Skip to main content

EMPIRICAL STUDY article

Front. Psychol., 11 October 2022
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Teaching and Learning in Higher Education: The Role of Emotion and Cognition View all 35 articles

Unpacking the interaction between foreign language learners’ emotion, cognition, and activity in the flipped classroom in higher education: A perezhivanie perspective

  • 1Department of World Languages, Dalian University of Foreign Languages, Dalian, China
  • 2Ganzhou Middle School, Ganzhou, China
  • 3Foreign Language College, Hainan Normal University, Haikou, China

Few studies have investigated learners’ emotional experiences and the interactions between emotion, cognition, and activity in the flipped foreign language classroom (i.e., a mixed teaching mode that combines in-class teaching and off-class self-directed learning). This study, from the perspective of perezhivanie (a concept from sociocultural theory), addressed these research gaps by exercising a longitudinal narrative study on a total of 32 Chinese-as-the-first-language university students of English who attended a 15-week English course with this teaching design. Among them, eight focal students were randomly selected for further evidence of the characteristics of the interactions between emotion, cognition, and activity. The results showed that the participants experienced more of positive emotions than negative emotions in the flipped classroom (FC) context, which supports the efficacy of the pedagogy. But most importantly, complex interactions between emotion, cognition, and activity were revealed. Generally, (negative) positive emotion, cognition, and activity were interconnected; however, what is also evident is that learners’ emotions either promoted or inhibited their cognitive functions, and positive and negative emotions did not necessarily correspond to positive and negative activities, respectively. This is due to the presence of dynamic, developmental, and historical sociocultural mediators in learners perezhivanija, be it teacher, peers, technology, teaching materials, teaching activities in an FC, or the learners’ previous English learning anecdotes, etc.

Introduction

All the learning processes unify cognition and emotion including second-language learning. However, researchers in the field of second-language acquisition (SLA) have primarily focused on learners’ cognitive performance, leaving their emotional experiences less explored (Swain, 2013; Dewaele and Li, 2018). Among the limited number of research addressing learners’ emotions in the field, a large majority of them targeted only a single emotion, such as anxiety, enjoyment, burnout, and boredom (Dewaele and MacIntyre, 2014; Jin and Zhang, 2019, 2021; Derakhshan et al., 2021; Li, 2022), despite the facts that second-language learning is a rather complex process whereby many positive and negative emotions can be experienced even in the same time window and that it is the positive or negative emotionality that motivates or demotivates learners over a long learning trajectory. Methodologically, research on emotions has heavily relied on using Likert-type scales. Albeit warranting a large dataset (Jiang and Dewaele, 2019; Li, 2021, 2022), the downsides of this research paradigm are the dichotomy of emotions into either positive or negative (Wang et al., 2021), and only a single form of emotional manifestation is attended to. Therefore, qualitative data are needed with its advantages of including the characteristics of typicality, complexity, and integrity, although the sample size is small.

In addition, though FC teaching as an alternative to replace teacher-led instruction in varying educational settings (van Alten et al., 2019), particularly in the second-language (L2) domain (Mehring and Leis, 2018), has advantages such as more frequent inter-student interactions before and after class (Roehl et al., 2013), flexibility (Buechler et al., 2014), and increased student engagement (Chetcuti et al., 2014; Malik et al., 2018), it also imposes new challenges on teachers and students alike. For example, the use of information technology in FC teaching can make learners feel difficult to concentrate (Qin et al., 2022). They also face greater pressure to engage in more interactive activities in the in-person part of the FC teaching (Malik et al., 2018). All new challenges are likely to influence foreign language learners’ emotions; consequently, it is necessary to conduct a more in-depth discussion on learners’ emotional experiences and influencing factors of learners’ emotion in FC teaching of foreign languages.

By far, scarce studies have explored how learners’ emotions form and develop in the FC from a perezhivanie (unity of emotion, cognition, and social activities from SCT) perspective to understand how learners interpret and emotionally relate to the teaching environment, which may shed some light on relevant studies. Thus, this study aims to explore the developmental process of learners’ emotions in the context of the FC through the lens of perezhivanie. To achieve this goal, we adopted a multi-dimensional narrative approach (Patton, 1990) to triangulate the findings by deploying written narratives at two time points (at the beginning and end of the semester), as well as on a longitudinal weekly basis lasting for 15 weeks. The synchronic and diachronic narrative data provided evidence not only on learners’ emotional states and its dynamic changes but also on the dialectical interactions between learners’ emotion, cognition, and activities in the FC context.

Literature review

This section provides interpretations of related concepts such as sociocultural theory, perezhivanie and drama, as well as a brief overview of existing research on learner emotion.

Sociocultural theory

Sociocultural theory (SCT) rooted in cultural-historical psychology established by Vygotsky and his colleagues (Lantolf and Thorne, 2006). Although SCT literally includes “social” and “cultural,” it is a theory of neither society nor culture, but one of mind that explains the laws of human beings’ mental functioning developed from social relationships and cultural artifacts (Qin et al., 2019; Qin, 2021, 2022; Qin and Ren, 2021). The philosophical origins of SCT are from the 18th and 19th centuries by Germany philosophers Kant’s and Hegel’s dialectics, as well as Marx and Engels’ works on sociology and economics critically drawing on Feuerbach’s tenets of materialism (Lantolf and Thorne, 2006).

Traditional psychology theories are keen to separate cognition from emotion (Vygotsky, 1987). However, SCT advocates their integrated and interactive contributions to the development of mental functioning by proposing the concept of perezhivanie. Originating from the Russian word perezhivat, perezhivanie refers to “how an individual is aware of, interprets, and affectively relates to a certain event” (Vygotsky, 1994, p. 341). It continuously develops in a dynamic manner since a very young age as individuals gain lived experiences in the real world. Perezhivanie thus implies the effect of the immediate and past sociocultural environment on individuals, more specifically social relationships and social activities (Vygotsky, 1994). Having said that, it must be noted that not all social relationships and social activities influence individuals’ development according to SCT but only those “dramatic” ones (also known as dramas) that cause one’s internal emotional conflicts and create critical perezhivaniya (Lantolf and Swain, 2019). Research from an SCT perspective typically uses critical perezhivaniya (plural form, or perezhivanija) as units of analysis, which automatically attends to tripartite interactions among emotion, cognition, and social environment (Roth and Jornet, 2013) and combines the past, the present, and the future (Veresov, 2017).

Perezhivanie

Perezhivanie refers to an individual’s lived or emotional experience, which is subjective thinking and feeling of the environment, that is, “how the individual is aware of, interprets, and affectively relates to a certain event” (Vygotsky, 1994, p. 341). As a unit of analysis, perezhivanie represents the dialectical unity of emotion and cognition. An individual’s perezhivanie exerts effects on the individual’s social activity and eventually builds the trajectory of development (Vygotsky, 1987; Golombek and Doran, 2014). Therefore, perezhivanie covers both objective environmental characteristics and subjective personal characteristics. The difference between experience and perezhivanie is that the former is a complete discrete event that can be divided according to time and be recalled through memories, while the latter is the ongoing conversion of social activities from the social to the individual. It is a dynamic and continuously updated unit that involves the interaction between situations, cognition, and emotions relative to the individual (Roth and Jornet, 2013). Vygotsky (1998) believes that the concept of perezhivanie can be used to analyze the influence of the social and cultural environment on the process of individual development. In short, individuals experience the current environment through their past perezhivanija and then form their new perezhivanija, and the current perezhivanija will have an impact on how individuals experience the new environment in future.

Drama

Vygotsky (1997) believed that researchers need to trace the individual history, that is, their past experience. Veresov (2017, p. 59) pointed out that social relations in the individual’s past experience would bring individual development, and not every social relation can be developed into individual psychological functions but only those “dramatic” social relations can. That is to say, only those social relations that influence the individual’s emotions can be used as the source of development (Lantolf and Swain, 2019). In a word, dramas are those events that cause the individual’s internal emotional conflicts, which is in line with the genetic law. The relationship between drama, perezhivanie, and development is that drama which brings rich emotions forms an individual’s critical perezhivanie, thus influencing the individual’s trajectory of development (Fleer et al., 2017). From a perezhivanie perspective, despite the same environment, some would feel it dramatic, thus gaining the opportunity to develop, while others may not feel the environment special to personal development.

Learners’ emotion in sociocultural theory

Previous learners’ emotion research from psychological perspectives would focus on specific types of emotion and attempt to explore the relation between emotional variables and other quantifiable variables (Khajavy et al., 2018; Li et al., 2020; Pawlak et al., 2020). However, emotion research from the SCT perspective does not delve into the constituent variables of emotion but records and describes emotional experience, traces the process of emotional development, and explores the interaction between learners and the sociocultural environment from a holistic view. For example, Swain et al. (2015) used narrative interviews to build a multilingual learning history of a participant, Grace and constructed a developmental trajectory by understanding her lived emotional experience (perezhivanie). Sampson (2020) followed 47 Japanese first-year college students and asked them to write down their feelings during English class every week. After quantifying the qualitative data, it was found that the sources of positive and negative emotions are mainly classroom activities, classmates, identity, teacher, and lesson. This kind of diachronic tracking of emotions and feelings of a group can record learners’ emotional states and sources of fluctuations and have important reference value for emotion research in SLA.

Learners’ emotion studies in flipped classroom

An FC is commonly referred to as the process of flipping what is traditionally carried out in the classroom to an independent homework activity before class. As such, lessons involve problem-solving and higher order thinking tasks, which are traditionally assigned to subsequent homework activities (Mehring, 2016, 2018; Låg and Saele, 2019). Vitta and Al-Hoorie (2020) defined FC as “…involves presentation of new content to learners to be independently studied before class, and then class time is devoted to reinforcing and engaging with the ‘flipped’ content.” In other words, in the FC, the in-person time aims to help students solve problems and to engage students in collaborative and hands-on activities (Bergmann and Sams, 2014; Mehring, 2018). The online time, an extension of in-class learning, makes use of digital resources to support students’ learning linguistic knowledge and intercultural knowledge (Hung, 2015, 2017; Chen Hsieh et al., 2017). In the traditional classroom, teachers inculcate knowledge in students, who often learn in a passive way. Instead, within the pedagogical framework of the FC, teachers assist in and guide learners through their learning process and thus allow greater learner autonomy (Evseeva and Solozhenko, 2015; Adnan, 2017).

The FC indeed can provoke both positive and negative learners’ emotions; however, so far, little research in EFL teaching has focused on both dimensions of emotions. Only Kruk and Pawlak (2022) discussed this systematically in second-language virtual classrooms, but mainly from a dualistic perspective, dividing positive and negative emotions into two non-interfering dimensions and only described characteristics of each, respectively. In the existing research on both sides of learners’ emotions in the FC, most of it has been teaching and learning in science classrooms. For example, Jeong et al. (2016) conducted a study in a general science course with sophomores in a university in Spain and concluded that students’ perception toward an FC included both negative (boredom and fear) and positive (fun and enthusiasm) emotions. González-Gómez et al. (2017) performed a comparative study between the traditional classroom (TC) and FC in a general science classroom with undergraduate students and concluded that in contrast to the TC, the FC aroused more of positive emotions than negative ones. However, in EFL classrooms, there are more studies focusing on one dimension of learners’ emotion in the FC, be it positive or negative. For example, Pan et al. (2022) inspected the effect of Massive Open Online Course (MOOC) and FC on EFL learners’ foreign language-speaking anxiety and attitude toward English learning and concluded that participants in both groups had positive attitudes toward technological-based instructional environments. Gok et al. (2021), however, did not find the FC triggered significantly higher anxiety, suggesting the complexity of the antecedents of emotions, and many factors might moderate the effect of pedagogical approaches on learners’ emotions.

Another strand of research focused on the effect of participating in classroom activities on emotions in the FC context. For example, Gok et al. (2021) found that pre-class preparation and in-class group work could alleviate learners’ anxiety. Abdullah et al. (2020) found that participating in the well-designed in-class and out-of-class activities provided students many opportunities to improve their self-confidence and significantly reduced their anxiety due to a creative, safe, comfortable, and encouraging learning environment forged by the teaching approach. Malik et al. (2018) evaluated the effectiveness of the FC approach by examining the relationship between the FC and student engagement at physical, behavioral, and emotional levels. The findings indicate that the FC improves learners’ physical and cognitive engagement; however, no improvement in their emotional engagement was observed.

Above all, we note that from an SCT perspective, the interaction between emotion, cognition, and activity should be examined if deeper understanding of learners’ emotion in the FC is to be achieved. More importantly, the cognitive, affective, and social interactions of individuals should be integrated as a whole. Although studies on neurosciences have proved the link and inseparability between cognition, emotion, and human behaviors due to mental trauma (Sarr-Jansman and Rowberry, 2018; Malaei et al., 2022; Quadt et al., 2022), and researchers from education (Yob, 1997) and psychology (Greenberg and Safran, 1987; Eich and Schooler, 2000; Schnitzspahn and Phillips, 2016; Belkhir, 2020) also noted the gap, only a few studies found this trend in SLA. However, most previous studies discussing the three components in one study in SLA were on second-language teacher education. For example, Tasker et al. (2010), Golombek and Doran (2014), Johnson and Worden (2014), Golombek (2015), Johnson and Golombek (2016), Johnson (2018, 2021), Agnoletto et al. (2021), discussed novice language teachers’ development due to emotional and cognitive dissonance during the practices of learning to teach. Even fewer studies focused on the interaction between the second-language learner’s emotion, cognition, and activities in a holistic way (unity of individuals and society). Only Moeller (2021) discussed about the significance of integrating feeling and thinking to optimize language learning, claiming that if positive emotions are activated, learners would put more efforts to language learning and show greater sense of efficacy. However, by far, no empirical studies on the interaction of the three components of perezhivanie have been found implemented among second-language learners in the FC.

In line with the previous discussion, the concept of perezhivanie views individuals’ emotions, cognitions, and activities (social activities engaged in) as inseparable and also as a unity of the dialectical relationship between the three (Swain, 2013; Lantolf and Swain, 2019). Furthermore, we note that existing research rarely describes the dynamic changes over time in learners’ emotions in the FC. Therefore, we argue that in this context, there is a need for a longitudinal study on learners’ emotions from a perezhivanie perspective. Based on this argument, the present study is guided by the following questions:

(1) What are learners’ emotional experiences in the context of the FC?

(2) In what ways are learners’ emotion, cognition, and activity combined in the context of the FC?

(3) What are the dramas (unit of analysis for perezhivanie) that cause the interaction between emotion, cognition, and activity?

Methodology

Context

The study was carried out in an EFL lesson using an FC approach at a Northeastern China university. The teacher for this lesson was a novice teacher with no prior experience in foreign language teaching in higher education. The classes for this lesson lasted 17 weeks. Review and final examination were conducted in weeks 16 and 17, and we only collected data from week 1 to week 15. In each week, students had one face-to-face session (90 mins), either integrated or listening and speaking class (two modules of EFL), supplemented by self-directed online learning through a platform called Unipus (90 mins). In addition, the students could communicate with the teacher through WeChat, a real-time communication tool like Facebook Messenger and email, if they had any questions.

Participants

The total participants were 32 sophomore undergraduates in semester 4, majoring in a foreign language other than English, but all took English as a foreign language course at a Northeast China university. There were 28 female and four male students, with an average age of 19.6 (SD = 1.1) years, who consented to participate in a written narrative task in weeks 1 and 15, respectively (weeks 16 and 17 were not included in the research because they were the time for review and final examination). Among them, eight students were randomly extracted from low-, medium-, and high-English level subgroups of the total sample achieved by allotting the 32 students to the three groups in terms of their scores in a national-level English examination. These focal students, representing different foreign language backgrounds and different levels of English proficiency, additionally accomplished narrative journals on a weekly basis over 15 weeks of the semester. We present the narrative diary data of the eight focal students after their consent. Table 1 reports in details the demographic information of eight focal students who consented the narratives to be included in the study anonymously.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participants’ demographic information.

Instruments and procedures

Narrative is a way of using language and other signs (images, gestures, etc.) to produce a coherent account that posits an interconnection between the past, the present, and the future events (Dick et al., 2017). Emotion and cognition are both developed from social activities (Gorbatkov, 2002; Golombek and Doran, 2014); therefore, cognition, emotion, and activity are inevitably unified in their narratives. Narrative analysis is interested in broader interpretive frameworks used by both the participant and the researcher to make sense of particular incidents in the individual’s lives, which is compatible to the “drama,” unit of analysis for perezhivanie, meaning the “dramatic social relations or events” that caused individual development (Fleer et al., 2017, p. 59). Researchers use narrative analysis to understand how participants construct stories from their own personal experiences, which contain the interaction of emotion, cognition, and activity, suiting for the current study. Johnson and Golombek (2002, p. 4) proposed that “the conceptualization of narrative inquiry in Dewey’s (1916; 1920; 1933) educational philosophy, which, at its core, argues that we are all knowers who reflect on experience, confront the unknown, make sense of it, and take actions.

A written narrative was adopted in this study. The total of 32 participants performed written narrative tasks in their native language, Chinese (minimally 200 words), in weeks 1 and 15, respectively. In week 1, they reported their language learning experiences prior to the current semester, including pre-college experiences. In week 15, they reported their emotional experiences in in-class learning, including the emotions experienced at the time points of presenting, answering questions, discussing, participating in other activities, and independent learning (watching online courses, etc.). In other words, the purposes of the narrative study on the participants were to understand their English learning experiences prior to, at the beginning of, and during FC learning, respectively (question 1). In total, 17,783 Chinese characters were collected.

To investigate the interaction between emotion, cognition, and activity (question 2) and locate the dramas that caused the change of perezhivanija or interaction of the three elements (question 3) in the FC context, weekly written narrative reports were collected from eight focal participants who consented to submit their reports. The contents pertained to pre-class online learning experiences, in-class activities, and after-class activities (see Table 2). In total, 29,319 Chinese characters were collected from this weekly narrative task.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Aspects to be covered in eight focal students’ written narratives.

Data analysis

Data analysis was conducted on NVivo 12 plus. The purposes were to elicit the participants’ emotional experiences in the FC context and the co-concurrences of emotion, cognition, and activities (or actions taken). For the former, we not only coded the emotions explicitly expressed by the participants in their self-narratives but also those hidden in the lines. For the latter, we adopted perezhivanie as the unit of analysis, that is, we identified the episodes in the participants’ self-narratives that contain dramas causing the interaction of emotion (E), cognition (C), and activity (A) and tried to make sense of the ways in which they co-existed. The first and the second authors independently analyzed the data. They achieved a high inter-rater consensus (90%). The two researchers discussed to solve the disagreements that occurred between them.

As shown in Table 3, we coded narrative data based on the three compositional elements of perezhivanie—emotion, cognition, and activity. As perezhivanie is historical and developmental as discussed before, we also coded learners’ language learning dramas or dramatic events that caused learners’ perezhivanija to change, either in the history (before college), prior to the term of the current study, or during the process of the term (in college). To get a clearer picture of FC teaching, we also traced those dramas that happened both in face-to-face classes, where the teacher presented in-person, and outside of the classroom where learners self-guided their learning with the help of assigned online digital resources, where the teacher presented through online support. In addition, we intended to clarify that the coding of emotion, cognition, and activities, respectively, are specifically justified, as given in the following text.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Coding scheme of the analysis of leaners’ written narratives.

Coding of learners’ emotions toward flipped classroom

As discussed earlier regarding learners’ emotion from the SCT perspective, the researchers do not specify positive or negative emotions. Therefore, for learners’ emotion in this study, we coded all kinds that emerged out of the data, including positive emotions and negative emotions (for details, see Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Coding scheme of varieties of learners’ emotions emerged in narrative data.

Coding of learner cognition toward flipped classroom

Belkhir (2020, p. 3) stated that the term “cognition” refers to “the process by which knowledge and understanding are developed in the mind.” The adjectival form “cognitive” means “connected with thinking or conscious mental processes.” Cognitive psychologist Matlin (2005, p. 2) defined “cognition” as a mental activity with various cognitive processes. In her view, cognition includes a wide range of mental processes, such as perception, memory, imagery, language, problem-solving, reasoning, and decision-making. She further described the cognitive approach as a theoretical stance that focuses mostly on people’s knowledge and their mental processes. However, for (L2) development from the SCT perspective, social interaction is not just a facilitative mechanism; cognition itself is social (Lantolf and Thorne, 2007). Speech (e.g., the written narrative in the present study), which is, of course, central in SCT, is regarded as an effective tool for thought and action during interaction with either the self and the others (Lantolf and Thorne, 2006). According to Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive development, cognitive abilities are acquired through social instruction and construction, and therefore, learners need to engage in social interactions for L2 development (Vygotsky, 1978). Therefore, in this study, we hold that how the learner perceives a teaching activity or the design of a task in the FC (which is certainly a social activity) renders the “teacher–task–learner” interaction. If a learner recorded his or her perceptions of FC in the narrative (a form of speech), it was regarded as the learner’s cognition toward FC, since perception reflects the individual’s thinking (mental process) of an activity or an event (a social interaction) as discussed above.

Coding of learners’ activities in flipped classroom

The coding of learners’ activities is only concerned with those activities that the learners participated in and recorded in the narratives, including in and out of the in-person classes both online and offline.

Results

Learners’ emotion characteristics and changes in flipped classroom

This section is composed of three subsections that present the participants’ emotional profile at the beginning of and during FC teaching as well as the interaction between emotion, cognition, and activity with the participants’ self-narrative excerpts. (The excerpts are italicized, and pseudonyms are used for the participants).

Emotional profile at the beginning of flipped classroom teaching

The participants reported experiencing 13 emotions in online and offline mixed FC classes at the beginning of the semester, including five positive-valence emotions and eight negative-valence emotions (see Figures 1, 2 for details). Positive emotions accounted for 35% of all the emotions reported and negative emotions 65%. Among the positive emotions, enjoyment was dominant and was mentioned in 25% of the entries; among the negative emotions, nervousness (22% of the entries), followed by boredom (18%) and anxiety (11%).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Overall positive emotions from pre- and post-written narratives by the 32 students.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Overall negative emotions from pre- and post-written narratives by the 32 students.

Emotional experiences in the process of flipped classroom teaching

The participants reported a larger portion of positive emotions than negative emotions during the process of FC teaching, although the total number of positive and negative emotions remained unchanged, in comparison to the beginning of this teaching mode (see Figures 1, 2). The dominant positive emotions included enjoyment, gratitude, interest, and excitement; negative emotions included fear, nervousness, embarrassment, sense of difficulty, and boredom. It is worth noticing that boredom decreased from 18% to 10% after FC pedagogy was adopted. Nervousness and anxiety occurred less frequently (see Figure 2). Many new positive emotions like gratitude, excitement, surprise, self-confidence, and sense of fulfillment appeared by the end of the research semester, proving the facilitative function of the FC in positivizing learners’ emotions.

The unity of learners’ emotion, cognition, and activity

The participants’ self-narratives highlight the unity of learners’ emotion, cognition, and activity, which features both linearity and non-linearity. One way of their combination is that negative emotions led to negative cognition to further negative behaviors. As the case of Vicky, her lower confidence in listening and speaking abilities (cognition) led to her denial of the usefulness of classroom presentation (emotion). Eventually she refused to do the presentation part in the classroom (activity)1 :

As my personal speaking and listening are not very good (C), I may not like the presentation part very much (E), and I don’t know if this thing improves learning (C). I only participated in power point file editing (A). (Excerpt from Vicky’s narrative in week 1).

Another student, Linda, felt difficulty in learning English well, so she did not take the online course seriously and did not believe in the value of the online lesson. Thus, negative emotion (I would not take online classes very seriously) triggered both negative cognition (I don’t think online lessons can help much for me) and activity (Instead of doing homework on the Internet, I prefer paper) at the same time. In what follows, both negative cognition and negative emotion caused negative behavior. For example, Linda did not understand the words taught by her teacher and felt troublesome to find out their meaning in the dictionary, so she did not pay attention to the online class:

I am very busy with my major studies (C). I want to learn English very well, but I feel so difficult to do it well (C). I would not take online classes very seriously (E). And, I don’t think online lessons can help much for me (C). Instead of doing homework on the Internet, I prefer paper (A). It is very convenient to mark it (C). Sometimes when I don’t understand the words taught by online teachers and it is troublesome to look it up in the dictionary (E), I would not watch online classes carefully and seriously (E). In general, I really don’t like online classes (E). (Excerpt from Linda’s narrative in week 15).

Jenny’s data showed that negative emotions could bring positive actions (or activities participated in) and cognition. She felt anxious because of a heavy workload with her major and did not feel satisfactory with her course grades. Both anxiety (emotion) and dissatisfaction (cognition) led to her increased self-planning behaviors (activity) and positive self-concept of planning ability (cognition):

Due to the heavy work from my major language courses, there may be delays in time plan (C). Before I took English class I concentrated on a lot of things, so I got anxious sometimes (E). After studying English this semester, I feel that I have improved a lot in terms of autonomous learning (C). When I was preparing for the CET 4 exam last semester, I didn’t really study down to earth because I was too ambitious to complete too many tasks at the same time (E). The final grade was not very satisfactory (C). This semester I was preparing for the CET 6 exam, I have set up a daily plan according to my own situation, and memorized words carefully every day (A). Although I don’t know whether my final grades will satisfy me, my self-planning ability has been improved (C). (Excerpt from Jenny’s narrative in week 15).

There was evidence showing that positive actions contribute to positive emotions and positive cognition. The student, Ryan, answered her teacher’s question well (activity) and was praised by the teacher, leading to her happiness (emotion) and greater engagement (activity) in the English class. Eventually, she believed that even if she would pass the important English test of CET 6 for college students in China and was determined to make more efforts to learn the language (cognition).

In a Listening and Speaking class, I answered a question very well (A). The teacher said ‘very good’, which made me very excited and happy for the whole class (E). When I felt the homework is boring (E), the teacher still examined our homework with passion and gave us feedback on WeChat group on time (A). I was often ashamed of being called to answer questions (E) because I didn’t preview the contents of the class in time before class (A), but the teacher kept waiting for my reply, reminded me and encourage me (A). So I am willing to learning English and take classes seriously (E). Even if I pass the CET 6, English learning is not over (C). (Excerpt from Ryan’s narrative in week 15).

There was a non-linear flow from positive cognition to positive cognition through positive action and positive emotion. In the case of Ryan, she recognized the teacher’s efforts in preparing the course (activity) and thus took each lesson carefully and was willing to complete homework (emotion). In the end, she thought doing homework was a great training for listening and writing (cognition).

I understand teacher took a lot of effort in preparing lessons (A), so I take each class very attentively (E). The teacher made me discover the joy of learning English (E). I am willing to complete my homework carefully after class (E). The seemingly boring transcribing homework (E) is actually a great training for listening and writing (C). (Excerpt from Ryan’s narrative in week 15).

Dramas accounted for the interaction between cognition, emotion, and activities in flipped classroom teaching

As discussed before, drama is the unit of analysis of a learner’s perezhivanie, which refers to “dramatic events” that cause conflicts or change of the learner’s perezhivanie—unity of cognition, emotion, and activities. Digging into the data, we found some typical dramas, mentioned later, some of which are common among students, and others are of individual characteristics.

Dramas of classroom activities

Among a variety of interactive activities, such as “textual structure analysis,” “sentence meaning induction,” and “passage theme extraction,” the participants particularly welcomed those which offered them a chance to be exposed to novel out-of-textbook knowledge, for example, watching movies and book recommendation. All the students perceive these activities helpful and beneficial to English learning (cognition, dramas 1 and 2). The underlined lines in the excerpt of drama 1, “learning English by watching an excerpt from the classic movie The Devil Wears Prada” attracted more attention from the learners and added more fun, and their perezhivanija or the interaction between activities, emotion, and cognition surely improved their learning motivation. In addition, drama 2 is the classroom activity of “recommending an interesting book.” Jim was extremely interested and serious in this activity. His perezhivanie even extended to future improvement of the presentation skills in other courses. Certainly, drama 2 is a very crucial social event in Jim’s learning experience.

Drama 12 :

In face-to-face classes, there are many interactions between the teacher and students (A), which can mobilize my attention (C). One of the most impressive scenes in this semester is learning English by watching an excerpt from the classic movie ‘The Devil Wears Prada’ (A-Drama). I feel that it adds some fun (E) to combine the boring content of the lesson with the movie clips. (Excerpt from Jenny’ narrative in week 15, June 29, 2021).

Drama 2:

When it came to the lesson about reading, the teacher asked the whole class to bring a book they like, write down the reasons to recommend it in English (A-Drama), and then the whole class randomly exchanged their books (A), which is very interesting (E). The teacher also participated and got my book. I was so happy (E). So, when preparing for the presentation, I was quite serious about it (A). It was a relatively smooth presentation for the first time, and I didn’t get stuck (A). Although there will be no English classes in the future, and there will be no English presentations, there will still be presentations in other courses, so it is better to prepare well (C). (Excerpt from Jim’s narrative in week 15, June 29, 2021).

Dramas of the teacher contribution

The teacher plays a crucial role in determining learners’ emotional experiences. By selecting well-designed tasks and interesting learning materials, teachers can highly promote their students’ positive emotions. In addition, their emotional support to students, including positive feedback following students’ performance, recognition, and encouragement, provides students a safe psychological environment wherein positive emotions are nurtured. In the case of Ryan, for example, the “dramatic events” related to her perezhivanie development is attributed to the responsible teacher who made her discover the joy of learning in the FC because the teacher would prepare “unique and carefully selected” materials and videos that she would use in class, which made her learn English more attentively out of interest (see drama 3). Rita had a similar experience of having a good responsible teacher (in previous learning history in the middle school) who led to the change of her perezhivanie, which means her learning activities, emotion, and cognition all changed toward positivity because of the teacher (see drama 4).

Drama 3:

Ryan: “Our teacher is cheerful and full of positive energy, sometimes quite humorous. Her classes have very rich content, from which I have always learned a lot of knowledge (C). The materials and videos prepared in each Listening and Speaking class are unique and carefully selected (A-Drama), not just those materials in the textbook. I understand teacher took a lot of effort in preparing lessons, so I take each class very attentively (C)Whenever I felt the homework is boring (E), I found the teacher still examined our homework with passion (E) and gave us feedback on WeChat group on time (A). “(Excerpt from Ryan’s narrative in week 15, June 28, 2021).

Drama 4:

Rita: My English was only 60/100 at a very low level when I was in the primary school, but in the middle school I met a terrific teacher who was very helpful and cared about me very much (A-Drama)I liked him (E) and I think I want to learn English well (C), so I started to like English (A), even now I still enjoy learning English very much (E). (Excerpt from Rita’s 2nd verbal narrative in week 15, July 1, 2021).

Dramas of peer contribution

Active peer dynamics in the classroom was conducive to the participants’ positive emotions. They reported that getting new knowledge shared by peers during the time of oral presentation (drama) led to their surprise and enjoyment, which is another drama. By listening to others’ presentation allowed the participants to know their classmates better and thus build inter-personal cohesion, which, in turn, contributed to their positive emotions of a good surprising gain in the class (as shown in drama 5).

Drama 5:

Jenny: When discussing everyone’s fashion choices (A), I found that everyone has different ideas (C-Drama). I was surprised (E) because I was exposed to different concepts, and I felt that it was also an opportunity to get to know others (C). (Excerpt from Jenny’s narrative in week 15, June 29, 2021).

Dramas of previous English learning history

Individual difference plays an important role here (Wang and Derakhshan, 2021). Learners’ levels of language proficiency determine to what extent they take part in classroom activities. Some of the current participants felt interested in their peer classmates’ presentations because their language capability allowed them to understand what was said. On the contrary, those low language proficiency tended to have negative feelings like boredom, apathy, and even helplessness because they neither could personally get involved in class activities nor understood others. Jack’s past English learning history shows he was learning science in high school and his self-awareness of his low proficiency made him lack self-confidence, which led to his disbelief in this class learning helpfulness.

Drama 6:

Jack: In high school, I studied science (A). Chinese and English have always been drags for me (E-Drama), but after college entrance examination, I came to this university to learn language by mistake. At the beginning, I tried (A), but then I didn’t understand it at all (C). I have been learning Spanish (A), so I have almost forgotten English (C). Now I’m a little bit self-defeating (E). (Excerpt from Jack’s narrative in week 15, June 29, 2021).

However, Zoey, on the opposite, was a bit unlucky. Once teased by peers on her “strange” English accent turned out to be her drama, which made her puzzled and feared speaking up again in class. To make things even worse, she was confused about the value of classroom learning because some boys in the class would love to make fun of her accent.

Drama 7:

Zoey: There was a semester in Grade One, every time when I was asked to speak up in the classroom (A), my peers would laugh at my accent (A-Drama)I don’t know why (C), maybe because there were too many boy classmates in the class and they loved to make fun of my accent (A)So ever since then I feared speaking up in class (E & A)(Excerpt from Zoey’s 2nd verbal narrative in week 15, July 1, 2021).

In a word, learners experienced an array of dramas in the FC, which could be emotional, cognitive, or behavioral “dramatic events” that aroused the interaction of the learner’s emotion, cognition, and activities or (actions taken), hence causing the dynamics of perezhivanie.

Discussion

Interplay of learners’ emotion, cognition, and actions (or activities)

This study intended to investigate learners’ emotional experiences in the FC and explore the different ways in which emotion, cognition, and action co-exist in this pedagogical context. Regarding the first question, the results showed that the participants experienced both positive and negative emotions in the FC, mirroring the findings in Li et al.’s (2020), Sampson (2020), and Kruk and Pawlak (2022). In comparison to negative emotions, more positive emotions were reported by the participants, particularly enjoyment, interest, confidence, surprise, and gratitude, showing that the FC can lead to emotional positivity, that is, a high ratio of positive to negative emotions (Jeong et al., 2016; González-Gómez et al., 2017). MacIntyre and Vincze (2017) revealed that positivity led to stronger language learning motivation. Thus, the teacher’s goal is never to erase learners’ negative emotions, which is also impossible, as negative emotions are inevitable part of learning. Instead, they should find ways to limit learners’ negative emotions to the point that negative emotions do not overwhelm the positive ones (Jin et al., 2021). To this end, exercising the FC pedagogy seems to be effective.

Narrative analysis also showed that emotion, cognition, and activity did not stand alone but united in a dramatic event (Greenberg and Safran, 1987; Vygotsky, 1994; Lantolf and Swain, 2019; Agnoletto et al., 2021). This finding reflects the construct of perezhivanie and suggests that sociocultural theory is a useful theoretical framework to research into emotion, cognition, or activity (Tasker et al., 2010; Golombek and Doran, 2014; Johnson and Worden, 2014; Golombek, 2015; Veresov, 2017; Johnson, 2018, 2021; Johnson and Golombek, 2018). Regarding the pattern of their combination, it was often shown in the qualitative data that negative (positive) emotion, cognition, and activity were interlocked, but occasionally negative emotions can also lead to positive cognition and actions, showing dialectical relations between the three (Vygotsky, 1994). As the case of Jenny, she felt anxious and dissatisfying with course grade, which made her improve self-planning ability and then forge a positive self-concept about herself. The implication is that negative emotions should not terrify learners and teachers (Li et al., 2020; Li, 2021, 2022). The key point is to find ways to manage negative emotions, which thus do not paralyze thinking and behaviors (Jiang and Dewaele, 2019; Jin et al., 2021), but bring positive outcomes to language learning. On this point, students can make use of their own agency to self-regulate their negative emotions, highlighting the importance of developing learners’ proper cognition of negative emotions and training their self-regulatory strategies of emotions.

Drama as an effective lens to observe the learners’ Perezhivanija displaying both historical and developmental characteristics

For the study of the development of individual higher mental functioning, Vygotsky (1997) believed that perezhivanie is historical. Accordingly, in this study, we traced participants’ English learning history in the far past (before college), near past (before the present term), and weekly data. We found that dramas that aroused the change of perezhivanija can be relative to learners’ past history, present events, or social relations (Swain, 2013; Ng, 2021). In the narrative, Zoey’s drama of being ridiculed by peers because of her accent is similar to the findings in the study by Jiang and Dewaele (2019) in which they found Chinese students have higher levels of foreign language anxiety, which may be attributable to Chinese educational background. This was also in contrast to Grace’s experience in the study by Swain et al. (2015). Instead of being inspired by the experience of being ridiculed like Grace, Zoey in the present study became afraid of speaking English. Golombek and Doran (2014) also claimed that how individuals interpret the lived experience would influence the way they interpret and react to the current situation.

Drama is also developmental over time. For example, a good teacher in the previous learning history exerted its effect on how students get along with English studies in college (narrative of Rita). In addition, drama also arouses future expected experience with learning, such as Jim who believes that the presentation skills learned in English class can be applicable to other subjects. Drama and perezhivanie are two essential concepts for understanding how the general genetic law of development works and how the social becomes the individual (Fleer et al., 2017). To be specific, drama brings rich emotions, forming individual’s critical perezhivanie, thus influencing individuals’ trajectory of development.

Implications

This study holds important implications for both research and teaching. This study shows that emotion does not occur alone but is closely combined with cognition and activity in a certain sociocultural context. Therefore, rather than encouraging isolationism, this study indeed advances a systematic perspective to emotion, cognition, and behavior studies incorporating social and relational factors. In this way, the ever-present cognitive approach to foreign language teaching (Swain, 2013) and the newly emerging emotional turn in language development studies (MacIntyre and Gregersen, 2012; Jin and Zhang, 2019, 2021) finally have a common ground on which they can work. In addition, in this study, although negative emotions were reported by the participants with their FC experiences, positive emotions took hold. This suggests that the FC in general is welcomed by the participants. With its mixed teaching modes that combined in-class teaching and out-of-class learning, the FC indeed can bring a brand new teaching and learning ecology in which learners are given more autonomy to explore after class, and teachers serve as a guide to solve the problems encountered by students during their explorations. Nevertheless, we must also note that the negative emotions that the participants reported should not be ignored since they might signal the disadvantages of the FC, which should be addressed by teachers in specific instructional contexts. Gregersen and MacIntyre (2014), inspired by the positive psychology movement, explained that negative emotions are not always bad as they can help learners to eliminate an obstacle, but they can be paralyzing if not properly dealt with during the teaching and learning process (Dewaele, 2015). Finally, what further emerged from the data was that learners prefer some activities specifically designed for the FC, which required them to prepare before class and present in class with all their research. Most of the participants positively commented on the activities they emotionally enjoyed during the in-class time, and this brings more implications to EFL teachers who are working in the FC mode. It is for sure worthwhile to organize more workshops working out more innovative designs compatible with the FC teaching design as it is of great difference to previous traditional classroom teaching (Gao et al., 2022).

Conclusion

This study established that the FC could effectively improve learners’ positive emotions in foreign language learning, although they might also experience negative emotions under this teaching mode. With no exception, all teaching approaches and methods have their own pros and cons and thus should be tailored to particular groups of students when applied. In addition, this study suggests that an SCT approach to language psychology gives deeper insights into emotions by providing a very useful analytic tool, perezhivanie, that sheds light on the complex interactions between emotion, cognition, and activity. Future studies might continue with the current study design but consider exploring in-depth what accounts for the complex interactions between emotion, cognition, and action in both learners and teachers, and in what ways learners and teachers can make these interactions beneficial.

Limitations and suggestions

This study has limitations. First, this study was only conducted on one class from a university. The number of participants were limited, and there was a lack of heterogeneity among the samples in many aspects like age, gender, and English learning experiences. Therefore, although the findings are quite inspiring, researchers are encouraged to interpret the findings with caution. Second, this study relied on purely the participants’ self-reports regarding their emotional experiences in the FC, based on which the efficacy of the FC was investigated, thus lacking of field observation to triangulate the narrative findings. In addition, the teacher was not included in this study. Thus, the pros and cons of FC teaching could not be viewed from the teacher’s perspective. Last but not least, we developed robust coding systems for meaning units emerged from the narratives on various kinds of emotions, cognition, and activities; the coding, though of referential value, still needs more shaping for future studies.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Dalian University of Foreign Languages. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

LQ contributed to the draft writing and data organization. LY collected the data and organized the data. YJ wrote the draft. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by the Grant Office of Humanities and Social Sciences of Education Bureau of Liaoning Province: 2020JYT02 and Dalian University of Foreign Languages.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ In this session, the sentences showing activity, emotion or cognition are marked with: (A) represents lines of “activities,” (C) represents “cognition,“(E) represents emotion.
  2. ^ In this session, the sentences showing dramas are annotated with underlines; (A) represents lines of “activities,” (C) represents “cognition,” (E) represents “emotion.”

References

Abdullah, M., Hussin, S., and Ismail, K. (2020). “Exploring the effects of flipped classroom model implementation on EFL learners’ self-confidence in English speaking performance,” in Recent advances in intelligent systems and smart applications, eds M. Al-Emran, K. Shaalan, and A. Hassanien (Cham: Springer), 223–241.

Google Scholar

Adnan, M. (2017). Perceptions of senior-year ELT students for flipped classroom: A materials development course. Comput. Assist. Lang. Learn. 30, 204–222. doi: 10.1080/09588221.2017.1301958

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Agnoletto, M., Dellagnelo, A., and Johnson, K. (2021). Perezhivanie in action: A novice teacher’s refraction of mediated dramatic events. Lang. Sociocult. Theory 8, 153–179. doi: 10.1558/lst.18163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belkhir, S. (2020). “Cognition and language learning: An introduction,” in Cognition and language learning, ed. S. Belkhir (Newcastle Upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing), 1–12.

Google Scholar

Bergmann, J., and Sams, A. (2014). Flipped learning: Gateway to student engagement. Washington, DC: International Society for Technology in Education.

Google Scholar

Buechler, D. N., Sealy, P. J., and Goomey, J. (2014). “Three pilot studies with a focus on asynchronous distance education,” in Proceedings of 121st ASEE annual conference & exposition; June 1, 2014, Indianapolis.

Google Scholar

Chen Hsieh, J. S., Wu, W.-C. V., and Marek, M. W. (2017). Using the flipped classroom to enhance EFL learning. Comput. Assist. Lang. Learn. 30, 1–21. doi: 10.1080/09588221.2015.1111910

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chetcuti, S. C., Hans, J. T., and Brent, J. P. (2014). “Flipping the engineering classroom: Results and observations with non-engineering students,” in Proceedings of 121st ASEE annual conference & exposition, June 1, 2014, Indianapolis, IN.

Google Scholar

Clément, R., Dörnyei, Z., and Noels, K. A. (1994). Motivation, self-confidence, and group cohesion in the foreign language classroom. Lang. Learn. 44, 417–448. doi: 10.1111/J.1467-1770.1994.TB01113.X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derakhshan, A., Kruk, M., Mehdizadeh, M., and Pawlak, M. (2021). Boredom in online classes in the Iranian EFL context: Sources and solutions. System 101:102556. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2021.102556

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewaele, J. M. (2015). On emotions in foreign language learning and use. Lang. Teach. 39, 13–15. doi: 10.37546/jalttlt39.3-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewaele, J.-M. (2021). “Enjoyment,” in The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition and individual differences, eds S. Li, P. Hiver, and M. Papi (New York, NY: Routledge), 190–206.

Google Scholar

Dewaele, J.-M., and Li, C. C. (2018). Editorial: Special issue “Emotions in SLA”. Stud. Second Lang. Learn. Teach. 8, 15–20. doi: 10.14746/ssllt.2018.8.1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewaele, J.-M., and MacIntyre, P. D. (2014). The two faces of Janus? Anxiety and enjoyment in the foreign language classroom. Stud. Second Lang. Learn. Teach. 4, 237–274. doi: 10.14746/ssllt.2014.4.2.5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewey, J. (1916). Democracy and education: An introduction to the philosophy of education. New York, NY: Macmillan.

Google Scholar

Dewey, J. (1920). Reconstruction and philosophy. Boston, MA: Beacon.

Google Scholar

Dewey, J. (1933). How we think. Chicago, IL: Henry Regnery.

Google Scholar

Dick, H., Segura, C., and Dennehy, N. (2017). Narrative in sociocultural studies of language. Anthropol. Oxford Bibliogr. 1–22. doi: 10.1093/obo/9780199766567-0180

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eich, E., and Schooler, J. W. (2000). Cognition/emotion interactions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Evseeva, A., and Solozhenko, A. (2015). Use of flipped classroom technology in language learning. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 206, 205–209. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.10.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleer, M., González Rey, F., and Veresov, N. (2017). Perezhivanie, emotions and subjectivity: Advancing Vygotsky’s legacy. Singapore: Springer.

Google Scholar

Gao, Y., Qin, L. L., and Gu, Q. (2022). Unpacking language teacher beliefs, agency, and resilience in the complex, unprecedented time: A mixed-method study. Front. Psychol. 13:958003. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.958003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gok, D., Bozoglan, H., and Bozoglan, B. (2021). Effects of online flipped classroom on foreign language classroom anxiety and reading anxiety. Comput. Assist. Lang. Learn. 1–21. doi: 10.1080/09588221.2021.1950191

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golombek, P. (2015). Redrawing the boundaries of language teacher cognition: Language teacher educators’ emotion, cognition, and activity. Mod. Lang. J. 99, 470–484. doi: 10.1111/modl.12236

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golombek, P. R., and Doran, M. (2014). Unifying cognition, emotion, and activity in language teacher professional development. Teach. Teach. Educ. 39, 102–111. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2014.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

González-Gómez, D., Jeong, J. S., and Cañada-Cañada, F. (2017). “How to improve the students’ affective dimension when learning science through a flipped-classroom setting: A case study,” in Proceedings of the 10th annual international conference of education, research and innovation; November 16-18, 2017, Seville.

Google Scholar

Gorbatkov, A. A. (2002). Dynamics of the link between positive and negative emotions. Vopr. Psikhol. 132, 140–160.

Google Scholar

Greenberg, L. S., and Safran, J. (1987). “Emotion, cognition, and action,” in Theoretical foundations of behavior therapy. Perspectives on individual differences, eds H. J. Eysenck and I. Martin (Boston, MA: Springer), 295–311.

Google Scholar

Gregersen, T., and MacIntyre, P. D. (2014). Capitalizing on language learners’ individuality: From premise to practice. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

Google Scholar

Hishikawa, K., Kusaka, T., Fukuda, T., Kohata, Y., and Inoue, H. (2019). Anxiety or nervousness disturbs the progress of birth based on human behavioral evolutionary biology. J. Perinat. Educ. 28, 218–223. doi: 10.1891/1058-1243.28.4.218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hung, H.-T. (2015). Flipping the classroom for English language learners to foster active learning. Comput. Assist. Lang. Learn. 28, 81–96. doi: 10.1080/09588221.2014.967701

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hung, H.-T. (2017). Design-based research: Redesign of an English language course using a flipped classroom approach. TESOL Q. 51, 180–192. doi: 10.1002/tesq.328

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeong, J. S., González-Gómez, D., and Cañada-Cañada, F. (2016). Students’ perceptions and emotions toward learning in a flipped general science classroom. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 25, 747–758. doi: 10.1007/s10956-016-9630-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, Y., and Dewaele, J.-M. (2019). How unique is the foreign language classroom enjoyment and anxiety of Chinese EFL learners? System 82, 13–25. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2019.02.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, Y. X., Dewaele, J.-M., and MacIntyre, P. D. (2021). Reducing anxiety in the foreign language classroom: A positive psychology approach. System 101:102604. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2021.102604

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, Y. X., and Zhang, L. J. (2019). A comparative study of two scales for foreign language classroom enjoyment. Percept. Mot. Skills 126, 1024–1041. doi: 10.1177/0031512519864471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, Y. X., and Zhang, L. J. (2021). The dimensions of foreign language classroom enjoyment and their effect on foreign language achievement. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling. 24, 948–962. doi: 10.1080/13670050.2018.1526253

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joe, A. I., Kpolovie, P. J., Osonwa, K. E., and Iderima, C. E. (2014). Modes of admission and academic performance in Nigerian universities. Merit Res. J. Educ. Rev. 2, 203–230.

Google Scholar

Johnson, K. E. (2018). Studying language teacher cognition: Understanding and enacting theoretically consistent instructional practices. Lang. Teach Res. 22, 259–263. doi: 10.1177/1362168818772197

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, K. E. (2021). Li, L. (2020). Language teacher cognition: A sociocultural perspective. Lang. Sociocult. Theory 7, 224–229. doi: 10.1558/lst.42530

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, K. E., and Golombek, P. (2002). Teachers’ narrative inquiry as professional development. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Johnson, K. E., and Golombek, P. R. (2018). Informing and transforming language teacher education pedagogy. Lang. Teach. Res. 24, 116–127. doi: 10.1177/1362168818777539

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, K. E., and Golombek, P. (2016). Mindful L2 teacher education: A sociocultural perspective on cultivating teachers’ professional development. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Johnson, K. E., and Worden, D. (2014). Cognitive/emotional dissonance as growth points in learning to teach. Lang. Sociocult. Theory 1, 125–150. doi: 10.1558/lst.v1i2.125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khajavy, G. H., MacIntyre, P. D., and Barabadi, E. (2018). Role of the emotions and classroom environment in willingness to communicate: Applying doubly latent multilevel analysis in second language acquisition research. Stud. Second Lang. Acquist. 40, 605–624. doi: 10.1017/S0272263117000304

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruk, M., and Pawlak, M. (2022). Understanding emotions in English language learning in virtual worlds. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Låg, T., and Saele, R. G. (2019). Does the flipped classroom improve student learning and satisfaction? A systematic review and meta-analysis. AERA Open 5, 1–17. doi: 10.1177/2332858419870489

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lantolf, J. P., and Swain, M. (2019). “Perezhivanie: The cognitive-emotional dialectic within the social situation of development,” in Contemporary language motivation theory: 60 years since Gardner and Lambert (1959), eds A. H. Al-Hoorie and P. D. MacIntyre (Bristol: Multilingual Matters), 80–105.

Google Scholar

Lantolf, J. P., and Thorne, S. L. (2006). Sociocultural theory and the genesis of second language development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Lantolf, J. P., and Thorne, S. L. (2007). “Sociocultural theory and second language learning,” in Theories in second language acquisition, eds B. van Patten and J. Williams (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 201–224.

Google Scholar

Leal, T., Slabakova, R., and Farmer, T. (2017). The fine tuning of linguistic expectations over the course of L2 learning. Stud. Second Lang. Acquisit. 39, 493–525. doi: 10.1017/S0272263116000164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, C. C. (2021). A control–value theory approach to boredom in English classes among university students in China. Mod. Lang. J. 105, 317–334. doi: 10.1111/modl.12693

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, C. C. (2022). Foreign language learning boredom and enjoyment: The effects of learner variables and teacher variables. Lang. Teach Res. doi: 10.1177/13621688221090324 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, C. C., Dewaele, J.-M., and Hu, Y. (2021). Foreign language learning boredom: Conceptualization and measurement. Appl. Linguist. Rev. 1–27. doi: 10.1515/applirev-2020-0124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, C. C., Dewaele, J.-M., and Jiang, G. Y. (2020). The complex relationship between classroom emotions and EFL achievement in China. Appl. Linguist. Rev. 11, 485–510. doi: 10.1515/applirev-2018-0043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Listyani, J. I., and Tananuraksakul, N. (2019). Motivation, self-confidence, and anxiety in English language learning: Indonesian and Thai students’ perspectives. Accents Asia 11, 54–77.

Google Scholar

MacIntyre, P. D., and Gregersen, T. (2012). Emotions that facilitate language learning: The positive-broadening power of the imagination. Stud. Second Lang. Learn. Teach. 2, 193–213. doi: 10.14746/ssllt.2012.2.2.4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacIntyre, P. D., and Vincze, L. (2017). Positive and negative emotions in motivation for second language learning. Stud. Second Lang. Learn. Teach. 7, 61–88. doi: 10.14746/ssllt.2017.7.1.4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malaei, F., Sohrabi, A., and Jahanitabesh, A. (2022). “An evolutionary perspective on emotion-cognition interactions,” in Proceedings of the EHBEA conference; April 19-22, 2022, Leipzig, Germany. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19101064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malik, Z. A., Khan, S. S., and Manzil, E. M. (2018). Exploring the relationship between student engagement and new pedagogical approaches. J. Educ. Technol. Systems 47, 170–192. doi: 10.1177/0047239518788281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, E., and Plunkett, K. (2012). The role of novelty in early word learning. Cogn. Sci. 36, 1157–1177. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2012.01239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matlin, W. M. (2005). Cognition. New York, NY: Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

Mehring, J. (2016). Present research on the flipped classroom and potential tools for the EFL classroom. Comput. Sch. 33, 1–10. doi: 10.1080/07380569.2016.1139912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehring, J. (2018). “The flipped classroom,” in Innovations in flipping the language classroom, eds J. Mehring and A. Leis (Singapore: Springer). doi: 10.1007/978-981-10-6968-0_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehring, J., and Leis, A. (2018). Innovations in flipping the language classroom: Theories and practices. Singapore: Springer Nature.

Google Scholar

Moeller, A. J. (2021). “The interplay of emotion, cognition, and learning in the language classroom,” in One vision—multiple perspectives: Central states conference on the teaching of foreign languages; March 11-13, 2021, Ishpeming, MI.

Google Scholar

Ng, C. (2021). Japanese students’ emotional lived experiences in English language learning, learner identities, and their transformation. Mod. Lang. J. 105, 810–828. doi: 10.1111/modl.12739

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pan, H., Xia, F., Kumar, T., Li, X., and Shamsy, A. (2022). Massive open online course versus flipped instruction: Impacts on foreign language speaking anxiety, foreign language learning motivation, and learning attitude. Front. Psychol. 13:833616. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.833616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patton, M. Q. (1990). Qualitative evaluation and research methods, 2nd Edn. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Pawlak, M., Zawodniak, J., and Kruk, M. (2020). The neglected emotion of boredom in teaching English to advanced learners. Int. J. Appl. Linguist. 30, 497–509. doi: 10.1111/ijal.12302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qin, L. L. (2021). Clear the clouds and see the sun in the world of Vygotsky’s ideas: An interview with James Lantolf. Int. J. TESOL Stud. 3, 101–113. doi: 10.46451/ijts.2021.01.08

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qin, L. L., He, Y. H., and Ouyang, X. B. (2019). A narrative study of the emotional effect on a novice second language teacher’s cognitive development. Mod. Foreign Lang. 42, 818–829.

Google Scholar

Qin, L. L. (2022). The “Wrong Love” between the zone of proximal development and scaffolding: An interview with Prof. James P. Lantolf. Chin. J. Appl. Linguist. 45, 138–149+152.

Google Scholar

Qin, L. L., and Ren, W. (2021). Investigating affordance in technology-enriched language learning environment through exploring students’ perezhivanija. Chin. J. Appl. Linguist. 44, 187–202,263. doi: 10.1515/CJAL-2021-0011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qin, L. L., Zhao, Y. Q., Ouyang, X. B., and Yao, L. (2022). Development of English learners’ agency in the cloud-based flipped classroom: A sociocultural theory perspective. Mod. Foreign Lang. 45, 381–393.

Google Scholar

Quadt, L., Critchley, H., and Nagai, Y. (2022). Cognition, emotion, and the central autonomic network. Auton. Neurosci. Basic Clin. 238:102948. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2022.102948

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahmat, N. H. (2020). Investigating the cycle or fear in foreign language learning. Eur. J. Foreign Lang. Teach. 5, 117–127. doi: 10.46827/ejfl.v5i1.3245

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roehl, A., Reddy, S. L., and Shannon, G. J. (2013). The flipped classroom: An opportunity to engage millennial students through active learning strategies. J. Fam. Consum. Sci. 105, 44–49. doi: 10.14307/JFCS105.2.12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, W., and Jornet, A. (2013). Situated cognition. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 4, 463–478. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sampson, R. J. (2020). The feeling classroom: Diversity of feelings in instructed L2 learning. Innov. Lang. Learn. Teach. 14, 203–217. doi: 10.1080/17501229.2018.1553178

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarr-Jansman, E. T. M., and Rowberry, D. J. (2018). “Cognition, emotion, and behavior,” in The challenges of nursing stroke management in rehabilitation centres, eds B. Buijck and G. Ribbers (Cham: Springer), 113–120.

Google Scholar

Schnitzspahn, K. M., and Phillips, L. H. (2016). “Emotion-cognition interactions,” in Encyclopedia of geropsychology, ed. N. Pachana (Singapore: Springer), 1–8.

Google Scholar

Shanty, H. (2019). Interest in foreign language learning. IDEAS. J. Eng. Lang. Teach. Learn. Linguist. Lit. 7, 134–143. doi: 10.24256/ideas.v7i1.731

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swain, M. (2013). The inseparability of cognition and emotion in second language learning. Lang. Teach. 46, 1–13. doi: 10.1017/S0261444811000486

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swain, M., Kinnear, P., and Steinman, L. (2015). Sociocultural theory in second language education: An introduction through narratives, 2nd Edn. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

Google Scholar

Tasker, T., Johnson, K., and Davis, T. (2010). A sociocultural analysis of teacher talk in inquiry-based professional development. Lang. Teach. Res. 14, 129–140. doi: 10.1177/1362168809353871

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teimouri, Y. (2018). Differential roles of shame and guilt in L2 learning: How bad is bad? Mod. Lang. J. 102, 632–652. doi: 10.1111/modl.12511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Alten, D. C. D., Phielix, C., Janssen, J., and Kester, L. (2019). Effects of flipping the classroom on learning outcomes and satisfaction: A meta-analysis. Educ. Res. Rev. 28:100281. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2019.05.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veresov, N. (2017). “The concept of perezhivanie in cultural-historical theory: Content and contexts,” in Perezhivanie, emotions and subjectivity: Advancing Vygotsky’s legacy, eds M. Fleer, F. González Rey, and N. Veresov (Singapore: Springer), 47–70.

Google Scholar

Vitta, J., and Al-Hoorie, A. (2020). The flipped classroom in second language learning: A meta-analysis. Lang. Teach. Res. 1–25. doi: 10.1177/1362168820981403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind and society: The development of higher mental processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky, Vol. 1. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L. S. (1994). “The problem of the environment,” in The Vygotsky reader, eds J. Valsiner and R. Van der Veer (Hoboken, NY: Blackwell), 338–354.

Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L. S. (1997). “The history of the development of higher mental functions,” in The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky, Vol. 4, ed. R. W. Rieber (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 1–252.

Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L. S. (1998). “The crisis at age seven,” in The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky, Vol. 5, ed. R. W. Rieber (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 289–296.

Google Scholar

Wang, Y. L., and Derakhshan, A. (2021). [Review of the book Investigating dynamic relationships among individual difference variables in learning English as a foreign language in a virtual world, by M. Kruk]. System 100:102531. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2021.102531

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y. L., Derakhshan, A., and Zhang, L. J. (2021). Researching and practicing positive psychology in second/foreign language learning and teaching: The past, current status and future directions. Front. Psychol. 12:731721. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.731721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weitoft, G., and Rosén, M. (2005). Is perceived nervousness and anxiety a predictor of premature mortality and severe morbidity? A longitudinal follow up of the Swedish survey of living conditions. J. Epidemiol. Commun. Health 59, 794–798. doi: 10.1136/jech.2005.033076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilang, J. (2022). Gratitude in foreign language learning. Arab World Engl. J. 13, 338–351. doi: 10.24093/awej/vol13no2.23

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yob, I. (1997). The cognitive emotions and emotional cognitions. Stud. Philos. Educ. 16, 43–57. doi: 10.1023/A:1004990702983

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yusriyah, H., Eryansyah, E., and Kurniawan, D. (2021). Interest and enthusiasm in learning English as a foreign language in a senior high school in Bangka Belitung Islands, Indonesia and how both relate to English achievement. IDEAS. J. Eng. Lang. Teach. Learn. Linguist. Lit. 9, 22–36. doi: 10.24256/ideas.v9i2.1919

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix

1. Direction for pre-semester narrative for all the participants:

Please describe your previous experience of English learning before college and what you have experienced in college in the flipped classroom. You may talk about events, persons, or any special artifacts that you can remember. Please write minimally 200 words.

2. Direction for post-semester narrative for all the participants:

Please describe your experience of English learning in the flipped classroom during this semester. You may talk about events, persons, or any special artifacts that you can remember. Please write minimally 200 words.

3. Direction for weekly narrative for 8 focal students

Please describe your experience of English learning in the flipped classroom during this week. You may talk about events, persons, or any special artifacts that you can remember. Please write minimally 200 words.

Keywords: perezhivanie, emotion-cognition-activity, dramas, sociocultural theory, flipped classroom

Citation: Qin L, Yao L and Jin Y (2022) Unpacking the interaction between foreign language learners’ emotion, cognition, and activity in the flipped classroom in higher education: A perezhivanie perspective. Front. Psychol. 13:1005237. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1005237

Received: 28 July 2022; Accepted: 15 September 2022;
Published: 11 October 2022.

Edited by:

Jian-Hong Ye, Beijing Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Don Yao, University of Macau, Macau SAR, China
Erkan Yüce, Aksaray University, Turkey

Copyright © 2022 Qin, Yao and Jin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lan Yao, yaolan1128@163.com; Yinxing Jin, busicontact@yahoo.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.