Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 10 November 2022
Sec. Organizational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Consumer Psychology in Tourism and Hospitality View all 33 articles

The mechanism of body–mind integration in the formation of destination attachment: A comparison of first-time and repeat tourists

Yinyin DongYinyin Dong1Ying Qu
Ying Qu2*
  • 1Tourism College, Hainan University, Haikou, Hainan, China
  • 2School of Tourism and Urban-Rural Planning, Zhejiang Gongshang University, Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China

By constructing a person-body–mind-place framework of destination attachment, this study explores the physical and mental formation mechanism of destination attachment and examines its dynamics between first-time and repeat tourists. The present study found that multisensory impressions can, directly and indirectly, affect destination attachment through emotional and cognitive psychological mediation. There are differences between first-time tourists and repeat tourists in terms of this mediation path. As the frequency of travel increases, the influence of multisensory impressions gradually increases. The formation of destination attachment is dominated by emotion for first-time tourists, whereas it is dominated by cognition for repeat tourists. Based on these findings, theoretical and practical implications are presented.

Introduction

As a representation of the human-land relationship, destination attachment has unique tourism marketing value (Ramkissoon, 2016), and exploring its formation mechanism is of great significance to tourism industry. However, previous studies tend to explain the formation of destination attachment in terms of either social construction (i.e., the perspective of the body) or subjective construction (i.e., the perspective of the mind) while ignoring the process of body–mind unity in the formation of destination attachment (Kastenholz et al., 2020). With the increasing rise of embodied theory emphasizing the “oneness of mind and body” in tourism research, its explanatory power for destination attachment has been recognized by some scholars (Yuksel et al., 2010). However, systematic integration studies have yet to be conducted.

Destination attachment is formed through the interaction of tourists with the tangible/intangible environment of the destination, and it is embodied in the process of travel experience (Loureiro, 2014). Tourists use their five senses to get information about a destination. Multisensory impressions are the first impression of tourists after entering the destination, which directly determines the perception, attitude, and behavior of tourists during the tour (Manosuthi et al., 2021). These impressions naturally become a key factor affecting destination attachment (Lv and McCabe, 2020). In addition, when tourists’ bodies are in a tourist situation, their psychological states may be activated (Krishna, 2012). Therefore, the interaction between tourists and the destination involves multisensory stimulation and emotional and cognitive psychological processes (Rakić and Chambers, 2012). Emotion and cognition coexist in attachment relationships as potential components of destination attachment (Scannell and Gifford, 2010). Although previous studies have recognized the importance of individual psychological factors on destination attachment (Prayag and Lee, 2019), they have not differentiated between emotional and cognitive pathways in shaping the different dimensions of destination attachment.

Moreover, due to their different travel experiences, first-time tourists may be more likely to trigger emotional reactions after receiving information through the senses. In contrast, with increased travel frequency, repeat tourists emphasize psychological meaning (Fuchs and Reichel, 2011). While previous studies have confirmed that a difference exists in the intensity of destination attachment between first-time and repeat tourists (Morais and Lin, 2010), determining how and in which ways first-time and repeat tourists diverge in the psychological mechanism of sensory impression influencing destination attachment has yet to be examined.

Therefore, this study aims to explore the relationship between multisensory impressions, emotional responses, cognitive responses, and destination attachment and identify differences in these relationships between first-time and repeat tourists. The shortcomings of existing research will be accounted for by achieving the above goals, and the mind–body mechanisms underlying the dynamic formation of destination attachment will be discovered.

Literature review and hypotheses development

Destination attachment and its formation mechanism

Rooted in the attachment theory, destination attachment is defined as the cognitive and emotional linkage that tourists establish with a tourism destination (Japutra, 2020), reflecting the extent to which an individual values and identifies with a particular environmental setting (Yuksel et al., 2010). The sense of physically being and feeling “at home” can be considered a sign that a tourist creates a connection to the destination. Although destination attachment has been mentioned as a multidimensional construct (Io and Wan, 2018), the two-dimensional division, which includes place identity and place dependence (Yuksel et al., 2010), is widely recognized by scholars (Liu et al., 2019).

Place identity refers to a tourist’s rich memories and affection for a particular destination, which are preserved in the definition of self. Place identity underscores the cognitive domain of a sense of place, which is related to symbolic meanings that a tourist ascribes to and self-identifies with that destination (Williams and Vaske, 2003). Place identity can be developed through positively balanced perceptions (Dwyer et al., 2019). Place dependence refers to the collection of social and material resources that meet the specific needs of tourists and represent the unique qualities of a place. Place dependence is a form of functional attachment, providing features and conditions that support achieving specific goals or desired activities (Williams and Vaske, 2003). Place dependence occurs when tourists show a functional need for a destination that is not transferable to another destination (Mlozi and Pesämaa, 2013). In the existing research on the measurement of the destination attachment model, place dependence and place identity are often used as second-order latent variables to conduct path tests (Hosany et al., 2017; Kastenholz et al., 2020). Although this can encompass the overall characteristics of destination attachment from a macro perspective, it ignores the complexity of the internal structure of destination attachment and the uniqueness of the formation mechanism of different dimensions (Mlozi et al., 2012). Therefore, the importance of refining the two-dimensional construct of destination attachment has been well approved (Mlozi et al., 2012).

Developing a market base made up of attached tourists is important, as they are less likely to change their choice of place despite the offerings of the alternatives (Loureiro, 2014). Destination attachment has the potential to improve satisfaction (Ramkissoon, 2016), foster attitudinal loyalty (Yi et al., 2018), expand word-of-mouth advocacy (Pandey and Sahu, 2020), and promote revisiting (Jian et al., 2021). Destination attachment is even seen as the key to enhancing the competitiveness of tourist destinations through increased patronage and profits (Dwyer et al., 2019). Exploring the formation mechanism of purposive attachment thus becomes an important topic.

There are two main paradigms of place attachment formation mechanisms. The earliest is social constructivism, which emphasizes physical practice and gives spatial meaning through interaction (Tuan, 1975). An abstract space becomes a meaningful place through experience (Kastenholz et al., 2020). Individuals are not directly attached to places but to the symbolism they represent. The classic “place ballet” view explains how people’s movement in space and time forms the process of place meaning (Seamon, 1980). However, this physically-based idea of the formation of place attachment is limited to theoretical discussion and lacks empirical evidence. Furthermore, the single view of physical practice ignores the complex psychological activities of tourists in the tourism situation.

The more recent subjective constructivism synthetically describes the formation of place attachment by a tripartite framework of “people-place-psychological processes” (Scannell and Gifford, 2010). Psychological processes link people with a place, thus dominating this research branch. However, existing research explores the effect of a comprehensive psychological variable that blends the cognitive and affective effects (e.g., motivations, satisfaction, personal involvement, destination image, perceived attractiveness, and self-congruity) on place attachment as a one-dimensional concept (Prayag and Ryan, 2012; Xu and Zhang, 2016; Riper et al., 2019; Tasci et al., 2022; Usakli et al., 2022). Little is known about the concrete effect of cognition and emotion in isolation on place attachment, especially on the different dimensions of place attachment. Moreover, this research branch also ignores the influence of physical elements in co-shaping place attachment. Therefore, the research gaps are two-fold. First, on the whole, the process of body–mind unity in forming destination attachment has yet to be studied. Second, regarding the mind branch, the separation effect of cognition and emotion on the different dimensions of destination attachment has to be delineated. The two gaps will be bridged together in this study.

Embodied theory and multisensory impressions

The embodied theory emphasizes that consumers’ physical senses first receive external marketing stimuli, which is then processed by consumers’ psychology, followed by affecting their attitudes and behaviors (Walther-Hansen, 2020). The senses are not independent of psychology but rather participate in the process of psychology. The mind must be understood in the context of its relationship to the body, which is the process of “body–mind oneness”. When the external environment stimulates different sensory cells of tourists, tourists first form different “sensations” and then produce a “conscious sensory experience” (Ji and King, 2018). In the research topic of the people-land relationship under the embodied paradigm, the body of tourists is similar to the plasma membrane of cells, which plays a role in material exchange and energy transfer (Vaske and Kobrin, 2001). In this connection between the inside and outside of the interface, the birth of a new people-land relationship occurs.

The concept of multisensory impressions first appeared in the field of sensory marketing and has recently attracted the interest of tourism scholars (Manosuthi et al., 2021). Fakfare et al. (2021) found that multisensory impressions are a good way to determine the perception of tourists’ degree of sensory stimulation when visiting a destination and can be summarized into five aspects: sight, hearing, taste, touch, and smell (Chen et al., 2021). Agapito et al. (2017) posited that multisensory impressions result from screening stimuli and experiences by tourists’ bodily senses. Usually, only those unique, profound, and valuable sensory stimuli and experiences will leave multisensory impressions. In recent years, multisensory experiences have been mentioned as an important prerequisite for destination attachment formation. Human geography studies have shown that the five senses are crucial for developing human-land relationships (Xiong et al., 2015). Tourists interact with destinations through their five senses (Agapito et al., 2017), stimulating an emotional preference for destinations. Multisensory impressions reflect the quality of the travel experience and link the objective physical environment with subjective emotional attachment (Agapito et al., 2014). Extraordinary multisensory impressions not only enhance tourists’ intimacy with the destination but may also trigger more identification with the destination (Lv and Wu, 2021). Multisensory impressions satisfy tourists’ needs for sensory pleasure and prompt tourists to reflect on the relationship between themselves and the destination, which is conducive to the formation of place dependence and place identity (Yang et al., 2021). The stronger the multisensory impression, the stronger the visitor’s attachment to the destination is likely to be. Therefore, this study proposes the following:

H1: Multisensory impressions have a significant positive effect on destination attachment.

H1a: Multisensory impressions have a significant positive impact on place dependence.

H1b: Multisensory impressions have a significant positive impact on place identity.

“Emotion-cognition” psychological systems

After external stimuli are input into the body of tourists through the senses, the tourists still need to go through a complex psychological process before they can have a conscious sensory experience, affecting subsequent behavioral choices (Jiang, 2020). Some scholars have proposed the “emotion-cognitive dual system model” to subdivide the complex psychological state of individuals (Alyahya and McLean, 2021). The dual system theory is also regarded as an information processing theory, which explains the formation of individual attitude preferences through two different information processing pathways (the limbic pathway of the emotional system and the central pathway of the cognitive system; Alyahya and McLean, 2021). Among them, the emotional system adopts the principle of intuition, requiring individuals to process information quickly with less effort. In contrast, the cognitive system adopts the principle of rationality, requiring individuals to use enough cognitive resources to comprehensively evaluate the content of the information. Emotional and cognitive responses trigger travel destination preferences (Michael et al., 2019).

Emotions represent distinct mental states characterized by episodes of intense feelings associated with a specific referent and instigate specific response behaviors, which are often unconsciously aroused. Emotions fundamentally shape the tourism experience; particularly, positive emotions are the core of hedonic tourism (Kim and Fesenmaier, 2015). Empirical evidence highlights the outstanding role of positive emotions in tourism, positively arousing tourist experiences connected to increased satisfaction, memorability, and loyalty (Agapito et al., 2017). Hosany and Gilbert (2010) developed the Destination Emotion Scale, arguing that positive emotion in tourism destinations involves a psychological state characterized by joy, love, and positive surprise.

However, tourists go beyond the superficial reception of sensory information and enter the consciousness domain of the mind (Wen and Leung, 2021), triggering deep-level cognition such as imagination, association, and thinking (Collins and Allard, 2001). Cognitive responses are the collection of all mental abilities and processes related to knowledge, memory, judgment, and even decision-making, which is a conscious mental process (Brown and Raymond, 2007). Tourists’ thinking and understanding of the destination environment occur throughout the entire tourism process. There is widespread support for cognitive assessment of destinations regarding goal congruence, certainty, and novelty (Choi and Choi, 2019).

Although emotion and cognition are independent psychological processes, they are closely related (Plass and Kalyuga, 2019). The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions suggests that positive emotions promote cognitive processing (Gable and Harmon-Jones, 2010). The emotions evoked by tourists interacting with the destination environment are all prerequisites for evaluating the cognitive experience of tourism (Tsai et al., 2020). Therefore, this study proposes the following:

H2: Emotional responses have a significant positive impact on cognitive responses.

Sensory marketing theory emphasizes that consumers interact with the outside world through their senses, affecting their emotions, attitudes, memories, and behaviors (Sthapit, 2019). Sensory stimuli, which are regarded as key tools for creating a tourism experience, are an important prerequisite for activating tourists’ emotional and cognitive responses. Tourists’ multisensory impressions partly explain the positive emotions associated with travel experiences (Yang et al., 2021). In the context of rural tourism, Kastenholz et al. (2020) verified the positive effects of multisensory impressions on emotions, and different sensory impressions have different effects on different dimensions of emotion. Lindstrom (2005) found that tactile experience stimulated tourists’ positive emotions and played a significant role in decision-making. Furthermore, tourists’ cognitive process of a destination is also deeply rooted in the body’s interaction with the world (Barsalou, 2008). Embodied tourism activities are also of great value in enhancing tourists’ perceptions (Walther-Hansen, 2020). Multisensory impressions can stimulate the perception and imagination of tourists, resulting in a more profound cognitive effect (Liu et al., 2019). Rich multisensory impressions play a crucial role in promoting tourists’ cognitive memory of a destination (Agapito et al., 2017). The stronger the sensory impression, the stronger the emotional and cognitive responses of visitors. Therefore, this study proposes the following hypotheses:

H3: Multisensory impressions have a significant positive impact on emotional responses.

H4: Multisensory impressions have a significant positive impact on cognitive responses.

Destination attachment is generally multifaceted, involving at least the cognitive interpretations of emotional responses to environmental stimuli relating to a particular geographical area (Hidalgo and Hernandez, 2001). Therefore, the formation of destination attachment involves both an emotional path and a cognitive path. First, attachment is regarded as an adaptive emotional response of an individual in a specific social relationship in psychology. Positive emotions are essential for building attachment to a destination (Grisaffe and Nguyen, 2011). High levels of positive emotions lead to strong destination attachment (Yan and Halpenny, 2019). Kim and Fesenmaier (2015) argued that positive emotions could activate on-site peak experiences and affect the recall of attachment memories. Fredrickson (2001) believed that tourists who receive more positive emotions through tourism experience might participate in more travel activities, promoting the development of place dependence. Io (2018) found that positive emotions promote tourist satisfaction and trigger tourists’ thinking about destination identity. Second, although a person’s attachment orientation is often conceptualized as a single global orientation toward close relationships, it is rooted in a complex network of cognitive processes. Current studies argue that a person’s cognitive evaluation of the experience is a necessary and sufficient condition for attachment to be formed (Gillath et al., 2009). Tourists’ cognitive assessments of destination travel experiences may alter a person’s attachment orientation. For example, the higher the perceived value of the landscape, the stronger the dependence on the destination (Brown and Raymond, 2007). When tourists associate their travel goals with themselves, they tend to identify with the destination (Barsalou, 2008). Therefore, this study proposes the following:

H5: Emotional responses have a significant positive impact on destination attachment.

H5a: Emotional responses have a significant positive impact on place dependence.

H5b: Emotional responses have a significant positive impact on place identity.

H6: Cognitive responses have a significant positive impact on destination attachment.

H6a: Cognitive responses have a significant positive impact on place dependence.

H6b: Cognitive responses have a significant positive impact on place identity.

Path difference between first-time and repeat tourists

It has been widely confirmed that there are differences in travel behaviors (e.g., motivation, experience, satisfaction, and revisit intention) between first-time and repeat tourists (Shanka and Taylor, 2004; Morais and Lin, 2010; Hsu et al., 2014). As the value of destination attachment becomes more pronounced, studies have begun to explore differences in destination attachment between first-time and repeat tourists. For example, Lewicka (2011) believed that tourists’ attachment to a destination gradually increases with the increase in travel frequency. Similarly, Vada et al. (2019) found that tourists more familiar with a destination are more likely to have a close relationship with the destination. Morais and Lin (2010) found that first-time tourists were mainly influenced by destination image, while repeat tourists were influenced by place attachment. Although these studies focused on the difference in the manifestation and effect of destination attachment, the differences in physical and mental mechanisms in the formation of destination attachment between first-time and repeat tourists have been ignored. Destination attachment is a product of the interaction of tourists with the destination. Tourists with different travel experiences have different degrees of received destination information through their senses, leading to differences in psychological activities and affecting the relationship between tourists and the destination.

For first-time tourists, due to the lack of prior experience with the destination, the landscape stimuli of the destination are a new experience for them, which may trigger intuitive emotional responses and do not require a lot of cognitive resources (Fuchs and Reichel, 2011). In addition, due to the short contact time with the destination, first-time tourists generally form a superficial attachment to the destination. Repeat tourists have a certain degree of knowledge about the destination (Shanka and Taylor, 2004). When repeat tourists are immersed in the scene again, the tourists will fully mobilize their cognitive resources to process the information received by the senses (Hwang et al., 2005). Generally speaking, the revisit is given more meaning, a process of spiritual awakening and self-reflection for tourists. Furthermore, with the increase in the frequency of travel, tourists’ attachment to the destination is more reflected in symbolic attachment. Accordingly, this study proposes the following:

H7: There are significant differences in the relational pathways of multisensory impressions, emotional responses, cognitive responses, and destination attachment between first-time tourists and repeat tourists.

Study design

Case study context

As the southernmost and only tropical coastal destination in China, Hainan Island has a long history with the development of coastal vacation tourism. However, in recent years, the characteristics of this destination entering a mature/stagnant period have become more prominent. Since 2012, the growth rate of domestic tourists in Hainan has been relatively low (10%), showing a steady trend with alternating slight declines and low recovery. Sanya, its core attraction area, has seen a more pronounced decline. The traditional functional marketing methods of Hainan destinations, which mainly focus on promoting destination attributes, are slightly exhausted. In contrast, relationship marketing methods emphasizing establishing an affective connection with the destination may provide an opportunity to reverse the unfavorable situation. Therefore, by taking Hainan Island as a case study, the “body–mind utility” shaping mechanism of destination attachment is explored.

Measures

The research questionnaire is divided into two parts. The first part is the central part of the questionnaire, including the scales of different variables, while the second part is the personal information of tourists. The measurement items of each variable in the model are from mature scales widely used in the relevant literature. The English scale has been appropriately modified according to the specific situation of the Hainan destination. The survey was first developed in English and then translated into Chinese by accredited translators. It was later translated back to English to ensure that the meanings of the survey items did not get lost during the process.

Among them, multisensory impressions refer to the research of Lv and McCabe (2020), Santos et al. (2019), and Fakfare et al. (2021), which covers the five dimensions of visual impression, auditory impression, gustatory impression, olfactory impression, and tactile impression. Emotional responses refer to research on destination emotion conducted by Hosany and Gilbert (2010), Hosany et al. (2015), and Hosany and Prayag (2013). The three dimensions of joy, love, and positive surprise were selected. Cognitive responses refer to the research conducted by Ma et al. (2019), Rivera et al. (2019), and Zheng et al. (2019) on cognitive assessment in tourism contexts. The three dimensions of goal consistency, certainty, and novelty were selected. Destination attachment refers to the research of Prayag and Lee (2019) and Liu et al. (2019), which includes the two dimensions of place dependence and place identity. All variables were measured using a 5-point Likert scale (1 = completely disagree, 5 = completely agree).

Before the formal investigation, this study conducted a pre-test to verify the reliability and validity of the scale. The pre-test was conducted in Sanya Bay, one of Hainan’s most famous and tourist-concentrated scenic spots. A total of 100 questionnaires were distributed through systematic random sampling, and 86 valid questionnaires were recovered. The pre-test results show that the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of each construct is greater than 0.7, indicating that the scale has good reliability; the factor loading of each item is above 0.7, indicating that the scale has good construct validity (Hair et al., 2021).

Data collection

Given the particularity of the formation of destination attachment (such as requiring a certain length of human-land interaction), by referring to the practice of previous research (e.g., Prayag and Lee, 2019), the present study excludes non-overnight excursionists who stay for less than 1 day and chooses the formal research time from October 5th to October 7th, 2021. This period is when Hainan tourism begins to enter its peak season, and China’s National Day holiday is longer, making it easy for tourists to immerse themselves in the tour. In such a context, the sample quality is high, thereby improving the validity of the sampling. In addition, to make the sample representative, the Sanya Phoenix Airport and Haikou Meilan Airport in Hainan Province (the leading import and export channels for Hainan tourism) were selected as the questionnaire distribution places, and the survey was mainly conducted by a random interception with an interval of every five tourists. Furthermore, to ensure the quality of the questionnaire collection, the questionnaire distributors consisted of postgraduates majoring in tourism management (7 students in total), who were more qualified for survey management.

First, the respondents were asked whether they planned to leave the island after traveling in Hainan. In this way, local residents, transfer passengers, and tourists who have just entered the island were excluded from the study. After getting a confirmed answer, the questionnaire was shown to the tourists. Due to the lengthy questionnaire questions, a small gift was given to express gratitude to the tourists. A total of 700 questionnaires were distributed during the period. Excluding those filled incompletely, 670 valid samples were recovered, and the effective recovery rate was 95.71%. The questionnaires split between first-time tourists and repeat tourists amounted to 332 and 338, respectively. Generally, the number of samples should be at least 10 times the number of variables (Hair et al., 2021). In addition, the G*Power program was used to calculate the sample size required, based on an effect of 0.15 for the predictors, a precision level of 5%, a test power of 0.97 and 32 predictors (Memon et al., 2020). The sample size required was 291 respondents. Thus, the current sample size (more than 300 for each group) was sufficient.

The sample was almost evenly divided between males and females for both groups. Most respondents were between the ages of 18–40 (first-timers: 82.8%; repeat tourists: 65.1%) and held a university degree (first-timers: 76.1%; repeat tourists: 76.2%). The majority of respondents were full-time employed professionals (first-timers: 50.5%; repeat tourists: 49.5%).

Data analysis

This study used partial least squares-structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) for analysis. The reasons are as follows. Firstly, PLS provides a variance-based predictive SEM method with the ability to analyze complex models and simultaneously handle reflective, formative, and higher-order model structures (Henseler et al., 2015). Secondly, PLS does not have data restrictions regarding normal distribution, randomness, and sample size, and the requirements are relatively loose, thereby providing flexibility for data analysis (Hair et al., 2021). Thirdly, PLS can effectively manage interfering data and missing values with good predictive and explanatory power.

Results

Measurement model

As shown in Tables 1, 2, firstly, the combined reliability (CR) and Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of each variable were greater than 0.7 in both the first-time and repeat groups, indicating that all scales had good reliability (Bagozzi and Yi, 1988). Secondly, the factor loadings of all items and the average variance extraction (AVE) in both groups were higher than the threshold of 0.5, indicating that the model had good convergent validity (Fornell and Larcker, 1981). Furthermore, for both first-time and repeat tourists, the values of the heterotrait-monotrait (HTMT) were all below 0.85, indicating that each variable had good discriminant validity (Hair et al., 2021). In addition, the variance inflation factor (VIF) value ranged from 2.124 to 3.581 for both groups, which was less than the threshold of 5.000, indicating that multicolinearity was not an issue in this research (Hair et al., 2021).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Assessment results of the measurement model.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. HTMT discriminant validity analysis results (first-time tourists/repeat tourists).

Structural model

As shown in Table 3, the R2 results of both groups (first-timers: between 38.1 and 78.9%; repeat tourists: between 55.2 and 64.9%) showed that the predictive ability of the model constructs reached a medium level or above (Hair et al., 2021). The Q2 values were all greater than 0 (first-timers: between 0.271 and 1.817; repeat tourists: between 0.502 and 1.752), indicating that the exogenous constructs had a certain predictive ability to endogenous constructs. The effect size (f2) values of the proposed structural model were all higher than the standard value of 0.02 for both groups (Hair et al., 2021). The standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) values of the model in the two groups were 0.065 and 0.061, respectively, which met the criterion of less than 0.08 proposed by Henseler et al. (2015), indicating that the model had a good model fit. The acceptability and invariance of the measurement model were also confirmed.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Structural model test results.

Direct effects test and multi-group analysis

First, the invariance of the two groups was assessed using the measurement invariance of composite models (MICOM) procedure of PLS-SEM, which showed that configural invariance, compositional invariance, equal mean values, and variances were all well-established (Hair et al., 2021). Hypothesis testing and multi-group analysis results can therefore be carried out. The test results are shown in Figures 1, 2, as well as in Table 4.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Conceptual model for first-time tourists. * indicates significance at p < 0.05; ** indicates significance at p < 0.01; and *** indicates significance at p < 0.001.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Conceptual model for repeat tourists. * indicates significance at p < 0.05; ** indicates significance at p < 0.01; and *** indicates significance at p < 0.001.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Hypothetical test and multi-group analysis.

The results show that multisensory impressions significantly and positively affected place dependence (first-timers: β = 0.156; p < 0.001; repeat tourists: β = 0.124; p < 0.001) across the two groups, H1a was thus supported. However, there was no significant difference between first-time tourists and repeat tourists for hypothesis H1a (βfirstrepeat = 0.032, p > 0.05). As for the relationship between multisensory impressions and place identity, H1b was partially supported across the two groups because the positive relationship existed not for first-timers but for repeat tourists (first-timers: β = 0.155, p > 0.05; repeat tourists: β = 0.181, p < 0.001). Moreover, the difference that multisensory impressions had on place identity between the two groups was significant (βfirstrepeat = −0.026, p < 0.001). Hypothesis H2 was also supported across the two groups, showing a significant relationship between emotional responses and cognitive responses (first-timers: β = 0.310, p < 0.001; repeat tourists: β = 0.327, p < 0.001). However, hypothesis H2 showed no significant difference between first-time tourists and repeat tourists (βfirst-βrepeat = −0.017, p > 0.05).

In addition, multisensory impressions were found to be positively related to emotional responses (first-timers: β = 0.907, p < 0.001; repeat tourists: β = 0.609, p < 0.001) and cognitive responses (first-timers: β = 0.564, p < 0.001; repeat tourists: β = 0.711, p < 0.001) across the two groups, thus supporting hypotheses H3 and H4. For both groups, multisensory impressions had a significant difference in emotional responses (βfirstrepeat = 0.298, p < 0.05) and cognitive responses (βfirstrepeat = −0.147, p < 0.05). Emotional responses significantly affected place dependence for the two groups (first-timers: β = 0.586, p < 0.001; repeat tourists: β = 0.373, p < 0.001). However, emotional response did not significantly affect place identity for the two groups (first-timers: β = 0.029, p > 0.05; repeat tourists: β = 0.043, p > 0.05). Thus, hypothesis H5a was accepted, while H5b was rejected. For both groups, emotional responses had a significant difference in place dependence (βfirstrepeat = 0.213, p < 0.001), but not in place identity (βfirstrepeat = −0.147, p > 0.05). Similarly, cognitive response significantly affected place dependence for repeat tourists, but not for first-timers (first-timers: β = 0.149, p > 0.05; repeat tourists: β = 0.169, p < 0.01). However, cognitive response significantly affected place identity for the two groups (first-timers: β = 0.312, p < 0.001; repeat tourists: β = 0.555, p < 0.001). Hypothesis H6a was partially supported, and H6b was accepted. Nevertheless, cognitive responses had a significant difference in place dependence (βfirstrepeat = 0.020, p < 0.01) and in place identity (βfirstrepeat = −0.243, p < 0.05) between the two groups. Based on this, therefore, the results partially support hypothesis H7.

Indirect effects test and multi-group analysis

According to the results in Table 4, the indirect effect of multisensory impressions on place dependence was significant through the emotional responses in both groups (first-timers: β = 0.474, p < 0.001; repeat tourists: β = 0.386, p < 0.001). Emotional responses partially mediate the relationship between multisensory impressions and place dependence across the two groups. Moreover, the difference that multisensory impressions had on place dependence through emotional responses between the two groups was significant (βfirstrepeat = 0.088, p < 0.001). However, the indirect effect of multisensory impressions on place identity was not significant through emotional responses for either of the groups (first-timers: β = 0.017, p > 0.05; repeat tourists: β = 0.296, p > 0.05). There was no difference between first-time tourists and repeat tourists (βfirstrepeat = −0.279, p > 0.05).

In addition, the indirect effect of multisensory impressions on place dependence was significant through cognitive responses for repeat tourists (β = 0.015, p < 0.01), but not for first-timers (β = 0.037, p > 0.05). Cognitive responses partially mediate the relationship between multisensory impressions and place dependence for repeat tourists. Moreover, the difference that multisensory impressions had on place dependence through cognitive responses between the two groups was significant (βfirstrepeat = 0.022, p < 0.05). However, in both groups, the indirect effect of multisensory impressions on place identity was significant through cognitive responses (first-timers: β = 0.313, p < 0.001; repeat tourists: β = 0.451, p < 0.001). Among the indirect effects of multisensory impressions on place identity, cognitive responses play a fully mediating role for first-time tourists. In contrast, cognitive responses play a partial mediating role for repeat tourists. Moreover, the difference that multisensory impressions had on place identity through cognitive responses between the two groups was significant (βfirstrepeat = −0.138, p < 0.001). Based on this, the results also partially support hypothesis H7.

Discussion and implications

Discussion

This paper constructs a structural equation model to explore the mind–body mechanism of destination attachment across the two groups of first-time and repeat tourists, contributing to understanding the formation of destination attachment. The results show that physical factors (multisensory impressions) and psychological factors (emotional and cognitive responses) are important antecedents of destination attachment. The effects of the antecedents differ between first-time and repeat tourists. In addition, emotional responses and cognitive responses significantly mediate the relationship between multisensory impressions and destination attachment, which varies between first-time and repeat tourists. Further discussion of the results has yielded several interesting insights.

Specifically, multisensory impressions were found to be an antecedent of place identity only for repeat tourists, while cognitive responses were an antecedent of place dependence only for repeat tourists. However, emotional responses were not a prerequisite for place identity for either group. The possible reason for this is that forming the place identity requires a long period of contact. Repeat tourists with rich travel experiences will directly generate place identity under the stimulation of multisensory impressions. However, due to the lack of travel experience to the destination for first-time tourists, the simple sensory impression cannot directly activate place identity. It requires the intermediary of cognitive psychology to achieve this (Fuchs and Reichel, 2011). Compared with shallow emotional responses, tourists’ cognitive activities at the destination are usually accompanied by deep thinking and more complex psychological activities (Ayduk et al., 2002). Therefore, impulsive emotions cannot affect place identity. Once cognitive activities occur, place identity will first be affected, which has been confirmed in both first-time tourists and repeat tourists. Moreover, the depth of cognition will affect its spillover validity. Comparatively speaking, repeat tourists have higher cognitive responses, and their cognitive responses affect place identity and slightly affect place dependence. This also proves that, compared with place dependence, place identity has higher requirements on physical and mental factors (Bolam et al., 2006).

In addition, repeat tourists showed stronger relationships on multisensory impressions→cognitive responses (βfirst = 0.564 < βrepeat = 0.711) and cognitive responses→place identity (βfirst = 0.312 < βrepeat = 0.555) than did first-time tourists. However, first-time tourists showed stronger relationships on multisensory impressions→emotional responses (βfirst = 0.907 > βrepeat = 0.609) and emotional responses→place dependence (βfirst = 0.586 > βrepeat = 0.373) than did the repeat tourists. Therefore, the results highlight that multisensory impressions can positively affect emotional and cognitive responses, but the effects on first-time and repeat tourists differ. Further, first-time tourists tend to choose the marginal path to process information when receiving the same sensory stimuli, and the intuitive, emotional responses dominate, which, in turn, mainly affect place dependence (Jiang, 2020). On the other hand, repeat tourists tend to choose the central route to process information, and the cognitive response of rational thinking dominates, mainly affecting place identity.

Correspondingly, the indirect effect size of multisensory impressions on place dependence through emotional responses for first-time tourists was much larger than for repeat tourists (βfirst = 0.474 > βrepeat = 0.386). The indirect effect size of multisensory impressions on place identity through cognitive responses for repeat tourists was much larger than for first-time tourists (βfirst = 0.313 < βrepeat = 0.451). This result suggests that first-time tourists rely more on the mediating role of emotional responses than repeat tourists in the indirect effects of sensory impressions on destination attachment. In contrast, repeat tourists rely more on the mediating role of cognitive responses than first-time tourists.

Considering the size of indirect effects, multisensory impressions exert a more considerable impact on place dependence under the mediation of emotional processing. In contrast, multisensory impressions exert a more considerable impact on place identity under the mediation of cognitive processing. For physical factors, if they want to achieve a greater impact on destination attachment, they must go through emotional or cognitive psychological processes. In the physical and mental mechanism of destination attachment, psychological factors play a dominant role, while physical factors play a fundamental role in triggering the mechanism. Moreover, physical factors are important antecedents of psychological factors. Sensory impressions directly affect emotional and cognitive responses and can also indirectly affect cognitive responses through emotional responses and realize a chain-mediated mediation of destination attachment.

By further identifying the intermediary type, it is found that the emotional response partially mediates for first-time tourists. In contrast, the cognitive response plays the role of complete mediation. For repeat tourists, the emotional and cognitive responses play partial mediation roles. That is to say, with the increase in travel frequency, the body’s role becomes increasingly important in shaping the attachment to the destination.

By combining the weight changes of the second-order structure of multisensory impressions, emotional responses, and cognitive responses, it can be determined that visual (βfirst = 0.938) and auditory impressions (βfirst = 0.863) have strong explanatory power for first-time tourists. In contrast, taste (βrepeat = 0.883) and tactile impressions (βrepeat = 0.874) have strong explanatory power for repeat tourists. Positive surprise (βfirst = 0.978) has strong explanatory power for first-time tourists, while love (βrepeat = 0.915) has strong explanatory power for repeat tourists. Novelty (βfirst = 0.919) has strong explanatory power for first-time tourists, while goal congruence (βrepeat = 0.900) has strong explanatory power for repeat tourists. It can be speculated that, for first-time tourists, visual and auditory impressions directly or indirectly affect destination attachment mainly through positive surprise and novelty cognition. For repeat tourists, taste and tactile impressions directly or indirectly affect destination attachment, mainly through love and goal congruence.

Theoretical implications

This paper proposes a people-body–mind-land framework of destination attachment formation, examining the body–mind utility effects therein. First, it verifies the effect of physical factors in the formation of destination attachment. The study found that while psychological factors play a leading role in shaping destination attachment, physical factors are the foundation. Although the direct effect of sensory impressions on destination attachment is not high, it can also indirectly influence destination attachment to a greater extent through the psychological mediating effects of emotion and cognition. As travel frequency increases, the body’s role becomes increasingly important. This affirms the value of the body in the formation mechanism of destination attachment and expands the applicability of the embodied theory in destination attachment research (Agapito et al., 2017).

Second, different from previous studies in which destination attachment was used as an overall construct (Skavronskaya et al., 2017), this study fine-grained the two-dimensional construct of destination attachment and examined the differences in the effects of mental factors on place dependence and place identity, respectively. Different psychological factors have different effects on different dimensions of destination attachment. Among them, place dependence is mainly driven by emotion. In contrast, place identity is mainly driven by cognition, which is consistent with the view posited by Backlund and Williams (2003) that the cognitive component is viewed as the place identity construct, and the emotional component is referred to as the place dependence construct. This study not only confirms the validity of the dual-system theory in explaining the psychology of destination attachment (Xu et al., 2019), but delineates different mechanisms of body–mind integration in motivating different attachment dimensions.

Finally, this study identifies differences in the body–mind mechanisms underlying destination attachment formation in first-time and repeat tourists. This study found that the physical and mental paths of first-time tourists are mainly as follows: multisensory impressions-emotional responses-place dependence, while the physical and mental paths of repeat tourists are multisensory impressions-cognitive responses-place identification. This means that the emotional response of first-time tourists is more prominent, while the cognitive response of repeat tourists is more prominent. This is in line with the position presented by Hwang et al. (2005) and Cao et al. (2021) to a certain extent: before the first-time tourists establish a connection with the destination, they mainly rely on the interaction between their senses and the physical environment to intuitively perceive and understand a destination. On the other hand, repeat tourists pay more attention to psychological meaning, and the level of place attachment is usually linked to the investment of cognitive resources. However, this may be different from the findings of Yolal et al. (2017) and Gursoy et al. (2014), who believe that first-time tourists place more importance on cognitive evaluation (service quality), while repeaters are loyal to their destination and rely more on emotional evaluation (satisfaction). The likely reason is that the context and destination of the studies are different, and the two studies ignore the underlying role of the body. Therefore, this study also provides a new perspective for exploring the differences in the travel behavior of first-time and repeat visitors from the perspective of body–mind integration.

Managerial implications

This study provides a reference for coastal destination marketing organizations (DMOs) to cultivate tourists’ attachment to the destination from an embodied perspective. Firstly, destination marketers can design rich multisensory experiences for tourists to highlight the uniqueness of coastal destinations in an integrated way. The five dimensions (sight, hearing, taste, touch, and smell) of sensory experiences can be connected, targeted, and creatively based on the preference of different tourist markets. In this way, creating a sensory experience can better meet the heterogeneous needs of different tourists and promote the emotional connection between tourists and the destination. In particular, destination marketers need to focus on conveying the multisensory landscapes to tourists rather than waiting for this to be initiated by tourists. This allows the priming effect of sensory experiences on destination attachment to be maximized in accordance with the expectations of the DMO rather than being elusive.

Secondly, since the formation of destination attachment is fundamentally a body–mind integration process, destination marketers can design destination landscape presentations based on the connection ways of tourists’ bodies and mind to induce attachment. To make a few examples. By rendering the visual impact of blue skies and white clouds or enhancing the olfactory enjoyment of fresh air, a strong and intuitive aesthetic experience will induce tourists’ positive emotions of joy, love, and surprise, which is beneficial to place dependence. By delivering historical allusions to tourists through the way of scrolling on the electronic screen or situational interpretation, the cognitive thinking of tourists on the destination culture can be aroused. By providing some embodied entertainment activities, such as allowing tourists to fish and work together with local residents, or encouraging tourists to taste local delicacies, tourists will be stimulated to reflect on the unique local way of life and the value identification of the destination can be strengthened.

In addition, the different needs and function mechanisms of multisensory experiences of first-time and repeat tourists must be considered in targeted marketing.

Visual and auditory impressions are prone to evoking the psychological process of first-time tourists, while taste and tactile impressions likely stimulate the psychological process of repeat tourists. Once the multisensory impressions occur, the emotional responses of first-time tourists will primarily be triggered, which affects place dependence. However, the cognitive responses of repeat tourists will primarily be triggered, which affects place identity. Therefore, it is important to deliver landscape resources, iconic attractions, convenient tourism facilities, and other material satisfaction for first-time tourists to develop functional attachments. In contrast, for those who return, it is necessary to cultivate their interaction with the destination and strengthen their social relations at the destination to enable more in-depth sensory senses and cognitive psychological processes. This suggestion concurs with those of Li et al. (2008).

Limitations and future research

This research has two limitations. First, this study only takes Hainan as a tourist destination as an example to verify the hypothesis model. The external validity of the conclusion requires further verification. In the future, the case location can be replaced, or a comparison of multiple coastal destinations can be conducted to investigate the mechanism of body–mind integration in the formation of destination attachment. Also, for a popular destination, there may always be a wide variety of tourism products, such as those that are religious, natural, or historical in nature. After tourists receive information through their senses, they will have different psychological preferences and reactions to various products. In future research, variables such as spiritual recovery, aesthetic perception, or cultural value can be added to the model to reflect the diversified reactions and refine the intermediary mechanism involved. The complex physical and mental mechanism of tourists’ destination attachment formation will be further explained.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Author contributions

YD: conceptualization, methodology, project administration, and writing – original draft. YQ: formal analysis, reviewing and editing, and correspond with the journal. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Agapito, D., Pinto, P., and Mendes, J. (2017). Tourists' memories, sensory impressions and loyalty: in loco and post-visit study in Southwest Portugal. Tour. Manag. 58, 108–118. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2016.10.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Agapito, D., Valle, P., and Mendes, J. (2014). The sensory dimension of tourist experiences: capturing meaningful sensory-informed themes in Southwest Portugal. Tour. Manag. 42, 224–237. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2013.11.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alyahya, M., and McLean, G. (2021). Examining tourism consumers’ attitudes and the role of sensory information in virtual reality experiences of a tourist destination. J. Travel Res. 61, 1666–1666. doi: 10.1177/00472875211037745

Google Scholar

Ayduk, O., Mischel, W., and Downey, G. (2002). Attentional mechanisms linking rejection to hostile reactivity: the role of “hot” versus “cool” focus. Psychol. Sci. 13, 443–448. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Backlund, E. A., and Williams, D. R. (2003). A quantitative synthesis of place attachment research: investigating past experience and place attachment. Paper presented at the northeastern recreation research symposium (pp. 320–325). Bolton landing, New York.

Google Scholar

Bagozzi, R. P., and Yi, Y. (1988). On the evaluation of structural equation models. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 16, 74–94. doi: 10.1007/BF02723327

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 617–645. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolam, B., Murphy, S., and Gleeson, K. (2006). Place-identity and geographical inequalities in health: a qualitative study. Psychol. Health 21, 399–420. doi: 10.1080/14768320500286526

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, G., and Raymond, C. (2007). The relationship between place attachment and landscape values: toward mapping place attachment. Appl. Geogr. 27, 89–111. doi: 10.1016/j.apgeog.2006.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, L., Qu, Y., and Yang, Q. (2021). The formation process of tourist attachment to a destination. Tour. Manag. Perspect. 38:100828. doi: 10.1016/j.tmp.2021.100828

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Q. P., Wu, J. J., and Ruan, W. Q. (2021). What fascinates you? Structural dimension and element analysis of sensory impressions of tourist destinations created by animated works. Asia Pacific J. Tourism Res. 26, 1038–1054. doi: 10.1080/10941665.2021.1941157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, H., and Choi, H. C. (2019). Investigating tourists’ fun-eliciting process toward tourism destination sites: an application of cognitive appraisal theory. J. Travel Res. 58, 732–744. doi: 10.1177/0047287518776805

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, N. L., and Allard, L. M. (2001). Cognitive representations of attachment: the content and function of working models. Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Interpersonal processes 2, 60–85.

Google Scholar

Dwyer, L., Chen, N., and Lee, J. (2019). The role of place attachment in tourism research. J. Travel Tour. Mark. 36, 645–652. doi: 10.1080/10548408.2019.1612824

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fakfare, P., Cho, G., Hwang, H., and Manosuthi, N. (2021). Examining the sensory impressions, value perception, and behavioral responses of tourists: the case of floating markets in Thailand. J. Travel Tour. Mark. 38, 666–681. doi: 10.1080/10548408.2021.1985042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist 56, 218–226. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fornell, C., and Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 18, 39–50. doi: 10.1177/002224378101800104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuchs, G., and Reichel, A. (2011). An exploratory inquiry into destination risk perceptions and risk reduction strategies of first time vs. repeat visitors to a highly volatile destination. Tour. Manag. 32, 266–276. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2010.01.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gable, P. A., and Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The effect of low versus high approach-motivated positive affect on memory for peripherally versus centrally presented information. Emotion 10, 599–603. doi: 10.1037/a0018426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillath, O., Giesbrecht, B., and Shaver, P. R. (2009). Attachment, attention, and cognitive control: attachment style and performance on general attention tasks. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 45, 647–654. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grisaffe, D. B., and Nguyen, H. P. (2011). Antecedents of emotional attachment to brands. J. Bus. Res. 64, 1052–1059. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2010.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gursoy, D., Chen, J. S., and Chi, C. G. (2014). Theoretical examination of destination loyalty formation. Int. J. Contemp. Hosp. Manag. 26, 809–827. doi: 10.1108/IJCHM-12-2013-0539

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J. F. Jr, Hult, G. T. M., Ringle, C. M., and Sarstedt, M. (2021). A primer on partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM). United States: Sage publications.

Google Scholar

Henseler, J., Ringle, C. M., and Sarstedt, M. (2015). A new criterion for assessing discriminant validity in variance-based structural equation modeling. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 43, 115–135. doi: 10.1007/s11747-014-0403-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hidalgo, M. C., and Hernandez, B. (2001). Place attachment: conceptual and empirical questions. J. Environ. Psychol. 21, 273–281. doi: 10.1006/jevp.2001.0221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hosany, S., and Gilbert, D. (2010). Measuring tourists’ emotional experiences toward hedonic holiday destinations. J. Travel Res. 49, 513–526. doi: 10.1177/0047287509349267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hosany, S., and Prayag, G. (2013). Patterns of tourists' emotional responses, satisfaction, and intention to recommend. J. Bus. Res. 66, 730–737. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2011.09.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hosany, S., Prayag, G., Deesilatham, S., Cauševic, S., and Odeh, K. (2015). Measuring tourists’ emotional experiences: further validation of the destination emotion scale. J. Travel Res. 54, 482–495. doi: 10.1177/0047287514522878

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hosany, S., Prayag, G., Van Der Veen, R., Huang, S., and Deesilatham, S. (2017). Mediating effects of place attachment and satisfaction on the relationship between tourists’ emotions and intention to recommend. J. Travel Res. 56, 1079–1093. doi: 10.1177/0047287516678088

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, J. L., Wang, T. C. T., and Huang, P. Y. H. (2014). Motivations for first-time and repeat backpackers in Shanghai. Tour. Manag. Perspect. 12, 57–61. doi: 10.1016/j.tmp.2014.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hwang, S. N., Lee, C., and Chen, H. J. (2005). The relationship among tourists’ involvement, place attachment and interpretation satisfaction in Taiwan’s national parks. Tour. Manag. 26, 143–156. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2003.11.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Io, M. U. (2018). The relationships between positive emotions, place attachment, and place satisfaction in casino hotels. Int. J. Hosp. Tour. Adm. 19, 167–186. doi: 10.1080/15256480.2017.1305315

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Io, M. U., and Wan, P. Y. K. (2018). Relationships between tourism experiences and place attachment in the context of casino resorts. J. Qual. Assur. Hosp. Tour. 19, 45–65. doi: 10.1080/1528008X.2017.1314801

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Japutra, A. (2020). The relations among attachment styles, destination attachment and destination satisfaction. Curr. Issue Tour. 23, 270–275. doi: 10.1080/13683500.2019.1639640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ji, M., and King, B. (2018). Explaining the embodied hospitality experience with ZMET. Int. J. Contemp. Hosp. Manag. 30, 3442–3461. doi: 10.1108/IJCHM-10-2017-0709

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jian, Y., Lin, J., and Zhou, Z. (2021). The role of travel constraints in shaping nostalgia, destination attachment and revisit intentions and the moderating effect of prevention regulatory focus. J. Destin. Mark. Manag. 19:100516. doi: 10.1016/j.jdmm.2020.100516

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, J. (2020). The role of natural soundscape in nature-based tourism experience: an extension of the stimulus–organism–response model. Curr. Issue Tour. 3, 1–20. doi: 10.1177/0047287519872827

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kastenholz, E., Marques, C. P., and Carneiro, M. J. (2020). Place attachment through sensory-rich, emotion-generating place experiences in rural tourism. J. Destin. Mark. Manag. 17:100455. doi: 10.1016/j.jdmm.2020.100455

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J., and Fesenmaier, D. R. (2015). Measuring emotions in real time: implications for tourism experience design. J. Travel Res. 54, 419–429. doi: 10.1177/0047287514550100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krishna, A. (2012). An integrative review of sensory marketing: engaging the senses to affect perception, judgment and behavior. J. Consum. Psychol. 22, 332–351. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2011.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewicka, M. (2011). Place attachment: how far have we come in the last 40 years? J. Environ. Psychol. 31, 207–230. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X. R., Cheng, C. K., Kim, H., and Petrick, J. F. (2008). A systematic comparison of first-time and repeat visitors via a two-phase online survey. Tour. Manag. 29, 278–293. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2007.03.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindstrom, M. (2005). Broad sensory branding. J. Prod. Brand. Manag. 14, 84–87. doi: 10.1108/10610420510592554

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Wang, S., and Jie, X. (2019). Experiential honeymoon tourism study from perspective of embodied cognition. In2019 5th international conference on information management (ICIM) (pp. 327–331). IEEE.

Google Scholar

Loureiro, S. M. C. (2014). The role of the rural tourism experience economy in place attachment and behavioral intentions. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 40, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhm.2014.02.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lv, X., and McCabe, S. (2020). Expanding theory of tourists’ destination loyalty: the role of sensory impressions. Tour. Manag. 77:104026. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2019.104026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lv, X., and Wu, A. (2021). The role of extraordinary sensory experiences in shaping destination brand love: an empirical study. J. Travel Tour. Mark. 38, 179–193. doi: 10.1080/10548408.2021.1889447

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, J., Gao, J., Scott, N., and Ding, P. (2019). Customer delight from theme park experiences: the antecedents of delight based on cognitive appraisal theory. Ann. Tour. Res. 42, 359–381. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2013.02.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manosuthi, N., Lee, J. S., and Han, H. (2021). Causal-predictive model of customer lifetime/influence value: mediating roles of memorable experiences and customer engagement in hotels and airlines. J. Travel Tour. Mark. 38, 461–477. doi: 10.1080/10548408.2021.1940422

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Memon, M. A., Ting, H., Cheah, J. H., Thurasamy, R., Chuah, F., and Cham, T. H. (2020). Sample size for survey research: review and recommendations. J. Applied Structural Equation Modeling 4, 1–20. doi: 10.47263/JASEM.4(2)01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michael, I., Ramsoy, T., Stephens, M., and Kotsi, F. (2019). A study of unconscious emotional and cognitive responses to tourism images using a neuroscience method. J. Islamic Marketing 10, 543–564. doi: 10.1108/JIMA-09-2017-0098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mlozi, S., and Pesämaa, O. (2013). Adventure tourist destination choice in Tanzania. Curr. Issue Tour. 16, 63–95. doi: 10.1080/13683500.2011.647807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mlozi, S., Pesämaa, O., Haahti, A., and Salunke, S. (2012). Determinants of place identity and dependence: the case of international tourists in Tanzania. Tourism Culture & Commun. 12, 97–114. doi: 10.3727/109830413X13575858951167

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morais, D. B., and Lin, C. H. (2010). Why do first-time and repeat visitors patronize a destination? J. Travel Tour. Mark. 27, 193–210. doi: 10.1080/10548401003590443

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pandey, A., and Sahu, R. (2020). Modeling the relationship between service quality, destination attachment and eWOM intention in heritage tourism. Int. J. Tourism Cities 8, 120–134. doi: 10.1108/IJTC-08-2019-0125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plass, J. L., and Kalyuga, S. (2019). Four ways of considering emotion in cognitive load theory. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 31, 339–359. doi: 10.1007/s10648-019-09473-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prayag, G., and Lee, C. (2019). Tourist motivation and place attachment: the mediating effects of service interactions with hotel employees. J. Travel Tour. Mark. 36, 90–106. doi: 10.1080/10548408.2018.1494087

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prayag, G., and Ryan, C. (2012). Antecedents of tourists’ loyalty to Mauritius: the role and influence of destination image, place attachment, personal involvement, and satisfaction. J. Travel Res. 51, 342–356. doi: 10.1177/0047287511410321

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rakić, T., and Chambers, D. (2012). Rethinking the consumption of places. Ann. Tour. Res. 39, 1612–1633. doi: 10.1016/j.annals.2011.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramkissoon, H. (2016). Place satisfaction, place attachment and quality of life: development of a conceptual framework for island destinations. Sustainable Island Tourism: Competitiveness and quality of life 2, 106–116. doi: 10.1079/9781780645421.0106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riper, C. J., Yoon, J. I., Kyle, G. T., Wallen, K. E., Landon, A. C., and Raymond, C. (2019). The antecedents of place attachment in the context of an Australian national park. J. Environ. Psychol. 61, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivera, D. E., Fa, M. C., and Villar, A. S. (2019). Delightful tourism experiences: a cognitive or affective matter? Tour. Manag. Perspect. 32:100569. doi: 10.1016/j.tmp.2019.100569

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santos, V., Caldeira, A., Santos, E., Oliveira, S., and Ramos, P. (2019). Wine tourism experience in the Tejo region: the influence of sensory impressions on post-visit behaviour intentions. Int. J. Marketing, Communication and New Media 5, 1–16. doi: 10.1108/IJEBR-07-2019-0436

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scannell, L., and Gifford, R. (2010). Defining place attachment: a tripartite organizing framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seamon, D. (1980). Body-subject, time-space routines, and place-ballets. The human experience of space and place 148, 65–72.

Google Scholar

Shanka, T., and Taylor, R. (2004). Discriminating factors of first-time and repeat visitors to wine festivals. Curr. Issue Tour. 7, 134–145. doi: 10.1080/13683500408667976

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skavronskaya, L., Scott, N., Moyle, B., Le, D., Hadinejad, A., Zhang, R., et al. (2017). Cognitive psychology and tourism research: state of the art. Tour. Rev. 4, 55–72. doi: 10.1108/TR-03-2017-0041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sthapit, E. (2019). Memories of gastronomic experiences, savoured positive emotions and savouring processes. Scand. J. Hosp. Tour. 19, 115–139. doi: 10.1080/15022250.2017.1402702

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tasci, A. D., Uslu, A., Stylidis, D., and Woosnam, K. M. (2022). Place-oriented or people-oriented concepts for destination loyalty: destination image and place attachment versus perceived distances and emotional solidarity. J. Travel Res. 61, 430–453. doi: 10.1177/0047287520982377

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsai, C. T., Hsu, H., and Chen, C. C. (2020). An examination of experiential quality, nostalgia, place attachment and behavioral intentions of hospitality customers. J. Hosp. Mark. Manag. 29, 869–885. doi: 10.1080/19368623.2020.1706684

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuan, Y. F. (1975). Place: an experiential perspective. Geogr. Rev. 65, 151–165. doi: 10.2307/213970

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Usakli, A., Kucukergin, K. G., Shi, D., and Okumus, F. (2022). Does self-congruity or functional congruity better predict destination attachment? A higher-order structural model. J. Destin. Mark. Manag. 23:100686. doi: 10.1016/j.jdmm.2021.100686

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vada, S., Prentice, C., and Hsiao, A. (2019). The influence of tourism experience and well-being on place attachment. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 47, 322–330. doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2018.12.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaske, J. J., and Kobrin, K. C. (2001). Place attachment and environmentally responsible behavior. J. Environ. Educ. 32, 16–21. doi: 10.1080/00958960109598658

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walther-Hansen, M. (2020). Making sense of recordings: How cognitive processing of recorded sound works. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Wen, H., and Leung, X. Y. (2021). Virtual wine tours and wine tasting: the influence of offline and online embodiment integration on wine purchase decisions. Tour. Manag. 83:104250. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2020.104250

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, D. R., and Vaske, J. J. (2003). The measurement of place attachment: validity and generalizability of a psychometric approach. For. Sci. 49, 830–840. doi: 10.1093/forestscience/49.6.830

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiong, J., Hashim, N. H., and Murphy, J. (2015). Multisensory image as a component of destination image. Tour. Manag. Perspect. 14, 34–41. doi: 10.1016/j.tmp.2015.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, Z., and Zhang, J. (2016). Antecedents and consequences of place attachment: a comparison of Chinese and Western urban tourists in Hangzhou, China. J. Destin. Mark. Manag. 5, 86–96. doi: 10.1016/j.jdmm.2015.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, Z., Zhang, H., Zhang, C., Xu, M., and Dong, N. (2019). Exploring the role of emotion in the relationship between museum image and tourists’ behavioral intention: the case of three museums in Xi’an. Sustain. For. 11:559. doi: 10.3390/su11030559

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, N., and Halpenny, E. A. (2019). Tourists' savoring of positive emotions and place attachment formation: a conceptual paper. Tour. Geogr. 5843, 1–21.

Google Scholar

Yang, F., Huang, A., and Huang, J. (2021). Influence of sensory experiences on tourists' emotions, destination memories, and loyalty. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 49, 1–13. doi: 10.2224/sbp.10010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yi, X., Fu, X., Jin, W., and Okumus, F. (2018). Constructing a model of exhibition attachment: motivation, attachment, and loyalty. Tour. Manag. 65, 224–236. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2017.10.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yolal, M., Chi, C. G. Q., and Pesämaa, O. (2017). Examine destination loyalty of first-time and repeat visitors at all-inclusive resorts. Int. J. Contemp. Hosp. Manag. 29, 1834–1853. doi: 10.1108/IJCHM-06-2015-0293

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuksel, A., Yuksel, F., and Bilim, Y. (2010). Destination attachment: effects on customer satisfaction and cognitive, affective and conative loyalty. Tour. Manag. 31, 274–284. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2009.03.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, D., Ritchie, B. W., Benckendorff, P. J., and Bao, J. (2019). The role of cognitive appraisal, emotion and commitment in affecting resident support toward tourism performing arts development. J. Sustain. Tour. 27, 1725–1744. doi: 10.1080/09669582.2019.1662029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: multisensory impressions, embodied theory, dual-system theory, emotional responses, cognitive responses, destination attachment

Citation: Dong Y and Qu Y (2022) The mechanism of body–mind integration in the formation of destination attachment: A comparison of first-time and repeat tourists. Front. Psychol. 13:1010589. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1010589

Received: 03 August 2022; Accepted: 20 October 2022;
Published: 10 November 2022.

Edited by:

Xinyuan Zhao, Macau University of Science and Technology, Macao SAR, China

Reviewed by:

I. Gusti Bagus Rai Utama, Dhyana Pura University, Indonesia
Ossi Pesämaa, Luleå University of Technology, Sweden

Copyright © 2022 Dong and Qu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ying Qu, rosemaryquying@126.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.