Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 24 November 2022
Sec. Positive Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic An Integrated Positive Psychology Approach Into Counseling in Different Settings View all 12 articles

Intuitive eating in Greek-Cypriot adults: Influence of gender and body mass

  • Department of Psychology, Neapolis University Pafos, Pafos, Cyprus

Introduction: To date, research on eating behaviors has largely taken a pathological approach. Researchers are increasingly taking a positive approach to explore adaptive eating styles. One such style that has recently received much research attention is intuitive eating. Recent work examining intuitive eating and its relationships with body mass and gender has yielded mixed findings. The current study explored the differential effects of gender and body mass on intuitive eating scores in a sample of Greek-Cypriot adults.

Method: A total of 1,312 adult participants (women n = 655; Mage = 34.49) completed the Intuitive Eating Scale-2 and provided demographic information.

Results: Our analyses revealed that men reported significantly higher intuitive eating scores than women. Body mass was significantly inversely associated with intuitive eating in both men and women. Additionally, a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) indicated significant interaction effects between gender and body mass on intuitive eating. These interaction effects were explored for each of the four subscales of intuitive eating, identifying differential associations for intuitive eating and BMI between men and women.

Discussion: Both gender and body mass are important factors which influence intuitive eating levels in Greek-Cypriot adults. Discussions of how these findings can inform future research, theory and practice are presented.

Introduction

Until recently, eating behavior research has largely taken a pathology-driven approach, focusing on the description, explanation and prediction of restrictive and disordered eating behavior. While such research has proven invaluable in guiding the diagnosis, prevention and treatment of disordered eating, this approach is limited as it focuses solely on the reduction of eating-related distress and does not take into consideration health-enhancing eating behaviors. More recently, scholars are increasingly taking a positive psychological approach, focusing on adaptive eating behaviors (i.e., those that are guided by the body’s internal physiological cues rather than external situational and socioemotional cues; Resch and Tylka, 2019). This work has elucidated the importance of adaptive eating behaviors not only in protecting against the development of disordered eating, but also in enhancing health and wellbeing (Tylka and Wilcox, 2006; Van Dyke and Drinkwater, 2014; Tylka et al., 2015). An adaptive eating style that has recently gained much research attention has been operationalized as intuitive eating (Tribole and Resch, 1995, 2012). Intuitive eating reflects a set of flexible eating behaviors that are guided by internal physiological hunger and satiety signals. Those who eat intuitively trust and depend on their internal cues to drive their eating behavior, avoid labeling foods as prohibited, eat for physical reasons rather than to cope with their emotions, and make food choices that support or enhance their body’s functioning and wellbeing (Tribole and Resch, 2012; Tylka and Kroon Van Diest, 2013; Resch and Tylka, 2019).

Intuitive eating has consistently been found to be closely related to psychosocial factors including facets of negative and positive body image. More specifically, researchers have reported associations for intuitive eating with lower levels of psychological distress, body image dissatisfaction, restrictive/disordered eating, and higher levels of quality of life, body appreciation, self-esteem, self-compassion and wellbeing (e.g., Augustus-Horvath and Tylka, 2011; Tylka et al., 2015; Webb and Hardin, 2016; Homan and Tylka, 2018; Linardon et al., 2020; Anastasiades and Argyrides, 2022; Gödde et al., 2022; Jackson et al., 2022; Teas et al., 2022). These findings have been replicated across a variety of ages, body mass categories, genders and cultural and ethnic groups (for a review, see Bruce and Ricciardelli, 2016; Linardon et al., 2021). Crucially, recent studies using experimental and prospective designs are yielding promising results with intuitive eating being found to lower symptoms of disordered eating and enhance body image, self-esteem, self-compassion, quality of life and psychological wellbeing (Bush et al., 2014; Humphrey et al., 2015; Carbonneau et al., 2017; Wilson et al., 2020; Babbott et al., 2022).

Accumulating empirical work provides strong support for intuitive eating being closely linked to body image. This highlights the importance of exploring the links between intuitive eating and other factors that have been found to play an important role in body image. Two such factors are gender and body mass. A higher body mass is often perceived as unhealthy and undesirable (Pearl et al., 2020). As such, individuals with a higher body mass have reported increased levels of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating (e.g., binge eating and emotional eating), and lower levels of body appreciation (Vartanian and Porter, 2016; Pearl et al., 2020). With regard to gender, there is much evidence suggesting that women face increased societal pressures to conform to unrealistic beauty ideals, with women generally reporting higher levels of body dissatisfaction and lower levels of body appreciation. Even though men also face increasing pressure to conform to ideals (i.e., an athletic, muscular physique; Frederick et al., 2022c), evidence suggests that the pressure and scrutiny women face is both more frequent and more severe (Grogan, 2016; Frederick et al., 2022a). Research exploring the links between intuitive eating and gender and body mass has yielded mixed findings (Linardon et al., 2021). However, a recent meta-analysis found intuitive eating to be (a) consistently negatively related to body mass, and (b) significantly higher in men than in women across a variety of age groups and cultures (Linardon et al., 2021). Despite this, the authors noted limited variability with respect to demographic characteristics (including gender, ethnicity, and culture) across studies examining intuitive eating.

One context in which intuitive eating has rarely been examined is in the Greek-Cypriot population. There are several reasons why intuitive eating is worth examining in this population. For instance, traditionally, the Cypriot diet is a Mediterranean diet, characterized by moderate consumption of seafood, meat and dairy, and high consumption of extra-virgin olive oil, whole grains, olives, legumes, nuts, seeds, and fruits and vegetables (Hidalgo-Mora et al., 2020). There is much research to support the health benefits of consuming a Mediterranean diet including improved life expectancy, quality of life, and lower incidence of chronic disease (Sánchez-Sánchez et al., 2020; Sezaki et al., 2022). In addition, some of the traditional food preparation and consumption practices in the Mediterranean region promote eating as an “embodied” process and may therefore affect intuitive eating behaviors. For instance, eating is largely a socially valued event where food is prepared socially, with care, and the sensory properties of food are acknowledged, discussed and savored (Sutton, 2009).

Surprisingly, however, obesity and overweight rates in Cyprus are among the highest in Europe, and continue to rise, with 33.8% of adults having overweight body mass, and 16.6% of adults having obesity (Eurostat, 2022). In addition, Cyprus has been found to have high levels of weight-related anxiety in relation to other European countries (Argyrides et al., 2019). This might be explained by the Westernization of eating habits with increased consumption of fast-foods (i.e., foods that are high in saturated fats and refined carbohydrates), facilitated by increased accessibility and ease of availability (e.g., using food delivery applications; Argyrides and Kkeli, 2015). Further, there remains a significant lack of public health initiatives and interventions aimed at the promotion of healthy eating behaviors (see Ministry of Health, 2008). This signifies the importance of examining intuitive eating in this population.

The current study

There is strong support for the clinical utility of intuitive eating in the realm of intervention efforts aimed at the prevention and treatment of disordered eating and obesity. The development of a detailed understanding of how intuitive eating is related to physical and psychosocial factors is essential for the design, implementation, and efficacy of such efforts. As such, the current study aimed to explore differences in intuitive eating scores based on gender and body mass in the Greek-Cypriot population. Based on previous findings (Linardon et al., 2021), we expected that men would report significantly higher intuitive eating scores than women, and that body mass would be significantly inversely associated with intuitive eating in both men and women. In addition, we explored whether the differences in intuitive eating scores across body mass categories differed for men and women (i.e., if there is an interaction effect between body mass and gender on intuitive eating scores).

Materials and methods

Measures

Demographics

A demographics questionnaire was completed by all participants in which they were asked to report their age, gender, ethnicity, height and weight. Body Mass Index (BMI) was calculated from height and weight as (kg/m2).

Intuitive eating

To measure intuitive eating, participants completed Intuitive Eating Scale-2 (IES-2; Tylka and Kroon Van Diest, 2013; Greek translation: Giannakou et al., 2022). The 23-item IES-2 comprises 4 subscales which assess the four facets of intuitive eating; Unconditional Permission to Eat (i.e., an individual’s willingness to eat when hungry and a refusal to label certain foods as forbidden; 6 items), Eating for Physical rather than Emotional Reasons (i.e., eating when one is physically hungry rather than to cope with emotional distress; 8 items), Reliance on Hunger and Satiety Cues (i.e., an individual’s trust in their internal hunger and satiety cues and reliance on these cues to guide eating behaviors; 6 items), and Body-Food Choice Congruence (i.e., a tendency to make food choices that honor one’s health and body functioning; 3 items). All items were rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 = strongly disagree, to 5 = strongly agree. An overall subscale score was computed as the mean of all items, with higher scores reflecting greater intuitive eating. Adequate internal consistency and construct validity have been reported for scores on the Greek translation of the IES-2 (Giannakou et al., 2022). For the current sample, the scale was found to be internally consistent (α = 0.87 for the Total Scale, α = 0.72 for Unconditional Permission to Eat, α = 0.90 for Eating for Physical Rather than Emotional Reasons, α = 0.86 for Reliance on Hunger and Satiety Cues, and α = 0.81 for Body-Food Choice Congruence).

Participants and procedure

The study was carried out in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki, and ethical approval was obtained from the Cyprus Bioethics Committee. Data collection took place between May 2022 and July 2022. Participants were recruited via advertisements placed on social media websites supplemented using a snowball sampling method. The study was advertised as a study about “eating styles and behaviors.” Those interested in participating were directed to and online questionnaire, hosted by Qualtrics1 and completed a pre-screener to determine eligibility for the study. Participants were eligible if (a) they were over 18 years of age (b) their preferred language was Greek, and (c) they were a citizen of Cyprus. Upon meeting the inclusion criteria, participants were provided with further information regarding the study requirements, including that participation was anonymous, voluntary and without remuneration. Provided digital informed consent before completing the online questionnaire with the measures listed above in a pre-randomized order. Attention checks were placed at two points in the questionnaire.

Data analysis

All data analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics Version 28. Missing data was managed using listwise deletion. Data were screened to ensure data quality as recommended when using online samples (Burnette et al., 2022; Moeck et al., 2022). This included checking (a) Internet Protocol addresses to identify whether any participant answered the questionnaire more than once (b) responses for age height and weight to identify nonsensical/improbable values and (c) failed responses to the attention checks. The screening identified six participants who entered nonsensical age/height values, and 10 participants who failed the attention checks, who were subsequently removed from the initial sample of N = 1,328, reducing the sample to N = 1,312.

Following this, data were assessed for normality, linearity, and homoscedasticity; all assumptions were met. Participant characteristics and intergroup differences based on gender and BMI category were assessed via examination of analysis of variance (ANOVA) and Chi-squared testing. The intercorrelations of the study variables were examined using a Pearson’s product moment correlation analysis, with r values of ≤0.10 being considered to have a small effect, ~0.30, a moderate effect, and ~0.50, a strong effect (based on Cohen, 1992). To examine subgroup differences in intuitive eating scores, a two-way multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA; Pillai’s trace correction) was conducted with gender and BMI category entered as independent variables and intuitive eating as the dependent variable. Effect sizes were examined, with partial eta-squared values of ≤0.01 being considered to a small effect, ~0.06 a moderate effect, and ~0.14 a strong effect (based on Cohen, 1988). For all analyses, p < 0.05 was considered significant.

Results

Participant characteristics

The participants of the study were 1,312 Greek Cypriot citizens and residents (women n = 655, men n = 657), ranging in age from 18 to 70 years (M = 34.49, SD = 10.90) and in self-reported body mass index (BMI) from 14.53 to 53.22 kg/m2 (M = 25.37, SD = 5.24). As can be seen in Table 1, on average, the men in this sample had significantly higher self-reported BMI than the women [F(1, 1,311) = 120.44, p < 0.001], and were significantly older than the women [F(1, 1,311) = 21.64, p < 0.001]. In addition, the distribution of participants across BMI groups differed significantly between men and women, χ2(3, 1,312) = 139.04, p < 0.001, with women having more participants in the lower BMI groups (i.e., underweight and normal weight), and men having more participants in the higher BMI groups (i.e., overweight and obese). No significant differences were observed for age differences between each of the BMI groups for men and women.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample characteristics.

Inter-correlations

As can be seen in Table 2, for women, the Pearson’s product moment correlation analysis indicated significant moderate inverse associations between BMI and intuitive eating (r = −0.35, p < 0.001), as well as all four of the subscales of intuitive eating. Similar results were observed for men, with moderate inverse associations for BMI and intuitive eating (r = −0.32, p < 0.001), as well as all the subscales of intuitive eating besides unconditional permission to eat, which did not reach significance.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Intercorrelations for study variables.

Comparison by gender and BMI category

The two-way MANOVA revealed a statistically significant overall interaction effect for gender and BMI category on the dependent variables [F(7, 1,304) = 7.19, p < 0.001]. The interaction effect was also significant for each of the dependent variables (see Table 3). Concerning the IES total score, men had a trend of higher scores in all BMI categories except in the underweight category (see Figure 1). Concerning the Unconditional Permission to Eat scale, males had similar scores in all weight categories except in the underweight category where scores were higher. Additionally, females were decreasing in scores and plateaued at the higher weight categories, implying that males are less affected overall by this variable than females, regardless of weight category (See Figure 2). Concerning the Eating for Physical Rather than Emotional Reasons scale, both genders had the same pattern of decreasing levels as body weight increased. However, this is not the case in the underweight category where females have higher levels than males (see Figure 3). Concerning Hunger Satiety Cues, the same pattern was present with males have a steady drop in levels of hunger satiety cues as their weight increased whereas in females there are similar scores in the underweight and normal weight categories and then decrease in the higher weight categories (see Figure 4). Lastly, concerning the Body-Food Choice Congruence scale, both genders have the same overall pattern of lower levels in the underweight category, increasing in the normal weight category and dropping again in the overweight and obese categories. However, this pattern was evident in men who had lower levels than women in the underweight and obese categories (see Figure 5).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Means and standard deviations for intuitive eating scores among BMI subgroups.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Interaction effect for intuitive eating total score. N = 1,312 (n = 657 for women, and n = 655 for men), p < 0.001.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Interaction effect for unconditional permission to eat. N = 1,312 (n = 657 for women, and n = 655 for men), p = 0.01.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Interaction effect for eating for physical rather than emotional reasons. N = 1,312 (n = 657 for women, and n = 655 for men), p < 0.001.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Interaction effect for reliance on hunger and satiety cues. N = 1,312 (n = 657 for women, and n = 655 for men), p < 0.001.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Interaction effect for body food choice congruence. N = 1,312 (n = 657 for women, and n = 655 for men), p < 0.001.

Concerning the main effects of gender, individual ANOVAs indicated a significant difference between males and females on the IES total Score [F(1,1,304) = 8.496, p = 0.004, η2 = 0.006] and the Eating for Physical Rather than Emotional Reasons Subscale [F(1,1,304) = 15.008, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.011]. In both cases, males scored significantly higher than females. No other gender differences were detected.

Concerning the main effects of BMI category, individual ANOVAs indicated a significant difference between the four BMI categories on all five scales (Table 4). Specifically, there was a significant main effect on the IES total Score [F(3,1,304) = 41.75, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.088], the Unconditional Permission to Eat [F(3,1,304) = 4.67, p = 0.003, η2 = 0.011], the Eating for Physical Rather than Emotional Reasons [F(3,1,304) = 38.68, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.082], the Hunger Satiety Cues [F(3,1,304) = 20.65, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.045] and the Body-Food Choice Congruence [F(3,1,304) = 17.16, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.038]. In all four BMI categories, most scale scores significantly differed between them (p’s of 0.03 to 0.000) with mean scores significantly decreasing as each BMI category increased, meaning that levels of total intuitive eating and its subscales were decreasing as body weight of participants increased. The only exceptions were found in: (a) the subscale of Unconditional Permission to Eat where individuals in the normal weight category did not significantly differ compared to the overweight and obese categories implying similar results between them (b) the Hunger Satiety Cues where individuals in the Underweight and Normal Weight categories had similar results and (c) the Body-Food Choice Congruence where individuals in the underweight category had similar results to the overweight and obese categories.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Gender by BMI category interaction statistics on all variables of interest.

Discussion

The current study aimed to explore differences in intuitive eating scores based on gender and BMI in Greek-Cypriot adults. Overall, our findings suggest that both gender and body mass are important factors which influence intuitive eating levels in Greek-Cypriot adults. More specifically, we found that men generally tend to have higher levels of intuitive eating than women, and that body mass is inversely associated with intuitive eating for both genders. In addition, significant interaction effects between body mass and gender were observed.

The finding that men tend to eat more intuitively than women was expected, and is in line with previous studies with similar findings (Linardon et al., 2021). This finding may be explained in the context of the societal pressures that men and women face. The pressures placed on women to conform to beauty ideals and the degree to which their bodies are objectified and scrutinized tend to be greater than for men, with women experiencing greater body-surveillance, thin-ideal internalization, appearance-related media pressures, and family pressures (Grogan, 2016; Frederick et al., 2022b). In addition, recent work has shown women to be more likely to follow a weight loss diet or to attempt to control their appearance through crash diets/fasting (Frederick et al., 2022a). Consequently, women may be more likely to impose restrictions on their food consumption and be less likely to rely on their internal physiological hunger and satiety cues to guide their eating behavior.

It is worth noting that our results found intuitive eating to be inversely associated with BMI, which was also expected in light of previous work (Linardon et al., 2021). This finding might also be explained from a sociocultural perspective, and common attitudes held by many societies which equate having a lower BMI to enhanced health and wellbeing, and a higher BMI with unattractiveness and laziness, physically “unfit” or “inactive” (Puhl and Heuer, 2009). Such attitudes, which come to be internalized by individuals with a higher BMI, are related to body functionality and functionality appreciation (i.e., acknowledging and appreciating the functions the body performs; Alleva et al., 2017; Alleva and Tylka, 2021). Indeed, individuals with higher BMI have been found to have higher levels of functionality appreciation (Soulliard et al., 2019; Soulliard and Vander Wal, 2019; Todd et al., 2019). Therefore, it may be that individuals with a higher BMI tend to be less inclined to trust their body’s physiological hunger and satiety cues, thus prioritizing these in guiding their eating behavior over external emotional and situational cues.

We also found significant interaction effects between gender and body mass on intuitive eating. More specifically, the strongest interaction effects were observed for IES total scores and eating for emotional rather than physical reasons, with men in all BMI categories except in the underweight category scoring higher on both. Based on this finding, it seems that having a higher BMI is more likely to negatively affect intuitive eating in women than in men. Examining these interaction effects for the individual intuitive eating subscales provided further insights. First, the results showed that unconditional permission to eat in men is less likely to be affected by BMI than it is in women. Put differently, as body mass increases, women seem to be much more likely to pose restrictions on their diet than men. In addition, women who are in the underweight category seem to be significantly more likely to eat for physical reasons rather than emotional reasons, than men who are in the underweight category. When it comes to relying on hunger and satiety cues to guide eating behavior, women in the normal weight category tend to score higher than men, who have higher scores for all other categories (i.e., underweight, overweight, and obese). Finally, being underweight or being obese seems to be associated with lower levels of body food choice congruence in men and higher in women. Overall, these findings identify specific facets of intuitive eating which may be important to explore in targeted intervention plans aimed at promoting intuitive eating in Greek-Cypriot adults.

Strengths and limitations

To the best of our knowledge, the current study was the first to compare levels of intuitive eating based on gender and body mass in the Greek Cypriot population. However, the findings should be considered in light of several limitations. Firstly, the correlational nature of the study design is somewhat limiting, as it does not allow for causal conclusions to be drawn. Secondly, the assessment of intuitive eating relies solely on self-report recall, which assumes participants’ accurate portrayal of their level of functioning and that their perception of their psychological functioning and eating behaviors are an accurate reflection of reality. For example, some studies found no relationship between self-report measures of dietary restraint behaviors and actual caloric intake over several weeks (Stice et al., 2004, 2010). This suggests that self-report dietary restraint scales often measure intentions or desires rather than actual behaviors. In addition, it cannot be ruled out that participants’ responses may have been affected by social and personal desirability, with participants giving responses according to how they want to feel or eat rather than how they actually do. Future studies would therefore do well to control for social desirability when examining intuitive eating using correlational designs.

The present work was also limited by the opportunistic sampling method and use of snowball sampling. For instance, the current study did not assess other variables which may influence eating styles (e.g., health-conditions, psychological distress, and type of diet). These factors may have had a confounding effect on the results, and future studies would do well to assess such additional variables.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Author contributions

MA and EA: conceptualization, investigation, data curation, methodology, and writing, reviewing and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

References

Alleva, J. M., and Tylka, T. L. (2021). Body functionality: a review of the literature. Body Image 36, 149–171. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2020.11.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alleva, J. M., Tylka, T. L., and Kroon Van Diest, A. M. (2017). The functionality appreciation scale (FAS): development and psychometric evaluation in U.S. community women and men. Body Image 23, 28–44. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anastasiades, E., and Argyrides, M. (2022). Healthy orthorexia vs orthorexia nervosa: associations with body appreciation, functionality appreciation, intuitive eating and embodiment. Eating and weight disorders: EWD. doi: 10.1007/s40519-022-01449-9 Advance online publication.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Argyrides, M., and Kkeli, N. (2015). Predictive factors of disordered eating and body image satisfaction in Cyprus: predictors of disordered eating in Cyprus. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 48, 431–435. doi: 10.1002/eat.22310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Argyrides, M., Kkeli, N., and Koutsantoni, M. (2019). The comparison of Cyprus to six other european countries on body image satisfaction, appearance investment and weight and appearance-related anxiety. Eur. J. Couns. Psychol. 8, 32–42. doi: 10.5964/ejcop.v8i1.182

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Augustus-Horvath, C. L., and Tylka, T. L. (2011). The acceptance model of intuitive eating: a comparison of women in emerging adulthood, early adulthood, and middle adulthood. J. Couns. Psychol. 58, 110–125. doi: 10.1037/a0022129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babbott, K. M., Cavadino, A., Brenton-Peters, J., Consedine, N. S., and Roberts, M. (2022). Outcomes of intuitive eating interventions: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eat. Disord. 1–31, 1–31. doi: 10.1080/10640266.2022.2030124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruce, L. J., and Ricciardelli, L. A. (2016). A systematic review of the psychosocial correlates of intuitive eating among adult women. Appetite 96, 454–472. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burnette, C. B., Luzier, J. L., Bennett, B. L., Weisenmuller, C. M., Kerr, P., Martin, S., et al. (2022). Concerns and recommendations for using Amazon MTURK for eating disorder research. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 55, 263–272. doi: 10.1002/eat.23614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bush, H. E., Rossy, L., Mintz, L. B., and Schopp, L. (2014). Eat for life: a work site feasibility study of a novel mindfulness-based intuitive eating intervention. Am. J. Health Promot. 28, 380–388. doi: 10.4278/ajhp.120404-QUAN-186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carbonneau, E., Bégin, C., Lemieux, S., Mongeau, L., Paquette, M.-C., Turcotte, M., et al. (2017). A health at every size intervention improves intuitive eating and diet quality in Canadian women. Clin. Nutr. 36, 747–754. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2016.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. (1st Edn.). Routledge.

Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 112, 155–159. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eurostat (2022). Body mass index (BMI) by sex, age and income quintile. Available at: https://appsso.eurostat.ec.europa.eu/nui/show.do?dataset=hlth_ehis_bm1i&lang=en (Accessed 15 January 2022).

Google Scholar

Frederick, D. A., Crerand, C. E., Brown, T. A., Perez, M., Best, C. R., Cook-Cottone, C. P., et al. (2022a). Demographic predictors of body image satisfaction: the U.S. Body Project I. Body Image 41, 17–31. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2022.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frederick, D. A., Pila, E., Malcarne, V. L., Compte, E. J., Nagata, J. M., Best, C. R., et al. (2022b). Demographic predictors of objectification theory and tripartite influence model constructs: the U.S. Body Project I. Body Image 40, 182–199. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2021.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frederick, D. A., Tylka, T. L., Rodgers, R. F., Convertino, L., Pennesi, J.-L., Parent, M. C., et al. (2022c). Pathways from sociocultural and objectification constructs to body satisfaction among men: The U.S. Body Project I. Body Image 41, 84–96. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2022.01.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giannakou, K., Kyprianidou, M., Basdani, E., Hadjimbei, E., and Chrysostomou, S. (2022). Cross-cultural validity of the intuitive eating Scale-2: psychometric evaluation in the Greek-Cypriot population. Mediterr. J. Nutr. Metab. 1–12, 1–12. doi: 10.3233/MNM-220037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gödde, J. U., Yuan, T. Y., Kakinami, L., and Cohen, T. R. (2022). Intuitive eating and its association with psychosocial health in adults: a cross-sectional study in a representative Canadian sample. Appetite 168:105782. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grogan, S. (2016). Body Image: Understanding Body Dissatisfaction in Men, Women and Children (3rd ed.). Routledge.

Google Scholar

Hidalgo-Mora, J. J., García-Vigara, A., Sánchez-Sánchez, M. L., García-Pérez, M.-Á., Tarín, J., and Cano, A. (2020). The Mediterranean diet: a historical perspective on food for health. Maturitas 132, 65–69. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2019.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Homan, K. J., and Tylka, T. L. (2018). Development and exploration of the gratitude model of body appreciation in women. Body Image 25, 14–22. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Humphrey, L., Clifford, D., and Neyman Morris, M. (2015). Health at every size college course reduces dieting behaviors and improves intuitive eating, body esteem, and anti-fat attitudes. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 47, 354–360.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2015.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, A., Sano, Y., Parker, L., Cox, A. E., and Lanigan, J. (2022). Intuitive eating and dietary intake. Eat. Behav. 45:101606. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2022.101606

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linardon, J., Messer, M., Helms, E. R., McLean, C., Incerti, L., and Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M. (2020). Interactions between different eating patterns on recurrent binge-eating behavior: a machine learning approach. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 53, 533–540. doi: 10.1002/eat.23232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linardon, J., Tylka, T. L., and Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M. (2021). Intuitive eating and its psychological correlates: a META-ANALYSIS. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 54, 1073–1098. doi: 10.1002/eat.23509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ministry of Health (2008). Εθνικές Οδηγείες Διατροφής και Άσκησης [National Guidelines for Diet and Exercise]. Available at: https://www.moh.gov.cy/Moh/MOH.nsf/All/ADDB0B13026ADB5AC2257A4C001DC85A?Open.

Google Scholar

Moeck, E. K., Bridgland, V. M. E., and Takarangi, M. K. T. (2022). Food for thought: commentary on Burnette et al. (2021) “concerns and recommendations for using Amazon MTURK for eating disorder research.”. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 55, 282–284. doi: 10.1002/eat.23671

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearl, R. L., Puhl, R. M., Himmelstein, M. S., Pinto, A. M., and Foster, G. D. (2020). Weight stigma and weight-related health: associations of self-report measures among adults in weight management. Ann. Behav. Med. 54, 904–914. doi: 10.1093/abm/kaaa026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puhl, R. M., and Heuer, C. A. (2009). The stigma of obesity: a review and update. Obesity 17, 941–964. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Resch, E., and Tylka, T. L. (2019). “Intuitive eating,” in Handbook of positive body image and embodiment. eds. T. L. Tylka and N. Piran (Oxford University Press), 68–79.

Google Scholar

Sánchez-Sánchez, M. L., García-Vigara, A., Hidalgo-Mora, J. J., García-Pérez, M.-Á., Tarín, J., and Cano, A. (2020). Mediterranean diet and health: a systematic review of epidemiological studies and intervention trials. Maturitas 136, 25–37. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2020.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sezaki, A., Imai, T., Miyamoto, K., Kawase, F., Shirai, Y., Abe, C., et al. (2022). Association between the Mediterranean diet score and healthy life expectancy: a global comparative study. J. Nutr. Health Aging 26, 621–627. doi: 10.1007/s12603-022-1811-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soulliard, Z. A., Kauffman, A. A., Fitterman-Harris, H. F., Perry, J. E., and Ross, M. J. (2019). Examining positive body image, sport confidence, flow state, and subjective performance among student athletes and non-athletes. Body Image 28, 93–100. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soulliard, Z. A., and Vander Wal, J. S. (2019). Validation of the body appreciation Scale-2 and relationships to eating behaviors and health among sexual minorities. Body Image 31, 120–130. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., Fisher, M., and Lowe, M. R. (2004). Are dietary restraint scales valid measures of acute dietary restriction? Unobtrusive observational data suggest not. Psychol. Assess. 16, 51–59. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.16.1.51

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., Sysko, R., Roberto, C. A., and Allison, S. (2010). Are dietary restraint scales valid measures of dietary restriction? Additional objective behavioral and biological data suggest not. Appetite 54, 331–339. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutton, D. (2009). The mindful kitchen, the embodied Cook: Tools, technology and knowledge transmission on a Greek Island mindful eating: Mindful cooking. Mater. Cult. Rev. 70, 63–68.

Google Scholar

Teas, E., Kimiecik, J., Ward, R. M., and Timmerman, K. (2022). Intuitive eating and biomarkers related to cardiovascular disease in older adults. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 54, 412–421. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2022.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todd, J., Aspell, J. E., Barron, D., and Swami, V. (2019). Multiple dimensions of interoceptive awareness are associated with facets of body image in British adults. Body Image 29, 6–16. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tribole, E., and Resch, E. (1995). Intuitive eating. New York: St. Martin’s Griffin.

Google Scholar

Tribole, E., and Resch, E. (2012). Intuitive eating: A revolutionary program that works. 3rd Edn. New York: St. Martin’s Griffin.

Google Scholar

Tylka, T. L., Calogero, R. M., and Daníelsdóttir, S. (2015). Is intuitive eating the same as flexible dietary control? Their links to each other and well-being could provide an answer. Appetite 95, 166–175. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tylka, T. L., and Kroon Van Diest, A. M. (2013). The intuitive eating scale–2: item refinement and psychometric evaluation with college women and men. J. Couns. Psychol. 60, 137–153. doi: 10.1037/a0030893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tylka, T. L., and Wilcox, J. A. (2006). Are intuitive eating and eating disorder symptomatology opposite poles of the same construct? J. Couns. Psychol. 53, 474–485. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.53.4.474

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Dyke, N., and Drinkwater, E. J. (2014). Relationships between intuitive eating and health indicators: literature review. Public Health Nutr. 17, 1757–1766. doi: 10.1017/S1368980013002139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vartanian, L. R., and Porter, A. M. (2016). Weight stigma and eating behavior: a review of the literature. Appetite 102, 3–14. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.01.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, J. B., and Hardin, A. S. (2016). An integrative affect regulation process model of internalized weight bias and intuitive eating in college women. Appetite 102, 60–69. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.02.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, R. E., Marshall, R. D., Murakami, J. M., and Latner, J. D. (2020). Brief non-dieting intervention increases intuitive eating and reduces dieting intention, body image dissatisfaction, and anti-fat attitudes: a randomized controlled trial. Appetite 148:104556. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: intuitive eating, wellbeing, eating behavior, adaptive eating, body image

Citation: Argyrides M and Anastasiades E (2022) Intuitive eating in Greek-Cypriot adults: Influence of gender and body mass. Front. Psychol. 13:1033720. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1033720

Received: 31 August 2022; Accepted: 07 November 2022;
Published: 24 November 2022.

Edited by:

Christos Pezirkianidis, Panteion University, Greece

Reviewed by:

Omer Horovitz, Tel-Hai College, Israel
Konstantinos Karampas, Panteion University, Greece

Copyright © 2022 Argyrides and Anastasiades. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marios Argyrides, bS5hcmd5cmlkZXMuMUBudXAuYWMuY3k=

ORCID: Marios Argyrides https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9936-7229
Elly Anastasiades https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8562-1177

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.