Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 10 November 2022
Sec. Educational Psychology

The effects of genre on the syntactic complexity of argumentative and expository writing by Chinese EFL learners

  • 1Soochow College, Soochow University, Suzhou, China
  • 2School of Foreign Languages, Soochow University, Suzhou, China

Genre researchers have found that writing in different genres involves different cognitive task loads and requires different linguistic demands. Generally speaking, narratives involve the description of events with a focus on people and their actions within a specific time frame, whereas non-narrative genres focus on making an argument or discussing ideas or beliefs in a logical fashion, thus resulting in distinct language features. However, the vast majority of genre-based studies have either focused on one single genre or made comparisons between narrative and non-narrative writing (mostly argumentative) in academic contexts without examining how EFL writers perform across non-narrative genres. Moreover, the measures used in quantifying the syntactic complexity of writing are varied, leading to inconsistent findings. This study investigated the effects of genre on the syntactic complexity of writing through comparing argumentative and expository compositions written by Chinese English-as-a-foreign-language (EFL) learners over one academic year. Fifty-two participants were asked to write eight compositions (with two genres alternated), four argumentative and four expository, which were parsed via the Syntactic Complexity Analyzer. The results with time as the within-subjects variable showed a significant development of syntactic complexity in both argumentative and expository compositions over one academic year. Meanwhile, the paired-sample t-test with genre as the within-subjects variable exhibited a higher syntactic complexity in argumentative compositions than in expository ones on most of the 14 measures examined at four time points over the year. Additionally, a two-way repeated measures analysis of variance with genre and time as independent variables ascertained an interactional effect of time and genre on some of the 14 measures. The present study tested and verified the impact genre exerts on the syntactic complexity of writing, providing implications for teachers to be more informed in teaching and assessing EFL writing and for students to be more conscious of genre difference in EFL writing.

Introduction

Over the past decades, research has shown that writing in different genres involves varied cognitive task loads (Kamberelis and Bovino, 1999; Beauvais et al., 2011; Bi, 2020) and requires different linguistic demands (Berman and Nir-Sagiv, 2004; Nippold, 2004; Beers and Nagy, 2011; Biber et al., 2016; Xu, 2020; Atak and Saricaoglu, 2021). Generally speaking, genres can be classified into narratives and non-narratives, and non-narratives can be further divided into expository, argumentative, persuasive, and the like. For the most part, narratives involve the description of events with a focus on people and their actions within a specific time frame, whereas non-narrative genres focus on making an argument or discussing ideas or beliefs in a logical fashion (Tian, 2014). To be specific, narrative essays tend to involve more third-person pronouns and use of the past tense; expository essays might contain more use of relative clauses and attributive adjectives in describing the theme; and argumentative essays prefer attributive clauses in demonstrating the statement (Brunner, 1986; Biber and Conrad, 2009; Berman and Slobin, 2013). In writing tasks across genres, English-as-a-foreign-language (EFL) writers are generally faced with a task that can be challenging in two aspects: On one hand, they need to identify the special discourse type in the required communicative context, and on the other hand, they need to utilize all their language resources to craft proper expressions to serve the specific context. For instance, Beauvais et al. (2011) found that students used different writing strategies to meet the demands of different writing genres. Compared to narrative writing, argumentative writing required more cognitive effort. Therefore, the students spent more time planning to write an argumentative text because it required a complex and sophisticated knowledge-transforming strategy.

Considering the differences in genres, teachers should both attach importance to writing complexity and take account of the effects of genre differentiation when assessing students’ proficiency level and determining their developmental trajectories in the target language (L2). Hyland (2007) contended that “effective teaching recognizes the wants, prior learning, and current proficiencies of students, but in a genre-based course, it also means, as far as possible, identifying the kinds of writing that learners will need to do in their target situations and incorporating these into the course” (p. 152). Likewise, when illustrating the criteria for assessing the Chinese “FLTRP Cup” English writing contest, Tian (2014) proposed that due attention should be paid to the differences between expository writing and argumentative writing in that both genres are expected to effectively address the topic and the task, but expository writing should present a clear thesis in a formal style and an objective tone; while argumentative writing should present an insightful position on the issue, and the position should be strongly and substantially supported or argued.

To date, the overwhelming majority of previous empirical studies assessing EFL writing have either focused solely on one single genre or made comparisons between narrative and non-narrative writing in academic contexts without examining how EFL writers perform across non-narrative genres (Qin and Uccelli, 2016). Moreover, in the actual teaching of writing, argumentative writing is generally the preferred option for practicing while other genres of writing are more or less ignored. Therefore, in writing instruction and practice, the need arises for students to have the opportunity to be exposed to different genres of writing and for teachers to provide students with specific and targeted guidance and instruction based on different styles of writing.

The present study, through comparing the syntactic complexity of students’ argumentative and expository compositions, attempts to contribute to the existing literature in two ways. First, by examining how students write in these two genres, we are able to see the changes in syntactic complexity in Chinese EFL learners’ English writing over one academic year. Second, by examining the differences in students’ argumentative and expository compositions, we are able to explore whether genre exerts any effects on syntactic complexity in Chinese EFL learners’ argumentative and expository compositions and, furthermore, if the effects of genre are retained over the course of 1 year.

Literature review

Genre in writing

Text genres are primarily divided into narrative and non-narrative in both academic and non-academic contexts (Brunner, 1986). Narratives focus on events and actions in settings performed by the characters, whereas non-narratives (e.g., argumentative, expository, descriptive) focus on ideas and concepts and express the unfolding of claims and argumentation in a logical fashion (Berman and Slobin, 2013). Genre has much to do with cognitive task complexity in relation to two competing hypotheses, namely the cognition hypothesis (Robinson, 2001, 2007) and the limited attentional capacity model (Skehan, 1998). Different genres place different levels of cognitive demands on students, with narrative being the least cognitively demanding, expository cognitively more demanding than narrative, and argumentative the most cognitively complex (Genung, 1900; Bain, 1967; Weigle, 2002). Yang (2014) explored four genres, namely narrative, expository, expo-argumentative, and argumentative, as regards different levels of cognitive demands and reasoning on the attentional resources of participants, finding that the argumentative essays were significantly more complex in global syntactic complexity features than the essays on the other rhetorical tasks. She interpreted her findings by fitting genre into the resource-directing dimension of Robinson’s cognitive hypothesis where reasoning and perspective-taking are involved. However, the 375 subjects were of varied proficiencies, including both English majors and non-English majors, and they were assigned different writing tasks, thus creating confusion as to the real cause underlying the differences.

When it comes to the actual writing, genre differences are reflected in the way words and phrases are selected as well as in the formation and connection of clauses or sentences that best describe their own characteristics (Biber and Conrad, 2009). Since the purpose of this study is to delve into the differences in syntactic complexity between argumentation and exposition, we shall focus on these two genres only and disregard the others herein.

Ruiz-Funes (2015) contended that expository and argumentative genres could be operationalized as the reasoning demands of cognitive task complexity and that they both involve a resource-directing feature of task complexity. Argumentation is a genre that invites a writer to give personal opinions and judgment on a debatable issue or statement and to take a stand on the issue/statement based on facts, generalizations, and reasoning. It is, more often than not, topic-oriented, which requires the writer to impose a logical structure to interrelate ideas in a coherent manner and to organize claims and arguments in a stepwise hierarchical format (Grabe, 2002), where clauses are often used to link ideas and enable complex expressions. By contrast, exposition invites the writer to explain and to provide information about something (not to take a side on something debatable or to argue on the topic) based on facts and generalizations of events and states. The ideas for expository production derive from general world knowledge and academic learning, and expository texts often express the unfolding of claims in containing the theme requirements of noun phrases using nominalizations as clause subjects that condense prior information and present what has already been put forward so that further comment can be made about it (Ravid, 2005).

Syntactic complexity in writing

In second language research, syntactic complexity can be viewed “as a valid and basic descriptor of L2 performance, as an indicator of proficiency and as an index of language development and progress” (Bulté and Housen, 2014, p. 43). Nonetheless, no consensus has been reached as to how to define the construct of syntactic complexity. In Gaies (1980), syntactic complexity was defined as the ability to express more ideas and more thoughts with the use of fewer words, while Skehan (1996) considered syntactic complexity as more elaborate language and more various sentence patterns. In Wolfe-Quintero et al. (1998), syntactic complexity was taken as the range of forms that surface in language production and the degree of sophistication of such forms, whereas Ortega (2015) stated that “syntactic complexity indexes the expansion of the capacity to use the additional language in ever more mature and skillful ways, tapping the full range of linguistic resources offered by the given grammar in order to fulfil various communicative goals successfully” (p. 82). Similarly, Lu (2011) considered syntactic complexity as the range of syntactic structures produced and the degree of sophistication of such structures, which is the way we chose to define syntactic complexity in the present study.

Syntactic complexity has been generally considered essential in assessing the performance of foreign language writing (Wolfe-Quintero et al., 1998; Housen and Kuiken, 2009; Lu, 2011; Staples et al., 2016; Yoon, 2017; Ana, 2018; Kyle and Crossley, 2018). For this reason, many researchers have attempted in the past decades to establish effective and reliable measures of syntactic complexity to assess language progression and judge the writing level and proficiency of learners (De Clercq and Housen, 2017; Ansarifar et al., 2018; Casal and Lee, 2019; Bi and Jiang, 2020; Kim, 2021; Huang et al., 2022; Li et al., 2022). Ortega (2003) showed cumulative evidence concerning syntactic complexity by assessing university students’ writing performance and overall proficiency in the target language through synthesizing 25 studies, testifying to the use of syntactic complexity as an indicator of writing quality and language proficiency. Guo et al. (2013) contended that we could, to some extent, anticipate language learners’ essay scores by looking at their language characteristics and structures in both integrated and independent writing.

Syntactic complexity is often conceptualized as a multidimensional construct, with each dimension requiring different appropriate measures (Norris and Ortega, 2009; Lu, 2011; Bulté and Housen, 2014). Norris and Ortega (2009) proposed that to assess the syntactic complexity of L2 production systematically, researchers should incorporate measures for global complexity, complexity by subordination, complexity via sub-clausal or phrasal elaboration, and possibly complexity by coordination. Bulté and Housen (2014) examined the factors of time and genre together with the relationship to writing quality for a time span of one semester based on corpus essays, suggesting that the effects of time by using measures that detected change or development did not necessarily capture a higher level of language proficiency or better writing quality. Jiang et al. (2019) examined the syntactic complexity of 410 narrative compositions across four writing proficiency levels with both large-grained and fine-grained measures, aiming to arrive at certain indicators that could best discriminate and predict writing proficiency; they concluded that the mean length of production unit and the number of dependent clauses per clause could better predict writing proficiency than other traditional large-grained measures.

Since most of the previous studies on syntactic complexity were directed with limited measuring indices (Biber et al., 2011; Bulté and Housen, 2012), such as mean words per T-unit (W/T), words per clause (W/C), or dependent clause ratio (DC/C), it is necessary to measure syntactic complexity with a relatively comprehensive range of indices. Therefore, this study has adopted for analysis the 14 measures Lu (2011) proposed in the Syntactic Complexity Analyzer (SCA), including the six measures Ortega (2003) examined, a further five reviewed in Wolfe-Quintero et al. (1998), and another three recommended in Wolfe-Quintero et al. (1998) for further research. With the 14 measures examined on different aspects over one academic year, this study will be both insightful and informative as regards the developmental trajectories of syntactic complexity in argumentative and expository writing and also the effects exerted by the two genres.

Previous studies on syntactic complexity in relation to genre differentiation

Measures of syntactic complexity have been proven to be sensitive to the genres of narrative and expository writing (Scott and Windsor, 2000; Beers and Nagy, 2009; Jeong, 2017) and those of narrative and argumentative writing (Crowhurst and Piche, 1979; Beers and Nagy, 2009; Lu, 2011; Qin and Uccelli, 2016; Yoon and Polio, 2016, 2017; Abdel-Malek, 2019; Jagaiah et al., 2020; Xu, 2020; Casal et al., 2021; Lu et al., 2021; Yu, 2021). Scott and Windsor (2000) found that students’ (9 and 11 years old) narratives had more clauses per T-unit than the expository summaries, while Crowhurst and Piche (1979) found that student (12 years old) narratives had fewer clauses per T-unit than persuasive essays (argumentative) and were not significantly different from the descriptive texts. Similarly, Ravid (2005) investigated two genres of L1 writing (narrative and expository) by writers ranging from fourth grade to adulthood and argued that expository texts, in which writers focused on abstract concepts and world knowledge, were more challenging to construct than narratives. The results revealed that children were likely to develop more complex language in expository texts despite the greater challenge posed by expository texts compared to narratives, which appeared less cognitively demanding.

Yoon and Polio (2017) examined narrative and argumentative essays written by 37 English-as-a-second-language (ESL) students to analyze development over time and genre differences. The results indicated strong genre differences in the area of linguistic complexity, with the syntactic complexity of argumentative essays being distinctly higher than that of narrative ones. In another study, Polio and Yoon (2018) investigated two automated systems, namely the L2 SCA and Coh-Metrix, as a way to capture variation in syntactic complexity across two genres. The results suggested that the syntactic complexity in argumentation was of greater magnitude when compared with that in narratives. Lu (2011) researched a corpus of college second language learners’ writing via the automatic SCA through examining 14 objective syntactic complexity measures to quantify second language writing development and progression. The results showed that the conditions of institution, genre, and time imposed a significant impact on the relationship between syntactic complexity and language performance, claiming that the clause was a potentially more informative unit of analysis than the T-unit. In the following year, Park (2012) examined the same 14 syntactic complexity measures as indices of language development in writing by college-level learners of English in Korea, also via SCA, revealing that genre made a great difference in regard to syntactic complexity in L2 learners’ writing, further confirming what had been found in Lu (2011).

With both genre and students’ proficiency taken into account, Beers and Nagy (2009) examined how syntactic complexity measures correlated with overall writing quality in two genres, narratives and expository essays. Their findings suggested that a clausal subordination measure (clauses per T-unit, C/T) was positively correlated with the quality of narratives but negatively with expository essays; conversely, there was no significant effect concerning the length of clause (W/C) on narrative writing, but statistical significance was found on expository writing. In another study, Beers and Nagy (2011) investigated the linguistic development of narrative, descriptive, compare/contrast, and persuasive compositions in different grade levels, and their results supported Crowhurst and Piche (1979) conclusion that narration places the fewest demands and argumentation the greatest demands on writers to make use of their syntactic resources.

Similarly, Yan and Zou (2019) examined differences in syntactic complexity in English among writers at two different proficiency levels and explored the relationship between syntactic complexity and compositions with two different genres. Compositions were also evaluated by SCA, gauging syntactic complexity at the global, clausal, and phrasal levels. The results indicated that the difference between the two genres reached a significant level in terms of C/T and complex nominals per clause (CN/C), but there was no significant relationship between syntactic complexity and L2 proficiency levels and no significant interactive effect between the genre factor and proficiency factor. Bae and Min (2020) investigated how syntactic patterns were different among four different genres, namely narrative, comparison, cause-effect, and argumentative, and three English proficiency levels in Korean L2 college students’ writing, examining the same 14 syntactic complexity measures as in Lu (2011). They found that syntactic complexity showed significant genre differences though no significant group differences of syntactic complexity were found among the L2 proficiency levels.

The findings yielded in previous studies have proven, to various degrees, the effects of genre on syntactic complexity in English writing, pointing to the different types of syntactic structures expected to be mastered in writing in different genres. However, the findings were mostly derived through comparisons between narratives and non-narratives and mostly between the narrative and argumentative genres. To fill the gap, the present study compares argumentative and expository compositions written by Chinese EFL learners over one academic year in an attempt to understand how genres affect syntactic complexities in EFL learners’ English writing and whether the genre effects are retained over 1 year.

Methodology

Research questions

1. What are the developmental changes in syntactic complexity over one academic year in Chinese EFL learners’ argumentative and expository compositions, respectively?

2. What are the effects exerted by genre on syntactic complexity in Chinese EFL learners’ argumentative and expository compositions, and are the effects of genre retained over one academic year?

Tool and measures

Lu (2010) designed the SCA, which can automatically analyze the complexity measures of L2 writing. The SCA can yield data on 14 measures concerning length of production unit, amount of subordination, amount of coordination, and degree of phrasal sophistication (see Table 1). These 14 measures were previously employed or recommended in the two large-scale research syntheses by Wolfe-Quintero et al. (1998) and Ortega (2003). In 2011, Lu reported the results of his own study with SCA, with regard to syntactic complexity, as fair and independent measuring indices of university students’ writing based on a corpus of data. Park (2012) also evaluated the same 14 syntactic complexity measures, in a cross-sectional study, as valid indices of developmental patterns in writing by college learners of English in Korea. With a view to comparing the present study with Lu (2011) and Park (2012), we also adopted the same 14 syntactic complexity measures for analysis, which are further classified into five categories, to gauge the degree of syntactic complexity in Chinese students’ L2 argumentative and expository compositions.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The five types of syntactic complexity measures examined in the present study.

Participants

The current study was positioned in a 1-year-long Comprehensive English course, including Comprehensive English III and Comprehensive English IV, which was given to second-year English majors in a renowned university in Jiangsu province, China. The participants came from two parallel classes of the same grade, with 28 and 31 students, respectively. Of the 59 participants, 52 were girls and seven were boys, aged between 19 and 21, all with about 10 years of formal English learning experience. Moreover, all the courses, including both compulsory and optional ones, for the two classes were given by the same teachers. Because of the absence of a teacher working as an exchange scholar in the United States, the participants were put in the same class for the Comprehensive English course over the whole academic year, thus overcoming other potential intervening causes or variables. Finally, it should be pointed out that because some participants failed to complete and hand in all of the eight assigned compositions, only 52 (out of 59) participants’ compositions were valid for analysis in the present study.

Data collection and analysis

Over the whole academic year, the participants were asked to write four argumentative and four expository compositions. The compositions were all finished in class, with a length of about 300 words and with no access to electronic devices. The writing topics were determined in relation to the contents of the textbook as well as the contemporary issues of the time. The data were collected at a regular time interval of 3–4 weeks, with the genres of argumentation and exposition alternated and the order of the topics counterbalanced to avoid a topic effect. To be specific, the topics for the first, third, fifth, and seventh compositions were argumentative, and those for the second, fourth, sixth, and eighth were expository (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The writing prompts for the argumentative and expository compositions.

For each writing task, the scoring of the compositions was done by two experienced raters in accordance with the assessment criteria proposed for Chinese “FLTRP Cup” English writing contest in relation to content, organization, and language (Tian, 2014). However, the feedback given to the participants was concerned only with lexical and grammatical errors and not related to the use of the language structures required by different genres.

In the process of data collection, we first coded each of the compositions by all the participants into specific syntactic complexity indices via the L2 SCA (as specified in Table 1). For research question 1, we conducted a one-way ANOVA with time as the within-subjects variable, aiming to examine the developmental changes in syntactic complexities in argumentative and expository compositions, respectively. For research question 2, we first conducted a paired-sample T-test with genre as the within-subjects variable to compare the syntactic complexities of the first argumentative compositions and the first expository ones by the 52 participants written at the beginning of the academic year, the purpose of which was to see whether differences existed between argumentative and expository compositions. After that, comparisons were done consecutively between the second argumentative and second expository, the third argumentative and third expository, and the fourth argumentative and fourth expository compositions to see if the effects of genre had been retained over one academic year. Moreover, we conducted a two-way repeated measures analysis of variance with both genre and time as independent variables and the 14 measuring indices of syntactic complexity as dependent variables, aiming to explore the interactional effects between time and genre on developmental changes in syntactic complexity in argumentative and expository compositions.

Results and discussion

Changes in syntactic complexity in argumentative and expository compositions by Chinese EFL learners over one academic year

To see if there were any changes in syntactic complexity in Chinese EFL learners’ argumentative and expository compositions, a one-way ANOVA was conducted with time as the within-subjects variable in the two genres, respectively, as can be seen in Tables 3, 4.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The effect of time on the syntactic complexity of argumentative compositions.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. The effect of time on the syntactic complexity of expository compositions.

Of the 14 measures examined, there was a linear growth in MLC in both argumentative and expository compositions over time, which concurs with the findings yielded in previous studies (Wolfe-Quintero et al., 1998; Xu et al., 2013). The significant and positive increase in MLC and MLT in argumentative compositions and MLC in expository compositions indicated students’ increased ability to produce longer expressions. Moreover, significant increase was also found in CP/C, CP/T, CN/C, and CN/T in both argumentative and expository compositions. Although the production of longer units does not necessarily equal complex and good language, it demonstrates the writer’s ability to form them in one sentence or clause by connecting different views and expressions, and the ability to master and use longer clauses and sentences does, to some extent, exhibit a higher level of language proficiency (Beers and Nagy, 2011). On one hand, we may attribute the longer mean length of production unit to the use of more clauses; on the other hand, the production of longer language units can be the result of phrasal elaboration. The increased values of CP/C, CP/T, CN/C, and CN/T in both argumentative and expository compositions suggest that complex phrases, such as attributive phrases, appositive phrases, and adjective phrase, as well as complex nominal expressions, can be used as a good way to increase the length of language production (Ansarifar et al., 2018).

It is interesting to see that the first argumentative and first expository compositions both had a relatively higher value of C/T, which means that more clauses were incorporated into the sentences in the students’ language production. However, the value of C/T experienced a drastic drop in both argumentative and expository writing over the course of the year, which concurs with Ortega (2015) view that as language learners advance to a higher level, they are more likely to produce language that is typically characterized by the use of phrasal expressions rather than progress at the clausal level. The development of coordination and particular structures in both argumentative and expository compositions, and meanwhile the decreased value of subordination in argumentative and minor increase in expository, indicated that the L2 writers moved toward the development of more phrasal components as compared to clausal components as they advanced in writing (Crossley and McNamara, 2014). This finding further corroborates Biber et al. (2011), who claimed that syntactic complexity could be better measured with the measuring indices of noun phrases rather than clauses embedded in sentences and that in writing academic essays, meaning was mostly expressed through the use of complex noun phrases rather than clause-level expressions. Moreover, the developmental trend concluded in the present study is in line with the developmental sequence proposed by Norris and Ortega (2009), where more advanced stages of development were defined by the extended use of nominalization. Likewise, Biber et al. (2013) proposed that the developmental stage of L2 learners is similar to the sequence theory and that there are specific developmental progressions in which grammatical form develops from finite dependent clauses to non-finite dependent clauses and to dependent phrases, and on the other hand, learners’ syntactic function progresses from the use of clausal constituents (e.g., direct object or adverbial) to the use of noun phrase modifiers (Biber and Conrad, 2009; Biber and Gray, 2010; Biber et al., 2013).

Effects of genre on syntactic complexity in Chinese EFL learners’ writing

To answer the second research question, the paired-sample t-test was first conducted with genre as the within-subjects variable between the first argumentative and the first expository compositions, and then comparisons were made, consecutively, between the second argumentative and expository compositions, the third argumentative and expository compositions, and the fourth argumentative and expository compositions on all of the 14 measures. Table 5 presents the statistically significant differences between syntactic complexities in argumentative and expository compositions at four time points over the year.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Mean differences (MD) in syntactic complexities and standard deviations by time point and genre.

Table 5 shows that the values of the argumentative compositions were significantly different from those of the expository ones written by the participants at Time 1. To be precise, on the length of the production units, MLC [t(−2.585), p = 0.013] and MLS [t(3.153), p < 0.05] were found to be of statistical significance; C/S [t(4.466), p = 0.000] was also of statistical significance concerning the effect of genre; on the level of subordination, C/T [t(3.248), p = 0.002], DC/C [t(3.641), p = 0.001], and DC/T [t(3.232), p = 0.002] were significantly and positively related to genre; on sentence coordination, T/S [t(2.558), p = 0.014] was the only measure that was of significance on the level of coordination; for the measuring indices in particular structures, a positive statistical significance was detected on VP/T [t(2.191), p = 0.033]. Inspections of the two group means indicate that learners produced overall more complex language in argumentative compositions than expository ones at Time 1 except for the negative relation on the measures of MLC [t(−2.585), p = 0.013] and CN/C [t(−3.906), p = 0.000]. Similar to the results yielded between the first argumentative and first expository compositions, at Time 2, significant effects were found on the length of the production units MLS [t(2.979), p = 0.004] and MLT [t(3.850), p = 0.000]; all the measures of subordination, namely C/T [t(2.6945), p = 0.010], CT/T [t(2.916), p = 0.005], DC/C [t(3.302), p = 0.002], and DC/T [t(3.009), p = 0.002], were found to be positively and significantly related to genre; CP/T [t(2.888), p = 0.006] was the only measure that was detected to be of significance on coordination measures; for particular structures measures, the differences concerning CN/T [t(2.950), p = 0.004] and VP/T [t(4.684), p = 0.000] were of statistical significance. Simply put, at Time 1 and Time 2, the genre effects and statistical significance were found mostly on the length of production unit, sentence complexity, and subordination.

However, at Time 3, the statistically significant differences were found more on coordination and particular structures. The results obtained at Time 4 were similar to those at Time 3. Put another way, for Time 3 and Time 4, the measures of coordination and particular structures were found to be significantly and positively related to genre. To be precise, at Time 3, there were significant genre effects on the measures of CP/C [t(3.061), p = 0.003], CP/T [t(2.747), p = 0.008], CN/T [t(2.040), p = 0.046] and CN/C [t(3.598), p = 0.001]. At Time 4, the genre effects were found on the measures of CP/C [t(−4.502), p = 0.000], CP/T [t(−4.249), p = 0.000], CN/C [t(3.916), p = 0.001], and VP/T [t(1.542), p = 0.002].

Through comparing the syntactic complexities in argumentative and expository compositions at four time points, we can conclude that the syntactic complexities of both argumentative and expository compositions in the earlier stage (the first semester in our research) were exhibited more on the measures of subordination and length of production unit, which indicated clausal progression, whereas the later period (the second semester in our research) saw a development of syntactic complexity on the measures of coordination and particular structures, both of which showcased the development of phrase-level complexity.

Moreover, the two-way repeated measures ANOVA was also conducted with genre and time as independent variables and the 14 measures of syntactic complexity as dependent variables (shown in Table 6). Along with the exact p-values, this study also reported the effect sizes and partial eta squared (ηp2), an appropriate measure for research designs involving within-subjects factors. Regarding the role that time played on the developmental pattern of syntactic complexity, the findings reveal that there was a significant main effect for time mainly on the measures of the length of production unit, namely MLC (p = 0.000, ηp2 = 0.055), MLT (p = 0.008, ηp2 = 0.029), and MLS (p = 0.043, ηp2 = 0.020), indicating that L2 writers produced longer expression in argumentative compositions than in expository compositions over the course of the year. On the coordination level, the significance was on all the three measures, namely CP/C (p = 0.000, ηp2 = 0.042), CP/T (p = 0.000, ηp2 = 0.048), and TS (p=0,003, ηp2 = 0.031). And similarly, all the three measures on the particular structures were found to be significantly related to time: CN/C (p = 0.000, ηp2 = 0.100), CN/T (p = 0.000, ηp2 = 0.053), and VP/T (p = 0.021, ηp2 = 0.023), showing that L2 learners tended to use more phrases in their compositions to express meaning and connect ideas. Different from the role that time played on syntactic complexity, the effect of genre was mainly found to be on the measures of the length of production unit (MLS: p = 0.050, ηp2 = 0.009; MLT: p = 0.055, ηp2 = 0.009), sentence complexity (C/S: p = 0.031, ηp2 = 0.011) and subordination (C/T: p = 0.032, ηp2 = 0.011, DC/C: p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.020, and DC/T: p = 0.009, ηp2 = 0.017), and VP/T (p = 0.000, ηp2 = 0.034) of particular structures. There were interactive effects of time and genre, and statistically significant difference was found on MLC (p = 0.031, ηp2 = 0.022), C/S (p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.031); all the measures of subordination, namely C/T (p = 0.027, ηp2 = 0.022), CT/T (p = 0.034, ηp2 = 0.021), DC/C (p = 0.026, ηp2 = 0.022), and DC/T (p = 0.035, ηp2 = 0.021)); CP/C (p = 0.000, ηp2 = 0.054) and CP/T (p = 0.000, ηp2 = 0.047) in coordination; and CN/C (p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.034) of particular structures.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Genre effects on syntactic complexity measures (two-way repeated measures ANOVA).

While comparing the findings from the present study with those obtained in previous research, we found that the adoption of different measuring indices may lead to different results in different types of genres. Nonetheless, most previous studies focused only on a few measures in contrasting narrative essays and non-narrative ones, and therefore it is difficult and unpersuasive in regard to determining if the indices taken are representative enough to capture the differences or whether there are other measures that are equally persuasive in demonstrating syntactic complexity.

The findings yielded in the present study are consistent with those obtained in Beers and Nagy (2009), who examined three measures, namely C/T, W/C, and W/T, and ascertained genre differences when using W/T and C/T as measures of syntactic complexity. Moreover, our results were comparable to those of Lu (2011), who found higher complexity measures in argumentative essays than in narratives under the condition of controlling for both time and institution on the measure of length of production unit (MLS).

On the whole, the majority of measures of statistically significant difference concerning genre effects were detected on the clausal level though the effects of time were exhibited more on the length of production unit, on coordination, and on particular structures, the last two of which being measures featuring the progression of phrase-level expressions. We may safely conclude that participants’ language production was affected by the type of writing tasks assigned to them and that between the two non-narrative types of writing, argumentative compositions exhibited higher syntactic complexity than expository compositions, which is in line with previous findings (Bulté and Housen, 2014; Kyle and Crossley, 2018). Put another way, argumentative compositions placed more reasoning demands on participants than expository ones, thus resulting in higher syntactic complexity.

Looking at the data in detail, we found that in the first semester (Time 1 and Time 2), the measuring indices were shown to discriminate more on subordination and the length of production unit, both of which were on the clausal level, despite the few measures found to be significantly related in coordination and particular structures. At Time 3 and Time 4, the effects of genre were found to be more on coordination and particular structures. This finding was consistent with the results reported in Lu (2011) and Xu et al. (2013) but partly inconsistent with the results that Ortega (2003) drew. Ortega ascribed the relatively small magnitude of C/T to advanced learners being more likely to simplify the clauses to shorter phrases and supported the claim that C/T may be lower at advanced levels as a result of reduction from clauses to phrases (2003). Lu (2011) found that for clauses per T-unit, significant differences were detected within persuasive compositions, in which there were more clausal-level expressions concerning subordination; whereas in our study, the significance was found in expository compositions rather than in argumentative ones. And for W/C (the corresponding index in our study is MLC), there were more W/C in descriptive compositions than in persuasive ones, but a statistically significant difference was not detected in the other two genres of writing. This was partly inconsistent with our study, in which the argumentative essays demonstrated longer lengths of clauses, with participants producing longer clauses in argumentative than in expository compositions.

To probe deeper into the causes underlying the difference, we excerpted some of the argumentative and expository compositions in the following:

Argumentative writing:

Student A: The booming catering industry has been capturing the hearts of more and more people especially the young, who like eating at stands or restaurants. Anyway, many others make a point to prepare and eat at home. Those who have good reasons to eat out argue that cooking at home wastes time as well as energy. With exhausting or maybe dull routine work, they do not bother to cook anymore. In addition, it seems that cuisine is more palatable and multiple at diners or restaurants, where they can just wait and soon enjoy the meals.

Student B: However, it’s not the only access to earn more and get promoted. To new graduates, finding a job earlier means more working experience and larger circle of professional friends, from whom they could seek supports when tackling challenges. They also have an edge over master degree (or up) holders in good energy. People in their mid-twenties would be more concentrated in working for they have little affairs to cope with in terms of family and seldom do they have to take a sick leave, compared with some middle-aged people.

Expository writing:

Student A: I’m preparing for long relaxing, a solo travel which will last for one year. I want to feel the romance of European towns, the spectacle of African savannah and the vast of the ocean. However, the luggage is an important matter which will affect my aesthetic experience. It cannot be too much or lacking. Therefore, besides all kinds of clothes and the cellphone used to pay, contact family and friends and take photos, I am to pick out following necessary articles.

Student B: Now as a college student, what I have learned from little experience in school is that the life here prepares for the life later. I entered XXX university last year and tried all kinds of activities I had never tried before, such as working as a volunteer in various corners of the city, participating in school organizations and staying up all night in the study room. I learned how to utilize my kindness to benefit people in need and effectively cooperate with partners. I realized that nobody can succeed with no venture. Thus I would push myself harder.

In analyzing the excerpts of participants’ compositions, we can see that, compared with expository compositions, there are more clauses in participants’ argumentative compositions, such as attributive clauses (…the young, who like eating at stands…), adverbial clauses (…they could seek supports when tackling challenges), and predicative clauses (it seems that cuisine is..). The use of more clauses in argumentative compositions, for one thing, discriminates the value of MLC with that of expository ones, and for another, it partly explains the high magnitude of C/T observed in our study. As with more clauses incorporated into a sentence, it is not surprising that the value of C/T gets higher accordingly. As opposed to more clauses found in argumentative compositions, in expository writing, there were more phrasal-level expressions, such as the romance of European towns, the spectacle of African savannah, the vast of the ocean, working as a volunteer in various corners of the city, participating in school organizations, and staying up all night in the study room.

In previous studies, researchers found that expository texts composed by student writers have more complex noun phrases (Ravid and Berman, 2010), more nominalized forms (Schleppegrell, 2001), and more relative and adverbial clauses (Scott and Windsor, 2000) as opposed to narratives, which suggests that the narrative and expository genres require different cognitive demands. The inherently different nature of narrative and non-narrative compositions, and even different types of writing within non-narrative ones, indicates that certain types of texts are believed to be linguistically and cognitively more demanding than others (Schleppegrell, 2001), and the frequency of exposure to different communicative contexts differs remarkably across individuals, leading to overall different structures and different levels of syntactic complexity. In the same vein, learners’ language production of argumentative and expository writing in syntax is characterized by distinctive forms and expressions. As Beers and Nagy (2009) stated, “writers need to be aware of stylistic options that will produce the most desirable impression on their readers most. Among these stylistic options are a variety of sentence-level syntactic structures through which meaning can be conveyed” (p. 196).

Moreover, in comparing the changes in syntactic complexity of both argumentative and expository writing, we found a positive progression in both genres of writing over the course of the year in most of the measures. But the developmental pattern and mean value of the syntactic complexity measures of argumentative and expository compositions are different. As for the mean length of production unit, all the three measures showed a generally upward trend with minor fluctuation. And the mean value of the MLC of argumentative compositions was consistently higher than that of expository ones despite the progression of both genres, and so was the syntactic complexity of MLS and MLT. As for other clause-level measures, like subordination, we see a generally decreased value of syntactic complexity in both genres of writing (although no significance was detected), which confirms previous findings that as learners advance to higher levels, they learn to use complex phrases rather than progress in clausal expressions (Ortega, 2003). Unlike the previous measuring indices, the four measuring indices on coordination behave differently in that students achieved more notable progress on CP/T and CP/C in expository writing than in argumentative writing and that the magnitude of C/T experienced a drastic decrease over the academic year. The relatively higher magnitude of C/T at the beginning was probably because when the students were trying to express the relationship among ideas, they were more likely to use more clauses to make their expressions smooth and coherent, such that more clauses were contained in each T-unit (Ortega, 2003). The progression pattern corroborated with other measures on the last set of indices, the particular structures. We found that all the three measures’ value increased value and that two of the measures (CN/C and VP/T) distinguished argumentative writing from expository writing positively and significantly.

To conclude, of the 14 measures we used to quantify syntactic complexity, it is clear that argumentative compositions and expository compositions are distinctively and significantly different from each other and that learners generally achieved more complex language production in both genres of writing over one academic year. Looking at the interactive effects of both time and genre, in spite of the progression of students’ writing in both genres, we found that the syntactic complexity of argumentative compositions was still higher than that of expository ones for the majority of measures and for most of the time points. In Biber and Conrad (2009) explanation, this was due to the fact that different types of writing entail distinct functional requirements, which could result in learners’ different language production. Moreover, argumentative writing involves more complex functional demand, and therefore the resources needed for completing it can be more than those required for expository writing. By looking at the developmental trend of the 14 measures over 1 year, we found that the linguistic progression or development proceeded in a similar, if not the same, way across the two genres, which is in line with our hypothesis concerning the syntactic complexity difference. In light of Robinson (2007) framework, we can attribute the difference between the two types of writing to the cognitive demand, which states that increased cognitive complexity (in our study, argumentative writing) imposes great effects on language production. While performing argumentative writing, which is a more complex task with more demands of logical and causal reasoning, participants tended to draw on their attention and memory resources to meet the requirements of more demanding cognitive resources and therefore produced language of relatively higher complexity. More importantly, time and instruction imposed essentially little influence on the linguistic progression between argumentative and expository writing. Therefore, we may safely attribute the different levels and progressions of syntactic complexity in argumentative and expository compositions to the inherently different natures and demands of the two genres of writing, which verifies our hypothesis that the distinctive cognitive resources required by the two types of writing genres play a crucial role in students’ writing syntactic complexity.

There is one more point worthy of special note in our study. In reading through all the argumentative and expository compositions, we found that when participants were required to produce expository compositions, some of them wrote by explaining their reasons and relating those reasons to instances for discussion. And they imposed a logical structure to interrelate ideas in a coherent manner and to organize claims and arguments in a stepwise hierarchical format, which is a typical way of organizing argumentative writing. We are not sure whether it was the organization of their expository compositions in an argumentative way that led to the relatively high value of C/T in the expository compositions, but we do know that some students were not quite aware of the requirements of different structures pertinent to different genres. Therefore, teaching EFL should not be understood as promoting a global English proficiency expected to be equally functional across different genres, but rather specific and genre-targeted instruction should be taken into consideration in the actual learning and teaching.

Conclusion

The findings of the present study are twofold. On one hand, we found that there was a statistically significant development of syntactic complexity in both argumentative and expository compositions and that the development was found more in phrasal-level measures (coordination and particular structures) than in clausal ones. This finding concurs with Biber and Gray (2010) that complex noun phrases can be much more appropriate measures of syntactic complexity compared to embedded clauses. It also echoes Biber et al. (2013), who highlighted the criticality of considering the distinctive grammatical features of academic register, i.e., phrasal modifiers when conducting syntactic complexity analysis in L2 writing research. On the other hand, genre was found to have affected the development of syntactic complexity in argumentative and expository compositions. To be precise, the syntactic complexity of argumentative essays was significantly higher than that of expository essays in some of the measuring indices, and this finding corroborates the findings of previous studies conducted between narrative and non-narrative genres (Beers and Nagy, 2011; Yoon and Polio, 2017), indicating that even within non-narrative types of writing, significant differences still exist. Moreover, the effects of genre were retained over one academic year, thus confirming the importance of controlling for the effects of relevant learner-, task-, and context-related factors in interpreting between-proficiency differences in syntactic complexity (Lu, 2011), and therefore conflating non-narratives (argumentative and expository in the present study) may overlook these differences.

The implications of the study are also twofold. First, the present study may help EFL learners and writing instructors gain a more in-depth understanding of how and in what forms the features of syntactic complexity are demonstrated. The lack of clause-level statistically significant development in both argumentative and expository essays may indicate that academic writing is characteristically dense with non-clausal phrases and complex noun phrases and that therefore phrases may be taken as more appropriate measures of syntactic complexity as opposed to embedded clauses. In academic prose, meaning is, for the most part, condensed into complex noun phrases rather than being expressed in clause forms, and therefore noun phrase modification, such as attributive adjectives and post-noun-modifying prepositional phrases, has a tendency to contribute to essay quality, which requires researchers and teachers to re-examine the traditional focus on clausal embedding and subordination measures based on the assumption that academic writing derives its complexity from the elaborate use of clausal construction and, for another, provides possible ways for them to place emphasis on the learning and application of learners’ phrasal-level knowledge. Second, learning a language can be taken as learning a range of distinct types of discourses, which are context-specific (Qin and Uccelli, 2016), and the knowledge and ability acquired in one particular genre practice does not necessarily mean that students can transfer their successful performance from one genre to another. Therefore, English teaching cannot be taken just as a way to promote students’ overall language level and performance, which is generally believed to serve the same function in different contexts, and students should also be able to draw inferences from one another. Both teachers and researchers should reflect on the different needs students will encounter in authentic communicative contexts in order to make informed decisions concerning which discourse practice to select for instruction. In the meantime, when assessing students’ language performance, especially in different genres of writing tasks, the effects of genre on students’ language production should also be taken into consideration since different genres of writing may entail specific syntactic expressions, and the progression of each genre can be distinct.

As with most studies, ours was not without its limitations. In the first place, the participants in this study were all from the same class and the same grade, leading to relatively high homogeneity, and so whether the findings of this research are generalizable needs to be further verified. Future research could involve subjects from different language proficiency levels, such as participants from different grades, institutions, and even different L1 backgrounds to investigate the syntactic development in students’ writing with language level and genre as interactive factors. Also, comparing results from EFL learners with those of native speakers may generate more robust findings concerning the effects of genre on syntactic complexity. Moreover, although one academic year is not considered short in regard to collecting data, it is more desirable to conduct research over a longer time span. Additionally, we adopted the 14 measures that Lu (2011) used for SCA, and it is undeniable that syntactic complexity includes various sorts of linguistic features, such as discourse features, passive sentences, and other linguistic resources used to distinguish one’s own position from that of others. Therefore, it would also be useful to employ more comprehensive and fine-grained measures for comparison. However, it is worth noting that in quantifying syntactic complexity, it is not the case that the more, the better, and therefore choosing indicators that represent a genuine and comprehensive measure of syntactic complexity matters most.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by School of Foreign Languages, Soochow University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

LP: conceptualization, research design, data analysis, and manuscript revision. RH: research design, data analysis, drafting, and manuscript revision. CC: data collection and data analysis. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abdel-Malek, M. (2019). Writing recounts of habitual events: investigating a genre-based approach. Foreign Lang. Ann. 52, 373–387. doi: 10.1111/flan.12383

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ana, C. L. M. (2018). Analysis of syntactic complexity in secondary education EFL writers at different proficiency levels. Assess. Writ. 35, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.asw.2017.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ansarifar, A., Shahriari, H., and Pishghadam, R. (2018). Phrasal complexity in academic writing: a comparison of abstracts written by graduate students and expert writers in applied linguistics. J. Engl. Acad. Purp. 31, 58–71. doi: 10.1016/j.jeap.2017.12.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atak, N., and Saricaoglu, A. (2021). Syntactic complexity in L2 learners’ argumentative writing: developmental stages and the within-genre topic effect. Assess. Writ. 47:100506. doi: 10.1016/j.asw.2020.100506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bae, J., and Min, S. (2020). Genre-based analysis of syntactic complexity in L2 college Students’ writing: pedagogic scope and directions. J. Asia Tefl. 17, 937–953. doi: 10.18823/asiatefl.2020.17.3.12.937

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bain, A. (1967). English Composition and Rhetoric (2nd Edn.). New York: Appleton & Company.

Google Scholar

Beauvais, C., Olive, T., and Passerault, J. M. (2011). Why are some texts good and others not? Relationship between text quality and management of the writing processes. J. Educ. Psychol. 103, 415–428. doi: 10.1037/a0022545

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beers, S. F., and Nagy, W. E. (2009). Syntactic complexity as a predictor of adolescent writing quality: which measures? Which genre? Read. Writ. 22, 185–200. doi: 10.1007/s11145-007-9107-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beers, S. F., and Nagy, W. E. (2011). Writing development in four genres from grades three to seven: syntactic complexity and genre differentiation. Read. Writ. 24, 183–202. doi: 10.1007/s11145-010-9264-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berman, R. A., and Nir-Sagiv, B. (2004). Linguistic indicators of inter-genre differentiation in late language development. J. Child Lang. 31, 339–380. doi: 10.1017/S0305000904006038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berman, R. A., and Slobin, D. I. (2013). Relating Events in Narrative: A Cross-Linguistic Developmental Study. New York: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Bi, P. (2020). Revisiting genre effects on linguistic features of L2 writing: a usage-based perspective. Int. J. Appl. Linguist. 30, 429–444. doi: 10.1111/ijal.12297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bi, P., and Jiang, J. (2020). Syntactic complexity in assessing young adolescent EFL learners’ writings: syntactic elaboration and diversity. System 91:102248. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2020.102248

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biber, D., and Conrad, S. (2009). Register, Genre, and Style. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Biber, D., and Gray, B. (2010). Challenging stereotypes about academic writing: complexity, elaboration, explicitness. J. Engl. Acad. Purp. 9, 2–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jeap.2010.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biber, D., Gray, B., and Poonpon, K. (2011). Should we use characteristics of conversation to measure grammatical complexity in L2 writing development. TESOL Q. 45, 5–35. doi: 10.5054/tq.2011.244483

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biber, D., Gray, B., and Poonpon, K. (2013). Pay attention to the phrasal structures: going beyond T-units—a response to WeiWei Yang. TESOL Q. 47, 192–201. doi: 10.1002/tesq.84

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biber, D., Gray, B., and Staples, S. (2016). Predicting patterns of grammatical complexity across language exam task types and proficiency levels. Appl. Linguis. 37, 639–668. doi: 10.1093/applin/amu059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunner, J. S. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Bulté, B., and Housen, A. (2012). “Defining and operationalising L2 complexity” in Dimensions of L2 Performance and Proficiency: Complexity, Accuracy and Fluency in SLA. eds. A. Housen, F. Kuiken, and I. Vedder (Amsterdam: John Benjamins), 21–46. doi: 10.1075/lllt.32.02bul

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bulté, B., and Housen, A. (2014). Conceptualizing and measuring short-term changes in L2 writing complexity. J. Second. Lang. Writ. 26, 42–65. doi: 10.1016/j.jslw.2014.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casal, J. E., and Lee, J. J. (2019). Syntactic complexity and writing quality in assessed first-year L2 writing. J. Second. Lang. Writ. 44, 51–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jslw.2019.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casal, J. E., Lu, X., Qiu, X., Wang, Y., and Zhang, G. (2021). Syntactic complexity across academic research article part-genres: a cross-disciplinary perspective. J. Engl. Acad. Purp. 52:100996. doi: 10.1016/j.jeap.2021.100996

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crossley, S. A., and McNamara, D. S. (2014). Does writing development equal writing quality? A computational investigation of syntactic complexity in L2 learners. J. Second. Lang. Writ. 26, 66–79. doi: 10.1016/j.jslw.2014.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowhurst, M., and Piche, G. L. (1979). Audience and mode of discourse effects on syntactic complexity in writing at two grade levels. Res. Teach. Engl. 13, 101–109. Retrieved from: http://www.jstor.org/stable/40170746

Google Scholar

De Clercq, B., and Housen, A. (2017). A cross-linguistic perspective on syntactic complexity in L2 development: syntactic elaboration and diversity. Mod. Lang. J. 101, 315–334. doi: 10.1111/modl.12396

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaies, S. J. (1980). T-unit analysis in second language research: applications problems and limitations. TESOL Q. 14, 53–60. doi: 10.2307/3586808

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Genung, J. F. (1900). The Working Principles of Rhetoric: Examined in Their Literary Relations and Illustrated With Examples. Boston: Ginn & Company.

Google Scholar

Grabe, W. (2002). “Narrative and expository macro-genres,” in Genre in the Classroom: Multiple Perspectives. ed. A. M. Johns (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 249–267.

Google Scholar

Guo, L., Crossley, S. A., and McNamara, D. S. (2013). Predicting human judgments of essay quality in both integrated and independent second language writing samples: a comparison study. Assess. Writ. 18, 218–238. doi: 10.1016/j.asw.2013.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Housen, A., and Kuiken, F. (2009). Complexity, accuracy, and fluency in second language acquisition. Appl. Linguis. 30, 461–473. doi: 10.1093/applin/amp048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y., Wu, J., Li, M., and Yang, Y. (2022). The trajectory of syntactic complexity development in L1 Chinese narrative writings of primary school children: a systematic 5-year longitudinal study. Assess. Writ. 52:100622. doi: 10.1016/j.asw.2022.100622

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyland, K. (2007). Genre pedagogy: language, literacy and L2 writing instruction. J. Second. Lang. Writ. 16, 148–164. doi: 10.1016/j.jslw.2007.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jagaiah, T., Olinghouse, N. G., and Kearns, D. M. (2020). Syntactic complexity measures: variation by genre, grade-level, students’ writing abilities, and writing quality. Read. Writ. 33, 2577–2638. doi: 10.1007/s11145-020-10057-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeong, H. (2017). Narrative and expository genre effects on students, raters, and performance criteria. Assess. Writ. 31, 113–125. doi: 10.1016/j.asw.2016.08.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, J., Bi, P., and Liu, H. (2019). Syntactic complexity development in the writings of EFL learners: insights from a dependency syntactically-annotated corpus. J. Second. Lang. Writ. 46:100666. doi: 10.1016/j.jslw.2019.100666

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamberelis, G., and Bovino, T. D. (1999). Cultural artifacts as scaffolds for genre development. Read. Res. Q. 34, 138–170. doi: 10.1598/RRQ.34.2.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, M. (2021). Exploring longitudinal changes in lexical and syntactic features in beginning-level EFL learner writing. System 103:102680. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2021.102680

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kyle, K., and Crossley, S. A. (2018). Measuring syntactic complexity in L2 writing using fine-grained clausal and phrasal indices. Mod. Lang. J. 102, 333–349. doi: 10.1111/modl.12468

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Nikitina, L., and Riget, P. N. (2022). Development of syntactic complexity in Chinese university students’ L2 argumentative writing. J. Engl. Acad. Purp. 56:101099. doi: 10.1016/j.jeap.2022.101099

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, X. (2010). Automatic analysis of syntactic complexity in second language writing. Int. J. Corp. Linguis. 15, 474–496. doi: 10.1075/ijcl.15.4.02lu

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, X. (2011). A corpus-based evaluation of syntactic complexity measures as indices of college-level ESL writers' language development. TESOL Q. 45, 36–62. doi: 10.5054/tq.2011.240859

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, X., Casal, J. E., Liu, Y., Kisselev, O., and Yoon, J. (2021). The relationship between syntactic complexity and rhetorical move-steps in research article introductions: variation among four social science and engineering disciplines. J. Engl. Acad. Purp. 52:101006. doi: 10.1016/j.jeap.2021.101006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nippold, M. A. (2004). “Research on later language development: international perspectives” in Language Development Across Childhood and Adolescence. ed. R. A. Berman (Amsterdam: Benjamins), 1–8. doi: 10.1075/tilar.3.04nip

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norris, J. M., and Ortega, L. (2009). Towards an organic approach to investigating CAF in instructed SLA: the case of complexity. Appl. Linguis. 30, 555–578. doi: 10.1093/applin/amp044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortega, L. (2003). Syntactic complexity measures and their relationship to L2 proficiency: a research synthesis of college-level L2 writing. Appl. Linguis. 24, 492–518. doi: 10.1093/applin/24.4.492

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortega, L. (2015). Syntactic complexity in L2 writing: Progress and expansion. J. Second. Lang. Writ. 29, 82–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jslw.2015.06.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, S. Y. (2012). A corpus-based study of syntactic complexity measures as development indices of college-level L2 learners’ proficiency in writing. Korean J. Appl. Linguis. 28, 139–160. Retrieved from http://uci.or.kr/G704-000491.2012.28.3.007. doi: 10.17154/kjal.2012.09.28.3.139

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polio, C., and Yoon, H. J. (2018). The reliability and validity of automated tools for examining variation in syntactic complexity across genres. Int. J. Appl. Linguist. 28, 165–188. doi: 10.1111/ijal.12200

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qin, W., and Uccelli, P. (2016). Same language, different functions: a cross-genre analysis of Chinese EFL learners’ writing performance. J. Second. Lang. Writ. 33, 3–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jslw.2016.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ravid, D. (2005). “Emergence of linguistic complexity in later language development: evidence from expository text construction” in Perspectives on Language and Language Development. eds. D. Ravid and H. Bat-Zeev Shyldkrot (Boston, MA: Springer), 337–355. doi: 10.1007/1-4020-7911-7_25

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ravid, D., and Berman, R. A. (2010). Developing noun phrase complexity at school age: a text-embedded cross-linguistic analysis. First Lang. 30, 3–26. doi: 10.1177/0142723709350531

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, P. (2001). Task complexity, task difficulty, and task production: exploring interactions in a componential framework. Appl. Linguis. 22, 27–57. doi: 10.1093/applin/22.1.27

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, P. (2007). “Criteria for classifying and sequencing pedagogic tasks,” in Investigating Tasks in Formal Language Learning. ed. M. P. García Mayo (Bristol, Blue Ridge Summit: Multilingual Matters), 7–26. doi: 10.21832/9781853599286-004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruiz-Funes, M. (2015). Exploring the potential of second/foreign language writing for language learning: the effects of task factors and learner variables. J. Second. Lang. Writ. 28, 1–19. doi: 10.1016/j.jslw.2015.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schleppegrell, M. J. (2001). Linguistic features of the language of schooling. Linguist. Educ. 12, 431–459. doi: 10.1016/S0898-5898(01)00073-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, C. M., and Windsor, J. (2000). General language performance measures in spoken and written narrative and expository discourse of school-age children with language learning disabilities. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 43, 324–339. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4302.324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skehan, P. (1996). A framework for the implementation of task-based instruction. Appl. Linguis. 17, 38–62. doi: 10.1093/applin/17.1.38

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skehan, P. (1998). A Cognitive Approach to Language Learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Staples, S., Egbert, J., Biber, D., and Gray, B. (2016). Academic writing development at the university level: phrasal and clausal complexity across level of study, discipline, and genre. Writ. Commun. 33, 149–183. doi: 10.1177/0741088316631527

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tian, Z. X. (2014). A Guide to “FLTRP Cup” English WRITING contest. Beijing: Foreign Language Teaching and Researching Press.

Google Scholar

Weigle, S. C. (2002). Assessing Writing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Wolfe-Quintero, K., Inagaki, S., and Kim, H. Y. (1998). Second Language Development in Writing: Measures of Fluency, Accuracy, & Complexity (No. 17). Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.

Google Scholar

Xu, W. (2020). Reframing genre-based pedagogy in a Chinese as a foreign language classroom: a transdisciplinary perspective. J. Educ. Res. 113, 452–461. doi: 10.1080/00220671.2020.1855095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, X. Y., Wang, W. M., Xiong, Y. Y., Jiang, J., Pan, X. Y., and Sun, N. H. (2013). A study of syntactic complexity in argumentative writing by Chinese English majors. For. Lang. Teach. Res. 45, 264–275.

Google Scholar

Yan, M., and Zou, F. (2019). Different genres and proficiency levels: relationships among syntactic complexity, genres and students with different L2 proficiency levels. English Lang. Lit. Stud. 9:85. doi: 10.5539/ells.v9n4p85

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, W. (2014). Mapping the relationships among the cognitive complexity of independent writing tasks, L2 writing quality, and complexity, accuracy and fluency of L2 writing. Doc. Dis. Georgia State Univ. 80–86. doi: 10.57709/5687326

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoon, H. J. (2017). Linguistic complexity in L2 writing revisited: issues of topic, proficiency, and construct multidimensionality. System 66, 130–141. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2017.03.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoon, H. J., and Polio, C. (2016). The linguistic development of students of English as a second language in two written genres. TESOL Q. 51, 275–301. doi: 10.1002/tesq.296

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoon, H. J., and Polio, C. (2017). The linguistic development of students of English as a second language in two written genres. TESOL Q. 51, 275–301. doi: 10.1002/tesq.296

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, S. (2021). Giving genre-based peer feedback in academic writing: sources of knowledge and skills, difficulties and challenges. Assess. Eval. High. Educ. 46, 36–53. doi: 10.1080/02602938.2020.1742872

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: genre, syntactic complexity, argumentative compositions, expository compositions, Chinese EFL learners

Citation: Pu L, Heng R and Cao C (2022) The effects of genre on the syntactic complexity of argumentative and expository writing by Chinese EFL learners. Front. Psychol. 13:1047117. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1047117

Received: 17 September 2022; Accepted: 17 October 2022;
Published: 10 November 2022.

Edited by:

Majid Elahi Shirvan, University of Bojnord, Iran

Reviewed by:

Danyang Zhang, Shenzhen University, China
Huiping Zhang, Northeast Normal University, China
Junjie Gavin Wu, Shenzhen Technology University, China

Copyright © 2022 Pu, Heng and Cao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Renquan Heng, cmVucXVhbmhlbmdAMTYzLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.