Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 25 March 2022
Sec. Cultural Psychology

The Role of Wisdom in the Cross-Cultural Adaptation of Chinese Visiting Scholars to Canada: A Mediation Model

  • 1School of Humanities and Social Sciences, Hubei University of Medicine, Shiyan, China
  • 2School of Education, Hangzhou Normal University, Hangzhou, China
  • 3Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 4Department of Psychology, Ningbo University, Ningbo, China

This study examines the role of wisdom in the cross-cultural adaptation of Chinese visiting scholars in Canada, as mediated by different coping styles. Path analysis was used to for hypotheses testing. The findings suggest that (1) wisdom measured by 3D-WS and Adult Self-Transcendence Inventory (ASTI), independently had direct correlation with social and psychological adaptation, and positively associated with engaged coping (active coping and proactive–reflective coping); (2) the independent effects of 3D-WS and ASTI on social adaptation, psychological adaptation, and life satisfaction were mediated by proactive–reflective coping; (3) wisdom, when measured by 3D-WS, promoted positive psychological adaptation through decreasing passive coping. This study shows that wisdom is a critical factor affecting cross-cultural adaptation, and the use of proactive–reflective coping is a wise way of handling future life challenges.

Introduction

The Science of Wisdom

Wisdom is considered an ideal endpoint of human development around the world. According to Erikson (1950), wisdom arises out of successful resolution and integration of various psychosocial conflicts that allow for better psychosocial adaptation. As a result, regardless of objective circumstances, wisdom is often related to a higher level of life satisfaction (Ardelt, 1997; Tornstam, 1997). Wise individuals are those who can integrate feelings, thoughts, and behaviors across intrapersonal, interpersonal, and transpersonal domains (Orwoll and Perlmutter, 1990; Orwoll and Achenbaum, 1993; Sternberg et al., 2019) and brilliantly apply their knowledge and judgment in real-life contexts. In general, wisdom is manifested in one’s way of handling daily events, managing one’s own life, making critical life decisions, and taking wise actions that would exert a significant and positive effect on human life.

In spite of different specific definitions of wisdom, most researchers—East and West—agree on the multidimensional nature of wisdom as an integration of knowledge, emotion, and virtue (Clayton and Birren, 1980; Randall and Kenyon, 2001; Baltes and Smith, 2008; Wang and Zheng, 2014; Ardelt and Ferrari, 2019). Additionally, by distinguishing first and third-person perspectives (Searle, 1992), psychological wisdom research can be differentiated into general and personal wisdom (Staudinger, 1999; Staudinger and Glück, 2011; Staudinger and Leipold, 2019). Personal wisdom relates to insight into one’s own self and life through personal experiences and insights, whereas general wisdom concerns the more detached insight into the lives of others and human life in general (Glück et al., 2013; Staudinger, 2013).

Since these different types of wisdom studies have different purposes, the measurement of personal wisdom should be distinguished from that of general wisdom. Thus, in this study, we choose to measure Chinese visiting scholars’ personal wisdom using the three-dimensional Wisdom Scale (3D-WS) of Ardelt (2003)—the combination of cognitive, reflective, and affective characteristics, and the Adult Self-Transcendence Inventory (ASTI) that focuses on self-transcendent wisdom (Orwoll and Perlmutter, 1990; Levenson et al., 2005). The former refers to more interpersonal domain, and the latter refers to a transpersonal and intrapersonal domain.

Based on lay and expert wisdom theories (Clayton and Birren, 1980; see also Weststrate et al., 2019), Ardelt proposed a self-report wisdom measure and identified three personal dispositions jointly required for wisdom (Ardelt, 1997, 2003; Ardelt and Ferrari, 2014). The cognitive dimension measures the individual’s deep understanding for truth and knowledge or experience of life. The reflective dimension refers to the individual’s willingness and ability to perceive reality and examine oneself from different angles, which implies self-examination and self-insight. The affective dimension reflects the individual’s sympathy and compassion for others (Ardelt, 2003; Glück, 2017).

In addition, this scale nicely captures both Western and Eastern implicit and explicit wisdom theories. Based on the work by Clayton and Birren (1980), the lay conceptions of wisdom described as cognitive (knowledgeable, experienced, intelligent, pragmatic, and observant), reflective (introspective and intuitive), and affective (understanding, empathetic, peaceful, and gentle), Holliday and Chandler (1986) also named the factors “exceptional understanding” as a mixture of reflective, “general competencies” and “judgment and communication skills” as mixture of cognitive, “interpersonal skills” as a mixture of affective. Similarly, from Sternberg’s theory (Sternberg, 1990a), “reasoning ability,” “sagacity,” “learning from ideas and environment,” “judgment,” “expeditious use of information,” and “perspicacity,” all of the dimensions contain cognitive, reflective, and affective wisdom attributes. These core components outlined above are all critical features of lay view (Glück and Bluck, 2011; Staudinger, 2013).

The scale of 3D-WS is also with explicit theories that are based on the Western and Eastern wisdom traditions (Takahashi and Bordia, 2000). The wisdom traditions of the West emphasize the cognitive dimension of wisdom (i.e., knowledge and analytical ability), Baltes and colleagues (Baltes and Smith, 1990; Baltes and Staudinger, 2000) defined wisdom as “an expert knowledge system in the domain, fundamental life pragmatics.” In the Eastern wisdom traditions, wisdom is characterized by flexibility, honesty, sensitivity, modesty, compassion, morality, and a balanced state of mind that is able to perceive and accept the reality of the present moment (Clayton and Birren, 1980; Hart, 1987; Nakasone, 1994; Levitt, 1999; Yang, 2014a; Wang and Zheng, 2014). Likewise, the explicit theory of Yang (2001, 2014b) and model of wisdom includes four factors, namely, (1) competencies and knowledge, (2) modesty and unobtrusiveness, (3) openness and profundity, and (4) benevolence and compassion. Furthermore, wisdom is considered a real-life process with three core elements: cognitive integration, embodied action, and positive effects for self and others. Wang and Zheng (2015) proposed an indigenous Chinese wisdom theory, where wisdom is defined as the integration of virtue and competence. Here, personal wisdom is manifested in daily life through processes that often involve a high level of intellectual competence (e.g., dialectical thinking, reflective thinking, innovative thinking, and critical thinking) and a high level of morality (e.g., moderation, honesty, responsibility, fairness, and compassion; Chen and Wang, 2014).

In general, all dimensions of 3D-WS can relate to the mainstream explicit wisdom theories from West and East.

Based on an analysis of the common elements of wisdom definitions provided in different cultures, Curnow (1999) first put forward self-transcendence as the essence of wisdom and identified four major features of wisdom: self-knowledge, integration, self-transcendence, and detachment, considered stages in the development of wisdom (Levenson et al., 2005). In terms of self-transcendence, the core feature is to transcend human biases, subjectivity, and self-centeredness. In turn, this can result in critical self-reflective knowledge, in line with the reflective dimension of the 3D-WS. In a recent study, Glück et al. (2013) found that the ASTI overlapped with a broad range of subscales of other wisdom measures including the 3D-WS (Ardelt, 2003), which assess several wisdom dimensions that have relationships with the sub-dimensions of the ASTI. However, these correlations are more strongly supported with North American and European samples, and not replicated in China or elsewhere.

Glück et al. (2013) compared four well-established wisdom measures: the SAWS (Webster, 2003), the 3D-WS (Ardelt, 2003), the ASTI (Levenson et al., 2005), and the Berlin wisdom paradigm (Baltes and Staudinger, 2000). Based on analysis of the content of researchers’ definitions and operationalizations of wisdom, the ASTI and the 3D-WS predominantly pertain to personal wisdom. Besides, the cognitive dimension of the 3D-WS also taps general wisdom, and an affective dimension of the 3D-WS relates to another important aspect of wisdom which refers to empathy-based concerning, namely, other-related wisdom. The ASTI presents its core aspect of wisdom as self-transcendence, which seems narrower than two other self-report scales, but the findings from this study support the idea that it taps a wide range of meanings of wisdom and self-transcendence is central to wisdom across all measures. Thus, this study adopts the 3D-WS and the ASTI which, combined, largely capture universal features of wisdom as mentioned above (Glück et al., 2013). Both measures of personal wisdom have been widely used in the international sample of different ages with relatively good overall reliability and validity (Ardelt, 2003; Levenson et al., 2005; Webster, 2007; Taylor et al., 2011; Thomas et al., 2017). They represent the two ways people may respond optimally to being a sojourner.

However, due to the major differences between Western and Eastern understandings of wisdom (Takahashi and Bordia, 2000; Birren and Svensson, 2005; Takahashi and Overton, 2005), Historically, Western wisdom is said to value knowledge and cognitive complexity through sophisticated reasoning and judgment, while Eastern wisdom has a more nuanced discussion of virtuous action (Cottingham, 1996; Magee, 1998; Takahashi and Bordia, 2000; Takahashi and Overton, 2005), so it is worth considering whether the definitions of wisdom and their components are universal in different cultures and religious traditions. Although the differences between Eastern and Western wisdom conceptions and assessments have been compared in some studies (e.g., Takahashi and Bordia, 2000; Yang, 2001; Takahashi and Overton, 2005; Kim and Knight, 2015), there is limited empirical evidence from Eastern countries.

Wisdom, Coping, and Acculturation

Acculturation refers to one’s psychosocial adaptation to a culture different from one’s native culture (Burnam et al., 1987). It is closely associated with sociocultural adaptation (Ward and Kennedy, 2001), acculturative stress (Kuo and Roysircar, 2004; Torres and Rollock, 2004) among others (Yoon et al., 2012). Many of the conceptual paradigms of acculturation proposed by several researchers share the similarity in that they include both internal and external domains (Kwan and Sodowsky, 1997). The internal domain usually refers to the psychological aspects of an individual’s orientation toward their larger cultural group and society, whereas the external domain usually refers to the behavioral aspects of an individual’s orientation toward their ethnic cultural group (Chia and Costigan, 2006). According to Berry (2006), there are two distinct perspectives of migrants’ cultural adaptation, namely, the cultural learning model (behavioral adaption, or doing well) and the “stress, coping, and adaptation” model (experience, or feeling well).

Cultural learning emphasizes teaching and training migrants in culture-specific skills in order to acculturate them in the new cultural environment (Ward, 2001). It is related to multiple factors, including the length of new culture residence, foreign language proficiency, cultural knowledge, and cultural distance. In contrast, psychological adaptation to stress focuses on immigrants’ emotional wellbeing and life satisfaction in the host culture, including their ability to cope with conflict and stress (Berry, 1997, 2006; Ward, 2001). Some studies show that migrants with different acculturation experiences react differently to stresses and adopt different coping strategies (Zheng and Berry, 1991; Yoshihama, 2002; Noh and Kaspar, 2003).

Importantly, Staudinger (2001) defined coping as social-cognitive processes of life reflection (i.e., life planning, life management, and life review) which were assumed to be essential for the development of wisdom-related knowledge and judgment. Reflection has been recognized as a core element of most definitions of personal wisdom (e.g., 3D-WS and ASTI) and a critical characteristic of wise individuals (Weststrate et al., 2019), emphasizing the motivation to think deeply and take different perspectives on experiences. Self-transcendent wisdom is deep transcendental insight into one’s self and the nature of reality, which primarily focuses on specific and basic existential issues like identity, reality, life purpose, meaning, and priorities (Staudinger, 2013). In addition, Linden et al. (2011) argued that wisdom could be understood as a psychological competency in coping with difficult, ambiguous, or unsolvable life problems, which helps people to tolerate or cope with complex and uncertain life situation in the future (Linden, 2014).

Recently, attention has been shifted to future-oriented proactive coping, that is, how one faces life instead of reacting to it after-the-fact. It includes one’s efforts to build up general resources to buffer future changes, address challenges, and facilitate personal growth (Safdar et al., 2009a). Safdar et al. (2003, 2009a,b, 2012, 2013) found that engaged coping strategies (i.e., meaning-focused and proactive coping) for acculturation-specific and generally stressful life-planning challenges, predict mental and physical health outcomes.

Foresight is also an increasingly acknowledged element in many lay theories and models of wisdom (Holliday and Chandler, 1986; Ji et al., 2001; Asadi, 2015; Weststrate et al., 2019; Ardelt et al., 2020). Some studies have found that wise reasoning about future conflicts better resolves conflicts (Huynh et al., 2016; Peetz and Grossmann, 2020), and training wise reasoning skills results in more balanced predictions about future emotional reactions (Grossmann et al., 2020; Peetz and Grossmann, 2020).

From the brief literature review above, we can see that wisdom is “a special coping strategy in which the individual brings together whatever intellectual and affective strengths are available to them to resolve a unique problem” (Bluck and Glück, 2004, p. 548). Some key characteristics of wisdom in response to real-life challenges involve self-transcendent meta-level responses (epistemic/intellectual humility, multi-angle considerations, appreciation of a given issue’s broader context) with moral grounding (Grossmann et al., 2020). Furthermore, wisdom is often considered to be the result of reflecting on life experiences, particularly challenging ones (Aldwin, 2007; Weststrate and Gluck, 2017; Munroe et al., 2021), manifested through expert life management and decision-making when dealing with stressful situations, social relationships, life tasks and goals, and life’s uncertainties (Baltes and Smith, 1990).

There have been some empirical studies of wisdom in the acculturation of ethnic minorities. Typically, ethnic minority research explores power disparities and their implications for equal access to social resources, investigating individuals’ psychological experiences through the understanding of the obstacles to be overcome, as well as the strengths on which they depend to do so (Coll et al., 1996). For example, Le (2008) found that European Americans exhibited a higher level of transcendent and practical wisdom than Vietnamese Americans. However, the wisdom experiences of Vietnamese refugees living in the United States might reflect the traumatic life experience of these immigrant participants as well as any difference between the Vietnamese culture and the US culture (Le, 2008, 2010). More universally, wise coping may help migrants better resolve psychological difficulties during acculturation process and lead to eventual adaptation in the host culture (Berry, 2006). People have the potential to use a wide range of coping strategies to achieve various outcomes (adaptations). Examining international students in Singapore, studies found that secondary coping mechanisms (e.g., acceptance and positive reinterpretation) predicted lower levels of perceived stress and subsequently reduced depressive symptoms (Ward et al., 1998, as cited in Ward and Kennedy, 2001). Similarly, Lin and Betz (2009) discovered a negative correlation between unconditional positive regard (i.e., self-compassion) and acculturative stress among Chinese international students.

To sum up, wise ways of dealing with stressful situations are theorized to be critical determinants of acculturation. Wise individuals acknowledge that values are relative, there is rarely absolute certainty, and contradiction is inherent in life. However, previous research is limited to immigrants who, with different acculturation needs, are qualitatively different from visiting scholars which are the interest of the present study.

International Cultural Exchange (Long-Term Visitors)

While many countries have programs that enable international professional exchanges, one of the largest groups is from mainland China. As the global knowledge economy is increasingly characterized by competition and internationalization, China has taken the lead in sending the largest proportions of visiting scholars abroad [Institute of International Education (IIE), 2019].1 Chinese visiting scholars are people from an academic institution, who visit a host university in the international culture to perform research on a topic the visitor is valued for. The groups are composed of China’s post-secondary institution researchers, faculty members, post-doctoral fellows, and backbone teachers. In recent years, China has implemented national policies to encourage the development of academic professionalism of its scholars and promote the cooperation between colleges in China and other countries. Post-secondary institutions are required to encourage global exchanges and visits which are often of great importance for faculty members who are interested in career advancement. Thus, the number of China’s visiting scholars who visit foreign countries to obtain global experiences has been raising every year, from 2,044 in the first year of CSC project that had been launched (Xue et al., 2015) to 12,900 participants by 2018 (China Scholarship Council, 2018). A visiting scholar can stay for a few months to a year in the host country, and stays may be extended as required by study programs. However, a visiting scholar and his/her family members are just temporarily exposed to the host culture since they will come back to China after the visiting period. At the same time, the visiting scholars have to face cultural challenges in behaviors and attitudes as long as the host culture interacts with their culture of origin. Triandis found that Westerners advocate individualism, while Easterners are on the collectivism end (Triandis, 1989; Singelis et al., 1995; Triandis and Gelfand, 1998). Nisbett and Peng conducted in-depth research on the differences between Chinese and Western thinking pattern, it is indicated that Western culture shows an analytic thinking orientation, while Chinese people show a holistic thinking orientation, etc. (Peng et al., 1997; Nisbett et al., 2001; Nisbett and Masuda, 2003; Masuda and Nisbett, 2006), besides Westerners emphasize more on independent self while Easterners tend to be interdependent self. Those deep cultural differences may have a significant impact on the social and psychological adaptation of Chinese visiting scholars in Canada.

Moreover, they may have different and similar acculturation experiences that are comparable to those of Chinese immigrants or international students, for instance, limited cross-cultural competence, culture shock, academic adaptation, and language barriers (Mori, 2000; Kim and Abreu, 2001; Lee et al., 2004; Russell et al., 2010). However, little has been done to explore the adaptation experiences of China’s visiting scholars. A survey of international visiting scholars’ experiences, especially for their cross-cultural challenging experiences, have the possibility of helping them to cope with or tolerate the complex and ambiguous situations in foreign countries, which is informative not only to China, but policy-makers, educators, professionals, and students of other countries. This study intends to offer practical suggestions and supports for visiting scholars in terms of pre-departure preparations, goal settings, and plans to maximize their exchange experience.

Current Study

This study examines the effects of coping styles (proactive–reflective, active, passive) as mediators between wisdom and adaptation among Chinese visiting scholars. Considering the strong theoretical links between wisdom and coping strategies, and the important role of wisdom in adaptation and wellbeing, we propose the following hypotheses:

1. Facing challenging and stressful life experiences during the exchange period, wisdom, as measured by the 3DWS and the ASTI, directly predicts adaptation and life satisfaction.

2. People with wisdom as measured by the 3DWS or the ASTI will differentially endorse different coping strategies (proactive–reflective, active, passive), and in turn exhibit different levels of social and psychological adaptation. Specifically, people who score higher on wisdom measures may show more engaged coping (proactive–reflective and active coping strategies) that help them acculturate more easily in Canada.

3. People who score higher on wisdom measures will have less psychological distress.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedure

Two hundred and one Chinese visiting scholars temporarily residing in Canada participated in the study. Hundred eighty-one usable responses were included for the final analyses, 20 were excluded because of their visiting period was not included in pandemic period or incomplete data for several key information (e.g., age and visiting period). Fifty-three (53%) of the participants were men, with age ranging from 22 to 59 years (M = 35.11, SD = 6.11). Among them, 124 (68.5%) were married, 49 (27.1%) were unmarried, 7 (3.9%) were divorced, and 1(0.5%) was widowed. The mean length of stay in Canada was 11.93 months (SD = 5.08). The length of residence ranging from 1 to 6 months was 3.9%, 7 to 12 months was 78.4%, and above 12 months was 17.7%.

The questionnaire was piloted with four Chinese visiting scholars through the online survey platform SoJump. During the formal data collection, through advertisements posted on WeChat groups, 201 participants consented and completed the survey through SoJump, and were paid 10RMB through WeChat’s red packet function.

Measures

The survey collected participants’ demographic data (gender, age, marital status, months of stay in Canada, previous exchange experiences, visiting purpose, English proficiency, and the perceived impact of COVID-19), responses to two wisdom measures (3D-WS and ASTI), coping styles, adaptation, and life satisfaction. All the measures were used in Chinese version for survey.

3D-WS

The adapted Chinese version of the 3D-WS was used to measure participants’ wisdom (Fu, 2019). The 39-item questionnaire consists of 14 cognitive, 12 reflective, and 13 affective items that have been applied to many culturally diverse samples (Benedikovicova and Ardelt, 2008; Bergsma and Ardelt, 2012; Bang and Montgomery, 2013; Glück et al., 2013; Bang and Zhou, 2014; Sahrani et al., 2014). Items were measured on a 5-point Likert scale, with higher scores indicating a higher level of wisdom development. In this study, the Cronbach alpha coefficient for 3D-WS was 0.94.

Adult Self-Transcendence Inventory

The ASTI (Koller et al., 2017) consists of 24 Likert scale items ranging from 1 (disagree strongly) to 4 (agree strongly), which conceptualizes wisdom as self-transcendence and to reflects a “decreasing reliance on externals for definition of the self, increasing interiority and spirituality, and a greater sense of connectedness with past and future generations” (Levenson et al., 2005). In this study, the Cronbach alpha coefficient for ASTI was 0.91.

Active and Passive Coping

The 20-item Simplified Coping Style Questionnaire (SCSQ) measures the tendency to which individuals respond to stress (Xie, 1998), including 12 items on active coping and 8 items on passive coping. All items are scored on a 4-point Likert scale (1 = do not do this at all, 4 = do this a lot) with higher scores indicating greater active/passive coping manners. In this study, the Cronbach alpha coefficients for active and passive coping were 0.87 and 0.81, respectively.

Proactive–reflective coping. The version of Proactive Coping Inventory (Greenglass et al., 1999) is a multidimensional coping inventory. The 14-item proactive coping and 11-item reflective coping subscales of the PCI were used to assess proactive and reflective coping. Participants were asked to indicate on a 4-point scale (1 = not at all true, 4 = completely true), the proactive coping scale combines autonomous goal setting with self-regulatory goal attainment cognitions and behavior, and the reflective coping scale describes simulation and contemplation about a variety of possible behavioral alternatives by comparing their imagined effectiveness and generating hypothetical plans of action. The correlation of the proactive coping scale average score to the reflective coping scale average score was 0.71.

In this study, we used Chinese version of the proactive coping scale and the reflective coping scale (Cheng and Greenglass, 2006; Wu et al., 2008), a single score was derived from these two subscales and was named proactive–reflective coping. The Cronbach alpha coefficient for our sample was 0.92.

Social Adaptation

Measurements of social adaptation consist of a 13-item Sociocultural Adaptation Scale (SAS, Ward and Kennedy, 1999) and a 9-item self-made work adaptation scale. The original SAS was made up of 41 items covering a variety of social situations and requires respondents to indicate the degree of difficulty they encounter in those social situations using a 5-point scale. An evolving program of cross-sectional research has convincingly demonstrated that the SAS is a reliable and valid measurement of cultural competence or behavioral adaptability in cross-cultural sojourners (Gao, 2014). In our study, some modifications have been made to the questionnaire and fewer items were included, as fitting the range of experiences of visiting scholars. Altogether five dimensions (interpersonal relationship, public situations, academic situations, academic achievement, social support) were included based on the original items. The Cronbach alpha coefficient for social adaptation was 0.75 in our sample.

Psychological Adaptation

The Zung (1965) Self-Rating Depression Scale (ZSDS) consists of 20 items that measure affective, cognitive, and physiological components of depression. Participants respond on a 5-point scale (1 = disagree strongly, 5 = agree strongly) and higher scores are associated with greater depression. It has been used extensively in cross-cultural research (Zung, 1969) and has consistently proven reliable and valid in multinational sojourner studies (e.g., Ward and Kennedy, 1999; Zhu, 2011; Gao, 2014). In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for psychological adaptation was 0.86.

Life Satisfaction

The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) was designed to assess the respondent’s overall judgment of their life in order to measure the concept of life satisfaction (Diener et al., 1985). The 5-item SWLS are global rather than specific in nature, allowing respondents to weight domains of their lives in terms of their own values (Frisch et al., 1992). In this study, the Cronbach alpha coefficient for SWLS was 0.90.

Common Method Bias Test

All data in this study were collected by the self-report method, which may lead to common method bias. We first controlled common method bias from measurement procedures, such as reverse-coded items and anonymous questionnaire filling. Then, Harman’s single-factor test was carried out before formal data analyses ((Podsakoff et al., 2003). The results showed that the total number of factors with eigenvalues greater than 1 was 37, and the variance of the first factor was 15.79% which was less than the critical value of 40%. Therefore, there was no obvious common method bias in the data of this study.

Data Analyses

Testing the hypotheses, structural equation modeling (SEM) was employed to investigate the relationship between wisdom and adaptation as mediated by coping styles using Mplus7.11, which provides the maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors (MLR). The model chi-square (χ2), χ2 to df ratio (χ2/df), the comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) were used to evaluate the model fit. The combination of model chi-square values accompanying p > 0.05, χ2 to df ratio < 2, CFI and TLI values>0.95, RMSEA and SRMR values<0.05 indicated a good model fit. Other researchers have discussed that CFI values between 0.90 and 0.95 are an acceptable fit (Kline, 2011).

Results

Correlation Analysis

Tables 1 and 2 present the descriptive statistics and correlations. Pearson’s correlation analyses (Table 2) indicated that 3D-WS and ASTI were significantly and positively correlated with social adaptation and psychological adaptation, but only ASTI was significantly correlated with SWLS. Active coping and proactive–reflective coping were both significantly and positively correlated with social adaptation, psychological adaptation, and SWLS; passive coping was negatively correlated with psychological adaptation but positively correlated with SWLS. 3DWS was positively correlated with active coping and proactive–reflective coping, but negatively correlated with passive coping. ASTI was positively correlated with active coping, passive coping, and proactive–reflective coping. Proactive–reflective coping was highly correlated with active coping. The correlation analysis provided evidence to support the test of mediation model.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Minimum, maximum, means, SDs, skewness, kurtosis, reliability.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The correlation matrix among variables.

Path Analysis

We applied path analysis to test the proposed mediation model as depicted in Figure 1, with estimates of the standardized path coefficients.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Standardized parameter estimates of the effects of wisdom [3DWS and Adult Self-Transcendence Inventory (ASTI)] on adaptation and SWLS mediated by coping styles. N = 181, Model fit: χ2 = 14.73, df = 11, χ2/df = 1.34, CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.97, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.04, root mean square residual (SRMR) = 0.04, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. SWLS = life satisfaction.

The mediation model showed a good fit, χ2(11) =14.73, p = 0.20 (χ2/df = 1.34), CFI =0.99, TLI =0.97, RMSEA =0.04 (90% CI = 0.00–0.09), SRMR =0.04. The effects of 3DWS and ASTI on social adaptation, psychological adaptation, and SWLS mediated by proactive–reflective coping were significant. 3DWS and ASTI negatively affected psychological adaptation but positively affected SWLS through passive coping. The paths from 3DWS and ASTI to social adaptation, psychological adaptation, and SWLS, as mediated by active coping were not significant.

We then examined the direct path coefficient from 3DWS and ASTI to social adaptation, psychological adaptation, and SWLS, without the mediators, after controlling for gender, age, visiting period, English proficiency, and the perceived impact of COVID-19. Results showed that the direct path coefficients from ASTI to social adaptation, psychological adaptation, and SWLS were statistically significant (β = 0.29, p < 0.001, β = 0.15, p < 0.05, β = 0.18, p < 0.05), and the direct path coefficients from 3DWS to social adaptation and psychological adaptation were statistically significant (β = 0.27, p < 0.01, β = 0.30, p < 0.001).

To evaluate the significance of indirect effects, bootstrapped standard errors were calculated (a bootstrap sample of 5,000 was specified) for the parameters and used to construct 95% confidence intervals (CIs). This was done because z statistics for indirect effects can be biased by deviations from distributional assumptions (MacKinnon, 2008). Significance of indirect effects on bootstrapping data is determined by whether zero is included within the 95% confidence interval. Confidence intervals including zero are considered non-significant. We estimated all possible indirect effects and the associated 95% bootstrap confidence intervals in the model. As displayed in Table 3, 3DWS and ASTI both exerted effects on social adaptation, psychological adaptation, and SWLS, through the indirect path via the mediating effect of proactive–reflective coping. Additionally, 3DWS exerted its effect on psychological adaptation through an indirect path via the mediating effect of passive coping.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Standardized indirect effects and 95% CI for the mediation model.

Discussion

The main objective of this study is to investigate the role of wisdom in the Chinese visiting scholars’ adaptation and life satisfaction and extend the literature by examining the mediation effects of coping styles underlying this association. Overall, the hypotheses were partially supported.

Wisdom (measured by 3DWS and ASTI) was directly correlated to social and psychological adaptation and was positively associated with engaged coping (active coping and proactive–reflective coping). The apparent positive impact of wisdom on the endorsement of engaged coping styles and adaptation may be attributed to the complex and multifaceted nature of wisdom which is a construct that involves expert life management and planning in selecting, optimizing, and compensating in the face of life challenges (Freund and Baltes, 2002a, 2002b) and manifests in how people cope with real-life problems (Glück et al., 2005; Grossmann et al., 2016). Wiser or less wise individuals act differently in how they handle difficult, uncertain, and challenging situations (Gluck and Bluck, 2013). In the face of difficulties, wiser individuals may place their lives and the challenges they encounter in a larger and meaningful context, which can facilitate positive coping. In this way, wisdom guides the coping behavior of Chinese visiting scholars. For instance, wiser individuals may apply engaged coping strategies when dealing with acculturation challenges.

Even though previous studies also suggested that high wisdom scorers were more engaged in coping with challenging events in their lives (Ardelt, 2005, 2010), in our study, only 3DWS was negatively correlated to passive coping, while ASTI showed a positive association. This may be due to the broad concept of wisdom and behavioral influence from the cultural setting. Based on the philosophical analysis of cross-cultural European and Asian wisdom, the ASTI (Levenson et al., 2005) defines wisdom as self-transcendence, that is, an inner peace independent of external disturbance and the feelings of unity with others and nature, which may have some consistency with some aspects of the Chinese Daoist philosophy. Daoism inherited identical nature-centric epistemology and methodology (Sivin, 1978). The influence of traditional Daoist attitude toward life and way of person thinking and making has been deeply imprinted in Chinese people (Feng, 2014; Tu and Guo, 2014). Wang and Wang (2021) elaborated further that in the view of the Lao-Zhuang (Daoist representatives Laozi and Zhuangzi) school of thought, Daoism values the doctrine of dao which includes the soft, weak, empty, simple, non-doing, and natural self-characteristics. Consistent with Daoist Self, one can comprehend and accept the laws of nature, live in unconstrained freedom, and take things as they come (Feng, 2014; Wang and Wang, 2020). Thus, Daoism advocates naturalness, effortlessness, and non-action, which might include some passive coping strategies, such as “I’ve been accepting the reality of the fact that it has happened” or “I’ve been thinking that time will change the status quo, the only thing to do is to wait.” Given that different cultures can shape or emphasize different dimensions of wisdom, people from different cultural backgrounds often attach importance to various aspects of wisdom (Takahashi and Bordia, 2000; Yang, 2001; Takahashi and Overton, 2005). Chinese visiting scholars’ behavior manifests the “inaction” in Chinese culture, which suggested that under the influence of Confucian and Daoist philosophy, some of the wiser individuals might choose to let things take their own course, especially during the COVID-19 pandemic.

This study also revealed that coping styles partially mediated the association between wisdom, adaptation, and life satisfaction. It is interesting to find that the path of ASTI, 3DWS → Proactive–reflective coping→ Social adaptation, Psychological adaptation, and SWLS were all significant. This indicates that wiser Chinese visiting scholars had a tendency to adopt future-oriented proactive coping strategies to prepare for potential future challenges, and in turn, it positively influences their adaptation and life satisfaction in Canada.

Coping is typically referred to as an emotional, behavioral, and cognitive efforts to manage the demands created by stress or conflict (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984). It usually occurs as a reaction to stressful events. Proactive coping is a process through which one prepares for potential future stressors and promote personal growth and wellbeing (Aspinwall, 1997; Aspinwall and Taylor, 1997). In other words, proactive–reflective coping demonstrates how an individual faces life rather than reacting to it. It is a wise way of coping and may help to prevent future threats to personal goals. Grossmann et al. explored the relationship between wisdom, future-directed reasoning, and affective forecasting. These studies found that wise reasoning about future conflicts can better help to solve conflicts (Huynh et al., 2016; Peetz and Grossmann, 2020). It is also found in the Chinese cultural sphere that those who can predict changes are considered wiser (Ji et al., 2001). The influential text The Classic of Changes (易经) has historically been used for diagnosing particular moments in time, including how to act wisely to avoid ill fortune. It may provide ideas to increase wisdom and make the proactive model possible (Kim et al., 2020). In addition, it was found by Mohammad and Zahra (2012) that all the proactive coping skills significantly and negatively correlated to depression. Kahana et al. (2012) also pointed out the individual’s psychological feelings can be affected by proactive coping strategies. The significance of proactive coping as an indicator of life satisfaction among undergraduate freshmen was shown in the study by Dwivedi and Rastogi (2017). All the above are in line with the significant path of the present study that wisdom→ proactive–reflective coping →adaptation (social and psychological adaptation) and life satisfaction. For the Chinese visiting scholars, adopting proactive–reflective coping strategies can help them enhance the planning and reflective ability to handle future challenges in the workplace and social context in Canada while improving their psychological wellbeing.

However, the effect of passive coping was only significantly when mediating between 3DWS, adaptation, and life satisfaction, and there was no significant mediating effect of active coping between wisdom (3DWS, ASTI), adaptation, and life satisfaction. This is possible because the investigation was implemented during the COVID-19 and the stressful events they encountered were in close relation to the pandemic. Personal wisdom is considered a quality or aspect of the person, whereas the actual use of coping strategies is a process that is not only influenced by individual qualities, but also by the context (Aldwin, 2007). Both active coping and passive coping are responses to stressful events that have already occurred (Greenglass, 2002), and the selected coping strategies the participants used will depend on how it is being applied in the face of the stressor. In the context of COVID-19, Chinese visiting scholars were all facing unexpected challenging events and were unable to respond to related stressors directly. The more adaptable individuals might be the ones who prepared themselves in advance. Thus, the mediating effect of active coping and passive coping were insignificant or only partially significant between wisdom, adaptation, and life satisfaction.

There are some limitations in the presented work that need to be addressed in the future. First, the concurrent influences on adaptation and life satisfaction were examined in a single testing occasion, which makes it difficult to determine the developmental pathways or draw causal conclusions from the relationship between wisdom, coping styles, adaptation, and life satisfaction in the interpretation of results. In the future, more longitudinal, experimental, or intervention studies could be employed to provide more information about within- and between-person differences in the relationship between wisdom, coping, and adaptation. Second, the generalizability of the findings is limited due to the relative homogeneity within this sample. All of our participants were Chinese visiting scholars in Canada. Future investigations should be broadened to other acculturation groups living in other countries and explore whether the relationships among wisdom, coping styles, adaptation, and life satisfaction found in the present study can be replicated. Third, on the basis of quantitative research, biographical interviews can help to explore in-depth personal wisdom and visiting scholars’ experience and elucidates within-person change.

Despite the limitations, the findings in this study still make a significant contribution to practice and research. First, the insight into the psychological mechanism underlying the association between wisdom and adaptation was substantially extended. In addition, one of the most intriguing findings is the significant path from wisdom to social, psychological adaptation, and life satisfaction, through proactive–reflective coping, which suggests that proactive–reflective coping may be an important mediating variable between wisdom, adaptation, and life satisfaction. It echoes a previous finding where proactive coping not only relates to depression negatively (Mohammad and Zahra, 2012) but also predicts positive results and promotes wellbeing (Greenglass and Fiksenbaum, 2009). Further research should examine wisdom and coping styles in more detail with sub-samples in order to determine the relations between wisdom and many other types of coping. Additionally, unraveling the relationships between wisdom, coping styles, and adaptation may enrich the theories of wisdom and adaptation based on different cultural background. The 3DWS and the ASTI were independent predictors of these coping and outcome measure, which reinforce that the notion that these scales are tapping different but equally important aspects of wisdom. Second, the findings in the study indicate that proactive–reflective coping strategies could help visiting scholars adapt more easily to life in Canada. Therefore, intervention programs focusing on improving or training their proactive coping skills might mitigate negative influences of psychological distress and enhance the quality of life in adaptation. In turn, it might help develop their wisdom when they learn to cope with their life challenges during the adaptation process. Lastly, this is a new study about the visiting scholars, which provides a new perspective for the related research of this group of people. It is better to start with a single group, but are there experiences typical for other groups, that can be comparable experiences for future direction of research.

In conclusion, the study seeks to contribute to investigating the relationship between wisdom and adaptation. It further suggests that wisdom, as a useful construct in the study of adaptation and acculturation, maybe a major contributor to positive outcome. Also, it reveals that individuals who report a higher level of wisdom may engage more in proactive–reflective coping strategies and are more satisfied with their lives. It is hoped that the present empirical study will inspire further discussions and debates on the wise-coping approach to adaptation and serve to stimulate further study to understand wisdom and its relationship to the adaption process and outcome. Besides, it is estimated that wisdom and adaptation might reciprocally interact with each other. Sophisticated study designs, such as cross-lagged panel studies, would help explore how mutual influence unfolds over time. All the answers to these questions will have far-reaching implications for practice, research, and policy-making in the future.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Hubei University of Medicine Ethics Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

DB, LZ, MF, and ZF contributed to conception and design of the study. DB, YC, and LZ organized the database and performed the statistical analysis. DB wrote the first draft of the manuscript. MF provided resources, supervision for the study, and reviewed the manuscript. MF, DB, and ZF contributed to manuscript revision. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was funded by the Humanities and Social Science Research Projects of Hubei Province (grant no: 21Y162).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all the Chinese visiting scholars for their participation. The authors thank some PhD students of the University of Toronto for their help with data collection.

Footnotes

1. ^https://opendoorsdata.org/

References

Aldwin, C. M. (2007). Stress, Coping, & Development: An Integrative Perspective (2nd Edn). New York, NY: Guildford Press.

Google Scholar

Ardelt, M. (1997). Wisdom and life satisfaction in old age. J. Gerontol. Psychol. Sci. 52B, P15–P27. doi: 10.1093/geronb/52b.1.p15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardelt, M. (2003). Empirical assessment of a three-dimensional wisdom scale. Res. Aging 25, 275–324. doi: 10.1177/0164027503025003004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardelt, M. (2005). How wise people cope with crises and obstacles in life. Revision 28, 7–20.

Google Scholar

Ardelt, M. (2010). Are older adults wiser than college students? A comparison of two age cohorts. J. Adult Dev. 17, 193–207. doi: 10.1007/s10804-009-9088-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardelt, M., and Ferrari, M. (2014). “Wisdom and emotions,” in The Oxford Handbook of Emotion, Social Cognition, and Problem Solving during Adulthood. eds. C. Herzog and P. Verhaeghen (New York, NY: Oxford University Press)

Google Scholar

Ardelt, M., and Ferrari, M. (2019). Effects of wisdom and religiosity on subjective well-being in old age and young adulthood: exploring the pathways through mastery and purpose in life. Int. Psychogeriatr. 31, 477–489. doi: 10.1017/S1041610218001680

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardelt, M., Ferrari, M., and Shi, W. (2020). “Implicit wisdom theories from around the world: variations by gender, age group, and nation,” in Handbook of Practical Wisdom in Business and Management. eds. B. Schwartz, C. Bernacchio, C. GonzálezCantón, and A. Robson (Springer), 1–30.

Google Scholar

Asadi, S. (2015). Characteristics of wise people based on implicit theories: regarding group and gender differences. J. Posit. Psychol. Res. 1, 51–61.

Google Scholar

Aspinwall, L. G. (1997). “Future-oriented aspects of social comparisons: A framework for studying health-related comparison activity,” in Health, Coping, and Well-Being: Perspectives from Social Comparison Theory. eds. B. P. Buunk and F. X. Gibbons (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 125–165.

Google Scholar

Aspinwall, L. G., and Taylor, S. E. (1997). A stitch in time: self-regulation and proactive coping. Psychol. Bull. 121, 417–436. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baltes, P. B., and Smith, J. (1990). “Toward a psychology of wisdom and its ontogenesis,” in Wisdom: Its Nature, Origins, and Development. ed. R. J. Sternberg (New York: Cambridge University Press), 87–120.

Google Scholar

Baltes, P. B., and Smith, J. (2008). The fascination of wisdom: its nature, ontogeny, and function. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 3, 56–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00062.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baltes, P. B., and Staudinger, U. M. (2000). Wisdom: A Metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue Toward excellence. Am. Psychol. 55, 122–136. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bang, H., and Montgomery, D. (2013). Wisdom and ego-identity for Korean and American late adolescents. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 44, 807–831. doi: 10.1177/0022022112466941

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bang, H., and Zhou, Y. (2014). The function of wisdom dimensions in ego-identity development among Chinese university students. Int. J. Psychol. 49, 434–445. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedikovicova, J., and Ardelt, M. (2008). The three-dimensional wisdom scale in cross-cultural context: A comparison between American and Slovak college students. Stud. Psychol. 50, 179–190.

Google Scholar

Bergsma, A., and Ardelt, M. (2012). Self-reported wisdom and happiness: An empirical investigation. J. Happiness Stud. 13, 481–499. doi: 10.1007/s10902-011-9275-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, J. W. (1997). Immigration, acculturation and adaptation. Appl. Psychol. Int. Rev. 46, 5–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.1997.tb01087.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, J. W. (2006). “Stress perspectives on acculturation,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Acculturation Psychology. eds. D. L. Sam and J. W. Berry (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 43–57.

Google Scholar

Birren, J. E., and Svensson, C. M. (2005). “Wisdom in history,” in A Handbook of Wisdom: Psychological Perspectives. eds. R. J. Sternberg and J. Jordan (New York: Cambridge University Press), 3–31.

Google Scholar

Bluck, S., and Glück, J. (2004). Making things better and learning a lesson: experiencing wisdom across the lifespan. J. Pers. 72, 543–572. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00272.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burnam, M., Hough, R., Karno, M., Escobar, J., and Telles, C. (1987). Acculturation and lifetime prevalence of psychiatric disorders among Mexican Americans in Los Angeles. J. Health Soc. Behav. 28, 89–102. doi: 10.2307/2137143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, H., and Wang, F. (2014). The experimental research on college students’ implicit cognition of wisdom. Psychological Development and Education. 30, 363–370. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2014.04.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, J., and Greenglass, E. R. (2006). Proactive Coping Inventory (PCI), Chinese Translation. Available at: www.psych.yorku.ca/greenglass/ (Accessed January 12 2006).

Google Scholar

Chia, A.-L., and Costigan, C. L. (2006). Understanding the multidimensionality of acculturation among Chinese Canadians. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 38, 311–324. doi: 10.1037/cjbs2006017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

China Scholarship Council (2018). Organization Introduction. Available at: https://www.csc.edu.cn/about (Accessed January 15, 2021).

Google Scholar

Clayton, V. P., and Birren, J. E. (1980). “The development of wisdom across the life span: A reexamination of an ancient topic,” in Life-Span Development and Behavior. Vol. 3. eds. P. B. Baltes and O. G. Brim, (Cambridge, Massachusetts: Academic Press), 103–135.

Google Scholar

Coll, C., Lamberty, G., Jenkins, R., McAdoo, H., Crnic, K., Wasik, B., et al. (1996). An integrative model for the study of developmental competencies in minority children. Child Dev. 67:1891. doi: 10.2307/1131600

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cottingham, J. (1996). Western Philosophy: An Anthology. New York: Blackwell.

Google Scholar

Curnow, T. (1999). Wisdom, Intuition, and Ethics. Farnham, Surrey, Burlington, UK: Ashgate Publishing.

Google Scholar

Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., and Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 49, 71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dwivedi, A., and Rastogi, R. (2017). Proactive coping, time perspective and life satisfaction: A study on emerging adulthood. J. Health Manag. 19, 264–274. doi: 10.1177/0972063417699689

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and Society. New York: W.W.Norton.

Google Scholar

Feng, Y.-L. (2014). Six Books of Zhen Yuan. Beijing, China: Zhonghua Book Company.

Google Scholar

Freund, A. M., and Baltes, P. B. (2002a). Life-management strategies of selection, optimization, and compensation: measurement by self-report and construct validity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 642–662. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.4.642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freund, A. M., and Baltes, P. B. (2002b). The adaptiveness of selection, optimization, and compensation as strategies of life management: evidence from a preference study on proverbs. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 57, P426–P434. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.5.P426

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frisch, M., Cornell, J., Villanueva, M., and Retzlaff, P. (1992). Clinical validation of the quality of life inventory. A measure of life satisfaction for use in treatment planning and outcome assessment. Psychol. Assess. 4, 92–101. doi: 10.1037//1040-3590.4.1.92

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fu, X. R. (2019). The Relationship between Wisdom Measurement and Well-Being of College Students. Doctoral dissertation. Nanjing Normal University, China.

Google Scholar

Gao, Y. (2014). A Study on the Intercultural Adaptability of Chinese Volunteer Teachers during their Preliminary Teaching Stage Overseas in Confucius Institutes in Hungry in 2014. Master dissertation. Beijing Foreign Language University, China.

Google Scholar

Glück, J. (2017). … and the wisdom to know the difference: scholarly success From a wisdom perspective. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 1148–1152. doi: 10.1177/1745691617727528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glück, J., and Bluck, S. (2011). Laypeople’s conceptions of wisdom and its development: cognitive and integrative views. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 66, 321–324. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gluck, J., and Bluck, S. (2013). “The MORE life experience model: A theory of the development of personal wisdom,” in The Scientific Study of Personal Wisdom. eds. M. Ferrari and N. Weststrate (New York, NY: Springer), 75–97.

Google Scholar

Glück, J., Bluck, S., Baron, J., and McAdams, D. (2005). The wisdom of experience: autobiographical narratives across adulthood. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 29, 197–208. doi: 10.1080/01650250444000504

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glück, J., König, S., Naschenweng, K., Redzanowski, U., Dorner-Hörig, L., Straßer, I., et al. (2013). How to measure wisdom: content, reliability, and validity of five measures. Front. Psychol. 4:405. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenglass, E. (2002). “Proactive coping,” in Beyond Coping: Meeting Goals, Vision, and Challenges. ed. E. Frydenberg (London: Oxford University Press), 37–62.

Google Scholar

Greenglass, E., and Fiksenbaum, L. (2009). Proactive coping, positive affect, and well-being: testing for mediation using path analysis. Eur. Psychol. 14, 29–39. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040.14.1.29

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenglass, E. R., Schwarzer, R., and Taubert, S. (1999). The Proactive Coping Inventory (PCI): A Multidimensional Research Instrument. Available at: http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/greenpci.htm (Accessed January 14, 2021).

Google Scholar

Grossmann, I., Gerlach, T. M., and Denissen, J. J. (2016). Wise reasoning in the face of everyday life challenges. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 7, 611–622. doi: 10.1177/1948550616652206

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossmann, I., Weststrate, N. M., Ardelt, M., Brienza, J. P., Dong, M., Ferrari, M., et al. (2020). Wisdom science in a polarized world: Knowns and unknowns. Psychol. Inq. 31, 103–133. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2020.1750917

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hart, William. (1987). The Art of Living. Vipassana meditation as taught by S. N. Goenka. San Francisco: Harper.

Google Scholar

Holliday, S. G., and Chandler, M. J. (1986). Wisdom: Explorations in Adult Competence. Basel, Switzerland: Karger.

Google Scholar

Huynh, A. C., Yang, D. Y. J., and Grossmann, I. (2016). The value of prospective reasoning for close relationships. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 7, 893–902. doi: 10.1177/1948550616660591

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ji, L.-J., Nisbett, R. E., and Su, Y. (2001). Culture, change, and prediction. Psychol. Sci. 12, 450–456. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00384

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahana, E., Kelley-Moore, J., and Kahana, B. (2012). Proactive aging: A longitudinal study of stress, resources, agency, and well-being in late life. Aging Ment. Health 16, 438–451. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2011.644519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, B. S. K., and Abreu, J. M. (2001). “Acculturation measurement: theory, current instruments, and future directions,” in Handbook of Multicultural Counseling. 2nd Edn. eds. J. G. Ponterotto, M. Casas, L. Suzuki, and C. M. Alexander (San Francisco, CA: SAGE), 394–424.

Google Scholar

Kim, J. J., Feng, Z., and Ferrari, M. (2020). Foresight and wisdom: the case of the classic of changes. Humanist. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/hum0000194

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S. Y., and Knight, B. G. (2015). Adaptation of the three-dimensional wisdom scale (3D-WS) for the Korean cultural context. Int. Psychogeriatr. 27, 267–278. doi: 10.1017/S1041610214002178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling (3rd Edn). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Koller, I., Levenson, M. R., and Glück, J. (2017). What do you think you are measuring? A mixed-methods procedure for assessing the content validity of test items and theory-based scaling. Front. Psychol. 8:126. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuo, B. C. H., and Roysircar, G. (2004). Predictors of acculturation for Chinese adolescents in Canada: age of arrival, length of stay, social class, and English reading ability. J. Multicult. Couns. Devel. 32, 143–154. doi: 10.1002/j.2161-1912.2004.tb00367.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwan, K., and Sodowsky, G. R. (1997). Internal and external ethnic identity and their correlates: A study of Chinese American immigrants. J. Multicult. Couns. Dev. 25, 51–67. doi: 10.1002/j.2161-1912.1997.tb00315.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York, NY: Springer Publishing.

Google Scholar

Le, T. N. (2008). Culture values, life experiences, and wisdom. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 66, 259–281. doi: 10.2190/AG.66.4.a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le, T. N. (2010). Life satisfaction, openness value, self-transcendence, and wisdom. J. Happiness Stud. 12, 171–182. doi: 10.1007/s10902-010-9182-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. S., Koeske, G. F., and Sales, E. (2004). Social support buffering of acculturative stress: A study of mental health symptoms among Korean international students. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 28, 399–414. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2004.08.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levenson, M. R., Jennings, P. A., Aldwin, C. M., and Shiraishi, R. W. (2005). Self-transcendence: conceptualization and measurement. Int. J. Aging Hum. 60, 127–143. doi: 10.2190/XRXM-FYRA-7U0X-GRC0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levitt, H. M. (1999). The development of wisdom: an analysis of Tibetan Buddhist experience. J. Humanist. Psychol. 39, 86–105. doi: 10.1177/0022167899392006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, S.-P., and Betz, N. E. (2009). Factors related to the social self-efficacy of Chinese international students. Couns. Psychol. 37, 451–471. doi: 10.1177/0011000009332474

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linden, M. (2014). “Promoting resilience and well-being with wisdom and wisdom therapy,” in Increasing Well-Being in Clinical and Educational Setting. Interventions and Cultural Context. eds. G. A. Fava and C. Ruini (Heidelberg: Springer, Heidelberg)

Google Scholar

Linden, M., Baumann, K., Lieberei, B., Lorenz, C., and Rotter, M. (2011). Treatment of posttraumatic embitterment disorder with cognitive behaviour therapy based on wisdom psychology and hedonia strategies. Psychother. Psychosom. 80, 199–205. doi: 10.1159/000321580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacKinnon, D. P. (2008). Introduction to Statistical Mediation Analysis. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Magee, B. (1998). The Story of Philosophy. New York: DK.

Google Scholar

Masuda, T., and Nisbett, R. E. (2006). Culture and change blindness. Cogn. Sci. 30, 381–399. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_63

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohammad, S., and Zahra, F. (2012). Proactive coping skills and depression of women teacher students. Asian J. Dev. Matters 5, 82–88.

Google Scholar

Mori, S. C. (2000). Addressing the mental health concerns of international students. J. Couns. Dev. 78, 137–144. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.2000.tb02571.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munroe, M., Al-Refae, M., Chan, H. W., and Ferrari, M. (2021). Using self-compassion to grow in the face of trauma: The role of positive reframing and problem-focused coping strategies. Psychol. Trauma. doi: 10.1037/tra0001164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakasone, R. Y. (1994). Spiritual journeys in aging: a Buddhist view. J. Relig. Health 33, 243–251. doi: 10.1007/BF02354915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbett, R. E., and Masuda, T. (2003). Culture and point of view. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 100, 11163–11170. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1934527100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., and Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychol. Rev. 108, 291–310. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noh, S., and Kaspar, V. (2003). Perceived discrimination & depression: moderating effects of coping, acculturation, and ethnic support. Am. J. Public Health 93, 232–238. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.93.2.232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orwoll, L., and Achenbaum, A. (1993). Gender and the development of wisdom. Hum. Dev. 36, 274–296. doi: 10.1159/000278214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orwoll, L., and Perlmutter, M. (1990). The study of wise persons: integrating a personality perspective. See Sternberg 1990. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, 160–178.

Google Scholar

Peetz, J., and Grossmann, I. (2020). Wise reasoning about the future is associated with adaptive interpersonal feelings after relational challenges. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 12, 629–637. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/8nwr4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, K., Nisbett, R. E., and Wong, N. Y. C. (1997). Validity problems comparing values across cultures and possible solutions. Psychol. Methods 2, 329–344. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.2.4.329

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Randall, W., and Kenyon, G. (2001). Ordinary Wisdom: Biographical Aging and the Journey of Life. Westport, USA: Praeger.

Google Scholar

Russell, J., Rosenthal, D., and Thomson, G. (2010). The international student experience: three styles of adaptation. High. Educ. 60, 235–249. doi: 10.1007/s10734-009-9297-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Safdar, S., Calvez, S., and Lewis, J. R. (2012). Multi-group analysis of the MIDA model: acculturation of Indian and Russian immigrants to Canada. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 36, 200–212. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2011.11.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Safdar, S., Choung, K., and Lewis, J. R. (2013). “A review of the MIDA model and other contemporary acculturation models,” in Immigration: Policies, Challenges and Impact. ed. E. Tartakovsky (Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publisher), 213–230.

Google Scholar

Safdar, S., Lay, C., and Struthers, W. (2003). The process of acculturation and basic goals: testing a multidimensional individual difference acculturation model with Iranian immigrants in Canada. Appl. Psychol. 52, 555–579. doi: 10.1111/1464-0597.00151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Safdar, S., Lewis, J. R., Greenglass, E., and Daneshpour, M. (2009a). “An examination of proactive coping and social beliefs among Christians and Muslims,” in Psychological aspects of social axioms. Springer, New York, NY, 177–196.

Google Scholar

Safdar, S., Struthers, W., and van Oudenhoven, J. P. (2009b). Acculturation of Iranians in the United States, the United Kingdom, and the Netherlands: A test of the multidimensional individual difference acculturation (MIDA) model. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 40, 468–491. doi: 10.1177/0022022108330990

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sahrani, R., Matindas, R. W., Takwin, B., and Mansoer, W. W. (2014). The role of reflection of difficult life experiences on wisdom. J. Indian Acad. Appl. Psychol. 40, 315–323.

Google Scholar

Searle, J. R. (1992). The Rediscovery of the Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press

Google Scholar

Singelis, T. M., Triandis, H. C., Bhawuk, D. P., and Gelfand, M. J. (1995). Horizontal and vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism: A theoretical and measurement refinement. Cross Cult. Res. 29, 240–275. doi: 10.1177/106939719502900302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sivin, N. (1978). On the word “Taoist” as a source of perplexity. With special reference to the relations of science and religion in traditional China. Hist. Relig. 17, 303–330. doi: 10.1086/462796

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staudinger, U. M. (1999). “Social cognition and a psychological approach to the art of life,” in Social Cognition and Aging. eds. T. M. Hess and F. Blanchard-Fields (Cambridge, Massachusetts: Academic Press), 343–375.

Google Scholar

Staudinger, U. M. (2001). Life reflection: A social-cognitive analysis of life review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 5, 148–160. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.5.2.148

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staudinger, U. M. (2013). “The need to distinguish personal from general wisdom: a short history and empirical evidence,” in The Scientific Study of Personal Wisdom. eds. M. Ferrari and N. Weststrate (Dordrecht: Springer)

Google Scholar

Staudinger, U. M., and Glück, J. (2011). Psychological wisdom research: commonalities and differences in a growing field. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 215–241. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131659

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staudinger, U. M., and Leipold, B. (2019). “The assessment of wisdom-related performance,” in Positive Psychological Assessment: A Handbook of Models and Measures. eds. M. W. Gallagher and S. J. Lopez (NE, Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association), 139–156.

Google Scholar

Sternberg, R. J. (1990a). “Wisdom and its relations to intelligence and creativity,” in Wisdom: Its Nature, Origins, and Development (Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press), 142–159.

Google Scholar

Sternberg, R., Nusbaum, H., and Glück, J. (2019). Applying Wisdom to Contemporary World Problems. 1st Edn. Salmon Tower in Midtown Manhattan, New York, US: Springer International Publishing.

Google Scholar

Takahashi, M., and Bordia, P. (2000). The concept of wisdom: a cross-cultural comparison. Int. J. Psychol. 35, 1–9. doi: 10.1080/002075900399475

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, M., and Overton, W. F. (2005). “Cultural foundations of wisdom: an integrated developmental approach,” in A Handbook of Wisdom: Psychological Perspectives. eds. R. J. Sternberg and J. Jordan (Cambridge University Press: New York), 32–60.

Google Scholar

Taylor, M., Bates, G., and Webster, J. D. (2011). Conceptualizing and measuring wisdom: a reply to Ardelt. Exp. Aging Res. 37, 256–259. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2011.554514

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, M. L., Bangen, K. J., Ardelt, M., and Jeste, D. V. (2017). Development of a 12-item abbreviated three-dimensional wisdom scale (3D-WS-12) item selection and psychometric properties. Assessment 24, 71–82. doi: 10.1177/1073191115595714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tornstam, L. (1997). Gerotranscendence: The contemplative dimension of aging. J. Aging Stud. 11, 143–154. doi: 10.1016/S0890-4065(97)90018-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torres, L., and Rollock, D. (2004). Acculturative distress among Hispanics: The role of acculturation, coping, and intercultural competence. J. Multicult. Couns. Devel. 32, 155–167. doi: 10.1002/j.2161-1912.2004.tb00368.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychol. Rev. 96, 506–520. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Triandis, H. C., and Gelfand, M. J. (1998). Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 118–128. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tu, Y. J., and Guo, Y. Y. (2014). The measurement of Taoist personality. Psychol. Explor. 34, 296–300.

Google Scholar

Wang, F.-Y. (2019). Chinese Cultural Psychology: A New Look, vol. 1 Shanghai Education Publishing House.

Google Scholar

Wang, Z.-D., and Wang, F.-Y. (2020). The Taiji model of self II: developing self models and self-cultivation theories based on the Chinese cultural traditions of Taoism and Buddhism. Front. Psychol. 11:540074. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.540074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z.-D., and Wang, F.-Y. (2021). Ternary Taiji models of the traditional Chinese self: centered on Confucian, Taoist, and Buddhist cultures. J. Humanist. Psychol. doi: 10.1177/00221678211016957

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, F., and Zheng, H. (2014). Exploration and Application of Psychological Theory of Wisdom. Shanghai Education Press.

Google Scholar

Wang, F., and Zheng, H. (2015). Integration of virtue and competence: the nature and category of wisdom. Nanjing Social Science 3, 127–133. doi: 10.15937/j.cnki.issn1001-8263.2015.03.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, C., and Kennedy, A. (1999). The measurement of sociocultural adaptation. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 23, 659–677. doi: 10.1016/S0147-1767(99)00014-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, C. (2001). “The A, B, Cs of acculturation,” in The handbook of culture and psychology. ed. D. Masumoto (New York, NY: Oxford), 411–445.

Google Scholar

Ward, C., and Kennedy, A. (2001). Coping with cross-cultural transition. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 32, 636–642. doi: 10.1177/0022022101032005007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, C., Leong, C-H., and Kennedy, A. (1998). “Self Construals, Stress, Coping and Adjustment during Cross-Cultural Transition.” in Annual Conference of the Society of Australasian Social psychologists, April 16-19, 1998; Christchurch, New Zealand.

Google Scholar

Webster, J. D. (2003). An exploratory analysis of a self-assessed wisdom scale. J. Adult Dev. 10, 13–22. doi: 10.1023/A:1020782619051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webster, J. (2007). Measuring the character strength of wisdom. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 65, 163–183. doi: 10.2190/AG.65.2.d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weststrate, N. M., Bluck, S., and Glück, J. (2019). “Wisdom of the crowd: exploring people’s conceptions of wisdom,” in Cambridge Handbook of Wisdom. eds. R. J. Sternberg and J. Glück (Cambridge University Press), 347–371.

Google Scholar

Weststrate, N. M., and Gluck, J. (2017). Hard-earned wisdom: exploratory processing of difficult life experience is positively associated with wisdom. Dev. Psychol. 53, 800–814. doi: 10.1037/dev0000286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, C. H., Chen, K. H., and YaO, G. (2008). Validation of the proactive coping scale in a sample of Chinese population. J. Psychol. Chin. Soc. 9, 103–120.

Google Scholar

Xie, Y. N. (1998). Reliability and validity of the simplified coping style questionnaire. Chin. J. Clin. Psych. 1998, 114–115.

Google Scholar

Xue, M., Chao, X., and Kuntz, A. M. (2015). Chinese visiting scholars’ academic socialization in US institutions of higher education: A qualitative study. Asia Pacific J. Educ. 35, 290–307. doi: 10.1080/02188791.2013.876389

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, S.-Y. (2001). Conceptions of wisdom among Taiwanese Chinese. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 32, 662–680. doi: 10.1177/0022022101032006002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, S.-Y. (2014a). “From personal striving to positive influences: exploring wisdom in real-life contexts,” in The Scientific Study of Personal Wisdom: From Contemplative Traditions to Neuroscience. eds. M. Ferrari and N. Weststrate (New York: Springer), 115–135. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-7987-7$46

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, S.-Y. (2014b). Wisdom and learning from important and meaningful life experiences. J. Adult Dev. 21, 129–146. doi: 10.1007/s10804-014-9186-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoon, E., Chang, C. T., Kim, S., Clawson, A., Cleary, S. E., Hansen, M., et al. (2012). A meta-analysis of acculturation/enculturation and mental health. J. Couns. Psychol. 60, 15–30. doi: 10.1037/a0030652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshihama, M. (2002). Battered women’s coping strategies and psychological distress: differences by immigrant status. Am. J. Community Psychol. 30, 429–452. doi: 10.1023/A:1015393204820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, X., and Berry, J. W. (1991). Psychological adaptation of Chinese sojourners in Canada. Int. J. Psychol. 26, 451–470. doi: 10.1080/00207599108247134

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, G. H. (2011). Cross Cultural Adaptation among International Students in P.R.C. Institutions of Higher Education. Doctoral dissertation. East China Normal University, China.

Google Scholar

Zung, W. (1965). A self-rating depression scale. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 12, 63–70. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1965.01720310065008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zung, W. (1969). A cross-cultural survey of symptoms in depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 126, 116–121. doi: 10.1176/ajp.126.1.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: wisdom, coping, adaptation, life satisfaction (SWLS), Chinese visiting scholars

Citation: Bao D, Zhou L, Ferrari M, Feng Z and Cheng Y (2022) The Role of Wisdom in the Cross-Cultural Adaptation of Chinese Visiting Scholars to Canada: A Mediation Model. Front. Psychol. 13:779297. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.779297

Received: 18 September 2021; Accepted: 28 February 2022;
Published: 25 March 2022.

Edited by:

William Tsai, New York University, United States

Reviewed by:

Fengyan Wang, Nanjing Normal University, China
Carolyn M. Aldwin, Oregon State University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Bao, Zhou, Ferrari, Feng and Cheng. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Dan Bao, tinabao113@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.