different scientific disciplines. According to the review, conceptual usage of playfulness in the literature is essentially two-fold: it is either taken as an individual’s desire or willingness for playful engagement in relation to context(s), or it is understood in terms of playful organization of the context. This conceptual synthesis makes it clearer how we position playfulness or gamefulness into a model of motivational dynamics. Correspondingly, playfulness can potentially be considered both as (1) background intent for (playful and gameful) explicit and implicit motive generation, and (2) structural organization of (playful and gameful) experience of engaging with activities in the context. Conforming to this idea, our model of motivational development (see Figure 1) incorporates playfulness both in a person’s dispositional motivation background (i.e., playfulness implied in motives) and contextual engagement (i.e., experientially playful organization of the context). Again, we assume that both the individual’s playful disposition and the contextual engagement of playfulness are interconnected. For example, if a person has a motive that reflects “a desire for playful engagement” it should be likely that the playfulness will also be embodied in the experiential organization of the context.

Research Questions and Hypotheses

Our survey targeted autonomous learners who participated in an online course on artificial intelligence and its societal impacts. Conforming to the model of motivational development presented above, the research focuses on the following three phases of the learning process: (1) motives to participate in the course, (2) contextual engagement during the course, and (3) learning outcomes. In line with this demarcation, the research questions of this study are: (1) How do playful and gameful intent for motive generation and for structural organization of a learning experience manifest themselves in self-report data about users’ motives to participate in an online course; (2) how are aspects of playful and gameful intent in motive generation related to playful and gameful organization of learning experiences in the engagement with the course; and (3) how are playful/gameful motives (the first item) and playful/gameful organization of experiences (the second item) associated with learning outcomes?

Consequently, the hypotheses of this study are as follows. First of all, because of the potential overlap between learning intents and a desire for playful engagement, we expect that this study will unveil structures in the motives of autonomous learners that resemble motives for playing games, as both of these modes of participation are profoundly voluntary and interactive.

H1: Course participants’ explicit motives to participate in an online learning course reflect their intent for learning but also resemble prevalent motives to play digital games.

Moreover, we expect to find gameful qualities in the experiences of situational engagement with learning activities. This is because learning activities share phenomenal qualities with game activities as both impose interactive tasks, challenges, goals, and performance evaluation (Lam et al., 2007; Adams, 2014; Karhulahti, 2015). Furthermore, gameful qualities of a situation are co-constituted both by the environment and the individual’s ways to organize the situation in a playful manner.

H2: Gameful qualities are not exclusive to games and gamified solutions but gameful qualities are also manifested in those learning situations and activities that are not strictly game-based or intentionally gamified.

Our third hypothesis concerns the relationship between motives and engagement. As there seems to be an agreement in the field of educational research that “motivation is a basis for subsequent engagement” (Martin, 2012, p. 305), we can expect a positive association between the two. Furthermore, we expect that the experienced gameful qualities parallel the experience of engagement with learning activities. This is because gameful experiences arguably presuppose active involvement (i.e., engagement) by the learner (Högberg et al., 2019).

H3a: Identified motive dimensions (H1) all predict a higher learner engagement with the online learning course.

H3b: Experienced gameful qualities (H2) of a learning situation are associated with learner engagement.

Since the gameful qualities of a situation are co-constituted by both the learning environment and the individual’s intent to organize the situation in a playful manner, we assume that learners’ playful motives contribute to the perception of gamefulness of learning. Individuals’ motives to participate are not to be understood as isolated from the ongoing experience, but rather as a meta-regulative resource they utilize before, throughout, and after their participation (see also discussion on engagement process in Janosz, 2012, p. 699).

H4: Experienced gameful qualities of the online course are not the same for all participants but are influenced by their motivation for organizing the learning situation playfully. Therefore, the explicit motives that participants hold predict how they experience the gamefulness of the learning situation.

Our final hypothesis considers the experienced learning outcomes, and how these associate with motives and engagement. Previous research (see Janosz, 2012) points out not only that motives are determinants of learning engagement, but also that engagement has an effect on student learning success. Therefore, we expect the following:

H5a: Contextual engagement is a precedent for learning outcomes.

H5b: Explicit motives are all associated with learning outcomes via contextual engagement.

Materials and Methods

The MAL Inventory Development

Research data was gathered using a survey targeted at students of Elements of AI, a MOOC developed by the University of Helsinki and the software company Reaktor. The course includes learning materials and assignments and is not purposefully gamified. For measuring explicit motives of participating in the course, the survey included an explorative Motives for Autonomous Learning (MAL) inventory. Since explicit motives are conceptualized to be relatively stable dispositions and “reasons people hold for initiating and performing voluntary behavior” (Reiss, 2004), they are considered well-suited for survey-based research setups (Beckmann and Heckhausen, 2018; Scheffer and Heckhausen, 2018; Schultheiss and Wirth, 2018). Motive questionnaires assess the self-attributed explicit schemata of individuals that direct and select their behavior, usually across situations (Thrash et al., 2012, pp. 133–134).

In the scale development process, we followed a five-step procedure presented by Phan et al. (2016). These steps consist of preliminary item pool generation, expert review of the item pool, a questionnaire pilot study, preferably two exploratory factor analyses (EFAs), and a theory-based confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) including its validity and reliability testing. Since the objective of this explorative study was to examine the dimensionality of the motives for the autonomous learning inventory and how these motives are associated with learning experiences and outcomes, our scale development did not include confirmatory phases for the MAL scale development. Psychometric validation of a scale necessitates at least two EFAs and a CFA with different survey data (Matsunaga, 2010), and both of these steps fell beyond the scope of the current study. As background material for designing a preliminary item pool for MAL, we used open-ended data originally collected by Reaktor about the students’ reasons for participation. These responses (n = 225) were first coded into potential motive categories based on recurring themes in the individual responses and a total of 34 motive categories were identified. The 34 categories were further classified into nine main categories: competence, mood management, achievement, social, boredom, autonomy, fun/entertainment, curiosity/interest, and fear of missing out. Both the 34 categories and the nine main categories were reviewed by three researchers and two industry operators as a part of the expert review phase of the scale development.

Next, the researchers went back to the data and selected those motive descriptions that captured different aspects of the nine main categories and the 34 sub-categories. However, not all nine preliminary motive categories were equally represented in the open-ended data. In order to balance the inventory and to construct new survey items for the motive categories, we reviewed a series of motive inventories that assess reasons to play digital games (Sherry et al., 2006; Yee, 2006; Demetrovics et al., 2011; Yee et al., 2012; Kahn et al., 2015; De Grove et al., 2017; Vahlo and Hamari, 2019). As a result, we constructed MAL inventory items (Table 1) for each of the preliminary nine categories as our objective was to include the preliminary MAL inventory in a questionnaire pilot study.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The 35-MAL Inventory and its descriptive statistics.

Before an EFA was made for the MAL inventory to investigate latent motive categories of the autonomous learners, we assessed the sampling adequacy of the 35-item inventory. The inventory passed the Kaiser–Myer–Olkin (KMO) test with a value of 0.88 and also the Bartlett test of sphericity (chi-square = 9,673, df = 595, p = 0.000), both of which indicated that a factor analysis was appropriate.

Measuring Engagement, Experience, and Outcomes

GAMEFULQUEST is an instrument for assessing the perceived gamefulness of using any service or system (Högberg et al., 2019). In the context of our study, “the service” refers to user experience when participating in an online learning course. Although GAMEFULQUEST has been psychometrically validated, we decided to do an EFA based on the fact that the instrument is new, and the three services used in the validation study were either standalone videogames (Zombies, Run!) or purposefully gamified systems (Duolingo and Nike+ Run Club). The validated 56-item GAMEFULQUEST inventory consists of seven dimensions: Accomplishment, Challenge, Competition, Guided, Immersion, Playfulness, and Social Experience (Högberg et al., 2019). Since our study focuses on the viewpoint of an autonomous learner and her motives, we decided to develop a total of eight new items for GAMEFULQUEST for measuring experienced autonomy as a part of a gameful experience. Similarly, to our approach with the MAL inventory, we assessed sampling adequacy for GAMEFULQUEST both in the validated form and as complemented by eight items that supposedly measured experienced autonomy while studying on the online course. Both the validated GAMEFULQUEST (KMO = 0.975, the Bartlett test of sphericity: chi-square = 27,407, df = 1,596, p = 0.000) and the version complemented by eight autonomy-based items (KMO = 0.978, Bartlett test of sphericity: chi-square = 31,083, df = 2016, p = 0.000) clearly passed the tests.

Scales for measuring student engagement are often developed for a school context thus necessitating modifications to any chosen instrument when applied to online learning. Engagement with the online course was studied using the Student Engagement Inventory (Lam et al., 2014). The 33-item scale explores three dimensions of engagement: Affective Engagement (nine items), Behavioral Engagement (12 items), and Cognitive Engagement (12 items). A number of the items were explicitly designed for a school context, thus, for the purposes of this study several items were excluded from our analyses on how motives to participate may predict engagement and whether experienced gamefulness and experienced engagement are correlated. The number of items was reduced to four items per dimension, and the resulting 12-item inventory Cronbach’s alphas (95% confidence intervals for the alphas) and coefficient omegas were for each dimension: Affective Engagement α = 0.872 (CI 0.93–0.95) and ω = 0.882, Behavioral Engagement α = 0.693 (CI 0.65–0.73) and ω = 0.7326, and Cognitive Engagement α = 0.8263 (CI 0.80–0.85) and ω = 0.8321.

According to Ames (1992, p. 267–268), in studying motivational climates it is essential to look into the learner’s perceptions of learning experiences rather than mere learner behavior. To measure participants’ perceptions of how the online course supports their motivation through different instructional strategies we used the 18-item version of the Motivating Instructional Contexts Inventory (MICI) validated (Cronbach’s α = 0.94) by Lam and Law (2007). The scale consists of six 3-item subscales: Challenge, Real-Life Significance, Curiosity, Autonomy, Recognition, and Evaluation. As the scale was developed for teacher-led classroom learning contexts, two subscales (Recognition and Evaluation) not suited to the online course studied here were excluded, and the remaining items were reworded to apply to a learning context where a teacher is not present. Furthermore, the four subscales used in the correlation analyses of this study were renamed to Challenge Support, Application Support, Curiosity Support, and Autonomy Support to better suit an online learning context. Cronbach’s α and McDonald’s ω were calculated for each subscale: Challenge Support ω = 0.69 (CI 0.65 to 0.73), ω = 0.71, Curiosity Support α = 0.64 (CI 0.59 to 0.68) and ω = 0.64, Application Support α = 0.857 (CI 0.84 to 0.87) and ω = 0.861, and Autonomy Support α = 0.70 (CI 0.66 to 0.74) and ω = 0.71.

Finally, we needed to measure participants’ perceptions of their learning outcomes. For this purpose we developed three items for assessing participants’ perceptions of learning about the course content (Learning the Topic, α = 0.762, CI 0.73 to 0.79, ω = 0.795), and three items to evaluate their perceptions of learning methods for online learning (Learning Method, α = 0.889, CI 0.87 to 0.90, ω = 0.892). Additionally, we used the 4-item Critical Reflection scale (Kember et al., 2000) to gauge the learning outcomes of critical thinking skills (α = 0.836, CI 0.81 to 0.86, ω = 0.837). A sum variable for each of these measures was calculated for investigating learning outcomes by means of multiple linear regressions between the above-mentioned constructs.

Participants and Data Collection

Data collection was conducted in two languages, English and Finnish. The survey was translated from English to Finnish using the committee approach. First, the survey was translated by the researchers, who are all competent in both languages. Next, a back-translation from Finnish to English was done by an outside translator. Then the back-translation was used to assess the original translation as well as the wording in the new instruments developed for this survey.

Survey participants were recruited through the Elements of AI course email list with the message targeted at students from Finland, Germany, Sweden, and the United Kingdom. To exclude respondents who had only recently registered on the course, the survey was further targeted at participants who had registered at least 6 months ago. The participants had the opportunity to participate in a raffle for a 200€ gift card. The resulting sample included 705 responses, with 54% of the respondents reporting they were female, 44.5% male, and 0.3% other, while 1.1% did not want to specify. The mean age of the sample was 47.8 (Min = 16, Max = 83). Three of the participants were minors. The Finnish national board on research integrity states that a person’s own consent for participation is sufficient when the participant is 15 or older. As our data gathering method was anonymous, we did not have a chance to inform the parents of participating minors. All participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

For the present study, it is important to note that as many as 82% of the respondents answered that they participated in the course out of their own interest. Only 10% of the respondents participated in the course as a part of workplace training, and 8% as a part of degree education. This observation is in line with our expectation that participation in MOOCs should be voluntarily initiated. And it indeed implies that the great majority of the survey respondents represent autonomous learners.

Results

MAL Inventory

Next, an exploratory factor analysis for the MAL inventory was made. The purpose of making an EFA was to analyze whether the MAL inventory measured latent motive dimensions, and to what extent these possible latent motive dimensions could be considered to be context-specific and to what extent, consequently, reoccurring across multiple contexts of autonomous experiencing.

We conducted a parallel analysis (Henson and Roberts, 2006) to identify how many factors should be extracted for the 35-item MAL inventory (Table 1). Both the parallel analysis (PA) with 100 iterations as well as the Kaiser eigenvalue >1 rule suggested that eight factors were to be extracted, and therefore we proceeded to investigate an eight-factor solution. Promax rotation was selected over orthogonal rotation methods, such as varimax rotation, since it is reasonable to assume that motives to participate are correlated to each other in all real-life situations (see Matsunaga, 2010, p. 100). We used factor loadings over 0.4 as a criterion of whether an inventory item had loaded on a factor (Hair et al., 2010), and considered three items to be the sufficient minimum number of items for a factor to be identified (Brown, 2015).

In scale validation studies, it is recommended to first remove items that do not meet the selected inclusion criteria, and then investigate again how many factors should be extracted for the inventory under investigation (Matsunaga, 2010). However, the objective of our study was not to validate the MAL inventory but rather to explore how all of the included motive items behaved in relation to each other. Therefore, we decided not to exclude items that showed loadings under 0.4 for all factors. Instead, we focused only on the latent dimensions and whether at least three items showed a loading over 0.4 on each of them.

In the eight-factor solution, no items loaded on the eighth factor with a loading over 0.4. Therefore we decided to extract seven factors. In the seven-factor solution, only one item “because of the certificate” loaded to the seventh factor with a loading over 0.4. Since the seventh factor was not properly identified, we rejected this solution and extracted a six-factor solution. In the six-factor solution, at least three items loaded on each factor with a loading that exceeded 0.4. This solution was thus retained, and we report the full factor loading table below (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Full factor loading table and uniqueness for the 35 Motives for Autonomous Learning inventory.

In the exploratory factor analysis eight motive items loaded on the first factor (Table 2). These items describe motives to participate, because an individual wants to keep up with the times and stay ahead of technological changes and innovations. A person motivated by this factor is curious about what the future might bring and participates in the course to explore new domains of knowledge which build her skills and appear to be useful in the future. We call this motive dimension Capability and it clearly refers to the value that participating in the course might bring for the individual in the future. This motive factor was also the most important reason for participating in the online course (mean 5.72).

The second factor indicates a motive for participating, because the learning experience is expected to be fun, challenging, and entertaining. Furthermore, a person motivated by this factor also took the course because participating made them feel good about themselves. We can also explore those items that showed loadings over 0.3 but under 0.4 on this factor and conclude that this motive dimension is also associated with curiosity, creativity, and interest in the subject matter as well as the fact that the person can freely choose when and how she participates in the course. Generally speaking, these motive items refer primarily to the learning experience itself instead of its possible consequences and future applications similarly to the Capability factor. Moreover, the items that loaded on this factor are similar to how intrinsically motivating and especially playful experiences have been described in the literature (Ryan and Deci, 2000; Sansone and Harackiewicz, 2000; Rheinberg and Engeser, 2018). Finally, the items that showed lower loadings on this factor concern the participant’s ability to act freely and autonomously. We call this motive factor Autotelic and this concept designates that the activity has a purpose in itself and that the learning experience is what draws us to participate in the course. Autotelic had the second highest mean value (4.71) and it can therefore be considered as the second most important motive to participate in the Elements of AI course.

Eight motive items loaded on the third factor. Three of these items cross-loaded between this factor and the sixth factor, but five items showed high loadings only on the third factor. These latter motive items describe reasons to participate in the course because it was considered useful for career development and professional development, because it was expected to open up new future possibilities, and because it was anticipated that participating would help to chase dreams. Furthermore, the items that cross-loaded between this factor and the sixth factor denote participating because of the certificate, the course credits, and because participating would be beneficial for building up one’s resumé. All of the above-mentioned items portray the Achievement motive factor which had the third highest mean value (3.74).

Three items loaded on the fourth factor. These motives indicate that an individual participates because the course was recommended to her, because she was encouraged to take the course, and because her friends or colleagues also took the course. We label this factor Social and it had the second lowest mean sum of the six factors (2.87).

Three items also loaded on the fifth factor. These items describe that a person participated in the course because she had too much free time, because she had nothing else to do, and because she was bored. Consequently, we call this motive factor Boredom and it was the least important reason for participating in the online learning course (mean 2.35).

The sixth factor also had three items that loaded on it. All of these three items showed a loading also on the Achievement factor. These items denote short-term prizes and Reward, and only one item “Because of the certificate.” The Reward factor was the fourth most important reason to participate in the course (mean 3.31).

Finally, we should also consider those items that did not show loadings over 0.4 on any of the six identified factors. As mentioned earlier, the items that described interest toward the course subject, curiosity, and autonomy all showed loadings over 0.3 on Autotelic. In addition to that, the items describing interest toward the subject matter also showed loadings over 0.3 on Capability, whereas the item “because I could study anywhere and anytime” cross-loaded between Autotelic and Boredom. The motive item “…because it was free” did not load on Autotelic but it did show a loading over 0.3 on Boredom. Finally, the three items which denoted fear of missing out, fear toward AI or robots, and desire to evaluate the course quality did not show loadings over 0.25 on any of the six factors.

The EFA reported in Table 2 suggests that motives for autonomous learning can be identified. The motives Capability, Autotelic, Achievement, Social, Boredom, and Reward reflect participants’ desire for learning, but are not otherwise drastically different from motives to play games (H1). Psychometric studies on motives for gameplay frequently report that social interaction, boredom, achievement, and especially qualities inherent to the experience itself (e.g., fun and challenge), are prevalent reasons to engage with games (Sherry et al., 2006; Yee, 2006; Demetrovics et al., 2011; Yee et al., 2012; Kahn et al., 2015; De Grove et al., 2017; Vahlo and Hamari, 2019). However, it is much rarer that a validated measure that assesses motives to play games would include dimensions that denote only short-term rewards and prizes (Reward) or players’ desire to build their skills and capabilities that would extend to their career development or their future prospects (Capability).

Gamefulness of the Contextual Engagement

Next, we continued to examine the second hypothesis of this study: “H2: Gameful qualities are not exclusive to games and gamified solutions but instead these qualities are manifested across all learning situations.” For this purpose, we made another exploratory factor analysis (EFA), this time for the GAMEFULQUEST measure. We investigated the number of factors in the 64-GAMEFULQUEST inventory by doing a parallel analysis test with 100 iterations. The PA test as well as Kaiser eigenvalue >1 test suggested eight factors for the 64-GAMEFULQUEST. Since GAMEFULQUEST is a validated measure, we utilized factor loading over 0.40 (promax rotation) as a criterion to determine whether an inventory item loaded on a factor (Hair et al., 2010). In the first iteration, no items loaded on the eighth factor of the 64-GAMEFULQUEST. After excluding a total of seven items from the original 56-GAMEFULQUEST and one of the additional autonomy-based items (“Was a chance for me to fulfill myself and live by my values”) which did not load on any factor, we made another PA test for the inventory. The test suggested a 6-factor solution. In the second iteration, one more item did not load on any of the factors. The PA test still suggested that six factors should be extracted from the inventory, and in the third iteration all remaining items loaded on a factor, and at least three items loaded on each factor. The full factor loadings are reported in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Factor loading table and uniqueness for GAMEFULQUEST, complemented by autonomy-based items (marked with “*” in the table).

The first factor consists of items that measure Accomplishment (e.g., “Encouraged me to strive for achievements”) and Playful (“Made me feel that I am finding new things”) in particular, but also to some extent Challenge (e.g., “Made me feel that in order to succeed I have to constantly improve”) and Guided (e.g., “Gave me useful feedback so that I can adapt”) in the validated 56-GAMEFULQUEST. In addition, seven of the eight items we developed for measuring experienced autonomy loaded on this factor. More precisely, the items that showed the highest loadings on the first factor were those that we developed for measuring experienced autonomy and those items that measure Accomplishment in the validated 56-GAMEFULQUEST. Since the first factor included items from four factors of the validated 56-GAMEFULQUEST as well as the new autonomy-based items, the factor could not be labeled similarly to any of the GAMEFULQUEST dimensions. We decided to call this factor Purposeful Play. The word “purposeful” refers to the experienced autonomy, and accomplishments that result in overcoming challenges in a feedback-providing environment. In addition to the clear reference to playfulness, the word “play” also refers to autonomy as play is frequently described as a voluntary act of expressing oneself freely.

The second factor consisted of the same items as a factor in the validated GAMEFULQUEST, and therefore we similarly call it Social Experience. The items that loaded on the third factor and the fourth factor were the same that loaded on the Guided and Competition in the validated GAMEFULQUEST, and we decided to retain these factor names. The fifth factor was very similar to the Immersion factor in the 56-GAMEFULQUEST, but the item “Made me immerse myself fully in what I was doing” cross-loaded with Purposeful Play. Since this item refers directly to immersion, we re-considered the name of the fifth factor and decided to call it Absorption instead of immersion. Finally, the sixth factor was similar to the Challenge factor of the GAMEFULQUEST, but again the item that includes the word “challenge” cross-loaded with Purposeful Play. Hence, we call this factor Effort, as the items that have the highest loading on this factor describe the work required from the participant more precisely than the challenges the course imposes on them.

In addition to the results of the EFA reported in Table 3, we also investigated whether a similar six-factor structure would be present in the validated 56-GAMEFULQUEST without the autonomy-based items fashioned by our research group. Applying the same EFA procedure as described above with the 64-GAMEFULQUEST resulted in a six-factor solution instead of an anticipated seven-factor solution (Högberg et al., 2019). In the six-factor solution, items that measure Accomplishment and Playful in the original GAMEFULQUEST still loaded on the same factor alongside with the item “Challenged me” from Challenge and “Gave me useful feedback so that I can adapt” from Guided. The other five factors were similar to the ones identified with the 64-GAMEFULQUEST and reported in Table 3.

The GAMEFULQUEST inventory did not measure the seven dimensions similar to the scale validation study (Högberg et al., 2019). Regardless of this, the inventory did measure gameful dimensions of the online course experience, which provides some support for the second hypothesis. By observing the mean sums for each of the identified factors, we can conclude that the experienced gameful qualities of participating in the online learning consisted mostly of Purposeful Play (mean 4.6) and Effort (mean 3.9) whereas Competition had a clearly lower mean value (2.9) than any of the other dimensions.

Next, we investigated how experiences of instructional practices of the learning situation were related to participants’ explicit motives for taking the course (Table 2) and their experienced gamefulness of the learning situation (Table 3). This was done by calculating correlations between sum variables of the four dimensions of the MICI and (1) the motives to participate in the course (MAL), as well as (2) dimensions of the experienced gamefulness of the situation (GAMEFULQUEST). The results of these correlations are reported in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Correlations (Spearman’s Rho) between the four Motivating Instructional Contexts Inventory (MICI) factors, and dimensions of experienced gamefulness and motives to participate in the course.

Experienced gamefulness in the form of Purposeful Play was found to be strongly correlated with all of the dimensions of the MICI. Experienced Social Connectedness and Guided of the GAMEFULQUEST were both correlated with Autonomy Support. In addition to these correlations, several dimensions of experienced gamefulness were weakly correlated with MICI dimensions. The motives of Capability and Autotelic were the only two participation reasons that were clearly associated with the MICI dimensions, and these correlations were the strongest between Real-Life Support and Capability, and Real-Life Support and Autotelic.

Engagement in the Learning Situation

We proceeded to calculate factor score variables for both MAL factors (Table 2) and GAMEFULQUEST dimensions (Table 3). We decided to use factor score variables in the following analyses instead of factor sums as factor scores provide information about how an item loads on every factor (DiStefano et al., 2009). Calculating factor scores is possible after making an EFA, and it was a reasonable approach in our situation in which we wanted to retain all inventory items and also items that showed cross-loadings between several factors (Tables 2 and 3). In order to investigate whether motives for autonomous learning (H3a) and perceived gamefulness of the learning experience (H3b) were associated with experienced engagement, we calculated factor sums for the Student Engagement Inventory (Lam et al., 2014).

Since the survey respondents were asked in the questionnaire to specify the reasons, that is, explicit motives, why they decided to take the online course, it is plausible to assume that motive factors are possible precedents for experienced engagement in a learning situation. To analyze the relationship between the motives (Table 2) and experienced engagement, we first calculated a linear regression between a combined motive sum variable and combined engagement sum variable. Both of these variables were constructed over all items in their corresponding inventories. The linear regression between the motive sum variable and the engagement sum variable showed that the motives combined do predict experienced engagement (coefficient 0.49, standardized error 0.04, p = 0.000, t = 12.87, β = 0.44, R2 = 19%). Next, we calculated multiple linear regressions between the identified motive factors and Affective engagement, Behavioral engagement, and Cognitive engagement. The results of these regressions are presented in Table 5.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Multiple regressions between autonomous motives to participate in a learning experience and experienced level of engagement.

Both Capability and Autotelic motives to participate predicted all three aspects of engagement. The effect of Capability on engagement was largest on Cognitive engagement whereas Autotelic was the main precedent for both Behavioral engagement and especially Affective engagement. Achievement and Reward motives were associated with Behavioral engagement whereas Social motives predicted lower Affective engagement, albeit weakly. Instead, the Boredom motive predicted a lower level of engagement from the perspective of all three engagement dimensions. A hypothesis of this study (H3a) was that all user motives would predict a higher level of engagement. If all motive factors are combined, the effect of motives on engagement is positive. But when each motive dimension is considered individually, it is revealed that not all motives are associated with a deeper level of engagement. On the contrary, especially participating out of Boredom was found to predict a significantly lower level of affective, behavioral, and cognitive engagement in a learning experience. Therefore, this hypothesis (H3a) of this study was not supported.

It was also hypothesized that experienced gamefulness of a situation would be associated with engagement. Since both perceived gamefulness and engagement are part of the learning situation and how a user experiences the motivational climate, it is reasonable to assume that experienced gamefulness would predict engagement or that engagement would be a precedent for experienced gamefulness. Therefore, we calculated correlations (Spearman’s Rho) between these two constructs to investigate their possible linkages (Table 6).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Correlations (Spearman’s Rho) between experienced gamefulness and dimensions of engagement in a learning experience.

Experienced Purposeful Play in a learning situation was strongly correlated with all of the three modes of engagement, most strongly with Affective engagement and Behavioral engagement. Of the other five dimensions of gameful experience, only Effort was moderately correlated with one dimension of engagement, Behavioral engagement. The other four dimensions of gameful experience had only weak or very weak linkages with engagement dimensions. These results did not provide support for hypothesis H3b: “Gameful qualities of a learning situation are associated with engagement.

Identifying Linkages Between Motives, Gamefulness, and Learning Outcomes

Following Masek and Stenros (2021), we have approached playfulness in a two-fold fashion: playfulness is the desire and intent for playful engagement and the playful organization of an experience. Similarly to Janosz (2012), we have also stated that participants’ motives are not to be understood as isolated from the experience, but rather as meta-regulative practices that participants engage with before, during, and after an activity. Following these demarcations, we hypothesized that gameful qualities of a learning situation are not identical between participants but are instead influenced by their motives to participate and organize the situation playfully (H4). In an effort to study this hypothesis, we calculated multiple regressions between MAL factor score variables and GAMEFULQUEST factor score variables.

All six motive dimensions were found to be associated with at least one dimension of GAMEFULQUEST. Experienced Purposeful Play was strongly predicted by the Autotelic motive to participate, but also by Capability, Achievement, and Reward. However, the Boredom motive predicted a lower level of experienced Purposeful Play in a learning situation. Social Connectedness was predicted by Autotelic and Social motives, but also by Achievement albeit only weakly. Experienced Guided was predicted by Achievement, Autotelic, and Social. Similarly, Competition was also predicted by Achievement and Social, but also by Boredom. Absorption was strongly predicted by Autotelic, and Capability and Social were both precedents for experienced Effort.

The motive dimensions explained 48 percent of the variance of Purpose Play and also over 20 percent of all other GAMEFULQUEST factors. The multiple regressions (Table 7) therefore suggest that gameful qualities of a learning situation are co-constituted by both the characteristics of the learning environment and by the participants motives to participate and organize the situation playfully. This supports the fourth hypothesis of this study.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Multiple regressions between MAL factor scores and GAMEFULQUEST factor scores.

The final hypotheses we investigate consider how explicit motives and experienced gamefulness influence learning outcomes. Based on how prior research has argued that motives are both determinants for engagement and learning (Janosz, 2012) and that gameful experiences also have a positive effect on learning outcomes (Hamari et al., 2016), we hypothesized that: H5a: Gameful experience is a precedent for learning outcomes and that H5b: Explicit motives are associated with learning outcomes via gameful experience.

The effects of MAL motive dimensions and GAMEFULQUEST categories were investigated by utilizing structural equation modelling (CB-SEM) and by constructing the model presented in Figure 1. The statistical analyses were made by statistical software Stata 16.2 using the maximum likelihood method. The variables in the model are all factor score variables with the exception of the outcome variables, all of which are sum variables.

The model described in Figure 2 and reported in Table 8 explained 19 percent of the variance in Learning Method, 31 percent of the variance in Learning the Topic, and 30 percent of the variance in Critical Reflection. Experienced Purposeful Play had a strong effect on Learning the Topic, and also the strongest effect of the six GAMEFULQUEST dimensions on Learning Method. The factor of Social Connectedness had the strongest effect on Critical Reflection, followed by Purposeful Play. The only effects that Guided and Competition had on the learning outcomes were negative. Finally, the MAL motive dimensions also had statistically significant effects on all learning outcomes via the experienced gamefulness of the learning situation. Again, Autotelic had the strongest effects of the motive dimensions, but also Capability and Social had positive impacts on learning. The effect of Achievement on learning was very weak, the Boredom motive had mostly negative influence on learning, although these effects were also very weak.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Structural model for investigating the direct effects of experienced gamefulness and indirect effects of explicit motives on learning outcomes.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. The direct effects of experienced gamefulness and indirect effects of motives on learning.

Discussion

Our guiding principle for this explorative study has been the idea that learning and playing, the activities that are still more or less considered through different academic contexts, carry phenomenal similarities that could be revealed by researching autonomous learners’ motives for participating in a MOOC that does not involve any intentionally gameful features. We conducted the investigation empirically by collecting survey responses from students of the free Elements of an AI online course. The collected self-report measures concerned their experiences of (1) motives to participate in the course, (2) contextual engagement during the course, and (3) learning outcomes. In regard to these domains of the learning process, we carried out three respective research tasks in which we investigated: (1) how playfulness/gamefulness was manifested in participants’ motives for learning, (2) how engagement with the course exhibited playful/gameful organization of experiences and how the measured gamefulness related to both learning motives and engagement, and finally (3) how playful/gameful motives and engagement associated with the learning outcomes.

The results of this study demonstrated that playfulness and autonomy were both prominent and significant factors across the learning process. Playfulness was here operationalized in a two-fold manner, conforming to Masek and Stenros’s (2021) conceptualization that focuses on both an individual’s playful dispositions (i.e., playful motives) and playful or gameful organization of learning experiences. Autotelic motives, in which the learning experience was expected to be fun, challenging, and entertaining, and which also denoted a person’s interest in experiencing autonomy, yielded the greatest (indirect) positive effect on learning outcomes, as well as the greatest effect on gameful organization of learning experiences (H5b). Of the motive categories, Autotelic motives most clearly highlighted the nature of a playful disposition. Moreover, several dimensions of experienced gamefulness in learning had direct positive effects on learning outcomes (H5a). An important thing to note here is that the playful and gameful dimensions that seemed to have the most significant role did not relate to short-term rewards or competition, but rather, they related to a more profound and playful predisposition that was associated with freedom of choice and autonomy. This parallels some previous notions of gamification researchers (Suh et al., 2018) asserting that gamification should strive for utilizing diverse types of gameful dynamics that help people to satisfy their long-term psychological needs. At least, our results imply that mere use of badges, points, and leaderboards would not be the best solutions for generating effective playfulness in learners. On the contrary, our results highlight that playfulness had a significant role in self-regulative motivational development during the whole process of learning: from a person’s playful attitude to the contextual engagement with learning activities that organize playful experiences and actualizes the person’s playful self.

The dimensions identified by the Motives for Autonomous Learning (MAL) inventory suggest that reasons to participate in online courses do not only inform us about learning motives but also, and perhaps even more importantly, about prevalent predispositions to engage with activities that enable and facilitate autonomous experiences. In this latter regard, it makes sense that the six factors Capability, Autotelic, Achievement, Social, Boredom, and Reward share profound similarities with motives to play digital games as participating in both of these activity types is indeed voluntary and autonomous (H1). These results indicate that models that are developed for measuring contextual motives may also generate knowledge about more general and fundamental predispositions that direct our activities across multiple contexts of experience. In game research literature, contextual and general approaches to gaming motives are often separated from each other (Ryan et al., 2006; Yee, 2006). Our research adds to this discussion by providing empirical evidence that reveals that it is not reasonable to establish a clear-cut distinction between contextual and general models, but rather to ask instead what we can learn about general human motivation by studying our reasons to engage with particular activities, such as learning and playing games.

It was revealed in this study that GAMEFULQUEST, an instrument developed for assessing experienced gamefulness of a situation, did not function entirely as was anticipated. The instrument did measure several dimensions similar to the model presented in the original validation study (Högberg et al., 2019), but Playfulness and Accomplishment did not form their own factors. Instead, these factors merged together with each other alongside the additional items we developed for measuring autonomy, thus forming a factor we call Purposeful Play (H2). The combination of those items that loaded on Purposeful Play reminds us of classic game definitions which argue that attainable, purposeful goals and autonomous players who are free to pursue mastery are necessary conditions for all games (Avedon, 1971; Avedon and Sutton-Smith, 1971; Mead, 2015). A reading of classic game definitions supports an interpretation of the EFA results (Table 3) according to which constitutive elements for all game experiences loaded on the Purposeful Play factor (autonomy, purposeful goal, challenge, and feedback) whereas frequently occurring elements of game experiences (Social, Guided, Competition, Absorption, and Effort) each formed a factor of their own. This is an important interpretation since Purposeful Play was revealed to be the factor that had a more significant effect on learning than the other factors of GAMEFULQUEST.

Purposeful Play did not only predict learning outcomes but this dimension was also more strongly correlated with affective, behavioral, and cognitive engagement than the other aspects of experienced gamefulness (Table 6). Again, the results revealed that not all factors of GAMEFULQUEST were meaningfully correlated with engagement (H3a). Competition, for instance, was not correlated at all with affective and cognitive engagement, and only weakly with behavioral engagement. Our investigation on how motives were associated with engagement furthermore revealed that the Autotelic motive was the strongest precedent for both affective and behavioral engagement whereas Capability and Autotelic both predicted a higher cognitive engagement in a learning experience. But it was found again that not all motives were positively related to engagement. The Social motive, for instance, was not associated with engagement at all, and the Boredom motive predicted a lower level of affective, behavioral, and cognitive engagement (H3b).

Furthermore, it was found that all four MICI dimensions, which relate to motivational support provided by the instructional context, strongly correlated with the Purposeful Play factor. Only MICI’s Autonomy Support dimension also had significant correlations to Social and Guided factors. This result implies that Purposeful Play exhibited a positive association with experiential dimensions of instructional support and thus arguably contributed strongly to the motivational climate of learning.

A regression between participation motives of an autonomous learner and experienced gamefulness of a learning situation revealed that motives are clearly related to how the learning situation is experienced and playfully organized by the learner. The Autotelic motive strongly predicted Purposeful Play and Absorption. The motive of Capability was associated with experienced Effort, and the Achievement motive with felt Competition. Again, not all motives predicted experienced gamefulness (e.g., Reward), and all motive factors were uniquely associated with GAMEFULQUEST dimensions (H4). The most striking effect revealed in this study is also the most interesting and potent. Autotelic was the main precedent for Purposeful Play, which was the most significant predictor for learning outcomes. These findings effectively reveal that experienced gamefulness is not to be regarded as a quality applicable only to games or gamified situations. Instead, aspects of gamefulness are present across learning situations, but they are also unequal from the perspective of expected learning outcomes.

Implications

All in all, this research has shed light on understanding motives not only as predispositions for engagement (cf., Thrash et al., 2012), but rather as meta-regulative resources that are immanently and dynamically involved during activities of the whole learning process. In other words, motives for participating in learning were not detached from contextual engagement. On the basis of the results, we would argue that the relationship between motives and engagement is reciprocal and co-constitutive in nature. It seems that motives are entangled with the experience of learning and, for example, how this experience is structured in terms of playfulness. As we have discussed earlier in this paper, playful motives represent only one, albeit very impactful, domain of motivational dispositions (i.e., motivation background) that has such a capacity to contribute to the organization of contextual experiences. For example, with the Capability motive, which particularly related to the development of one’s competence, there was a related emphasis on situationally experiencing Effort. Our suggestion is that future studies investigating motives and engagement should more broadly further investigate the co-constitutional nature of motives during engagement with learning activities.

In regard to practical implications, the results of this study strongly put forward an idea that playfulness or gamefulness does not have to be explicitly manifested in the design of the course material of the learning environment. As we have seen in this particular case, playfulness was exhibited as a prominent experiential aspect of participating in an online course, even though the course was not intentionally gamified. In other words, playfulness seemed to emerge as a property of the learner’s intent and the personal ways of organizing the learning experience. This aptly reminds us of Sicart’s (2014, p. 11) assertion that “play is appropriative, in that it takes the context in which it exists and cannot be totally predetermined by such context.” In practice, it means that gamification of learning, hence implementing game-like elements in the learning activities or the context, does not determine playful/gameful experiences. Gamification, rather, is about constructing the learning environments in a manner that affords play, that is, promotes (or at least avoids denying) the possibility of playful/gameful attitudes and experiences for the autonomous learner.

Limitations and Future Research Directions

The present work has certain limitations that should be considered when interpreting its results and making conclusions. One of the prominent limitations relates to the fact that, instead of adopting a longitudinal methodological approach, only a single point of measurement was used for investigating different phases of the learning process. Another thing to keep in mind is that the present study focused on the users of only a single MOOC. In the future, similar research design should be replicated in studies that are targeted at different types of learning, for gaining a better understanding of the effect of different contexts in terms of different pedagogical approaches, learning topics, and motivational climates. It is also important to consider replicating the survey in a more experimental fashion, for instance in the immediate situation in which learning takes place. By doing so, the impact of motives on the ongoing experience could be investigated by making use of multi-method approaches including psycho-physiological measures and qualitative interviews. Moreover, comparative studies could also be targeted toward the users of (entertainment) game applications, and their experiences regarding motivational development.

In terms of playfulness, we may consider our study as an empirical case of Masek and Stenros’s (2021) conceptual synthesis. Hence, our findings illustrate how playfulness is reciprocally present in the motivational orientation that directs our contextual actions, as well as in the ways these contextual actions organize our experiences. The findings provide support for this conceptual framework in which playfulness is seen within two ontological stances—as an attitude and an activity—both of which are tied to contextual engagement. And finally, in terms of the basic need satisfaction of the self-determination theory (Deci and Ryan, 1991), our findings also remind us researchers not to consider basic needs, regarding autonomy, competence and relatedness, merely as background factors of motivation, but rather as meta-regulative resources for continuous motivational development.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent from the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin was not required to participate in this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

JV, KT, and TV contributed to conception, design of the study, and wrote sections of the manuscript. JV organized the data and performed and reported the statistical analyses. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work is funded by Business Finland, the government organization for innovation funding (9214/31/2019).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to express their gratitude to all the participants in the study and would specifically like to thank Oskari Koskela, Mikko Myllykoski, and Antti Päivinen for their help in planning and organizing the study. The authors also wish to thank all the collaborating partners at Reaktor and University of Helsinki for their valuable support.

References

Aarseth, E. (2007). “I fought the law: transgressive play and the implied player,” in DiGRA ’07–Proceedings of the 2007 DiGRA International Conference: Situated Play, The University of Tokyo; September, 2007, 130–133.

Google Scholar

Adams, E. (2014). Fundamentals of Game Design. 3rd Edn. San Francisco: New Riders.

Google Scholar

Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: goals, structures, and student motivation. J. Educ. Psychol. 84, 261–271. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.84.3.261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., and Reschly, A. L. (2006). Measuring cognitive and psychological engagement: validation of the student engagement instrument. J. Sch. Psychol. 44, 427–445. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2006.04.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avedon, E. M. (1971). “The structural elements of games,” in Study of Games. eds. E. Avedon and B. Sutton-Smith (New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.), 419–426.

Google Scholar

Avedon, E., and Sutton-Smith, B. (eds.) (1971). Study of Games. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Google Scholar

Bahn, D. (2007). Orientation of nurses towards formal and informal learning: motives and perceptions. Nurse Educ. Today 27, 723–730. doi: 10.1016/j.nedt.2006.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beckmann, J., and Heckhausen, H. (2018). “Motivation as a function of expectancy and incentive,” in Motivation and Action. 3rd Edn. eds. J. Heckhausen and H. Heckhausen (New York: Routledge), 163–220.

Google Scholar

Bogost, I. (2007). Persuasive Games: The Expressive Power of Videogames. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Brown, T. A. (2015). Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research. 2nd Edn. New York: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Bruner, J. S., Jolly, A., and Sylva, K. (eds.) (1976). Play: Its Role in Development and Evolution. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

Google Scholar

Brunstein, J. C. (2018). “Implicit and explicit motives,” in Motivation and Action. 3rd Edn. eds. J. Heckhausen and H. Heckhausen (New York: Routledge), 369–405.

Google Scholar

Caillois, R. (2001). Man, Play and Games. Chicago: University of Illinois Press.

Google Scholar

Charsky, D. (2010). From edutainment to serious games: A change in the use of game characteristics. Games Cult. 5, 177–198. doi: 10.1177/1555412009354727

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christenson, S. L., Reschly, A. L., and Wylie, C. (eds.) (2012). Handbook of Research on Student Engagement. Vol. 840. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Coleman, J. S. (1971). “Learning through games,” in Study of Games. eds. E. Avedon and B. Sutton-Smith (New York: John Wiley & Sons), 322–329.

Google Scholar

Crawford, C. (1984). The Art of Computer Game Design. Berkeley, California: Osborne/McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Curtis, H. S. (1915). Education Through Play. New York: Macmillan.

Google Scholar

De Grove, F., Breuer, J., Chen, V. H. H., Quant, T., Rabindra, R., and Van Looy, J. (2017). Validating the digital games motivation scale for comparative research between countries. Commun. Res. Rep. 34, 37–47. doi: 10.1080/08824096.2016.1250070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (1991). “A motivational approach to self: integration in personality” in Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Perspectives on Motivation 38. ed. R. Dienstbier (Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press), 237–288.

Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2012). “Motivation, personality, and development within embedded social contexts: An overview of self-determination theory” in The Oxford Handbook of Human Motivation. ed. R. M. Ryan (New York: Oxford University Press), 85–107.

Google Scholar

Demetrovics, Z., Urbán, R., Nagygyörgy, K., Farkas, J., Zilahy, D., Mervó, B., et al. (2011). Why do you play? The development of the motives for online gaming questionnaire (MOGQ). Behav. Res. Methods 43, 814–825. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0091-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deterding, S., Dixon, D., Khaled, R., and Nacke, L. (2011). “From game design elements to gamefulness: defining gamification,” in Proceedings of MindTrek’11; September 28–30, 2011, Tampere, Finland.

Google Scholar

Di Paolo, E. A., Rohde, M., and De Jaegher, H. (2010). “Horizons for the enactive mind: values, social interaction, and play,” in Enaction Toward a New Paradigm for Cognitive Science. eds. J. Stewart, O. Gapenne, and E. A. Paolo (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press), 33–87.

Google Scholar

Di Paolo, E., and Thompson, E. A. (2014). The enactive approach. In The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition, ed. L. Shapiro (New York: Routledge), 68–76.

Google Scholar

Diseth, Å., Pallesen, S., Brunborg, G. S., and Larsen, S. (2010). Academic achievement among first semester undergraduate psychology students: the role of course experience, effort, motives and learning strategies. High. Educ. 59, 335–352. doi: 10.1007/s10734-009-9251-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DiStefano, C., Min, Z., and Mîndrilã, D. (2009). Understanding and using factor scores: considerations for the applied researcher. Pract. Assess. Res. Eval. 14, 1–11. doi: 10.7275/da8t-4g52

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccles, J., and Wang, M.-T. (2012). “Part I commentary: so what is student engagement anyway?” in Handbook of Research on Student Engagement. eds. S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, and C. Wylie (New York: Springer), 133–145.

Google Scholar

Elliot, A. J., and Thrash, T. (2001). Achievement goals and the hierarchical model of achievement motivation. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 13, 139–156. doi: 10.1023/A:1009057102306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faiella, F., and Ricciardi, M. (2015). Gamification and learning: a review of issues and research. J. Learn. Knowledge Soc. 11, 13–21. doi: 10.20368/1971-8829/1072

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Froese, T., and Stewart, J. (2012). Enactive cognitive science and biology of cognition: A response to Humberto Maturana. Cybernetics Hum. Knowing 19, 61–74.

Google Scholar

Gee, J. P. (2003). What Video Games Have to Teach us About Learning and Literacy. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

Google Scholar

Gibson, J. J. (1977). “The theory of affordances,” in Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing: Towards an Ecological Psychology. eds. R. Shaw and J. Brandsforth (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum associates), 67–82.

Google Scholar

Goffman, E. (1959). The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. New York: Anchor Books.

Google Scholar

Goffman, E. (1961). Encounters. Two Studies in the Sociology of Interaction. Mansfield Centre, CT: Martino Publishing.

Google Scholar

Gollwitzer, P. M., and Oettingen, G. (2012). “Goal attainment” in The Oxford Handbook of Human Motivation. ed. R. M. Ryan (New York: Oxford University Press), 208–231.

Google Scholar

Groos, K. (1908). The Play of Man. New York, London: D. Appleton and Company.

Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., and Anderson, R. E. (2010). Multivariate Data Analysis. New Jersey: Pearson Education.

Google Scholar

Hamari, J., Koivisto, J., and Pakkanen, T. (2014). “Do persuasive technologies persuade? a review of empirical studies,” in Persuasive Technology: 9th International Conference (LNCS 8462), PERSUASIVE 2014; May 21–23, 2014, Padua, Italy.

Google Scholar

Hamari, J., Shernoff, D. J., Rowe, E., Coller, B., Asbell-Clarke, J., and Edwards, T. (2016). Challenging games help students learn: An empirical study on engagement, flow and immersion in game-based learning. Comput. Hum. Behav. 54, 170–179. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.07.045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henson, R. K., and Roberts, J. K. (2006). Use of exploratory factor analysis in published research: common errors and some comment on improved practice. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 66, 393–416. doi: 10.1177/0013164405282485

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, E. T. (2008). Culture and personality: variability across universal motives as the missing link. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2, 608–634. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00075.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Högberg, J., Hamari, J., and Wästlund, E. (2019). Gameful experience questionnaire (GAMEFULQUEST): an instrument for measuring the perceived gamefulness of system use. User Model. User Adap. Inter. 29, 619–660. doi: 10.1007/s11257-019-09223-w

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huizinga, J. (1949). Homo Ludens: A Study of the Play-Element in Culture. United Kingdom: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Janosz, M. (2012). “Part IV commentary: outcomes of engagement and engagement as an outcome: Some consensus, divergences, and unanswered questions,” in Handbook of Research on Student Engagement. eds. S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, and C. Wylie (New York: Springer), 695–703.

Google Scholar

Kahn, A. S., Shen, C., Lu, L., Ratan, R. A., Coary, S., Hou, J., et al. (2015). The Trojan player typology: A cross-genre, cross-cultural, behaviorally validated scale of video game play motivations. Comput. Hum. Behav. 49, 354–361. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.03.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karhulahti, V.-M. (2015). Defining the videogame. Game Stu. 15

Google Scholar

Kember, D., Leung, D. Y. P., Jones, A., Loke, A. Y., McKay, J., Sinclair, K., et al. (2000). Development of a questionnaire to measure the level of reflective thinking. Assess. Eval. High. Educ. 25, 381–395. doi: 10.1080/713611442

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinnunen, J., Taskinen, K., and Mäyrä, F. (2020). Pelaajabarometri 2020: Pelaamista koronan aikaan. Tampereen yliopisto: Tampere.

Google Scholar

Kowert, R., and Quandt, T. (2016). The Video Game Debate. Unravelling the Physical, Social, and Psychological Effects of Video Games. New York: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Kraft, I. (1971). “Pedagogical futility in fun and games?” in Study of Games. eds. E. Avedon and B. Sutton-Smith (New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.), 326–329.

Google Scholar

Lam, S. F., Jimerson, S., Wong, B., Kikas, E., Shin, H., Veiga, F. H., et al. (2014). Understanding and measuring student engagement in school: the results of an international study from 12 countries. Sch. Psychol. Q. 29, 213–232. doi: 10.1037/spq0000057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, S. F., and Law, Y. K. (2007). The roles of instructional practices and motivation in writing performance. J. Exp. Educ. 75, 145–164. doi: 10.3200/JEXE.75.2.145-164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, S. F., Pak, T. S., and Ma, W. Y. K. (2007). “Motivating instructional contexts inventory” in Issues in the Psychology of Motivation. ed. P. R. Zelick (Hauppauge, NJ: Nova Science), 119–136.

Google Scholar

Leino, O. T. (2010). Emotions in Play on the Constitution of Emotion in Solitary Computer Game Play. (Copenhagen, Denmark: IT University of Copenhagen.

Google Scholar

Lieberman, J. N. (1977). Playfulness: Its Relationship to Imagination and Creativity. New York: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Mace, W. M. (1977). “James J. Gibson’s strategy for perceiving: ask not what’s inside your head, but what’s your head inside of,” in Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing: Towards an Ecological Psychology. eds. R. Shaw and J. Brandsforth (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 43–65.

Google Scholar

Martin, A. J. (2012). “Part II commentary: motivation and engagement: conceptual, operational, and empirical clarity,” in Handbook of Research on Student Engagement. eds. S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, and C. Wylie (New York: Springer), 303–311.

Google Scholar

Masek, L., and Stenros, J. (2021). The meaning of playfulness: A review of the contemporary definitions of the concept across disciplines. J. Comp. Game Culture 12, 13–37.

Google Scholar

Matsunaga, M. (2010). How to factor-analyze your data right: Do’s, Don’ts, and how-to’s. Int. J. Psychol. Res. 3, 97–110. doi: 10.21500/20112084.854

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, R. E. (2019). Computer games in education. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 70, 531–549. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-102744

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., and Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychol. Rev. 96, 690–702. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.690

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mead, G. H. (2015). Mind, Self & Society. The Definitive Edition. Chicago and London: The University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Noë, A. (2009). Out of our Heads: Why you are Not your Brain, and Other Lessons from the Biology of Consciousness. United Kingdom: Macmillan.

Google Scholar

Phan, M. H., Keebler, J. R., and Chaparro, B. S. (2016). The development and validation of the game user experience satisfaction scale (GUESS). Hum. Factors 58, 1217–1247. doi: 10.1177/0018720816669646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piaget, J. (1952). Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood. New York: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc.

Google Scholar

Prensky, M. (2001). Digital Game Based Learning. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Proyer, R. T. (2013). The well-being of playful adults: adult playfulness, subjective well-being, physical well-being, and the pursuit of enjoyable activities. Eur. J. Hum. Res. 1, 84–98. doi: 10.7592/EJHR2013.1.1.proyer

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Przybylski, A., Rigby, C. S., and Ryan, R. M. (2010). A motivational model of video game engagement. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 14, 154–166. doi: 10.1037/a0019440

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiss, S. (2004). Multifaceted nature of intrinsic motivation: The theory of 16 basic desires. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 8, 179–193. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.8.3.179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rheinberg, F., and Engeser, S. (2018). “Intrinsic motivation and flow,” in Motivation and Action. 3rd Edn. eds. J. Heckhausen and H. Heckhausen (New York: Routledge), 579–622.

Google Scholar

Ryan, R., and Deci, L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: classic definitions and new directions. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 25, 54–67. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1999.1020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., Rigby, C. S., and Przybylski, A. (2006). The motivational pull of video games: A self-determination theory approach. Motiv. Emot. 30, 344–360. doi: 10.1007/s11031-006-9051-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sailer, M., and Homner, L. (2020). The gamification of learning: A meta-analysis. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 32, 77–112. doi: 10.1007/s10648-019-09498-w

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sansone, C., and Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation. San Diego: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Scheffer, D., and Heckhausen, H. (2018). “Trait theories of motivation,” in Motivation and Action. 3rd Edn. eds. J. Heckhausen and H. Heckhausen (New York: Routledge), 67–112.

Google Scholar

Schultheiss, O. C., and Wirth, M. M. (2018). “Biopsychological aspects of motivation.” in Motivation and Action. 3rd Edn. eds. J. Heckhausen and H. Heckhausen (New York: Routledge), 407–451.

Google Scholar

Schunk, D. H., and Usher, E. L. (2012). “Social cognitive theory and motivation” in The Oxford Handbook of Human Motivation. ed. R. M. Ryan (New York: Oxford University Press), 13–27.

Google Scholar

Sherry, J. L., Lucas, K., Greenberg, B. S., and Lachlan, K. (2006). “Video game uses and gratifications as predictors of use and game preference,” in Playing Video Games. Motives, Responses, and Consequences. eds. P. Vorderer and J. Bryant (New York: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group), 213–224.

Google Scholar

Sicart, M. (2014). Play Matters. Cambridge, MA, London: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Sinatra, G. M., Heddy, B. C., and Lombardi, D. (2015). The challenges of defining and measuring student engagement in science. Educ. Psychol. 50, 1–13. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2014.1002924

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skinner, E. A., and Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the classroom: reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and student engagement across the school year. J. Educ. Psychol. 85, 571–581. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.85.4.571

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skinner, E. A., and Pitzer, J. R. (2012). “Developmental dynamics of student engagement, coping, and everyday resilience,” in Handbook of Research on Student Engagement. eds. S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, and C. Wylie (New York: Springer), 21–44.

Google Scholar

Smith, P. K. (2005). “Play: types and functions in human development,” in Origins of the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Child Development. eds. B. J. Ellis and D. F. Bjorklund (New York, London: Guilford Press), 271–291.

Google Scholar

Squire, K. (2003). Video games in education. Int. J. Int. Games 2, 49–62.

Google Scholar

Stenros, J. (2014). In defence of a magic circle: the social, mental and cultural boundaries of play. Trans. Digital Games Res. Assoc. 1, 147–185. doi: 10.26503/todigra.v1i2.10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suh, A., Wagner, C., and Liu, L. (2018). Enhancing user engagement through gamification. J. Comput. Inf. Syst. 58, 204–213. doi: 10.1080/08874417.2016.1229143

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suits, B. (1978). The Grasshopper. Games, Life and Utopia. Peterborough: Broadview Press.

Google Scholar

Sutton-Smith, B. (1983). “Piaget, play, and cognition, revisited” in The Relationship Between Social and Cognitive Development. ed. W. F. Overton (Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 229–249.

Google Scholar

Sutton-Smith, B. (2009). The Ambiguity of Play. Cambridge and London: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in Life. Biology, Phenomenology, and the Sciences of Mind. Cambridge: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Thompson, E., and Stapleton, M. (2009). Making sense of sense-making: reflections on enactive and extended mind theories. Topoi 28, 23–30. doi: 10.1007/s11245-008-9043-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thrash, T. M., Maruskin, L. A., and Martin, C. C. (2012). “Implicit-explicit motive congruence” in The Oxford Handbook of Human Motivation. ed. R. M. Ryan (New York: Oxford University Press), 141–156.

Google Scholar

Tuuri, K., Parviainen, J., and Pirhonen, A. (2017). Who controls who? Embodied control within human-technology choreographies. Interact. Comput. 29, 494–511. doi: 10.1093/iwc/iww040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vahlo, J. (2017). An enactive account of the autonomy of videogame gameplay. Game Stu. 17

Google Scholar

Vahlo, J., and Hamari, J. (2019). “Five-factor inventory of intrinsic motivations to gameplay (IMG),” in Proceedings of the 52nd Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences, 2476–2485, Hawaii, United States. January 7–11, 2019.

Google Scholar

Vahlo, J., Smed, J., and Koponen, A. (2018). Validating gameplay activity inventory (GAIN) for modeling player profiles. User Model. User Adap. Inter. 28, 425–453. doi: 10.1007/s11257-018-9212-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., and Rosch, E. (1992). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cumberland: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Virtanen, T. E., Lerkkanen, M. K., Poikkeus, A. M., and Kuorelahti, M. (2014). Student behavioral engagement as a mediator between teacher, family, and peer support and school truancy. Learn. Individ. Differ. 36, 201–206. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L. S. (2004). Imagination and creativity in childhood. J. Russ. East Eur. Psychol. 42, 7–97. doi: 10.2753/RPO1061-0405280184

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weber, A., and Varela, F. J. (2002). Life after Kant: natural purposes and the Autopoietic foundations of biological individuality. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 1, 97–125. doi: 10.1023/A:1020368120174

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitton, N., and Moseley, A. (2014). Deconstructing engagement: rethinking involvement in learning. Simul. Gaming 45, 433–449. doi: 10.1177/1046878114554755

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wouters, P., and Van Oostendorp, H. (2017). Overview of Instructional Techniques to Facilitate Learning and Motivation of Serious Games. In Instructional Techniques to Facilitate Learning and Motivation of Serious Games (Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing), 1–16.

Google Scholar

Yee, N. (2006). Motivations for play in online games. J. Cyber Psychol. Behav. 9, 772–775. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2006.9.772

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yee, N., Ducheneaut, N., and Nelson, L. (2012). “Online gaming motivation scale.” in CHI ’12: Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, May 5–10, 2012, Austin, United States.

Google Scholar

Zainuddin, Z., Chu, S. K. W., Shujahat, M., and Perera, C. J. (2020). The impact of gamification on learning and instruction: A systematic review of empirical evidence. Educ. Res. Rev. 30:100326. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2020.100326

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zepke, N., and Leach, L. (2010). Improving student engagement: ten proposals for action. Act. Learn. High. Educ. 11, 167–177. doi: 10.1177/1469787410379680

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: playfulness, games, autonomous learner, online learning, motives, motivation, engagement, self-determination

Citation: Vahlo J, Tuuri K and Välisalo T (2022) Exploring Gameful Motivation of Autonomous Learners. Front. Psychol. 13:825840. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.825840

Received: 30 November 2021; Accepted: 24 January 2022;
Published: 25 February 2022.

Edited by:

Ramón Chacón-Cuberos, University of Granada, Spain

Reviewed by:

Noelia Parejo-Jiménez, University of Granada, Spain
Wooyoung (William) Jang, University of West Georgia, United States

Copyright © 2022 Vahlo, Tuuri and Välisalo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jukka Vahlo, anVra2EubC52YWhsb0BqeXUuZmk=

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.