Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 28 April 2022
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic Emotion Processing in Autism Spectrum Disorders View all 9 articles

Associations Between Autism Symptomatology, Alexithymia, Trait Emotional Intelligence, and Adjustment to College

  • Department of Psychology, Loyola University Chicago, Chicago, IL, United States

It has been asserted that the socio-emotional challenges associated with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) may be explained, in part, by the higher rates of alexithymia in individuals with autism. Alexithymia refers to difficulties in identifying one’s own emotional states and describing those states to others. Thus, one goal of the present study was to examine levels of alexithymia in relation to ASD symptomatology and trait emotion intelligence (EI). Trait EI is a multifaceted concept that captures emotional competencies and behavioral dispositions A second goal was to assess whether alexithymia, ASD symptomatology and trait EI served as significant predictors of adjustment to college, including academic, social, and personal-emotional adjustment. In addition to keeping with the spectrum nature of autism, our research strategy allowed us to capture those students who may not have received a formal diagnosis of ASD but report symptoms that can be indicative of ASD. This includes women, who are less likely to receive a diagnosis of ASD even when ASD symptomatology is present. The results of the study showed that students reporting higher levels of ASD symptomatology also reported significantly higher levels of alexithymia and lower trait emotional intelligence (trait EI) than those with less or no symptomatology. Alexithymia was also negatively related to trait EI, and both alexithymia and ASD symptomatology were found to be significant predictors of trait EI. However, only trait EI was a significant predictor of adjustment to college and only for social adjustment. These findings suggest that support programs that develop trait EI skills may improve the college experience for students with ASD, regardless of alexithymia or ASD symptomatology.

Introduction

Alexithymia is a multifaceted personality construct originally introduced by Nemiah et al. (1976) to describe patients who experience difficulties in identifying their own emotions due to impoverished representations of their emotional states. Put simply, alexithymia consists of an inability to identify one’s own emotional states and describe those emotional states to others. An important expansion on the definition of alexithymia to include the effects on interpersonal relationships was put forth by Bagby and Taylor (1997), suggesting that without knowledge of their own emotional experiences, individuals with alexithymia may not be able to readily imagine themselves in another person’s situation and therefore, can appear unempathetic and ineffective in modulating the emotional states of others.

Alexithymia and Autism

Research has shown that alexithymia may underlie medical, psychiatric, and behavioral problems that are affected by difficulties with affect regulation, including autism spectrum disorder (Lumley et al., 2007). Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental (neurodiverse) disorder characterized by social-emotional difficulties and restrictive and repetitive interests and behaviors (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). In individuals with ASD, the prevalence of alexithymia has been reported to be between 40 and 65%. In contrast, in neurotypical (NT) populations the prevalence ranges from 10 to 13% (Taylor et al., 1997; Salminen et al., 1999; Hill et al., 2004; Berthoz and Hill, 2005).

It has been suggested that the socio-emotional difficulties associated with ASD may be the result of co-occurring alexithymia (Bird and Cook, 2013; Kinnaird et al., 2019). In fact, it has been argued that the socio-emotional challenges associated with ASD may be better explained by the higher rates of alexithymia in this population, rather than autism per se (Bird and Cook, 2013). Providing support for this assertion, Cook et al. (2013) found that alexithymia, but not ASD traits, predicted reduced emotion facial recognition when participants were asked to label emotions portrayed on crossed-morphed faces. In other research, Kätsyri et al. (2008) found that individuals with autism were less accurate in labeling strongly degraded images of facial emotions when compared to neurotypical controls. However, this effect was no longer significant when alexithymia was used as a covariate in their study. Finally, research has shown that alexithymia, and not ASD, predicted difficulties in both facial and vocal emotion recognition (Heaton et al., 2012; Allen et al., 2013).

However, not all studies have shown this pattern of findings. For example, Stephenson et al. (2019) found that ASD symptomatology, but not alexithymia, predicted reduced eye fixation in an emotion identification task with dynamic and static stimuli requiring identification of targeted emotions. Moreover, others have rightly noted that while alexithymia rates in individuals with ASD may be elevated in comparison to their neurotypical peers, “alexithymia is neither necessary nor sufficient for an autism diagnosis, nor is it universal among autistic individuals” (Bird and Cook, 2013, p. 724). Finally, high rates of alexithymia have been found in individuals without ASD but with other disorders (e.g., substance abuse, addictive behaviors). In fact, alexithymia is considered a transdiagnostic disorder because it is a common deficit in disorders besides ASD (Grynberg et al., 2012).

Trait Emotional Intelligence and Autism

Such disparate findings across emotion studies have led some to speculate that the type of tasks used to assess the independent contributions of autism and alexithymia to emotion processing skills can affect outcomes (Kinnaird et al., 2019). In the present study, we examined whether alexithymia, or ASD traits, served as a better predictor of trait emotional intelligence (trait EI). Trait EI was targeted for several reasons. Firstly, trait EI is a multifaceted concept of emotion processing that is believed to capture emotional competencies and behavioral dispositions (Mikolajczak et al., 2006). According to Petrides et al. (2007a) and others (e.g., O’Connor et al., 2019), these emotional competencies span four areas: sociability (emotion management, assertiveness, social awareness), emotionality (emotion perception, empathy, emotion expression), self-control (emotion regulation, impulsiveness, stress management) and wellbeing (trait optimism, trait happiness, self-esteem). Secondly, past research has shown robust relations between trait EI and personality (e.g., Newsome et al., 2000), social competency (Mavroveli et al., 2007), perspective-taking in social situations (Schutte et al., 2001), and psychological adjustment (Engelberg and Sjoberg, 2004; Chapman and Hayslip, 2005). Relatedly, trait EI has been shown to predict social network quality, loneliness, depression, and life satisfaction (e.g., Laborde et al., 2014; Andrei et al., 2015). Thirdly, research has shown that programs designed to improve trait EI can be successful, producing beneficial outcomes in work (management skills) and school (teacher training) settings, as well as improvements in the quality of relationships (see Kotsou et al., 2019, for a review). Finally, while trait EI is related to alexithymia, it appears to be a distinct construct. For example, Mikolajczak et al. (2006) found that trait EI was a significant predictor of both psychological and somatic symptoms over and above alexithymia.

Although trait EI has been studied infrequently in ASD, Gökçen et al. (2014) found that scores on the Autism Quotient (AQ; Baron-Cohen et al., 2001) were negatively correlated with global trait EI. AQ scores were also negatively correlated with wellbeing, emotionality, sociability and empathy factors assessed on the trait EI measure they administered. In both adolescents and adults with ASD, trait EI has also been shown to predict interpersonal relationship scores and social stress (e.g., Montgomery et al., 2012). However, research has found that “trait” and “ability” EI skills differ in ASD. Ability EI can be defined as the interrelated set of cognitive abilities and skills that include recognizing emotions and the complex relations between emotions, reasoning and problem solving (Mayer et al., 2000; Petrides and Furnham, 2000; Brackett and Mayer, 2003; Lopes et al., 2003). Ability EI reflects how individuals think and reason about social situations, whereas trait EI provides insight about how to apply that knowledge in socio-emotional situations. Importantly, Montgomery et al. (2010) found that ability EI did not differ in individuals with and without ASD (although see Boily et al., 2017). They asserted that when provided with enough time to reason through information, high-functioning individuals with ASD are not impaired in the cognitive processes involved in decoding emotional situations. However, when needing to apply that reasoning in social situations, they may struggle to do so. According to Montgomery et al. (2012), individuals with ASD “have the knowledge and cognition to understand and reason about emotional information, but their application (of it) in natural settings is impaired” (p. 9).

Alexithymia, Trait Emotional Intelligence, and Adjustment to College

Such assertions support findings showing that knowledge and understanding of emotions are not significantly impaired in ASD (Harms et al., 2010), but that the application of that knowledge in real-time socio-emotional situations may be a challenge. In college settings, this may be particularly important as students are not only learning how to navigate the academic demands of college, but the social and emotional ones as well. Past research has shown that students with ASD traits often perform at levels comparable to neurotypical students academically (e.g., Jackson et al., 2018). Nevertheless, students with ASD are considerably more likely than both neurotypical students and students with other disabilities to drop out of college (Sanford et al., 2011; Taylor and Seltzer, 2011; Shattuck et al., 2012). In fact, students with ASD often report that their academic efforts and abilities will enable them to succeed (e.g., Jackson et al., 2018). In contrast, studies have shown that students with ASD exhibit less social and personal-emotional adjustment to college, feeling that they are not fully integrated into the social milieu of school (Trevisan and Birmingham, 2016). They also report heighted levels of anxiety and other personal-emotional adjustment issues (Accardo, 2017; Cox et al., 2017; Davidson et al., 2021). These socio-emotional challenges are believed to lead to feelings of disconnect in the college setting.

However, while alexithymia, ASD symptomatology and trait EI may be associated with socio-emotional issues, to our knowledge they have not been explored as potential underlying factors to college adjustment issues. Without this knowledge, it is difficult to design support programs for students with ASD that address the factors leading to socio-emotional challenges at college.

The Present Study

In light of the studies cited, a primary goal of the present study was to examine whether alexithymia and ASD symptomatology uniquely predict trait EI. Also of interest was the extent to which alexithymia, ASD symptomatology and trait EI are associated with academic and socio-emotional adjustment to college in students with varying or no ASD symptomatology. In keeping with the spectrum nature of ASD, ASD symptomatology was treated as a continuous variable, given that ASD traits fall along a continuum in the general population (Constantino and Todd, 2005; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). This allowed us to include students that exhibit ASD symptomatology without a formal diagnosis, and differs from studies that have focused almost exclusively on students with a formal ASD diagnosis recruited through their campus student accessibility offices. Such practices may exclude students, given that students with ASD are known to under-identify with these offices (e.g., White et al., 2011). Additionally, studies have shown that students may exhibit ASD traits without a formal diagnosis, especially high-functioning individuals (e.g., Newman et al., 2009; Cox et al., 2017). This is particularly true for females who are more likely than males to be under-diagnosed (Lai et al., 2019; Hull et al., 2020). Thus, women are often under-represented in studies on autism, particularly those studies with high-functioning individuals (Hull et al., 2020). Others have used a similar strategy to examine how symptoms associated with ASD impact functioning, regardless of diagnosis (e.g., Trevisan and Birmingham, 2016; Dijkhuis et al., 2020; Lei et al., 2020). With this information in mind, the following research questions were addressed:

1. What are the relations between ASD symptomatology and alexithymia, trait EI and adjustment to college? Do adults with higher levels of ASD symptomatology present higher levels of alexithymia and lower levels of trait EI than those reporting less or no symptomatology? How is ASD symptomatology related to college adjustment variables? Additionally, do the patterns of relations change for the DSM-5 compatible subscale scores (i.e., the social communication and interaction and the restricted interests and repetitive behaviors subscales on the SRS-2) on these study variables?

2. How does alexithymia relate to trait EI and college adjustment? Does a negative relation exist between alexithymia and trait EI? That is, are higher levels of alexithymia associated with lower levels of trait EI? Is alexithymia related to negative adjustment to college (academic, social, personal-emotional)?

3. Which is the better predictor of trait EI, ASD symptomatology or alexithymia?

4. To what extent does ASD symptomatology, alexithymia and trait EI predict adjustment to college?

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 150 college students at a private university in a large city in the Midwest region of the U.S. participated (Mage = 20;03, range = 18;04–25;02). Students were predominately (67%) Caucasian and female (82%). These demographics are consistent with the demographics of the school and the introductory psychology classes in which students were recruited. A post hoc power analysis was conducted to establish whether our study had an adequate sample size to determine at least a medium effect (Cohen’s d = 0.5) at an alpha level of 0.05. The results revealed the study had sufficient power for our analyses (Power = 0.83). Table 1 provides additional information about our sample. Note that because individuals reporting higher levels of ASD symptomatology could differ in important ways from those reporting less or no ASD symptomatology, we also provide additional information about the students in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics.

The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS-2; Constantino and Gruber, 2012) and students’ ASD diagnostic history, if provided, were used to determine ASD symptomatology. The SRS-2 is a widely used measure that was chosen because it has been shown to capture symptomatology that can be indicative of ASD (Chan et al., 2017). Specifically, the SRS-2 is a 65-item questionnaire that identifies the presence and severity of social impairment associated with ASD, as well as repetitive and restricted behaviors (Constantino and Gruber, 2012). On the SRS-2, individuals rate items about their behaviors during the past 6 months using a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not true) to 4 (almost always true). Scores on the SRS-2 are presented as T-scores (μ = 50, SD = 10) and take into account respondents’ gender, with T-scores of 60 and above indicative of clinically significant difficulties associated with ASD. In our sample, 89 students scored below threshold for ASD symptomatology on the SRS-2 (Mage = 20;05, range = 18;10–23;07) and 62 students scored above threshold for symptomatology that could be indicative of ASD on the SRS-2 (Mage = 20;01, range = 18;04–25;02). On the SRS-2, severity of symptoms can be further delineated, with scores of 60T–65T in the mild range, 66T–75T in the moderate range and 76T or higher in the severe range (Constantino and Gruber, 2012). T-scores in the range of 59 and below are generally not associated with clinically significant ASD symptomatology. Table 1 presents the percentage of students in our study falling within those ranges and participant characteristics of those scoring above and below the cut-off.

The SRS-2 was used because its scores corroborate gold-standard diagnostic tools of ASD (e.g., ADOS-2 and ADI-R) and the constructs that are considered essential for an ASD diagnosis (Bölte et al., 2008; Reszka et al., 2014). Specifically, two components of the SRS-2 have been validated (i.e., social communication and interaction and restricted interests and repetitive behavior) to corroborate ASD diagnostic criteria outlined by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013; Frazier et al., 2014).

The strategy of using the SRS-2 to examine ASD symptomatology regardless of formal ASD diagnostic history, has been used by others, including when assessing the connections between ASD symptomatology and mood disorders (Morie et al., 2019). Although not without limitations, this method has become increasingly common when working with samples with high-functioning females and with on-line samples, where observational confirmation of a diagnosis is impossible. In our sample, the SRS-2 had good internal consistency (α = 0.78).

With that said, 13 students (9 males, 4 females) indicated that they had received an ASD diagnosis and were receiving services for an autism diagnosis at the university’s student accessibility office. Although analyses were underpowered to detect significant differences between those with and without a formal diagnosis of ASD, the scatterplots of the 13 participants were compared to the larger sample. The responses of those with a previous ASD diagnosis aligned with the distribution of the larger sample across all measures and outliers were not detected. Of the students scoring below threshold for ASD symptomatology (≤T59), none reported that they had been diagnosed with ASD.

Measures

Alexithymia

The Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS; Bagby et al., 1994) is a widely used 20-item self-report measure. Participants rate statements on a 5-point scale, with 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The TAS-20 consists of three subscales: difficulty identifying feelings (DIF), difficulty describing feeling (DDF) and externally oriented thinking (EOT). Sample items include: “When I am upset, I don’t know if I am sad, frightened or angry.” (DIF), “It is difficult for me to find the right words for my feelings.” (DDF), and “I prefer talking to people about their daily activities rather than their feelings.” (EOT). In our sample, the total score on the TAS-20 was found to have adequate internal consistency (α = 0.76).

Trait Emotional Intelligence

The Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire-Short Form (TEIQue-SF; Petrides, 2009) is a 30-item measure based on EI theory, which conceptualizes EI as a personality trait. The TEIQue-SF provides a total, or global, trait EI score, along with scores on four trait EI factors: wellbeing (e.g., trait happiness and self-esteem), self-control (e.g., emotion regulation, impulsiveness), emotionality (e.g., trait empathy), and sociability (e.g., emotion management, assertiveness). Two items from each of the 15 facets of the 153-item long version of the TEIQue are included on the short form, based primarily on their correlations with the corresponding total facet scores (Cooper and Petrides, 2010). In our sample, the TEIQue-SF was found to have good internal consistency (α = 0.86).

Adjustment to College

The Student Adaption to College Questionnaire (SACQ; Baker and Siryk, 1999) is a 67-item self-report measure that assesses students’ adjustment to college. On this measure, students rate items using a 9-point scale, ranging from 1 (doesn’t apply to me at all) to 9 (applies very closely to me) to determine adjustment across four subscales (academic, social, personal-emotional, institutional affiliation). Although the scale provides a full-scale score, most studies focus on adjustment as measured by its four subscales. These include academic adjustment assesses how well the student manages the academic demands of school including the adequacy of their studying and academic efforts, as well as their attitudes toward their course of study. Social adjustment captures the degree to which the student has integrated themselves into the social milieu of college whereas personal-emotional adjustment reflects students’ psychological and physical wellbeing. Finally, institutional attachment captures how much a student identifies with and is emotionally attached to their university. Although scores on institutional attachment were gathered, they were not analyzed for the present study.

The SACQ was selected because it is one of the most widely used measures of college adjustment and has been well-validated, with the four SACQ domains associated with grade point average, use of campus services, and attrition (Beyers and Goossens, 2002; Credé and Niehorster, 2012). Moreover, the measure has been used previously in college students with clinical and subclinical levels of ASD symptomatology (Trevisan and Birmingham, 2016; White et al., 2016). On the SACQ, raw scores are converted to T-scores (μ = 50, SD = 10). T-scores are continuous and take into account year in school and sex of student. In our sample, internal consistency was found to be adequate on the subscales: academic (α = 0.77), social (α = 0.71), and personal-emotional adjustment (α = 0.79).

Procedure

Following IRB approval and informed consent, students completed the randomized measures through a link to a secure online platform. When done, all participants were debriefed about the study.

Results

Preliminary Analyses

All data analyses were performed using IBM SPSS (v. 27.0; Chicago, IL, United States). Preliminary analyses were conducted on the dependent variables to ensure appropriateness of parametric procedures. This included checks for the normality of distributions and for outliers. Outliers were defined as values that were ≥ three standard deviations from the mean and were not part of the normal distribution (Cohen et al., 2003). Skewness was defined as variable values greater than ± 2.0 whereas kurtosis was defined by values greater than ± 7.0 (West et al., 1995). Moreover, the normality of the cumulative distribution was examined on each of the dependent variables (alexithymia, trait EI, college adjustment variables and SRS-2). These checks showed normally distributed data for all dependent variables in the sample and the groups in Table 1 (skewness ranged from –1.46 to 1.70; kurtosis ranged from –2.10 to 2.49) and non-significant Levene’s tests of homogeneity of variance between groups in Table 1 [F(1, 46) ≤ 2.57, p ≥ 0.11].

All analyses of total and subscale scores maintained the continuous nature of the variables. In addition, raw scores from each measure were used for all data analyses. Effect sizes and covariance inflation factor (VIF) were also calculated. VIF measures how much the variance of an independent variable is influenced or inflated by its correlation with the other independent variables and is important to determine when using regression analyses. VIF results and effect sizes are given in the tables.

Relations Between Autism Spectrum Disorder Symptomatology and Study Variables

Our first research question explored the relations between ASD symptomatology and alexithymia, trait EI and college adjustment through Pearson correlations tests. In terms of the associations between ASD symptomatology and alexithymia, higher SRS-2 raw scores were related to higher alexithymia total scores on the TAS-20, r(144) = 0.78, p < 0.001. Additionally, higher scores on the SRS-2 were associated with higher scores on each of the subscales of the TAS-20. This included difficulty identifying feelings (DIF), r(149) = 0.74, p < 0.001, difficulty describing feelings (DDF), r(149) = 0.68, p < 0.001, and externally oriented thinking (EOT), r(146) = 0.50, p < 0.001. As shown in Table 2, the same pattern of findings held for the both DSM-5 compatible subscales of the SRS-2 (i.e., social communication and interaction and restricted interests and repetitive behaviors). Lastly, higher ASD symptomatology scores were also related to elevated alexithymia scores that were at or above clinical threshold (≥61) for alexithymia, r(142) = 0.57, p < 0.001.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Relations between study variables.

Additionally, ASD symptomatology was related to lower trait EI scores, r(149) = –0.72, p < 0.001. As shown in Table 2, the two dimensional subscales of the SRS-2 were each significantly related to lower trait EI. Finally, significant correlations were also found between ASD symptomatology and social adjustment, r(141) = –0.28, p < 0.001, and personal-emotional adjustment to college subscales, r(142) = –0.31, p < 0.001, on the SACQ. However, ASD symptomatology was not significantly related to academic adjustment, r(141) = 0.06, p = 0.508.

Our second set of research questions examined the associations between alexithymia and trait EI and alexithymia and college adjustment. As expected, a significant negative relation was found between alexithymia and trait EI, r(143) = –0.68, p < 0.001. This pattern extended to each of the subscales of the TAS-20, which were also negatively associated with trait EI (see Table 2). Although we were not sure whether alexithymia would be related to college adjustment, significant negative relations were found between alexithymia and social adjustment, r(135) = –0.20, p < 0.001, and personal-emotional adjustment, r(136) = –0.30, p < 0.001, to college. However, alexithymia was not related to academic adjustment, r(135) = 0.06, p = 0.458 (see Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Regression results for research questions.

Predictors of Trait Emotion Intelligence and College Adjustment

Given these significant patterns of correlations, our third research question explored whether ASD symptomatology or alexithymia was a better predictor of trait EI. Simultaneous regression analyses showed that the overall model was significant, F(2, 142) = 87.77, p < 0.001, adjusted R2 = 0.55. Moreover, both alexithymia and ASD symptomatology were found to be significant predictors of trait EI. These results are shown in Table 3.

To address our final research question, we used simultaneous regression analyses to explore the best predictors of academic, social and personal-emotional adjustment to college in students. Predictors included alexithymia, ASD symptomatology and trait EI total scores. The analyses showed that the overall models were significant for social adjustment to college, F(3, 131) = 7.73, p < 0.001, adjusted R2 = 0.13, and personal-emotional adjustment, F(3, 131) = 6.61, p < 0.001; adjusted R2 = 0.11. However, the overall model for academic adjustment was not significant, F(3, 131) = 0.311, p < 0.817, adjusted R2 = 0.007. Our results show that only trait EI was a significant predictor of social adjustment (β = 0.406, p = 0.002), whereas alexithymia and ASD symptomatology were not significant predictors of academic, social or personal-emotional adjustment to college. These results are shown in Table 2.

Discussion

The findings of the present study are consistent with past studies (e.g., Hill et al., 2004; Berthoz and Hill, 2005), with greater levels of ASD symptomatology associated with significantly higher total and subscale alexithymia scores. Additionally, higher ASD symptomatology scores were also related to elevated scores that were at or above clinical threshold (≥61) for alexithymia. These findings are important because the presence of co-occurring ASD symptomatology and alexithymia increases the chances of mood disorders and other psychiatric conditions that impinge upon both socio-emotional and general functioning (Grabe et al., 2008; Kinnaird et al., 2019).

In fact, it has been asserted that it is alexithymia and not autism per se, that contributes to emotion impairments (Bird and Cook, 2013). Support for this assertion has been found on different emotion recognition tasks (Kätsyri et al., 2008; Cook et al., 2013). In the present study, the connections between alexithymia and trait emotional intelligence (trait EI) were explored because trait EI reflects a set of important emotional competencies, including social awareness, emotion perception, and emotion regulation (Petrides et al., 2007b). It has also been shown to have significant associations with self-monitoring and empathic perspective (Schutte et al., 2001), measures of psychological adjustment (Chapman and Hayslip, 2005) and social network quality and life satisfaction (Laborde et al., 2014; Andrei et al., 2015).

In the present study, higher levels of alexithymia were related to lower trait EI. However, higher ASD symptomatology was also related to lower trait EI. This finding is consistent with those of Gökçen et al. (2014), who showed that higher scores on the Autism Quotient scale were negatively correlated with global trait EI as well as wellbeing, emotionality, sociability and empathy factors on the trait EI measure. This raised the question about whether alexithymia or ASD symptomatology served as a better predictor of trait EI. In our regression model, it was found that each were significant predictors of trait EI. These results suggest that alexithymia and ASD symptomatology both contribute to trait EI, which would be consistent with the fact that not all individuals with ASD exhibit elevated levels of alexithymia whereas social-emotional difficulties are common in ASD (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). However, because trait EI encompasses a range of emotion processing abilities, it will be important for future studies to delineate the unique contributions of ASD symptomatology and alexithymia on specific emotion skills.

Finally, while scores on emotion measures may be important, they do not necessarily reflect everyday adjustment or functioning. Thus, our final research question explored whether alexithymia, ASD symptomatology and trait EI were significant predictors of adjustment to college. Previous research has shown that adjustment to college can take different forms, including academic (e.g., how well the student manages the academic demands), social (e.g., the degree to which the student has integrated themselves into the social milieu of college) and personal-emotional (e.g., students’ psychological and physical wellbeing) adjustment (Credé and Niehorster, 2012). Although significant correlations were found between adjustment to college and study variables, including alexithymia and ASD symptomatology, of the predictors assessed (i.e., alexithymia, ASD symptomatology and trait EI), only trait EI was a significant predictor and only for social adjustment. Thus, study findings provide evidence of the potential role trait EI may play in college adjustment, particularly in terms of a student’s integration into the social milieu of college. This assertion is consistent with previous studies showing that in the general population, trait EI is associated with overall wellbeing and social-personal success (e.g., Petrides et al., 2007b; Andrei et al., 2015).

Our results are also noteworthy in terms of their implications. In the general population, trait EI can be developed through intervention and training programs, producing beneficial outcomes in different settings (e.g., work, school, relationships; see Kotsou et al., 2019, for a review). Thus, future studies of trait EI are needed, including those that examine the potentially beneficial outcomes of training programs in the college setting, especially in students who show co-occurring ASD symptomatology and alexithymia. Exploring the impact of trait EI training programs on alexithymia would also be valuable.

Limitations and Future Directions

Although we feel our results are compelling, limitations in the research must be acknowledged. First, self-report measures of alexithymia, such as the TAS-20, assume that individuals with alexithymia can accurately gauge their emotion abilities (Lane et al., 1996; Lumley et al., 2007). Although this is a legitimate concern, studies have shown significant associations between self-report measures of alexithymia and observational measures, as well as parent/other reports (e.g., Kooiman et al., 2002; Milosavljevic et al., 2016; Maroti et al., 2018). Others have shown that there are cognitive and affective components to alexithymia that are not captured by the TAS but may have bearing on college performance (Ziermans et al., 2019). Additionally, ASD symptomatology was determined based on a self-report measure and therefore likely represents a “high-functioning” group, limiting the generalizability of the findings. Nevertheless, Frazier et al. (2014) found that the SRS-2 exhibited measurement invariance across age, sex, and reporter (self vs. others). Others have used this sampling strategy to include individuals that may not have received a formal diagnosis of ASD but score above threshold on one or more self-report measures of ASD symptomatology (e.g., Trevisan and Birmingham, 2016; Dijkhuis et al., 2020; Lei et al., 2020). It has also been suggested that the SRS-2 may be capturing symptoms of other conditions, such as elevated levels of anxiety (South et al., 2017). We would concur and acknowledge that it can be difficult to tease apart co-occurring conditions with ASD. Thus, these findings do not preclude the possibility that other factors could be better predictors of trait EI and adjustment to college.

It must also be acknowledged that a different pattern of results could have been found if we had limited the study to only those with a formal diagnosis of ASD. In fact, it has been asserted that individuals with a formal diagnosis of ASD and are at or above threshold for alexithymia are a distinct subgroup, and that identification of that subgroup may have important implications for accurately determining the role of alexithymia in ASD (Kinnaird et al., 2019). Based on our findings, we would add that identifying this subgroup may be important for determining who may benefit the most by training programs of trait EI.

Nevertheless, our findings revealed that neither ASD symptomatology nor alexithymia predicted students’ adjustment to college. Rather, the only significant predictor of college adjustment was trait EI. These findings may reflect the fact that high-functioning individuals with ASD, or elevated levels of alexithymia, are not necessarily at a disadvantage in the college setting. With that said, programs that develop trait EI may also have a positive impact on alexithymia. Thus, college support programs that focus on developing trait EI may be valuable resource for students with and without ASD symptomatology.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board at Loyola University Chicago. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

DD designed and executed the study, analyzed the data, and wrote up the results. DM assisted with the data analyses and write-up of the study. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Accardo, A. L. (2017). College-bound young adults with ASD: Self-reported factors promoting and inhibiting success. Div. Autism Dev. Disabil. 4, 36–46.

Google Scholar

Allen, R., Davis, R., and Hill, E. (2013). The effects of autism and alexithymia on physiological and verbal responsiveness to music. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 432–444. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1587-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association [APA] (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5, 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Google Scholar

Andrei, F., Siegling, A. B., Aloe, A. M., Baldaro, B., and Petrides, K. V. (2015). The incremental validity of the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire (TEIQue): a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Pers. Assess. 98, 261–276. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2015.1084630

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagby, R. M., and Taylor, G. J. (1997). “Affect dysregulation and alexithymia,” in Disorders of Affect Regulation: Alexithymia in Medical and Psychiatric Illness, eds G. J. Taylor, R. M. Bagby, and J. D. A. Parker (Cambridge: University Press), 26–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagby, R. M., Taylor, G. J., and Parker, J. D. (1994). The twenty item Toronto Alexithymia Scale—II. convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity. J. Psychosom. Res. 38, 33–40. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90006-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, R. W., and Siryk, B. (1999). SACQ: Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., and Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Autism Dev. Disord. 31, 5–17. doi: 10.1023/a:1005653411471

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berthoz, S., and Hill, E. L. (2005). The validity of using self-reports to assess emotion regulation abilities in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Eur. Psychiatry 20, 291–298. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2004.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beyers, W., and Goossens, L. (2002). Concurrent and predictive validity of the student adaptation to college questionnaire in a sample of European freshman students. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 62, 527–538. doi: 10.1177/00164402062003009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bird, G., and Cook, R. (2013). Mixed emotions: The contribution of alexithymia to the emotional symptoms of autism. Transl. Psychiatry 3:e285. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.61

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boily, R., Kingston, S. E., and Montgomery, J. M. (2017). Trait and ability emotional intelligence in adolescents with and without autism spectrum disorder. Can. J. Sch. Psychol. 32, 282–298. doi: 10.1177/0829573517717160

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bölte, S., Poustka, F., and Constantino, J. N. (2008). Assessing autistic traits: cross-cultural validation of the social responsiveness scale (SRS). Autism Res. 1, 354–363. doi: 10.1002/aur.49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brackett, M. A., and Mayer, J. D. (2003). Convergent, discriminant, and incremental validity of competing measures of emotional intelligence. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29, 1147–1158. doi: 10.1177/0146167203254596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, W., Smith, L. E., Hong, J., Greenberg, J. S., and Mailick, M. R. (2017). Validating the social responsiveness scale for adults with autism. Autism Res. 10, 1663–1671. doi: 10.1002/aur.1813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, B. P., and Hayslip, B. Jr. (2005). Incremental validity of a measure of emotional intelligence. J. Pers. Assess. 85, 154–169. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa8502_08

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., and Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 3rd Edn. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Google Scholar

Constantino, J. N., and Gruber, C. P. (2012). Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS-2), 2nd Edn. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Constantino, J. N., and Todd, R. D. (2005). Intergenerational transmission of subthreshold autistic traits in the general population. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 655–660. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, R., Brewer, R., Shah, P., and Bird, G. (2013). Alexithymia, not autism, predicts poor recognition of emotional facial expressions. Psychol. Sci. 24, 723–732. doi: 10.1177/0956797612463582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, A., and Petrides, K. V. (2010). A psychometric analysis of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire-Short Form (TEIQue-SF) using item response theory. J. Pers. Assess. 92, 449–457. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2010.497426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, B. E., Thompson, K., Anderson, A., Mintz, A., Locks, T., Morgan, L., et al. (2017). College experiences for students with autism spectrum disorder: personal identity, public disclosure, and institutional support. J. Coll. Stud. Dev. 58, 71–87. doi: 10.1353/csd.2017.0004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Credé, M., and Niehorster, S. (2012). Adjustment to college as measured by the student adaptation to college questionnaire: a quantitative review of its structure and relationships with correlates and consequences. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 24, 133–165. doi: 10.1007/s10648-011-9184-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidson, D., DiClemente, C. M., and Hilvert, E. (2021). Experiences and insights of college students with autism spectrum disorder: an exploratory assessment to inform interventions. J. Am. Coll. Health. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2021.1876708 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dijkhuis, R., de Sonneville, L., Ziermans, T., Staal, W., and Swaab, H. (2020). Autism symptoms, executive functioning and academic progress in higher education students. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 50, 1353–1363. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04267-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engelberg, E., and Sjoberg, L. (2004). Emotional intelligence, affect intensity, and social adjustment. Pers. Individ. Differ. 37, 533–542. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frazier, T. W., Ratliff, K. R., Gruber, C., Zhang, Y., Law, P. A., and Constantino, J. N. (2014). Confirmatory factor analytic structure and measurement invariance of quantitative autistic traits measured by the social responsiveness Scale-2. Autism 18, 31–44. doi: 10.1177/1362361313500382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gareth, J., Witten, D., Hastie, T., and Tibshirani, R. (2013). An Introduction to Statistical Learning: With Application in R. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Gökçen, E., Petrides, K. V., Hudry, K., Frederickson, N., and Smillie, L. D. (2014). Sub-threshold autism traits: the role of trait emotional intelligence and cognitive flexibility. Br. J. Psychol. 105, 187–199. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grabe, H. J., Frommer, J., Ankerhold, A., Ulrich, C., Groger, R., Franke, G. H., et al. (2008). Alexithymia and outcome in psychotherapy. Psychother. Psychosom. 77, 189–194. doi: 10.1159/000119739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grynberg, D., Chang, B., Corneille, O., Mauragem, P., Vermeulen, N., Berthoz, S., et al. (2012). Alexithymia and the processing of emotional facial expressions (EFEs): systematic review, unanswered questions and further perspectives. PLoS One 7:e42429. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harms, M. B., Martin, A., and Wallace, G. L. (2010). Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychol. Rev. 20, 290–322. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9138-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heaton, P., Reichenbacher, L., Sauter, D., Allen, R., Scott, S., and Hill, E. (2012). Measuring the effects of alexithymia on perception of emotional vocalizations in autistic spectrum disorder and typical development. Psychol. Med. 42, 2453–2459. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712000621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, E., Berthoz, S., and Frith, U. (2004). Brief report: cognitive processing of own emotions in individuals with autism spectrum disorder and in their relatives. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 229–235. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000022613.41399.14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hull, L., Petrides, K. V., and Mandy, W. (2020). The female autism phenotype and camouflaging: a narrative review. Rev. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 7, 306–317. doi: 10.1007/s40489-020-00197-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, S. L., Hart, L., Brown, J. T., and Volkmar, F. R. (2018). Brief report: self-reported academic, social, and mental health experiences of post-secondary students with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 48, 643–650. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3315-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kätsyri, J., Saalasti, S., Tiippana, K., von Wendt, L., and Sams, M. (2008). Impaired recognition of facial emotions from low-spatial frequencies in Asperger syndrome. Neuropsychologia 46, 1888–1897. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinnaird, E., Stewart, C., and Tchanturia, K. (2019). Investigating alexithymia in autism: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur. Psychiatry 55, 80–89. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2018.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kooiman, C. G., Spinhoven, P., and Trijsburg, R. W. (2002). The assessment of alexithymia: a critical review of the literature and a psychometric study of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20. J. Psychom. Res. 53, 1083–1090. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(02)00348-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotsou, I., Mikolajczak, M., Heeren, A., Gregoire, J., and Leys, C. (2019). Improving emotional intelligence: a systematic review of existing work and future challenges. Emot. Rev. 11, 151–165. doi: 10.1177/1754073917735902

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laborde, S., Dosseveille, F., Guillén, F., and Chávez, E. (2014). Validity of the trait emotional intelligence questionnaire in sports and its links with performance satisfaction. Psychol. Sports Exerc. 15, 481–490. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, M. C., Kassee, C., Besney, R., Bonato, S., Hull, L., Mandy, W., et al. (2019). Prevalence of co-occurring mental health diagnoses in the autism population: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 6, 819–829. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30289-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane, R. D., Sechrest, L., Reidel, R., Weldon, V., Kaszniak, A., and Schwartz, G. E. (1996). Impaired verbal and nonverbal emotion recognition in alexithymia. Psychosom. Med. 58, 203–210. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199605000-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lei, J., Brosnan, M., Ashwin, C., and Russell, A. (2020). Evaluating the role of autistic traits, social anxiety, and social network changes during transition to first year of university in typically developing students and students on the autism spectrum. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 50, 2832–2851. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04391-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopes, P. N., Salovey, P., and Strauss, R. (2003). Emotional intelligence, personality, and the perceived quality of social relationships. Pers. Individ. Differ. 35, 641–658. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00242-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lumley, M. A., Neely, L. C., and Burger, A. J. (2007). The assessment of alexithymia in medical settings: implications for understanding and treating health problems. J. Pers. Assess. 89, 230–246. doi: 10.1080/00223890701629698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maroti, D., Lilliengren, P., and Bileviciute-Ljungar, I. (2018). The relationship between alexithymia and emotional awareness: a meta-analytic review of the correlation between TAS-20 and LEAS. Front. Psychol. 9:453. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mavroveli, S., Petrides, K. V., Rieffe, C., and Bakker, F. (2007). Trait emotional intelligence, psychological well-being and peer-rated social competence in adolescence. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 25, 263–275. doi: 10.1348/026151006X118577

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., and Salovey, P. (2000). “Models of emotional intelligence,” in Handbook of Human Intelligence, ed. R. J. Sternberg (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 396–420. doi: 10.1017/cbo9780511807947.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikolajczak, M., Luminet, O., and Menil, C. (2006). Predicting resistance to stress: incremental validity of trait emotional intelligence over alexithymia and optimism. Psicothema 18(Suppl), 79–88.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Milosavljevic, B., Carter Leno, V., Simonoff, E., Baird, G., Pickles, A., Jones, C. R., et al. (2016). Alexithymia in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: Its relationship to internalising difficulties, sensory modulation and social cognition. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 46, 1354–1367. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2670-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montgomery, J. M., McCrimmon, A. W., Schwean, L., and Saklofske, D. H. (2010). Emotional intelligence in asperger syndrome: implications of dissonance between cognition and affect. Educ. Train. Dev. Disabil. 45, 566–582.

Google Scholar

Montgomery, J. M., Stoesz, B. M., and McCrimmon, A. W. (2012). Emotional intelligence, theory of mind, and executive functions as predictors of social outcomes in young adults with Asperger Syndrome. Focus Autism Other Dev. Disord. 28, 4–13. doi: 10.1177/1088357612461525

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morie, K. P., Jackson, S., Zhai, Z. W., Potenza, M. N., and Dritschel, B. (2019). Mood disorders in high-functioning autism: the importance of alexithymia and emotional regulation. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 49, 2935–2945. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04020-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nemiah, J. C., Freyberger, H., and Sifnéos, P. E. (1976). “Alexithymia: A view of the psychosomatic process,” in Modern Trends in Psychosomatic Medicine, Vol. 3, ed. O. W. Hill (London: Butterworths), 430–439.

Google Scholar

Newman, L., Wagner, M., Cameto, R., and Knokey, A.-M. (2009). The Post-High School Outcomes of Youth with Disabilities up to 4 Years After High School. A Report from the National Longitudinal Transition Study-2 (NLTS2) (NCSER 2009-3017). Menlo Park, CA: SRI International.

Google Scholar

Newsome, S., Day, A. L., and Catano, V. M. (2000). Assessing the predictive validity of emotional intelligence. Pers. Individ. Differ. 29, 1005–1016. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00250-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connor, P. J., Hill, A., Kaya, M., and Martin, B. (2019). The measurement of emotional intelligence: a critical review of the literature and recommendations for researchers and practitioners. Front. Psychol. 10:1116. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, K. V. (2009). Technical Manual for the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaires (TEIQue). London: London Psychometric Laboratory.

Google Scholar

Petrides, K. V., and Furnham, A. (2000). On the dimensional structure of emotional intelligence. Pers. Individ. Differ. 29, 313–320. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00195-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, K. V., Perez-Gonzalez, J. C., and Furnham, A. (2007a). On the criterion and incremental validity of trait emotional intelligence. Cogn. Emot. 21, 26–55. doi: 10.1080/02699930601038912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, K. V., Pita, R., and Kokkinaki, F. (2007b). The location of trait emotional intelligence in personality factor space. Br. J. Psychol. 98, 273–289. doi: 10.1348/000712606X120618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reszka, S. S., Boyd, B. A., McBee, M., Hume, K. A., and Odom, S. L. (2014). Brief report: concurrent validity of autism symptom severity measures. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 466–470. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1879-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salminen, J. K., Saarijärvi, S., Äärelä, E., Toikka, T., and Kauhanen, J. (1999). Prevalence of alexithymia and its association with sociodemographic variables in the general population of Finland. J. Psychosom. Res. 46, 75–82. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(98)00053-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanford, C., Newman, L., Wagner, M., Cameto, R., Knokey, A. M., and Shaver, D. (2011). The Post-High School outcomes of Young Adults with Disabilities up to 6 Years After High School. Menlo Park, CA: SRI International.

Google Scholar

Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Bobik, C., Coston, T. D., Greeson, C., Jedlicka, C., et al. (2001). Emotional intelligence and interpersonal relations. J. Soc. Psychol. 141, 523–536. doi: 10.1080/00224540109600569

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shattuck, P. T., Narendorf, S. C., Cooper, B., Sterzing, P. R., Wagner, M., and Taylor, J. L. (2012). Postsecondary education and employment among youth with an autism spectrum disorder. Pediatrics 129, 1042–1049. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

South, M., Carr, A. W., Stephenson, K. G., Maisel, M. E., and Cox, J. C. (2017). Symptom overlap on the srs-2 adult self-report between adults with asd and adults with high anxiety. Autism 10, 1215–1220. doi: 10.1002/aur.1764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephenson, K. G., Luke, S. G., and South, M. (2019). Separate contributions of autistic traits and anxious apprehension, but not alexithymia, to emotion processing in faces. Autism 23, 1830–1842. doi: 10.1177/1362361319830090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M., and Parker, J. D. A. (1997). Disorders of Affect Regulation: Alexithymia in Medical and Psychiatric Illness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511526831

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, J. L., and Seltzer, M. M. (2011). Employment and post-secondary educational activities for young adults with autism spectrum disorders during the transition to adulthood. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 41, 566–574. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1070-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevisan, D., and Birmingham, E. (2016). Examining the relationship between autistic traits and college adjustment. Autism 20, 719–729. doi: 10.1177/1362361315604530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

West, S. G., Finch, J. F., and Curran, P. J. (1995). “Structural equation models with nonnormal variables: Problems and remedies,” in Structural Equation Modeling: Concepts, Issues, and Applications, ed. R. H. Hoyle (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.), 56–75.

Google Scholar

White, S. W., Ollendick, T. H., and Bray, B. C. (2011). College students on the autism spectrum: prevalence and associated problems. Autism 15, 683–701. doi: 10.1177/1362361310393363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, S. W., Richey, J. A., Gracanin, D., Coffman, M., Elias, R., LaConte, S., et al. (2016). Psychosocial and computer-assisted intervention for college students with autism spectrum disorder: preliminary support for feasibility. Educ. Train. Autism Dev. Disabil. 51, 307–317.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ziermans, T., de Bruijn, Y., Dijkhuis, R., Staal, W., and Swaab, H. (2019). Impairments in cognitive empathy and alexithymia occur independently of executive functioning in college students with autism. Autism 23, 1519–1530. doi: 10.1177/1362361318817716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism, alexithymia, emotional intelligence, college adjustment, emotion processing

Citation: Davidson D and Morales D (2022) Associations Between Autism Symptomatology, Alexithymia, Trait Emotional Intelligence, and Adjustment to College. Front. Psychol. 13:813450. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.813450

Received: 11 November 2021; Accepted: 28 March 2022;
Published: 28 April 2022.

Edited by:

Tim Ziermans, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Umberto Granziol, University of Padua, Italy
Mikle South, Emory Autism Center, United States

Copyright © 2022 Davidson and Morales. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Denise Davidson, ddavids@luc.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.