Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 06 June 2022
Sec. Health Psychology

How Did People Cope During the COVID-19 Pandemic? A Structural Topic Modelling Analysis of Free-Text Data From 11,000 United Kingdom Adults

  • 1Institute of Education, University College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 2Department of Behavioural Science and Health, University College London, London, United Kingdom

Background: The COVID-19 pandemic has had substantial impacts on lives across the globe. Job losses have been widespread, and individuals have experienced significant restrictions on their usual activities, including extended isolation from family and friends. While studies suggest population mental health worsened from before the pandemic, not all individuals appear to have experienced poorer mental health. This raises the question of how people managed to cope during the pandemic.

Methods: To understand the coping strategies individuals employed during the COVID-19 pandemic, we used structural topic modelling, a text mining technique, to extract themes from free-text data on coping from over 11,000 UK adults, collected between 14 October and 26 November 2020.

Results: We identified 16 topics. The most discussed coping strategy was ‘thinking positively’ and involved themes of gratefulness and positivity. Other strategies included engaging in activities and hobbies (such as doing DIY, exercising, walking and spending time in nature), keeping routines, and focusing on one day at a time. Some participants reported more avoidant coping strategies, such as drinking alcohol and binge eating. Coping strategies varied by respondent characteristics including age, personality traits and sociodemographic characteristics and some coping strategies, such as engaging in creative activities, were associated with more positive lockdown experiences.

Conclusion: A variety of coping strategies were employed by individuals during the COVID-19 pandemic. The coping strategy an individual adopted was related to their overall lockdown experiences. This may be useful for helping individuals prepare for future lockdowns or other events resulting in self-isolation.

Introduction

The COVID-19 pandemic subjected people worldwide to a range of adversities, from isolation at home to loneliness, worries about and experiences of catching the virus, troubles with finances, difficulties acquiring basic needs, and boredom (Chandola et al., 2020; Wright et al., 2020; Brodeur et al., 2021; ONS, 2021). While some of these experiences have also been reported during previous epidemics (Brooks et al., 2020), the COVID-19 pandemic was unprecedented in its global size, transmissibility, and uncertain timeframe. As a result, there were serious concerns that people would be unable to cope and there would be a substantial rise in mental illness, self-harm and suicide globally (Holmes et al., 2020; Mahase, 2020). To a certain extent, this was borne out, with data showing rises in depression and anxiety at the start of the pandemic in many countries around the world (Banks and Xu, 2020; Schippers, 2020; Pierce et al., 2021). However, the COVID-19 pandemic also highlighted resilience amongst many groups, manifested as either levels of anxiety, depression and life satisfaction returning relatively quickly to pre-pandemic levels (Fancourt et al., 2021; Pierce et al., 2021), or with certain groups such as older adults only experiencing small changes to their mental health (Fancourt et al., 2021), or not showing any signs of worsened mental health at all (Saunders et al., 2021). This raises the question of how people managed to cope during the pandemic.

How people cope is an important factor underlying the relationship between experiencing stressors and subsequent mental health. Coping is generally defined as the cognitive and behavioural efforts that are used to manage stress (Lazarus and Folkman, 1991). There is much debate as to whether certain coping strategies are more beneficial than others and in what contexts. For example, strategies that aim to reduce and resolve stressors may be more effective in supporting mental health (Taylor and Stanton, 2007), but avoidant strategies may be helpful in reducing short-term stress (Taylor and Stanton, 2007). While avoidant strategies may have some benefits, they can also lead to more harm as no direct actions are taken to reduce the stressor, potentially resulting in feelings of helplessness or self-blame (Suls and Fletcher, 1985). Numerous sociodemographic, personality, and social factors are known to influence how people cope with stress (Bolger and Zuckerman, 1995; Christensen et al., 2006). For example, personality type can influence the severity of the stressor experience by facilitating or constraining use of coping strategies (Bolger and Zuckerman, 1995; Connor-Smith and Flachsbart, 2007). Additionally, effects of personality on coping are facilitated by their consequences for level of engagement with stressors, and similarly, approach to rewards (Connor-Smith and Flachsbart, 2007; Leszko et al., 2020).

A number of studies have examined coping during the COVID-19 pandemic, using quantitative (Park et al., 2020; Agha, 2021; Fluharty and Fancourt, 2021), qualitative (Ogueji et al., 2021; Sarah et al., 2021), and mixed methods approaches (Chew et al., 2020; Dewa et al., 2021). Quantitative studies have examined the association between coping strategies and the level and trajectories of symptoms of poor mental health during the pandemic (Agha, 2021; Fluharty et al., 2021). For example, coping strategies involving withdrawal, avoidance and substance use to evade the source of stress have been shown to partially mediate trajectories of depression and anxiety during COVID-19 (Freyhofer et al., 2021, p. 19). Further, a study examining predictors of coping found COVID-19 specific experiences contributed to choice of coping strategy (Fluharty and Fancourt, 2021). For instance, experiencing financial adversity (such as job loss and major cut in income) was associated with problem-focused, emotion-focused, and avoidant coping. Additionally, two qualitative studies collected free text responses on how people were coping during the COVID-19 pandemic (Ogueji et al., 2021; Sarah et al., 2021). Both studies found the most common strategies employed were centred around socially-supported coping. However, these studies had small sample sizes and were both recruited over social media, which may have biased the sample towards this result. Further, the studies were narrative in nature and did not compare coping strategies across sociodemographic groups or in a formal manner to peoples’ lockdown experiences.

There has also been a methodological challenge with the studies on coping during COVID-19 carried out so far. Existing quantitative studies have typically relied on closed-form responses to survey items (e.g., Likert responses). An issue with this approach is that responses are restricted to those that the researcher has thought of in advance – an issue that is particularly salient given the novelty of the COVID-19 pandemic. While qualitative approaches allow for more flexibility in responses, the small sample sizes typical of qualitative studies restrict the questions that can be asked of the data – specifically, those that statistically relate individual characteristics and circumstances to topics raised.

Text-mining methods offer the benefits of quantitative and qualitative approaches, enabling the extraction of themes from large-scale free-text data that can be summarised numerically and related to participant characteristics (e.g., age, sex, and personality traits) using standard statistical methods. To our knowledge, two studies have used text-mining methods to analyse free-text survey data on coping during the COVID-19 pandemic in the United Kingdom (Rogers et al., 2020; Hampshire et al., 2021). Both found that visiting nature and green space, keeping active, and using videoconferencing to keep in touch with family and friends were common strategies employed to improve wellbeing. However, both studies used data from the first months of the pandemic, when the situation was relatively novel and social isolation had not been extended for long. Therefore, this study aimed to explore the breadth of coping strategies adopted over a longer period of the pandemic, and how these strategies related to participants’ demographic, socioeconomic and personality characteristics and to their lockdown experiences. To achieve this, we used structural topic modelling (STM; Roberts et al., 2014) – a text-mining technique – and free-text data from 11,000 United Kingdom adults that was collected seven months after lockdown was first introduced in the United Kingdom.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We used data from the COVID-19 Social Study; a large panel study of the psychological and social experiences of over 70,000 adults (aged 18+) in the United Kingdom during the COVID-19 pandemic. The study commenced on 21 March 2020 and involved online weekly data collection for 22 weeks with monthly data collection thereafter. The study is not a random sample and therefore is not representative of the United Kingdom population, but it does contain a heterogeneous set of individuals. Participants were recruited in three ways. First, convenience sampling was used, including promoting the study through existing networks and mailing lists (including large databases of adults who had previously consented to be involved in health research across the United Kingdom), print and digital media coverage, and social media. Second, more targeted recruitment was undertaken focusing on groups who were anticipated to be less likely to take part in the research via our first strategy, including (i) individuals from a low-income background, (ii) individuals with no or few educational qualifications, and (iii) individuals who were unemployed. Third, the study was promoted via partnerships with third sector organisations to vulnerable groups, including adults with pre-existing mental health conditions, older adults, carers, and people experiencing domestic violence or abuse. Full details on sampling, recruitment, data collection, data cleaning and sample demographics are available at https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/JM8RA. The study was approved by the UCL Research Ethics Committee (12467/005) and all participants gave informed consent.

A one-off free-text module was included in the survey between 14 October and 26 November 2020. Participants were asked to write responses to eight questions on their experiences during the pandemic and their expectations for the future. Here, we used responses to a single question: What have been your methods for coping during the pandemic so far and which have been the most or least helpful? (see Supplementary Table 1 for the full list of questions asked during the module). 30,950 individuals participated in the data collection containing this survey module (43.4% of participants with data collection by 26 November 2020). Responses to the free-text questions were optional. 12,536 participants recorded a response to the question on coping (40.5% of eligible participants). Of these, 11,073 (88.3%) provided a valid record, the definition of which is provided in a following section.

The period 14 October–26 November was seven months into the pandemic in the United Kingdom and overlapped with the beginning of the second wave of the virus. As such, participants could reflect on their experiences during a strict lockdown from March 2020, the relaxation of that lockdown over the summer of 2020, and the start of new restrictions being brought in for the second wave. Supplementary Figure 1 shows 7-day COVID-19 caseloads and confirmed deaths, along with the Oxford Policy Tracker, a numerical summary of policy stringency (Hale et al., 2020), across the study period. An overview of the key developments in the pandemic across the data collection period is provided in the Supplementary Information.

Predictors of Topic Proportions

Structural topic modelling allows for inclusion of covariates in the estimation model, such that the estimated proportion of a free-text response devoted to a given topic can differ according to document metadata (e.g., characteristics of its author). To predict topic proportions, we included variables for age, sex, ethnicity, country of residence, education level, living arrangement, keyworker status, self-isolation status, diagnosed psychiatric condition, long-term physical health conditions, and Big-5 personality traits.

Country of residence (England, Scotland, Wales, Northern Ireland), sex (male, female), ethnicity (White, Non-White), age (modelled with basis splines [B-Splines] with four degrees of freedom (Perperoglou et al., 2019) to account for potential non-linear association), education level (GCSE or below, A-levels or equivalent, degree or above), and keyworker status (as working in health, social care or support sectors, or work involving in medicines or PPE production or distribution) were each measured at baseline interview. Long-term physical health conditions (0, 1, 2+) was measured using a multiple-choice question on medical conditions. Included conditions were high blood pressure, diabetes, heart disease, lung disease, cancer, any other clinically-diagnosed chronic physical health conditions, or any disability. Psychiatric diagnosis (yes, no) was measured with the same multiple choice question using items on clinically diagnosed depression, clinically diagnosed anxiety, and any other clinically diagnosed mental health problem. Both variables were collected at baseline interview. Self-isolation status was defined as staying at home at any point due to existing medical condition or being categorised as high risk. This variable was collected at data collections between 21 March 2020 and 04 July 2020.

Personality was measured at baseline interview using the Big Five Inventory (BFI-2; Soto and John, 2017), which measures personality on five domains and 15 facets: openness (intellectual curiosity, aesthetic sensitivity, and creative imagination), conscientiousness (organisation, productiveness, and responsibility), extraversion (sociability, assertiveness, and energy level), agreeableness (compassion, respectfulness, and trust) and neuroticism (anxiety, depression, and emotional volatility). Each item was scored on a 5-point scale (1 = “strongly disagree”, 5 = “strongly agree”). We used the sum Likert score for each domain (range 3–15). Higher scores indicate higher levels of the trait.

Data Cleaning

We performed topic modelling using unigrams (single words). Free-text responses were cleaned using an iterative process. The main steps were as follows. Popular hyphenated words were collapsed into non-hyphenated form and spaces were removed between words that could have been hyphenated (e.g., “pre-pandemic” and “pre pandemic” became “prepandemic”). Punctuation mistakes (e.g., full stops between words) were replaced with whitespace unless the full stop denoted an initialism or a URL. Full stops were removed between initialisms – e.g., U.K. became UK – and “www.” was removed from URLs. Responses were tokenized into lower-case unigram form and “stop” words (common words such as “the” and “and”) were removed. Stop words were identified with the onix, SMART, and snowball dictionaries (Silge and Robinson, 2016), excluding 38 words that we deemed to be relevant to the current topic. We identified spelling mistakes with the hunspell spellchecker (Ooms, 2018), and amended these manually if they had FOUR or more occurrences, and replaced using the hunspell suggested word function otherwise. Where the algorithm provided multiple suggestions, the word with the highest frequency across responses was used. To reduce data sparsity, in the STM analysis, we further stemmed words using the Porter (1980) algorithm, dropped responses if they contained fewer than five words, and dropped words if they appeared in fewer than five responses (Banks et al., 2018). Data cleaning was carried out in R version 3.6.3 (R Core Team, 2020) using the tidyverse (Wickham et al., 2019), stringi (Gagolewski, 2020), qdap (Rinker, 2020), hunspell (Ooms, 2018), SnowballC (Bouchet-Valat, 2020), and tidytext (Silge and Robinson, 2016) packages.

Data Analysis

We performed several quantitative analyses. First, as not all participants chose to provide a response, we ran a logistic regression model to explore the predictors of providing a free-text response. We used the variables defined above as predictor variables (to simplify interpretation, we converted age to categories; 18–29, 30–45, 46–59, 60+). Second, we used STM, implemented with the stm R package (Roberts et al., 2019), to extract topics from responses. STM treats documents as a probabilistic mixture of topics and topics as a probabilistic mixture of words. It is a “bag of words” approach that uses correlations between word frequencies within documents to define topics. As noted, STM allows for inclusion of covariates in the estimation model, and we included the variables defined above. There was only a small amount of item missingness (n = 113), so we used complete case data.

We ran STM models from 2 to 30 topics and selected the final models based on visual inspection of the semantic coherence and exclusivity of the topics and close reading of exemplar documents representative of each topic (documents with highest proportion of text estimated as belonging to a given topic). Semantic coherence measures the degree to which high probability words within a topic co-occur, while exclusivity measures the extent to that a topic’s high probability words have low probability for other topics. After selecting a final model, we carried out three further analyses. First, we decided upon narrative descriptions for the topics based on high probability words, high “FREX” words (a weighted measure of word frequency and exclusivity), and exemplar texts. Second, we ran multiply-adjusted linear regression models estimating whether topic proportions were related to author characteristics defined above (again categorising age into four groups to aid interpretability). For comparability with categorical variables, Big-5 personality trait variables were scaled such that a 1-unit change was equal to a 2 SD difference (Gelman, 2008). (Topic proportions were the dependent variables in these regressions.) Third, to explore which coping strategies may have been particularly effective, we used linear regression to examine whether topic proportions predicted lockdown experiences. Lockdown experiences were measured with three separate items on enjoying lockdown (How much have you enjoyed lockdown? 1. Not at all, 7. Very much), missing lockdown (Do you feel you will miss being in lockdown? 1. Not at all, 7. Very much), and feelings about future lockdowns (How do you feel about the prospect of any future lockdowns? 1. I would dread it, 7. I would really look forward to it). These variables were collected between 11 and 18 June 2020. We ran a separate regression for each lockdown experience variable, with each given variable regressed upon topic proportions added to the model simultaneously. We did not include intercepts in this regression, so coefficients can be interpreted as predicted means when all text is devoted to a specific topic. Individuals with item-missingness on the lockdown experience variables were dropped in this analysis (n = 2,203).

Results

Descriptive Statistics

A total of 11,073 individuals provided a valid free-text response. Descriptive statistics for respondents are displayed in Table 1, with figures for the total eligible sample also shown for comparison. There were some differences between those who provided a (valid) response and those that did not. Supplementary Figure 2 displays the results of logistic regression models exploring the predictors of providing a response. Responders were disproportionately female, of older age, more highly educated, more likely to live alone, and to have self-isolated than non-responders. They were also more open, conscientious, and extraverted, on average.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics.

Descriptive statistics for the lockdown experience variables are displayed in Figure 1. Responses were varied, but more responses were recorded below the midpoint of the scales than above for each question. A higher mean response was given for the enjoyed lockdown question than for the other questions. The modal response to the will miss lockdown question was “not at all” (31.6%).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Descriptive statistics. Lockdown experience variables.

A word cloud of the forty most frequently used words for each question is displayed in Figure 2. Many of the words refer to activities or time use (e.g., walking, reading, exercise, routine) or to social factors (e.g., friends, family, talking, zoom).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Word cloud. Forty most frequently used words across responses. Words sized according to number of responses they appear in.

Coping Strategies

We selected a 16 topic solution. Short descriptions are displayed in Table 2, along with exemplar quotes and topic titles that we use when plotting results. Topics are ordered according to the estimated proportion of text devoted to each topic. Correlations between the topic proportions are displayed in Supplementary Figures 3, 4.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Topic descriptions.

The largest topic (Topic 1; 8.82% of text; Thinking positively) included individuals who had tried to see the positives in the situation, to remember that others were in relatively worse situations, and recognise that the pandemic would pass. Topic 6 (7.34%; Taking one day at a time) similarly, related to a general cognitive coping strategy, including text on individuals taking each day as it came and imposing structure on their time. This topic overlapped with Topic 12 (4.79%; Keeping routines), which related to people keeping routines, particularly with exercise. Similarly, Topic 10 (5.76%; Keeping busy) related to participants filling their time (“keeping busy”) in generally non-specific ways.

Most other topics related to spending time on specific activities. Topic 3 (7.88%; Engaging in creative activities) related to individuals engaging in arts, hobbies, or crafts as a way of coping. Topic 5 (7.77%; Consuming media) included text from participants who reported spending their time listening to music and radio or watching TV and films. Topic 4 (7.82%; Walking and spending time in nature) related to individuals who had used the opportunity to take long walks and get into nature, while Topic 15 (4.22%; Coping through exercise) included text from individuals who found exercise had a positive effect. Topic 8 (6.57%; Talking to family and friends) and Topic 11 (5.34%; Contacting others) including responses on keeping in contact with family, friends and colleagues, the latter referring to the use of online technologies in particular. Topic 9 (6.37%; Doing DIY and gardening) referred to individuals spending time gardening or completing “odd jobs” at home. Topic 14 (4.23%; Doing online activities) included participants spending time on activities online, including classes, courses, and group sessions (such as singing groups) as well as functional online activities such as ordering supermarket deliveries.

Amongst the remaining topics, Topic 2 (8.14%; Engaging in harmful behaviours) included individuals who reported self-harming or increasing alcohol consumption or comfort eating, though the latter two were reported in several cases as improving mood (at least in the short term). Topic 16 (4.13%; Avoiding the news) meanwhile included text on individuals actively avoiding coverage on COVID-19 as a coping strategy. Topic 7 (6.94%; Following the rules) referred specifically to attempts to reduce risk by following guidelines (e.g., mask wearing). Finally, Topic 13 (4.49%; Mixture of themes) surfaced exemplar texts that did not contain a clear, consistent theme.

Topic Proportions and Author Characteristics

The results of regressions exploring the association between topic proportions and author characteristics are displayed in Figures 35. A sizeable number of coefficients were statistically significant when using Bonferroni-corrected p-values (p < 0.05/352 comparisons; see Supplementary Tables 2, 3 for full regression results). However, effect sizes were generally small.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Association between document topic proportion and participant’s Big-5 personality traits (+95% confidence intervals). Results displayed as marginal effects (difference in topic proportion according to change in independent variable). Continuous independent variables are standardized such that a one unit change is equal to a 2 SD difference (Gelman, 2008), Derived from OLS regression models including adjustment for gender, ethnicity, age, education level, living arrangement, psychiatric diagnosis, long-term physical health conditions, self-isolation status, Big-5 personality traits and keyworker status.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Association between document topic proportion and demographic characteristics (+95% confidence intervals). Results displayed as marginal effects (difference in topic proportion according to change in independent variable). Derived from OLS regression models including adjustment for gender, ethnicity, age, education level, living arrangement, psychiatric diagnosis, long-term physical health conditions, self-isolation status, Big-5 personality traits and keyworker status. Reference categories are provided in the plot titles.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Association between document topic proportion and participants’ socioeconomic and health characteristics (+95% confidence intervals). Results displayed as marginal effects (difference in topic proportion according to change in independent variable). Derived from OLS regression models including adjustment for gender, ethnicity, age, education level, living arrangement, psychiatric diagnosis, long-term physical health conditions, self-isolation status, Big-5 personality traits and keyworker status. Reference categories are provided in the plot titles.

Regarding Big-5 personality traits (Figure 3), individuals high in trait openness devoted more text to topics such as engaging in creative activities (Topic 3); conscientious individuals devoted more text on keeping busy (Topic 10), walking and spending time in nature (Topic 4), and spending time on DIY or gardening (Topic 9); extravert individuals wrote more on contacting others (Topic 8 and Topic 11) and less on spending time consuming media (Topic 5) or doing DIY or gardening (Topic 9); agreeable individuals devoted more text on avoiding the news (Topic 16), spending time talking to family and friends (Topic 8) and in harmful behaviours (Topic 2); and neurotic individuals wrote more on consuming media (Topic 5) and – surprisingly –less on keeping routines (Topic 12) and following the guidelines (Topic 7). However, associations were small in each case: a 2 SD increase in the relevant trait was associated with a less than 2.5% point difference in proportion of text devoted to a given topic.

There were also differences according to demographic characteristics (sex, country, age, and ethnicity; Figure 4). Some of these differences were relatively sizeable. Notably, females devoted more text to discussing creative activities (Topic 3; 3.0%, 95% CI = 2.4, 3.6%) and less text to discussing following the rules (Topic 7; −4.4%, 95% CI = −5.1, −3.7%). Adults aged 60+ wrote less on engaging in harmful behaviours than adults aged 18–29 (Topic 2; −5.1%, 95% CI = −6.8, −3.5%) and more on following the rules (Topic 7; 3.0%, 95% CI = 1.7, 4.2%) and doing DIY and gardening (Topic 9; 3.1%, 95% CI = 1.7, 4.5%). Differences according to country and ethnicity were generally smaller.

Finally, there were differences according to socio-economic and health characteristics (Figure 5), but effect sizes were less than 3% points in each case. Individuals with degree-level education or above devoted less text to thinking positively (Topic 1) and individuals with psychiatric diagnoses devoted more text to discussing engaging in harmful behaviours (Topic 2; 1.9%, 95% CI = 1.1, 2.7).

Associations Between Topic Proportions and Lockdown Experiences

The results of regressions assessing the association between lockdown experiences and topic proportions are displayed in Figure 6. Engaging in creative activities (Topic 3), DIY and gardening (Topic 9) and keeping a routine (Topic 12) were associated with greater enjoyment of first lockdown. Creative activities were also related to feeling more positive (or less negative) about a future lockdown and expecting to miss the first lockdown more. Following the rules (Topic 7), keeping busy (Topic 10), and thinking positively (Topic 1) were related to anticipating missing lockdown less. Talking to family and friends was generally related to worse lockdown experiences (though confidence intervals overlapped mean values).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Association between lockdown experiences and document topic proportions (+95% confidence intervals). Results displayed as predicted lockdown experience values where proportion devoted to a given topic is 100%. Derived from OLS regression models adjusting for all estimated proportions for all topics simultaneously. Dashed line represents mean value for the respective lockdown experience variable.

Discussion

We identified 16 overarching topics of how people were coping during lockdown in the United Kingdom. The most discussed coping strategy was ‘thinking positively’ and involved themes of gratefulness and positivity. Numerous topics were centered around activities and hobbies including ‘walking and spending time in nature’, ‘coping through exercise’, ‘doing DIY and gardening’, and ‘engaging in creative activities’. Other themes were digitally oriented, including ‘consuming media’ and ‘doing online activities’, or were socially-supportive, including ‘contacting others’ and ‘talking to friends and family’. Other strategies were more focused on staying in control such as ‘keeping routines’, ‘focusing on one day at a time’, ‘keeping busy’, and ‘following government guidelines’. However, some respondents reported adopting more avoidant strategies including ‘engaging in harmful behaviours’ and ‘avoiding the news’.

Many of the core topics we identified echo those found in other coping research conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic, including reports of ‘embracing lockdown’, feeling hope, and the uptake of numerous hobbies and activities (Al-Tammemi et al., 2020; Park et al., 2020; Rogers et al., 2020; Hampshire et al., 2021; Ogueji et al., 2021; Sarah et al., 2021). However, our findings extend some of this previous research by elucidating specific activities within previous broad themes identified (for example, DIY and gardening), and, with regards to previous text mining studies, show that topics identified (e.g., using outdoor space, video-conferencing, keeping routines) were used across a longer time frame that just the beginning of the pandemic (Rogers et al., 2020; Hampshire et al., 2021). In line with previous research, a number of known sociodemographic, personality, and health predictors were associated with coping choice during the pandemic (Park et al., 2020; Ahmed et al., 2021; Fluharty and Fancourt, 2021).

It is tempting when considering coping to attempt to categorise strategies into adaptive vs maladaptive strategies: those that could have supported mental health and experiences during COVID-19 vs. those that exacerbated negative experiences. However, the effectiveness and suitability of coping strategies depends strongly on factors such as the context, the timescale over which the coping strategy is employed, the outcome the strategy is being employed to deal with, whether the strategy occurs in isolation or alongside other strategies, and one’s flexibility to modify their use of the strategy according to situational demands (Folkman and Moskowitz, 2004). Therefore, this study did not attempt to make such simple categorisations. Nevertheless, a number of notable associations between the strategies employed and the experiences of the individuals using them did emerge. For example, it was notable that the largest topic was “thinking positively”. According to Fredrickson’s (2001) broaden and build theory, positive psychological approaches to stressful situations have a critical adaptive purpose to help prepare individuals for future challenges. Positive thinkers typically use problem-focused, functional and efficient coping strategies, which corroborates the findings from this study that thinking positively was associated with taking one day at a time, keeping routines, and keeping busy; all proactive coping strategies (Naseem and Khalid, 2010). By employing such approaches, individuals can appraise stressful situations as less threatening, thereby reducing the potential impact of the external stressful situation (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984). Thus, daily positive emotions can moderate stress reactivity (Ong et al., 2006), and are also associated with a lower risk of developing depression following stressful societal events (Fredrickson et al., 2003). Conversely, negative thinking is associated with coping becoming dysfunctional. This was seen amongst 8% of the sample who reported engaging in harmful behaviours. Notably, these harmful behaviours were most common amongst younger adults (aged 18–29) and people with pre-existing psychiatric diagnoses. This corroborates previous research. For example, there is evidence generally that younger adults are more likely to engage in avoidant coping strategies outside of pandemic circumstances (Hamarat et al., 2002; Chen et al., 2018). But it is notable that these groups have had consistently poorer psychological experiences across the pandemic. This fits with research showing that engaging in avoidant and harmful behaviours is associated with greater psychological reactivity both during the pandemic and outside it (Thompson et al., 2018; Fluharty et al., 2021). Thus data reported here could help to explain some of the coping mechanisms that could underlie such differential experiences.

Additionally, we found that individuals who engaged in creative activities had the most positive lockdown experiences. The association between creative activities and more enjoyable lockdown experiences is consistent with findings that creative activities are beneficial for a range of mental, social, physical, and wellbeing outcomes (Fancourt et al., 2019). The results are also consistent with a recent analysis of the COVID-19 Social Study suggesting that individual’s used arts activities during lockdown as a way to regulate their emotions (Mak et al., 2021) and an analysis that found longitudinal associations between creative activities during lockdowns and improvements in depression, anxiety and well-being (Bu et al., 2021). It is possible that those who were able to engage in creative activities had other advantages, such as higher incomes, larger houses, or fewer caring responsibilities. However, some of the typical barriers to engagement in the arts changed during lockdown as some activities shifted to a virtual platform (e.g., physical attendance, cost effective) (Mak et al., 2021), which may mean that some people had an enjoyable lockdown experience as they were able to participate in activities they would have otherwise been unable to.

There was also an association between engaging in DIY and gardening and more positive experiences during lockdown. This echoes previous work on the longitudinal associations between outdoor activities during lockdowns and improvements in mental health and wellbeing (Bu et al., 2021; Stock et al., 2021). Being outdoors during lockdowns may have helped to remove individuals from stressful home environments and has also been shown to aid recovery from mental exhaustion, increase one’s sense of vitality (physical and mental energy), and increase physical activity, which in turn can support better mental health and coping (Kaplan, 1995; Markevych et al., 2017). However, it is also notable that we did not find associations between some strategies and experiences. For example, in other studies consuming media during COVID-19 has been associated with poorer mental health (Bu et al., 2021), but we did not find any association with people’s enjoyment of lockdown or attitudes towards potential future lockdowns. This supports suggestions that the effects of coping strategies depend on a wide-range of factors including the specific outcomes in question and adds weight to the importance of considering coping during COVID-19 as a complex phenomenon (Folkman and Moskowitz, 2004). Our results also highlighted the importance of socially-supportive strategies during the pandemic. Recent evidence suggests socially-supportive strategies (e.g., talking to friends and family, social media, contacting others) have been the most commonly employed coping strategies during the COVID-19 pandemic (Al-Tammemi et al., 2020; Ogueji et al., 2021; Sarah et al., 2021), and in other infectious disease outbreaks (Chew et al., 2020). Socially-supportive coping during the pandemic has been associated with faster decreased in mental health symptoms during the first lockdown, suggesting it was a particularly effective form of coping (Fluharty et al., 2021). While the most frequently reported strategy in the current study was not socially-supportive (thinking positive), there were two different coping strategies (11.91%) related to a socially-supportive theme (contacting others and talking to friends and family). Previous research has also reported use of such strategies amongst specific populations such as keyworkers and people living alone as ways of maintaining resilience and combatting loneliness, but it was previously unclear whether such usage was above and beyond that of other demographic groups (May et al., 2021). Our results suggest that not only did these groups use such strategies but they were more likely to do so than non-keyworkers and people not living alone.

This study had several strengths. We used rich qualitative data from over 11,000 United Kingdom adults representing a wide range of demographic groups. By using open-ended free-text data, we were able to analyse spontaneous responses and thus were not limited to coping strategies, activities, or styles we had thought of in advance. Some of the coping strategies were related to participant characteristics in the expected direction – for instance, people with pre-existing mental health conditions were more likely to report engaging in harmful behaviours (in line with previous research that this group is more likely to use avoidant coping). This suggests that our models extracted consistent and meaningful themes. While structural topic models are novel in the coping literature, our results show that such models can complement and bridge qualitative and quantitative approaches, providing insights not easily attained with either approach on its own. A further strength of this study was that we used data from 7 to 8 months after the first lockdown, allowing for an assessment of coping strategies across an extended period of the pandemic.

Nevertheless, this study had several limitations. Not all of the topics identified a single theme consistently and associations with participant characteristics could be driven by idiosyncratic texts. Our sample, though heterogeneous, was not representative of the United Kingdom population. Respondents to the free-text question were also biased towards the more highly educated. This may have generated bias in the topic regression results. While it is plausible that participants discussed coping strategies that they deemed most important, participants may have employed multiple coping strategies and not written about them all. Further, across the long timespan of the pandemic, individuals may have adopted different strategies at different points. Responses may have been biased towards those salient at the time (e.g., those used recently). Moreover, individuals may not interpret or be aware of a behaviour as a coping strategy, though it has that effect – for instance, increasing consumption of alcohol or fatty or sugary foods. A final limitation was that, while we included a wide set of predictors in our models, many relevant factors were unobserved. Associations may be biased by unobserved confounding.

Conclusion

Sixteen different coping strategies employed by adults in the United Kingdom during the COVID-19 pandemic were identified through text-mining participant free test responses to the COVID-19 Social Study. Some strategies reported were more cognitive (or “antecedent-focused”), either based around attentional deployment (both focusing attention onto the pandemic by focusing on following the rules or distracting oneself from events by avoiding the news), problem solving (e.g. drawing on social support) or cognitive change (e.g. trying to think more positively about things) (Gross, 2001). Others were response focused, involving the use of hobbies, exercise or substances to cope. Some coping strategies reported help to explain why certain groups have coped better than others in the pandemic, reporting lower scores of anxiety and depression. This finding may be useful for helping individuals prepare for future lockdowns or other events resulting in self-isolation. Socially supportive coping also emerged as an important coping strategy used by certain groups at higher risk of poor mental health such as keyworkers and people living alone, highlighting the importance of supporting individuals at risk of increased loneliness and lower social support during pandemics to connect with others. However, more research is needed around coping strategies involving potentially harmful and risky behaviours to identify if such behaviours predict poorer mental and physical health during pandemics and how they can be avoided. Overall, this study sheds light onto the important topic of how people adapted to the challenging circumstances of the COVID-19 pandemic and how coping strategies varied by sociodemographic factors. Given that individuals’ roles in pandemics (i.e., survivor, healthcare, patient, caregiver, general population) can also affect how we cope (Chew et al., 2020), future research may want to extend the findings here to explore the interaction between coping strategies, individual roles and subsequent mental health trajectories.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available due to stipulations made by the ethics committee. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to LW, liam.wright@ucl.ac.uk.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by UCL Research Ethics Committee (12467/005). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

All authors conceived and designed the study. LW curated the data and conducted the data analysis. LW and MF agreed on narrative titles for the topics and wrote the first draft. All authors provided critical revisions, read, and approved the submitted manuscript.

Funding

This COVID-19 Social Study was funded by the Nuffield Foundation (WEL/FR-000022583), but the views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily the Nuffield Foundation. This study was also supported by the MARCH Mental Health Network funded by the Cross-Disciplinary Mental Health Network Plus initiative supported by United Kingdom Research and Innovation (ES/S002588/1), and by the Wellcome Trust (221400/Z/20/Z). DF was funded by the Wellcome Trust (205407/Z/16/Z). This study was also supported by HealthWise Wales, the Health and Car Research Wales initiative, which is led by Cardiff University in collaboration with SAIL, Swansea University. The funders had no final role in the study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication. All researchers listed as authors are independent from the funders and all final decisions about the research were taken by the investigators and were unrestricted.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The researchers are grateful for the support of a number of organisations with their recruitment efforts including: the UKRI Mental Health Networks, Find Out Now, UCL BioResource, HealthWise Wales, SEO Works, FieldworkHub, and Optimal Workshop.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.810655/full#supplementary-material

References

Agha, S. (2021). Mental well-being and association of the four factors coping structure model: a perspective of people living in lockdown during COVID-19. Ethics Med. Public Health 16:100605. doi: 10.1016/j.jemep.2020.100605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahmed, O., Hossain, K. N., Siddique, R. F., and Jobe, M. C. (2021). COVID-19 fear, stress, sleep quality and coping activities during lockdown, and personality traits: a person-centered approach analysis. Pers. Individ. Dif. 178:110873. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.110873

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Al-Tammemi, A. B., Akour, A., and Alfalah, L. (2020). Is it just about physical health? an internet -based cross-sectional study exploring the pyschological impacts of COVID-19 pandemic on university students in jordan using kessler psychological distress scale. Front. Psychol. 11:562213. doi: 10.21203/rs.3.rs-29439/v1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banks, G. C., Woznyj, H. M., Wesslen, R. S., and Ross, R. L. (2018). A review of best practice recommendations for text analysis in R (and a User-Friendly App). J. Bus. Psychol. 33, 445–459. doi: 10.1007/s10869-017-9528-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banks, J., and Xu, X. (2020). The mental health effects of the first two months of lockdown during the COVID-19 pandemic in the UK*. Fiscal Stud. 41, 685–708. doi: 10.1111/1475-5890.12239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolger, N., and Zuckerman, A. (1995). A framework for studying personality in the stress process. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69, 890–902. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.5.890

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouchet-Valat, M. (2020). SnowballC: Snowball Stemmers Based on the C ‘Libstemmer’ UTF-8 Library (0.7.0) [Computer Software]. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=SnowballC (accessed May 13, 2022).

Google Scholar

Brodeur, A., Clark, A. E., Fleche, S., and Powdthavee, N. (2021). COVID-19, lockdowns and well-being: evidence from google trends. J. Public Econ. 193:104346. doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2020.104346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, S. K., Webster, R. K., Smith, L. E., Woodland, L., Wessely, S., Greenberg, N., et al. (2020). The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: Rapid review of the evidence. Lancet 395, 912–920. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bu, F., Steptoe, A., Mak, H., and Fancourt, D. (2021). Time use and mental health in UK adults during an 11-week COVID-19 lockdown: a panel analysis. Br. J. Psychiatry 219, 551–556. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2021.44

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chandola, T., Kumari, M., Booker, C. L., and Benzeval, M. (2020). The mental health impact of COVID-19 and lockdown-related stressors among adults in the UK. Psychol. Med. 1–10. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720005048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y., Peng, Y., Xu, H., and O’Brien, W. H. (2018). Age differences in stress and coping: problem-focused strategies mediate the relationship between age and positive affect. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 86, 347–363. doi: 10.1177/0091415017720890

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chew, Q. H., Wei, K. C., Vasoo, S., Chua, H. C., and Sim, K. (2020). Narrative synthesis of psychological and coping responses towards emerging infectious disease outbreaks in the general population: practical considerations for the COVID-19 pandemic. Singapore Med. J. 61, 350–356. doi: 10.11622/smedj.2020046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christensen, U., Schmidt, L., Kriegbaum, M., Hougaard, C. Ø, and Holstein, B. E. (2006). Coping with unemployment: does educational attainment make any difference? Scand. J. Public Health 34, 363–370. doi: 10.1080/14034940500489339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connor-Smith, J. K., and Flachsbart, C. (2007). Relations between personality and coping: a meta-analysis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93, 1080–1107. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.6.1080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewa, L. H., Crandell, C., Choong, E., Jaques, J., Bottle, A., Kilkenny, C., et al. (2021). CCopeY: a mixed-methods coproduced study on the mental health status and coping strategies of young people during COVID-19 UK lockdown. J. Adolesc. Health 68, 666–675. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2021.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fancourt, D., Finn, S., and World Health Organization, Regional Office for Europe, and Health Evidence Network (2019). What Is The Evidence On The Role Of The Arts In Improving Health And Well-Being?: A Scoping Review. Available online at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK553773/ (accessed May 13, 2022).

Google Scholar

Fancourt, D., Steptoe, A., and Bu, F. (2021). Trajectories of anxiety and depressive symptoms during enforced isolation due to COVID-19 in England: a longitudinal observational study. Lancet Psychiatry 8, 141–149. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30482-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fluharty, M., and Fancourt, D. (2021). How have people been coping during the COVID-19 pandemic? Patterns and predictors of coping strategies amongst 26,016 UK adults. BMC Psychol. 9:107. doi: 10.1186/s40359-021-00603-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fluharty, M., Bu, F., Steptoe, A., and Fancourt, D. (2021). Coping strategies and mental health trajectories during the first 21 weeks of COVID-19 lockdown in the United Kingdom. Soc. Sci. Med. 279, 113958. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.113958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folkman, S., and Moskowitz, J. T. (2004). Coping: pitfalls and promise. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 55, 745–774. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology. the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Am. Psychol. 56, 218–226. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.56.3.218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., and Larkin, G. R. (2003). What good are positive emotions in crisis? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 365–376. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freyhofer, S., Ziegler, N., de Jong, E. M., and Schippers, M. C. (2021). Depression and anxiety in times of COVID-19: how coping strategies and loneliness relate to mental health outcomes and academic performance. Front. Psychol. 12:682684. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.682684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagolewski, M. (2020). Stringi: Character String Processing Facilities (1.5.3) [Computer Software]. Available online at: https://stringi.gagolewski.com/ (accessed May 13, 2022). 0

Google Scholar

Gelman, A. (2008). Scaling regression inputs by dividing by two standard deviations. Stat. Med. 27, 2865–2873. doi: 10.1002/sim.3107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2001). Emotion regulation in adulthood: timing is everything. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 10, 214–219. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00152

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hale, T., Angrist, N., Cameron-Blake, E., Hallas, L., Kira, B., Majumdar, S., et al. (2020). Oxford COVID-19 Government Response Tracker. Blavatnik School of Government. Available online at: https://www.bsg.ox.ac.uk/research/research-projects/coronavirus-government-response-tracker (accessed May 13, 2022).

Google Scholar

Hamarat, E., Thompson, D., Aysan, F., Steele, D., Matheny, K., and Simons, C. (2002). Age differences in coping resources and satisfaction with life among middle-aged, young-old, and oldest-old adults. J. Genet. Psychol. 163, 360–367. doi: 10.1080/00221320209598689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampshire, A., Hellyer, P. J., Trender, W., and Chamberlain, S. R. (2021). Insights into the impact on daily life of the COVID-19 pandemic and effective coping strategies from free-text analysis of people’s collective experiences. Interface Focus 11:20210051. doi: 10.1098/rsfs.2021.0051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmes, E. A., O’Connor, R. C., Perry, V. H., Tracey, I., Wessely, S., Arseneault, L., et al. (2020). Multidisciplinary research priorities for the COVID-19 pandemic: a call for action for mental health science. Lancet Psychiatry 7, 547–560. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30168-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: toward an integrative framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 15, 169–182. doi: 10.1016/0272-4944(95)90001-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal, And Coping. Berlin: Springer Pub. Co.

Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1991). The Concept Of Coping. New York, NY: Columbia University Press, 206.

Google Scholar

Leszko, M., Iwański, R., and Jarzębińska, A. (2020). The relationship between personality traits and coping styles among first-time and recurrent prisoners in poland. Front. Psychol. 10:2969. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahase, E. (2020). Covid-19: Mental health consequences of pandemic need urgent research, paper advises. BMJ 369:m1515. doi: 10.1136/bmj.m1515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mak, H. W., Fluharty, M., and Fancourt, D. (2021). Predictors and impact of arts engagement during the COVID-19 pandemic: analyses of data from 19,384 adults in the covid-19 social study. Front. Psychol. 12:626263. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.626263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markevych, I., Schoierer, J., Hartig, T., Chudnovsky, A., Hystad, P., Dzhambov, A. M., et al. (2017). Exploring pathways linking greenspace to health: theoretical and methodological guidance. Environ. Res. 158, 301–317. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2017.06.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

May, T., Aughterson, H., Fancourt, D., and Burton, A. (2021). Stressed, uncomfortable, vulnerable, neglected’: a qualitative study of the psychological and social impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on UK frontline keyworkers. SocArXiv [Preprint] doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2021-050945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naseem, Z., and Khalid, R. (2010). Positive thinking in coping with stress and health outcomes: literature review. J. Res. Reflect. Educ. 4, 42–61.

Google Scholar

Ogueji, I. A., Okoloba, M. M., and Demoko Ceccaldi, B. M. (2021). Coping strategies of individuals in the United Kingdom during the COVID-19 pandemic. Curr. Psychol. 1–7. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-01318-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ong, A. D., Bergeman, C. S., Bisconti, T. L., and Wallace, K. A. (2006). Psychological resilience, positive emotions, and successful adaptation to stress in later life. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 730–749. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

ONS, (2021). Coronavirus And The Social Impacts On Great Britain—Office for National Statistics. Available online at: https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/healthandsocialcare/healthandwellbeing/bulletins/coronavirusandthesocialimpactsongreatbritain/11june2021 (accessed May 13, 2022).

Google Scholar

Ooms, J. (2018). Hunspell: High-Performance Stemmer, Tokenizer, and Spell Checker (3.0) [Computer software]. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=hunspell (accessed May 13, 2022).

Google Scholar

Park, C. L., Russell, B. S., Fendrich, M., Finkelstein-Fox, L., Hutchison, M., and Becker, J. (2020). Americans’ COVID-19 stress, coping, and adherence to CDC guidelines. J. Gen. Internal Med. 35, 2296–2303. doi: 10.1007/s11606-020-05898-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perperoglou, A., Sauerbrei, W., Abrahamowicz, M., and Schmid, M. (2019). A review of spline function procedures in R. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 19:46. doi: 10.1186/s12874-019-0666-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierce, M., McManus, S., Hope, H., Hotopf, M., Ford, T., Hatch, S. L., et al. (2021). Mental health responses to the COVID-19 pandemic: a latent class trajectory analysis using longitudinal UK data. Lancet Psychiatry 8, 610–619. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(21)00151-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porter, M. F. (1980). An algorithm for suffix stripping. Program 14, 130–137. doi: 10.1108/eb046814

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2020). R: A Language And Environment For Statistical Computing (3.6.3) [Computer Software]. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Rinker, T. (2020). Qdap: Quantitative Discourse Analysis Package (2.4.3) [Computer software]. Available online at: https://github.com/trinker/qdap (accessed May 13, 2022).

Google Scholar

Roberts, M. E., Stewart, B. M., and Tingley, D. (2019). stm: an r package for structural topic models. J. Stat. Softw. 91, 1–40. doi: 10.18637/jss.v091.i02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, M. E., Stewart, B. M., Tingley, D., Lucas, C., Leder-Luis, J., Gadarian, S. K., et al. (2014). Structural topic models for open-ended survey responses. Am. J. Political Sci. 58, 1064–1082. doi: 10.1111/ajps.12103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, N. T., Waterlow, N. R., Brindle, H., Enria, L., Eggo, R. M., Lees, S., et al. (2020). Behavioral change towards reduced intensity physical activity is disproportionately prevalent among adults with serious health issues or self-perception of high risk during the UK COVID-19 lockdown. Front. Public Health 8:575091. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2020.575091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarah, K., Oceane, S., Emily, F., and Carole, F. (2021). Learning from lockdown—Assessing the positive and negative experiences, and coping strategies of researchers during the COVID-19 pandemic. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 236:105269. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105269

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saunders, R., Buckman, J. E. J., Fonagy, P., and Fancourt, D. (2021). Understanding different trajectories of mental health across the general population during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychol. Med. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721000957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schippers, M. C. (2020). For the greater good? The devastating ripple effects of the Covid-19 crisis. Front. Psychol. 11:577740. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.577740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silge, J., and Robinson, D. (2016). tidytext: text mining and analysis using tidy data principles in R. J. Open Source Softw. 1:37. doi: 10.21105/joss.00037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soto, C. J., and John, O. P. (2017). The next Big Five Inventory (BFI-2): Developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 113, 117–143. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stock, S., Bu, F., Fancourt, D., and Mak, H. W. (2021). Going outdoors, neighbourhood satisfaction and mental health and wellbeing during a COVID-19 lockdown: a fixed-effects analysis. PsyArXiv [Preprint] doi: 10.31234/osf.io/8pjmt

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suls, J., and Fletcher, B. (1985). The relative efficacy of avoidant and nonavoidant coping strategies: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 4, 249–288. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.4.3.249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S. E., and Stanton, A. L. (2007). Coping resources, coping processes, and mental health. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 3, 377–401. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, N. J., Fiorillo, D., Rothbaum, B. O., Ressler, K. J., and Michopoulos, V. (2018). Coping strategies as mediators in relation to resilience and posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 225, 153–159. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.08.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., McGowan, L., François, R., et al. (2019). Welcome to the tidyverse. J. Open Source Softw. 4:1686.

Google Scholar

Wright, L., Steptoe, A., and Fancourt, D. (2020). Are we all in this together? Longitudinal assessment of cumulative adversities by socioeconomic position in the first 3 weeks of lockdown in the UK. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 74, 683–688. doi: 10.1136/jech-2020-214475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: COVID-19, mental health, coping (C), free-text analysis, structural topic modeling, text mining

Citation: Wright L, Fluharty M, Steptoe A and Fancourt D (2022) How Did People Cope During the COVID-19 Pandemic? A Structural Topic Modelling Analysis of Free-Text Data From 11,000 United Kingdom Adults. Front. Psychol. 13:810655. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.810655

Received: 07 November 2021; Accepted: 04 May 2022;
Published: 06 June 2022.

Edited by:

Tina Cartwright, University of Westminster, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Michaéla C. Schippers, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Netherlands
Chrissy H. Roberts, University of London, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2022 Wright, Fluharty, Steptoe and Fancourt. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Liam Wright, liam.wright@ucl.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.