Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 14 December 2022
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

Structural validity of the Norwegian version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire in children aged 3–6 years

Updated
\r\nKatrine Nyvoll Aadland*Katrine Nyvoll Aadland1*Arne LervgArne Lervåg2Yngvar OmmundsenYngvar Ommundsen3Eivind AadlandEivind Aadland1
  • 1Department of Sport, Food and Natural Sciences, Faculty of Education, Arts and Sports, Western Norway University of Applied Sciences, Sogndal, Norway
  • 2Department of Pedagogy, Religion and Social Studies, Faculty of Education, Arts and Sports, Western Norway University of Applied Sciences, Sogndal, Norway
  • 3Department of Sport and Social Sciences, Norwegian School of Sport Sciences, Oslo, Norway

Introduction: This study examined the structural validity of the teacher-report Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) in Norwegian preschoolers aged 3–6 years. We tested the original five-factor structure, the five-factor structure with two broader second-order factors, and a three-factor structure, all suggested in the literature. Since the positively worded items in SDQ have been shown to introduce noise, we also examined all three structures with a positive construal method factor for these items.

Methods: Preschool teachers from 43 preschools completed the SDQ questionnaire for 1,142 children [48% girls, mean age 4.3 (SD 0.9) years]. Confirmatory factor analyses were used to estimate and compare the six models. Measurement invariance was tested across sex (multi-group approach) and age (multiple-indicator multiple-cause approach).

Results: The original five-factor structure of SDQ was supported, where the model fit improved when including a method factor for positively worded items. Both models showed scalar invariance across sex and age. The second-order and the three-factor structures were not supported.

Conclusion: We recommend using the original five-factor structure when using SDQ for both clinical and research purposes in young children and adding a method factor when using structural equation modeling.

Introduction

The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) is a widely used instrument for measuring mental health in children and adolescents and is available in more than 80 languages.1 The questionnaire is developed for use in clinical and educational settings, as well as for screening and research (Goodman, 1997), and has parent-, teacher-, and self-report versions. SDQ was developed for children aged 3–16 years with an emphasis on strengths as well as difficulties to increase acceptability for use in general, healthy populations (Goodman, 1997). It assesses children’s behaviors, emotions, and relationships with 25 items equally divided between the five dimensions: conduct problems, emotional symptoms, hyperactivity, peer problems, and prosocial behavior (Goodman, 1997).

Validation studies generally support the original five-factor structure of SDQ across different languages and age groups (Smedje et al., 1999; Goodman, 2001; Van Roy et al., 2008; Stone et al., 2010; Mieloo et al., 2012; Niclasen et al., 2012; Ezpeleta et al., 2013; Croft et al., 2015; Kersten et al., 2016; McAloney-Kocaman and McPherson, 2017). A systematic review of 27 studies found strong evidence for the five-factor structure also in young children (Kersten et al., 2016). Seventeen of the included studies used confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and were of fair quality. Nine of these studies had preschool teachers as informants of which eight studies reported acceptable to good model fit for the five-factor structure. The original studies that solely included young children (aged 3–7) used the SDQ versions in English (Hill and Hughes, 2007; Downs et al., 2012), German (Downs et al., 2012), Spanish (Hill and Hughes, 2007; Downs et al., 2012), Dutch (Mieloo et al., 2012), and Danish (Niclasen et al., 2012). More recent studies have also supported the five-factor structure of SDQ in Scottish (McAloney-Kocaman and McPherson, 2017) and Swedish (Dahlberg et al., 2019) samples.

Norwegian versions of the SDQ have been validated in different age groups (Rønning et al., 2004; Van Roy et al., 2008; Sanne et al., 2009; Bøe et al., 2016), but the structural validity of the Norwegian teacher-report form for preschool children is not known. Sanne et al. (2009) found an acceptable goodness of fit for Goodman’s five-factor model for both the Norwegian parent- and teacher-report versions of SDQ in children aged 7–9 years. Furthermore, the findings by Rønning et al. (2004), Van Roy et al. (2008), and Bøe et al. (2016) generally support a five-factor structure of the Norwegian self-report form for adolescents, but all studies suggest that modifications should be considered to improve its structural validity. More research is therefore needed to validate the structure of the Norwegian SDQ, particularly for young children.

Several alternative structures of the SDQ have been suggested. A highly cited study by Goodman et al. (2010) suggested using fewer, broader domains in the general population with low rates of disorders by grouping the items into one externalizing and one internalizing factor in addition to the prosocial behavior factor (Goodman et al., 2010). Ezpeleta et al. (2013) found support for both the original five-factor structure and a second-order model with internalizing and externalizing as second-order factors in 3-year-old children (using the SDQ version for 3- to 4-year-olds). However, others have found a better fit for the original five-factor structure than a three-factor structure (internalizing, externalizing, and prosocial behavior as factors) in preschoolers (Croft et al., 2015; McAloney-Kocaman and McPherson, 2017). Some studies have also suggested a six-factor structure that includes a method factor for the positively worded items (i.e., a “positive construal factor”), as these items tend to cluster together, in addition to the original five factors (Van Roy et al., 2008; McAloney-Kocaman and McPherson, 2017). Van Roy et al. (2008) identified a positive construal factor in their study of adolescents (10–19 years) but found that the factor had a modest role compared with the original factors. In a sample of 4-year-olds, McAloney-Kocaman and McPherson (2017) compared a three-factor model (with internalizing, externalizing, and prosocial behavior as factors), a five-factor model (with the original five factors), and a six-factor model (with a method factor in addition to the original five factors) for the standard parent-report SDQ in children and youth (3–16 years) and found that the six-factor solution provided the best model fit, although the original five-factor structure also was supported.

Some studies have tested differences in scores for the SDQ domains across sex and age (Downs et al., 2012; Mieloo et al., 2012; Niclasen et al., 2012). For example, Niclasen et al. (2012) found that boys scored higher on the hyperactivity, conduct problems, and peer problems scales and lower on the emotional symptoms and prosocial behavior scales compared to girls and that younger children scored higher on the hyperactivity and conduct problems scales compared to older children. However, few studies have examined the measurement invariance of the SDQ across sex and age, which is a prerequisite for such analyses. To the best of our knowledge, a study by Dahlberg et al. (2019), who found strong measurement invariance across sex, age, and parents’ education levels for parent- and teacher-report versions of SDQ in 3- to 5-year-olds, is the only previous study performing invariance testing of the SDQ structure in young children.

Against this background, more research is needed to facilitate the interpretation of the SDQ across populations and purposes (McAloney-Kocaman and McPherson, 2017). In the present study, we aimed to examine the structural validity of the teacher-report SDQ in Norwegian preschoolers aged 3–6 years, including evaluation of measurement invariance over sex and age. We examined six hypothesized models in the prevailing literature: Model 1: the original five-factor structure (Goodman, 1997; Figure 1), Model 2: a second-order model with internalizing and externalizing as second-order factors in the original five-factor structure (Goodman et al., 2010; Figure 2), Model 3: a three-factor model with internalizing, externalizing, and prosocial behavior as factors (Figure 3; Goodman et al., 2010; McAloney-Kocaman and McPherson, 2017), Model 4: the original five-factor structure with a positive construal method factor for the positively worded items (Van Roy et al., 2008; McAloney-Kocaman and McPherson, 2017; Figure 4), Model 5: the second-order model with a method factor (Figure 5), and Model 6: the three-factor model (internalizing, externalizing, and prosocial behavior) with a method factor (Figure 6).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Original five-factor structure (Model 1).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Second-order model with internalizing and externalizing as second-order factors from the original five-factor structure (Model 2).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Three-factor model with internalizing, externalizing, and prosocial behavior as factors (Model 3).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Original five-factor structure with a positive construal method factor for the positively worded items (Model 4).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Second-order model with internalizing and externalizing as second-order factors from the original five-factor structure with a positive construal method factor for the positively worded items (Model 5).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Three-factor model (internalizing, externalizing, and prosocial behavior) with a positive construal method factor for the positively worded items (Model 6).

Materials and methods

The present study used baseline data from the cluster randomized controlled trial Active Learning Norwegian Preschool(er)s (ACTNOW) (Aadland et al., 2020), collected during the autumn of 2019 and 2020. ACTNOW was conducted in the Sogn og Fjordane region in Western Norway and included 1,265 children (response rate 82%) aged 3–5 years from 46 preschools (response rate 82%) that fulfilled the inclusion criteria of having ≥ six 3- to 4-year-old children enrolled. Preschool teachers in three preschools refrained from completing the SDQ questionnaire; hence, 1,142 children (48% girls) attending 43 preschools were included in the present study.

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire

The SDQ is a brief measure of psychosocial strengths and difficulties in 3- to 16-year-old children. The SDQ asks about 25 attributes (10 positives, 14 negatives, and one neutral attribute) across five domains of five items each: (1) emotional symptoms scale; (2) conduct problems scale; (3) hyperactivity scale; (4) peer problems scale; and (5) prosocial behavior scale. All but the last scale is also summed to provide a total difficulties score (range 0–40). SDQ uses a 3-point Likert scale to indicate the extent to which each attribute applies to a child [“not true” (score 0), “somewhat true” (score 1), or “certainly true” (score 2)] (Goodman, 1997, 2001). Positively worded items on scales 1–4 (item 7: Obeys, item 11: Friend, item 14: Popular, item 21: Reflect, and item 25: Attends) were scored reversely. The SDQ was scored according to the protocol (Goodman, 1997; see text footnote 1). We used the standard Norwegian (Nynorsk) version of the SDQ available from the SDQ website (see text footnote 1). Preschool teachers completed the questionnaire for each child enrolled in their department.

Ethics statement

The procedures and methods used in ACTNOW conform to the ethical guidelines defined by the Word Medical Association’s Declaration of Helsinki and its subsequent revisions (World Medical Association, 2013). The study protocol was approved by the Norwegian Centre of Research Data (NSD) (reference number 248220) and the Institutional Ethics Committee. We obtained written consent from a parent or guardian of each child prior to the assessment.

Statistical methods

Children’s age and difficulties scores are provided as mean and standard deviation (SD). These analyses were conducted using the SPSS software, version 27.0 (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Armonk, NY: IBM Corp., USA).

Bivariate correlations between the five SDQ scales (sum scores), CFAs, and measurement invariance testing were conducted in Mplus, version 8 (Muthén and Muthén, Los Angeles, CA, USA). Due to the use of ordinal data and children being nested within preschools, the complex method with the robust weighted least squares mean and variance adjusted (WLSMV) estimator and delta parameterization was used in all CFAs and measurement invariance analyses. This also implies that missing data were handled by this estimator.

We examined the six hypothesized models illustrated in Figures 16. Due to the large sample size, multiple indices in addition to the chi-square test statistics (χ2) were used to assess model fit for the CFAs: A comparative fit index (CFI) and Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) ≥ 0.95, a root mean squared error of approximation (RMSEA) ≤ 0.06, and a standardized root mean squared residual (SRMR) ≤ 0.08 were considered good model fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999). In addition, we used modification indices to evaluate potential modifications to improve model fit. Internal consistency estimates are provided as coefficient omega (ω), estimated in Mplus.

As a secondary analysis, since items in the SDQ previously have been shown to cross-load (Goodman, 2001; Van Roy et al., 2008; Sanne et al., 2009) and studies have shown that the exploratory equation modeling (ESEM) approach is more flexible than the CFA approach (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2009; Xiao et al., 2019; Wei et al., 2022), we also used the ESEM syntax (with geomin rotations) to estimate the exploratory factor structure. Factor loadings ≥ 0.320 (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2001) were interpreted as substantive significant. We see these analyses as secondary, because they are solely data-driven and because our aim of the present study was to validate a questionnaire that has been developed from theory and is thoroughly validated and used in research and clinical practice for other age groups.

Models with good fit were tested for measurement invariance across age and between girls and boys. We used a multiple-indicator multiple-cause (MIMIC) approach to assess measurement invariance across age, as this approach allows for the use of age as a continuous variable. In the MIMIC models, the latent factors and the observed indicators were regressed on age in three steps as explained by Morin et al. (2016). In the first step (null model), age has no effect on the latent means, and the indicator intercepts as the direct paths for both the latent factors and the observed indicators are constrained to zero. In the second step (saturated model), we freely estimated the direct paths from age to all the observed indicators keeping the paths from age to the latent factors constrained to zero. In the third step (invariant model), we freely estimated the paths from age to the latent factors and age constrained the paths from age to the observed indicators to zero. We interpreted the results from these models such that if the saturated and invariant models had a better model fit than the null model, age was a predictor. If the saturated model had a better model fit than the invariant model, differential item functioning (DIF) existed as a function of age. If this was the case, partial invariance was estimated by including the direct effects of age on SDQ items showing large modification indices in addition to the direct effect on latent factors. Similar model fit indices for the saturated and invariant models supported measurement invariance for age.

We used a multi-group approach to measurement invariance testing across sex. To evaluate measurement invariance across sex, three nested models were compared by comparing increasingly restrictive models (i.e., b against a and c against b): (a) equivalence of the structure (configural invariance), (b) equivalence of factor loadings (weak invariance), and (c) equivalence of item intercepts (strong invariance).

We used a non-significant χ2 and the criteria proposed by Chen (2007) (n > 300) to indicate measurement invariance for both the MIMIC and nested models: ΔCFI < –0.010, supplemented by ΔRMSEA < 0.015 or ΔSRMR < 0.030 for loading/weak invariance, and ΔCFI < –0.010, supplemented by ΔRMSEA < 0.015 or ΔSRMR < 0.010 for intercept/strong invariance. A p-value ≤ 0.05 was used to indicate statistical significance in all analyses.

The outputs from all models are available from https://osf.io/rza5y/ (doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/RZA5).

Results

Children with data on at least one item on the SDQ were included in the analysis (n = 1142, 48% girls). We included 1130–1141 observations on each item (0.5% missing observations in total). The mean age of the children was 4.3 years (SD 0.9), and the mean difficulties score was 7.79 (SD 5.44) [6.50 (5.00) in girls and 8.98 (5.56) in boys]. Children’s scores are presented in Supplementary Table 1, and bivariate correlations between scales are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Bivariate correlation matrix for all scales (sum scores) in Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ).

Confirmatory factor analysis

The original five-factor model (Model 1) suggested by Goodman (1997) showed a good model fit for CFI (0.954), RMSEA (0.035), and TLI (0.948) but not for SRMR (0.089) (Supplementary Figure 1). The internal consistency for the five factors was all above 0.80 (emotional symptoms; ω = 0.851, conduct problems; ω = 0.801, hyperactivity; ω = 0.903, peer problems; ω = 0.858, and prosocial behavior; ω = 0.903). Several modifications were suggested, with the highest modification index for item 13: Unhappy. This item was suggested to cross-load on all factors (modification indices range 55.33–71.82). These cross-loadings might indicate that item 13 is not well suited for the youngest children. The wording of the item is “Ofte lei seg, nedfor eller på gråten” (“Often unhappy, down-hearted or tearful”), where the last part, “på gråten” (“tearful”), possibly is problematic, since children at this age tend to cry for many reasons and in different situations. For this reason, we omitted item 13 in further analyses. Removing item 13 from the five-factor model resulted in a better model fit for all indices (CFI = 0.969, RMSEA = 0.030, TLI = 0.965, and SRMR = 0.080), all within the accepted criteria. The modification indices further suggested cross-loadings for some items and correlations between items. We allowed correlations for items with reasonable similarities within the same factor (i.e., item 2 with item 10, item 23 with item 6, item 9 with item 20, and item 24 with item 16). These minor modifications (correlations), except for the correlation between item 9 and item 20, were also seen in the five-factor models by Goodman et al. (2010). After taking these modifications into account, model fit indices for the original five-factor model (Model 1) with modifications were all within the criteria for good model fit (Table 2), with standardized factor loadings ≥ 0.386 (Figure 7).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Model fit indices for the confirmatory factor analysis of the six tested models of the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) structure (n = 1142).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Accepted five-factor model (Model 1). All paths are significant unless marked non significant (NS). Item 13 is not included in the confirmatory factor analysis (CFA).

Adding a positive construal factor for all positively worded items to the five-factor structure (Model 4) increased the model fit indices (Table 2). All positively worded items, except item 11: Friend, loaded significantly on the method factor, with factor loadings ranging from 0.176 to 0.718. Both items 21: Reflect and 25: Attends had higher factor loadings on the method factor than their original hyperactivity factor (Figure 8 for the model with modifications and Supplementary Figure 2 for the model without modifications).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Accepted five-factor model with a positive construal method factor (Model 4). All paths are significant unless marked non significant (NS). Item 13 is not included in the confirmatory factor analysis (CFA).

The more parsimonious second-order model (Model 2), with internalizing and externalizing as second-order factors, did not have a different model fit than the original five-factor model (Table 2). However, we observed a Heywood case with a negative residual variance in the peer problems factor and a low correlation (0.40) between the two first-order factors for the internalizing factor, questioning the second-order structure. The same was found for the second-order model with a method factor (Model 5); it showed good model fit but a low correlation (0.43) between the first-order factors.

For the three-factor model (Model 3), model indices were poorer than for other models, and only the RMSEA was within the criteria for good model fit (Table 2). The variance for the internalizing factor was low (0.090), questioning its role in the model. The modification indices suggested several modifications. The highest modification index was suggested for the positively worded item 14: Popular on the prosocial behavior factor, which might be explained by common variance that will be accounted for in the three-factor model including the methods factor taking positively worded items into account. Hence, we did not add this cross-loading in the three-factor model. The correlation between items 16: Clingy and 24: Afraid added in the other models was also included in the three-factor model. This modification gave a better model fit (Supplementary Table 2) but resulted in insignificant variance in the internalizing factor (0.076, p = 0.093). The internal consistency for the three factors was all above 0.87 (internalizing; ω = 0.871, externalizing; ω = 0.909, and prosocial behavior; ω = 0.903).

Adding a method factor to the three-factor model (Model 6) resulted in a better model fit than for Model 3; however, only the RMSEA was within the criteria for good model fit (Table 2). The model fit indices were better for the modified three-factor model with a method factor (correlation between item 16 and item 24), but still, only within the criteria of good model fit for the RMSEA (Supplementary Table 2). The variance in the internalizing factor was not significant. Factor loadings ranged between 0.301 and 0.951. However, the modification indices suggested a wide range of modifications.

Finally, we supplemented our accepted five-factor models (Models 1 and 4) from the CFA with an ESEM where all SDQ items were allowed to load on five factors. The model fit for the ESEM was good for all indices [CFI = 0.989, TLI = 0.981, RMSEA = 0.022 (0.017–0.027), and SRMR = 0.040] and similar to the model fit for the five-factor models. All items had the highest loading on their original factor, except for item 19: Bullied, which had higher loading on the conduct problems factor. In addition, we observed a cross-loading for item 2: Restless on the peer problems factor. All other cross-loadings were for the positively worded items on the prosocial behavior factor, which supports the inclusion of a method factor in SEM analysis of the SDQ (item 7: Obeys, also cross-loaded on the hyperactivity factor). All factor loadings for the ESEM are shown in Supplementary Table 3. Furthermore, model fit indices for all CFAs and ESEMs are shown in Supplementary Table 2.

Measurement invariance

Since only the five-factor models (Models 1 and 4) were found to have good model fit, we only performed measurement invariance testing for these models. Both models showed scalar invariance both across sex (Table 3) and age (Table 4), showing that the structure of the SDQ did not differ between girls and boys and across the age range of 3–6 years. Age explained less than 2.3% of the variance in each of the original five factors (in Models 1 and 4) and 8.4% of the method factor [e.g., in the five-factor model with method factor (Model 4), the explained variances of age were 0.6, 0, 0.5, 1.7, 0.3, and 8.4% of the emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity, peer problems, and prosocial behavior factors, and the method factor, respectively].

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Measurement invariance testing for the accepted models across sex.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Multiple-indicator multiple-cause (MIMIC) models for age in accepted models.

Discussion

The present study aimed to examine the structural validity of teacher-reported SDQ in Norwegian preschoolers aged 3–6 years, including evaluation of measurement invariance over sex and age. By testing six hypothesized structural models in the prevailing literature, we found an acceptable model fit for the original five-factor structure (Goodman, 1997), and a superior fit for the original five-factor structure with a positive construal method factor for the positively worded items (Van Roy et al., 2008; McAloney-Kocaman and McPherson, 2017). Although the model fit for the second-order model was good, the low correlation between the first-order factors emotional symptoms and peer problems of the internalizing factor questions the convergent validity of this model. The model fit for the three-factor structure and the three-factor structure with a method factor was not supported. In addition, there was no significant variation among the children in the internalizing factor, questioning the validity of the three-factor model in the current sample. The five-factor structure—with and without a methods factor—had scalar invariance across sex and age (3–6 years).

Our findings support previous studies and confirm the validity of the original five-factor structure of the SDQ in preschool-aged children (Downs et al., 2012; Mieloo et al., 2012; Croft et al., 2015; Kersten et al., 2016; McAloney-Kocaman and McPherson, 2017; Dahlberg et al., 2019). Similar to previous studies, some modifications within the five-factor structure were indicated in the present study. As item 13: Unhappy cross-loaded on all factors, we chose to omit this item in our analysis because these cross-loadings might be explained by the fact that children of young age simply react with crying for a wide range of reasons, not only when being unhappy. Goodman (2001) also observed a cross-loading for item 13 but only to the conduct problems factor. However, their sample included older children (5–15 years). In addition to omitting item 13, we added minor modifications to the model, that is, four correlations between items of similar meaning/wording within the same factors. Three of these four correlations were the same minor modifications as in the five-factor model by Goodman et al. (2010). Generally, the small cross-loadings observed (and ignored) in the present and previous studies are expected as the children are young, and the domains may be less distinctive than in older children, making differentiation across sub-domains through preschool teacher reports more challenging. Van Roy et al. (2008) suggested that especially emotional and behavioral difficulties are less distinct constructs in young children than in older children.

Research has suggested using broader subscales for SDQ in low-risk, epidemiological samples (Goodman et al., 2010). Goodman et al. (2010) found support for a second-order model where the second-order factor of internalizing is indicated by the first-order factors emotional symptoms and peer problems, and externalizing by conduct problems and hyperactivity, for different SDQ forms and raters in children aged 5–15 years. In their study, the correlations between the first-order factors within the internalizing and externalizing factors were between 0.66–0.71 and 0.71–0.81, respectively. Our second-order model, including preschool children as rated by preschool teachers, showed very low convergent validity for the internalizing factor, where the correlation between the emotional symptoms and peer problems was 0.344. Goodman et al. (2010) further examined whether the five first-order factors could be replaced by the three factors internalizing, externalizing, and prosocial behavior but found a poor fit. Neither the present study nor McAloney-Kocaman and McPherson (2017) found support for a three-factor structure in preschool samples. The present study observed a lack of significant variation among the children in the internalizing factor. Together, evidence on the second-order model and the three-factor model suggests that these broader subscales should not be used in preschool children.

The internal consistency for the factors within the five-factor model was high in the present study with all coefficient Omegas above 0.80 (range 0.80–0.90). These findings are similar to those by Ezpeleta et al. (2013) reporting Omegas 0.91 for the total score and from 0.75 to 0.93 for the five domains of SDQ (version 3–4 years) in their sample of 3-year-old preschoolers. Other studies have mainly provided internal consistency coefficients such as Cronbach’s alpha, which are lower than Omega. In the systematic review by Kersten et al. (2016), Cronbach’s alpha for the teacher-report form of SDQ from 26 studies was on average 0.82 for the total score and ranged between 0.49 and 0.69 for the factors.

Although the present study supported the original five-factor structure, the inclusion of a method factor for the positively worded items provided a superior fit to the data. This finding is consistent with the finding by McAloney-Kocaman and McPherson (2017) using parent-reported SDQ in Scottish children, which to our knowledge is the only previous study examining this structural model in a sample of preschoolers. In both studies, the inclusion of a method factor resulted in lower factor loadings on the original factors for all positively worded items (in our study, not for item 11: Friend), and for some of these items also a lower factor loading on the original factor than on the method factor. In the present study, both items 21: Reflect and 25: Attends had higher factor loadings on the method factor than the original hyperactivity factor, whereas the Scottish study observed similar or higher factor loadings on the method factor than their original factors for all items except items 21 and 25 from the hyperactivity factor and item 7: Obeys from the conduct problems factor. These two studies’ findings indicate that the items with low loadings on the original factors after adding the method factor are less relevant as they do not provide substantive information for their factors. The significant factor loadings on the method factor, which for some items were high (ranges in the present study 0.17–0.54 and the Scottish study 0.32–0.51), might indicate that the positively worded items reflect method variance that needs to be accounted for in a separate positive construal method factor. Several previous studies have highlighted the noise associated with the positively worded items, but they all appear to agree that this noise is tolerable to gain acceptance for the use of the questionnaire in general healthy populations (Van Roy et al., 2008; McAloney-Kocaman and McPherson, 2017). In other words, although some of the positively worded items are less relevant for their factors, they should better be included to gain acceptability from the respondents.

The five-factor structure—with and without a method factor—was equal across sex and age, meaning that these groups’ latent means can be compared. This finding is consistent with the Swedish study by Dahlberg et al. (2019) examining the five-factor structure in preschool-aged children. To the best of our knowledge, no previous studies have examined the measurement invariance for the five-factor structure with a method factor.

The present study has several strengths and limitations. First, we included a large study sample, providing good evidence for the factor structure (including measurement invariance across sex and age) of the Norwegian teacher-report form of SDQ in young children. However, the sample was not representative of Norwegian preschoolers as children from a limited geographical area in the western part of Norway were included. Second, the strength of our analysis was the adjustment for the preschool level, as the same teacher completed the SDQ for all children attending their department in the preschool. A limitation of our study is that we included only teacher reports, while multi-informant approaches for both research and clinical purposes have been recommended (Stone et al., 2010). However, previous studies have shown that teacher-reported SDQ has higher levels of internal consistency than parent-reported SDQ (Kersten et al., 2016), and teachers in preschools have better opportunities than parents to judge how children relate to others (Stone et al., 2010).

Conclusion and implications

The present study found support for the original five-factor structure of the SDQ (Goodman, 1997), but not a three-factor structure, in a sample of Norwegian preschoolers. However, we found a superior fit for a model with the five-factor structure including an additional positive construal method factor for the positively worded items. The inclusion of a method factor weakened the factor loadings for the positively worded items on their original factors. Despite the positively worded items that may provide measurement noise, it has been suggested that their inclusion may be important for acceptability for the SDQ by teachers completing the form. Thus, the present study points to the importance of including such a method factor to account for this noise. Good internal consistency (ω) for all factors (≥ 0.80) and measurement invariance across both sex and age provide evidence for the use of the five factors to compare latent means for sex and age groups. Hence, we recommend using the original five factors when using SDQ for both clinical and research purposes in young children and adding a method factor when using structural equation modeling. We further recommend excluding item 13: Unhappy when using SDQ in young children.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusion of this article will be made available by the authors, on reasonable request.

Ethics statement

The study protocol was approved by the Norwegian Centre of Research Data (NSD) (reference number: 248220) and the Institutional Ethics Committee. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

KA performed the data collection, analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript draft. EA obtained funding for the study, contributed to data analysis, and drafted the manuscript. AL contributed to data analysis and drafted the manuscript. YO drafted the manuscript. All authors conceived the idea for the manuscript, read, commented on, and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This ACTNOW study was funded by the Research Council of Norway (grant number: 287903), the County Governor of Sogn og Fjordane, the Sparebanken Sogn og Fjordane Foundation, and the Western Norway University of Applied Sciences.

Acknowledgments

We would like to express our gratitude to all children, parents, and preschool teachers for their participation in the study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1024918/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Figure 1 | Confirmatory factor analysis of the five-factor model (no modifications).

Supplementary Figure 2 | Confirmatory factor analysis of the five-factor model with a method factor (no modifications).

Supplementary Table 1 | Proportions of scoring on each category for all items of Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ).

Supplementary Table 2 | Model fit indices for all tested models.

Supplementary Table 3 | Exploratory equation modeling (ESEM) of Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ).

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.sdqinfo.org

References

Aadland, E., Tjomsland, H. E., Johannessen, K., Nilsen, A. K. O., Resaland, G. K., Glosvik, O., et al. (2020). Active learning Norwegian Preschool(er)s (ACTNOW) - design of a cluster randomized controlled trial of staff professional development to promote physical activity, motor skills, and cognition in preschoolers. Front. Psychol. 11:1382. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asparouhov, T., and Muthén, B. (2009). Exploratory structural equation modeling. Struct. Equ. Modeling 16, 397–438. doi: 10.1080/10705510903008204

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bøe, T., Hysing, M., Skogen, J. C., and Breivik, K. (2016). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ): Factor structure and gender equivalence in Norwegian adolescents. PLoS One 11:e0152202. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0152202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct. Equ. Modeling. 14, 464–504. doi: 10.1080/10705510701301834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Croft, S., Stride, C., Maughan, B., and Rowe, R. (2015). Validity of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire in preschool-aged children. Pediatrics 135, E1210–E1219. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-2920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dahlberg, A., Ghaderi, A., Sarkadi, A., and Salari, R. (2019). SDQ in the hands of fathers and preschool teachers-psychometric properties in a non-clinical sample of 3-5-Year-Olds. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 50, 132–141. doi: 10.1007/s10578-018-0826-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Downs, A., Strand, P. S., Heinrichs, N., and Cerna, S. (2012). Use of the teacher version of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire with German and American preschoolers. Early Educ. Dev. 23, 493–516. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2010.532082

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ezpeleta, L., Granero, R., de la Osa, N., Penelo, E., and Domenech, J. M. (2013). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (3-4) in 3-year-old preschoolers. Compr. Psychiatry 54, 282–291. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, A., Lamping, D. L., and Ploubidis, G. B. (2010). When to use broader internalising and externalising subscales instead of the hypothesised five subscales on the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ): Data from British parents, teachers and children. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 38, 1179–1191. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9434-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 38, 581–586.

Google Scholar

Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 40, 1337–1345. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, C. R., and Hughes, J. N. (2007). An examination of the convergent and discriminant validity of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. School Psychol. Q. 22, 380–406. doi: 10.1037/1045-3830.22.3.380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Modeling 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kersten, P., Czuba, K., McPherson, K., Dudley, M., Elder, H., Tauroa, R., et al. (2016). A systematic review of evidence for the psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 40, 64–75. doi: 10.1177/0165025415570647

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McAloney-Kocaman, K., and McPherson, K. (2017). Factor structure and reliability of the parent-informant strengths and difficulties questionnaire in a Scottish preschool sample. Early Educ. Dev. 28, 368–376. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2017.1228367

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mieloo, C., Raat, H., van Oort, F., Bevaart, F., Vogel, I., Donker, M., et al. (2012). Validity and reliability of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire in 5-6 year olds: Differences by gender or by parental education? PLoS One 7:8. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morin, A. J. S., Arens, A. K., and Marsh, H. W. (2016). A bifactor exploratory structural equation modeling framework for the identification of distinct sources of construct-relevant psychometric multidimensionality. Struct. Equ. Modeling 23, 116–139. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2014.961800

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niclasen, J., Teasdale, T. W., Andersen, A. M. N., Skovgaard, A. M., Elberling, H., and Obel, C. (2012). Psychometric properties of the danish strength and difficulties questionnaire: The SDQ assessed for more than 70,000 raters in four different cohorts. PLoS One 7:e32025. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0032025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rønning, J. A., Handegaard, B. H., Sourander, A., and Mørch, W.-T. (2004). The strengths and difficulties self-report questionnaire as a screening instrument in Norwegian community samples. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 13, 73–82. doi: 10.1007/s00787-004-0356-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanne, B., Torsheim, T., Heiervang, E., and Stormark, K. M. (2009). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire in the Bergen child study: A conceptually and methodically motivated structural analysis. Psychol. Assess. 21, 352–364. doi: 10.1037/a0016317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smedje, H., Broman, J. E., Hetta, J., and von Knorring, A. L. (1999). Psychometric properties of a Swedish version of the “Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire”. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 8, 63–70. doi: 10.1007/s007870050086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stone, L. L., Otten, R., Engels, R. C. M. E., Vermulst, A. A., and Janssens, J. M. A. M. (2010). Psychometric properties of the parent and teacher versions of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire for 4- to 12-year-olds: A review. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 13, 254–274. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0071-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics, 4th Edn. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Google Scholar

Van Roy, B., Veenstra, M., and Clench-Aas, J. (2008). Construct validity of the five-factor strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ) in pre-, early, and late adolescence. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 49, 1304–1312. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01942.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, X., Huang, J., Zhang, L., Pan, D., and Pan, J. (2022). Evaluation and comparison of SEM, ESEM, and BSEM in estimating structural models with potentially unknown cross-loadings. Struct. Equ. Modeling 29, 327–338. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2021.2006664

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Medical Association (2013). World medical association declaration of Helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA 310, 2191–2194. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiao, Y., Liu, H., and Hau, K.-T. (2019). A comparison of CFA, ESEM, and BSEM in test structure 519 analysis. Struct. Equ. Modeling 26, 665–677. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2018.1562928

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mental health, preschool children, confirmatory factor analysis, psychometrics, SDQ

Citation: Aadland KN, Lervåg A, Ommundsen Y and Aadland E (2022) Structural validity of the Norwegian version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire in children aged 3–6 years. Front. Psychol. 13:1024918. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1024918

Received: 22 August 2022; Accepted: 17 November 2022;
Published: 14 December 2022.

Edited by:

Antonio Zuffiano, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Reviewed by:

Tatiana Marci, University of Padua, Italy
Sebastian Dys, Simon Fraser University, Canada

Copyright © 2022 Aadland, Lervåg, Ommundsen and Aadland. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Katrine Nyvoll Aadland, katrinea@hvl.no

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.