Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 07 February 2022
Sec. Health Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Social Determinants of Psychological Illness and Well-Being Across the Life Course View all 9 articles

‘Kindness by Post’: A Mixed-Methods Evaluation of a Participatory Public Mental Health Project

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
  • 2Division of Psychiatry, Faculty of Brain Sciences, University College London, London, United Kingdom

Background: Random acts of kindness can improve wellbeing. However, less is known about the impacts of giving and receiving acts of kindness with strangers on wellbeing and loneliness. Therefore, this study’s objectives were to evaluate a participatory public mental health project involving sending and receiving a card with goodwill messages, to understand how such acts of kindness influence wellbeing and loneliness, and to investigate the potential mechanisms underlying the project’s impacts.

Materials and Methods: This study was an analysis of anonymized service evaluation data collected in the ‘Kindness by Post’ (KBP) project in 2020. It used a mixed-methods single-group design and data from 289 participants. Changes in wellbeing, loneliness, sense of belonging and hope from baseline (12th–14th February) to follow-up (26th February–2nd March) were analyzed using linear or multinomial logistic regression. Regression models also examined the associations between changes in wellbeing and baseline loneliness or participation level. Free text responses about experiences and suggestions for the project were analyzed using thematic analysis.

Results: Participants had a small, but statistically significant improvement, in wellbeing equating to 0.21 standard deviations (95% CI: 0.12–0.30) after taking part in the project, as well as improvements in loneliness, sense of belonging and hope. How lonely a participant was at baseline and whether participants both sent and received a kindness card were not associated with improvements in wellbeing. In the qualitative analysis, a desire to help others emerged as the main motivator to take part in the card exchange. Participants reported enhanced personal fulfillment, leading to improvements in wellbeing. Receiving a card could make people feel special and cherished, which was reported to establish a sense of connection with others, with potential benefits for reducing loneliness.

Conclusion: This study provided preliminary evidence that the KBP project might improve wellbeing, loneliness, sense of belonging and hope. Sending a kindness card in this project played a predominant role in wellbeing enhancement, and receiving a kindness card could reduce loneliness. This study suggests that the KBP project can be replicated in more contexts in the future, and might improve wellbeing and loneliness in large communities.

Introduction

This paper reports a mixed methods evaluation of an innovative participatory public health program: the Kindness by Post (KBP) project, in which participants send and receive cards with a message of kindness from another participant. KBP aims to enhance wellbeing and social connection and reduce feelings of loneliness for those taking part.

Improving wellbeing and reducing loneliness are important ways to improve public mental health. Wellbeing, which comprises both positive affective experiences (Ryan and Deci, 2001, p. 143, 144) and positive psychological functioning (Ryff, 1989, p. 1077), is protective against developing depression (Wood and Joseph, 2010) and associated with a range of positive health (Ryff, 2014) and social outcomes (Lyubomirsky et al., 2005a, p. 39).

Loneliness, defined as a subjective negative emotional state that arises from a discrepancy between someone’s desired and actual social relationships (Perlman and Peplau, 1981, p. 39; de Jong-Gierveld, 1998; Valtorta and Hanratty, 2012, p. 518), involves intimate, relational and collective dimensions (Cacioppo et al., 2015). Like wellbeing, loneliness also predicts a range of mental and physical health outcomes (Cacioppo and Hawkley, 2003; Murberg, 2004; Hawkley and Cacioppo, 2010, p. 219; Wang et al., 2018, p. 11), including depression (Lee et al., 2021). Loneliness may also make people perceive themselves as having poor social skills, which leads to low self-esteem (Cacioppo et al., 2000), which may consequently decrease wellbeing (Apaolaza et al., 2013, p. 5).

Therefore, interventions which can enhance wellbeing and prevent or reduce loneliness are of high interest to help individuals and reduce the burden on public health. Psychological interventions have been developed to improve wellbeing (Weiss et al., 2016) and which show promise for reducing loneliness (Masi et al., 2011). However, most psychological interventions require support from expert clinicians, which is expensive and might not be accessible to all people. Less costly, scalable ways of supporting people to enhance their own wellbeing and address loneliness are also desirable.

Giving kindness, receiving kindness and creative activities (three elements of the KBP project evaluated in our study) have all been shown to improve wellbeing. Performing kind acts, such as holding a door for another, greeting strangers or helping others with academic work, has been found to improve wellbeing (Otake et al., 2006; Layous et al., 2013, p. 1299; Ouweneel et al., 2014; Layous et al., 2017; Curry et al., 2018, p. 4; Kaffke, 2018). It is suggested that kind behaviors that are courteous or altruistic may help people recognize their abilities to help others, which cultivates positive feelings to themselves (Lyubomirsky et al., 2005b). Consequently, positive experiences may promote positive emotions in the long term, leading to a higher level of wellbeing (Pressman et al., 2015). As well as the person performing the kind act, people who receive kindness could also have improved wellbeing. People who received kindness have been found to show higher levels of smiling expressions, which reflected more sincere joy, compared to those who did not interact with people performing kind acts (Pressman et al., 2015). A thematic analysis found that receiving kindness was reported to increase wellbeing beyond experiencing pleasure but also self-confidence, self-actualization and sense of mastery (Filep et al., 2017). In addition to enhancing wellbeing, acts of kindness may also connect the giver and receiver because the receiver may feel acknowledged and valued by the giver. Furthermore, engaging in something new such as doing creative work is also encouraged as a way to improve wellbeing. Conner et al. (2018) demonstrated that people who had done more creative activities (e.g., artistic ones) or developed original ideas reported a higher level of daily flourishing. Therefore, creative acts may help to achieve a positive mood and improve wellbeing, particularly if these acts also provide an opportunity to give and receive kindness.

If kind acts can enhance people’s wellbeing, it is worthwhile organizing such kindness activities into more extensive and comprehensive programs in the general population (Sin and Lyubomirsky, 2009). Previous research has only demonstrated an overall small positive effect size of kindness acts on primarily the actors (Curry et al., 2018, p. 19), and there is limited evidence showing the psychological impacts on receiving kindness from strangers. There is a lack of evidence regarding the effects of kindness programs where people both perform kind acts to strangers and receive kindness from strangers, and whether this mutuality leads to bigger impacts on wellbeing and loneliness than only giving or receiving kindness. Importantly too, although there is growing evidence suggesting that kindness to strangers leads to wellbeing promotion (Dunn et al., 2008), there is still insufficient understanding about the potential mechanisms underlying the relationships between kind acts toward strangers and enhancement of people’s wellbeing. Therefore, it is essential to further investigate the experiences of kind acts for improving wellbeing. Furthermore, kindness behaviors can provide social support in which people encounter social interactions. Cacioppo et al. (2015) noted that actions that provide mutual social support and increase social interactions with others could reduce loneliness. Concerning the strong associations between loneliness and wellbeing (Houghton et al., 2016; Emerson et al., 2020), it is also worth determining whether simple kindness behaviors could build connectedness between individuals, which may be an effective means to reduce loneliness.

This study will add to the developing evidence base regarding acts of kindness to and from strangers in promoting wellbeing and reducing loneliness. It aims to examine the effectiveness of a brief, self-administrated kindness program that was organized among the general population. The public health program ‘KBP’ is run nationally online across the UK by the Mental Health Collective (MHC), a non-profit community interest company. In the KBP project, participants send a handmade or bought card that includes kind messages to a randomly allocated stranger, and receive a similar card from a different randomly allocated stranger, who is likely to be a different person. It has been used in a variety of social contexts, including for new students at university, for the public during the COVID-19 lockdown, and for people observing Ramadan. This study used data collected in a card exchange for Valentine’s Day in 2020—the ‘Great British Valentine’ (GBV). The exchange sought to help participants at a time which may be difficult for many, as people without a partner or in a troubled romantic relationship may experience low mood or loneliness during the Valentine’s Day period (Otnes et al., 1994). The KBP project mobilizes several mechanisms for improving wellbeing, discussed above. First, it involves an act of kindness to a stranger, which has an established evidence base for improving wellbeing. Second, in contrast to most random acts of kindness projects, KBP also has a reciprocal element of giving and receiving, which may increase connections with others. Third, the creative element of card-making and kind message-writing in KBP may also be helpful for wellbeing promotion. As an inexpensive, potentially highly scalable program, it is therefore of substantial interest to evaluate the KBP project and understand how it is experienced by participants.

Mixed methods, combining quantitative and qualitative approaches, were used to evaluate the KBP project. There are two main research questions in the quantitative analysis: (1) What are the impacts of taking part in the KBP project on participants’ wellbeing, loneliness, hope and sense of belongingness; and (2) whether baseline loneliness, the extent of participation in the card exchange and age relate to wellbeing changes? Our primary hypothesis is that participants would have an increase in wellbeing, measured by the Short Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Wellbeing Scale (SWEMWBS) (Stewart-Brown et al., 2009), from baseline to follow-up after taking part in the KBP project. Regarding secondary outcomes, we hypothesize that participants’ scores on measures of loneliness, hope and belongingness will improve from baseline to follow-up following the card exchange. Additionally, it is hypothesized that people with lower baseline loneliness scores would have more improvements in wellbeing at follow-up. It is also predicted that people who both sent and received a card would have more improvements in wellbeing compared to those who partially took part in the program (who only gave or only received a card). Lastly, the KBP is predicted to be equally effective across all age groups.

We will use qualitative analysis of participants’ free texts online comments to explore their experience of this program, its perceived benefits and the potential mechanisms by which any perceived effects may have been achieved.

Materials and Methods

Study Design and Setting

The current study reports an analysis using anonymized data collected by the KBP program organizers in the 2020 GBV card exchange. The research comprises a cohort study, employing a within-subject design.

Participants

All participants in GBV who completed pre- and post-outcome measures were included in this study. To take part in GBV, people had to be aged 16 years or above with a postal address in the United Kingdom; there were no other exclusion criteria. Participants were required to sign up for GBV online. Consistent with our ethical approval, the current study only used the data from the adult participants, aged 18 or above.

Ethical Approvals

The study was approved by the UCL Research Ethics Committee on 9th July 2020 (REC reference 18307/001).

Procedure

The GBV card exchange was broadly advertised in newspapers, broadcast, and social media. People who were interested in this project could sign up on the MHC website. Participants registered to take part were first invited to complete the ‘Before questionnaire’ online. They were informed that the data could be shared with external organizations anonymously for research purposes. People gave their consent to this by proceeding with the questionnaire. The baseline data collection was conducted from the 12th of January to the 14th of February 2020. One week before Valentine’s Day, each participant was asked to send a homemade card or letter with goodwill messages to a stranger who was randomly allocated by a computer algorithm. The stranger’s postal address and instructions regarding how to send a card were sent to the participant’s account. In return, each participant would receive a card from another stranger during the week of Valentine’s Day. If participants had not received a card, there was a back-up system that allowed participants to ask the program organizers to arrange for a ‘replacement’ card from a volunteer. Participants were informed that there was no guarantee of receiving a card because the sending process from the stranger was completely voluntary, and not receiving a card was nothing personal. Participants could withdraw from the project at any time they wished. After the card exchange, participants were contacted again by email on the 26th February and invited to complete the online ‘After Questionnaire.’ They were reminded again about their anonymized data being shared and that they could give their consent by completing the questionnaire. Participants were sent a second reminder by email if they did not respond to the questionnaires. The follow-up data collection was closed on the 2nd March 2020. Participants responding outside the data collection windows were excluded from the analysis.

Measures

At baseline and follow-up, participants completed online self-report measures of:

• Wellbeing, using the 7-item Short Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Wellbeing Scale (SWEMWBS) (Stewart-Brown et al., 2009);

• Loneliness, using the 3-item University of California at Los Angeles (UCLA) Loneliness Scale version-3 (Russell, 1996);

• Belongingness, using four items drawn from the General Belongingness Scale (GBS) (Malone et al., 2012);

• Hope, using a single item from the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1996).

Participants reported their gender, ethnicity and age group at baseline. They reported whether or not they had sent and had received a card and provided brief free-text feedback about their experiences of the project at follow-up. There were four questions covering the specific sending or receiving experiences as well as their overall impressions and suggestions for the project. Further information regarding the study measures and how they were scored is provided in Supplementary Material 1.

All data were downloaded into a Microsoft Excel file by KBP staff. Free-text data were checked to remove any personally identifying information, such as names. Multiple and duplicate responses from the same person were identified by checking the sources such as email addresses of the responses. For participants who completed the measures more than once at the same timepoint in either before- or after-questionnaires, all their responses were removed, unless the responses at the same timepoint were identical, then one of the responses was saved. An anonymized dataset was thus produced, containing no personal identifiers or codes that could be used to link the data back to identifiable individuals. This anonymized dataset was then shared with the researcher at UCL through the secure UCL Dropbox system.

Quantitative Analysis

We summarize demographic characteristics of the sample as well as baseline and follow up measures of wellbeing, loneliness, sense of belonging and hope using descriptive statistics. To explore how representative our sample was of GBV participants, we compared participants who had completed both before and after questionnaires with those who had only completed the before questionnaire using linear regression and chi squared tests.

We used Pearson’s and Spearman’s rank correlation tests to check that our four outcome measures of wellbeing, loneliness, sense of belonging and hope were not collinear to each other at baseline: i.e., that they were measuring distinct, different concepts, and that it is therefore of interest to treat and report them all as separate outcomes in our paper. We used an established threshold of | r| < 0.7 (Dormann et al., 2013) as evidence that measures were not collinear.

We calculated standardized scores for wellbeing, loneliness and sense of belonging at baseline and follow up, standardized by the mean and standard deviation of the measure at baseline. For each participant, we calculated changes in wellbeing, loneliness and sense of belonging from baseline to follow up for scores on both original and standardized scales as outcomes for the analysis. Change in hope was recategorized into three groups (negative change, no change, and positive change). A new binary variable was generated based on the sending and receiving experiences of the participant to represent the level of participation in the program (full vs. partial; see Supplementary Material 1).

For Research Question 1, we estimated change in wellbeing, loneliness and sense of belonging from baseline to follow up using separate linear regression models, without adjustment for any covariates. Results are presented on both the original measurement scale and as standardized effect sizes. We used multinomial logistic regression to examine whether there was any improvement in hope after taking part in the program.

To explore whether the program’s effectiveness was associated with either baseline loneliness or full vs. partial participation for Research Question 2, we fitted separate univariable linear regression models with change in wellbeing as the outcome and baseline loneliness and participation level respectively as the single explanatory variable. In order to see whether changes in wellbeing differ across age groups, we conducted a univariable linear regression model with change in wellbeing as the outcome and age group as the single explanatory variable. We checked the assumptions of regression models through the construction of appropriate histograms and normal quantile plots. All analyses were performed using Stata v16.

Qualitative Analysis

The current study uses the standards for reporting qualitative research (SRQR) to report the qualitative analysis (O’Brien et al., 2014). This study used a thematic analysis to capture the pattern of the meaning of the experiences and feedback reflected by the participants. The qualitative analysis processes were guided by the thematic approach developed by Braun and Clarke (2012). There were 388 participants who completed the after-questionnaire, which contained the free-text responses, and the current analysis used the transcripts of the 289 participants who completed both before- and after- questionnaires (see Figure 1). However, the other 99 transcripts were also checked once a coding frame had been developed to determine whether there were any additional novel and distinctive codes generated. The qualitative transcripts were analyzed using NVivo software version 12.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Participants included in the Kindness by Post analyses.

Unit of Analysis

All free-text comments to the four questions were merged as an individual transcript for each participant. All 289 participant transcripts were analyzed.

Researcher Characteristics and Reflexivity

The lead researcher (CW), who had the main role in coding the transcripts, has an academic psychology background. Her personal experience of loneliness and lack of belongingness during time living abroad alone for several years made her interested in determining whether the KBP project made people feel more connected to another. The other two researchers involved in analysis of qualitative data are academic researchers with anthropology (EP) and social work (BL-E) backgrounds and are the Coordinator and a Co-investigator of the UKRI-funded Loneliness and Social Isolation in Mental Health Research Network, respectively. They were involved in the discussion regarding the coding framework and brought their perspectives from their own personal, professional and academic experiences to the analysis. All the researchers had participated in KBP, which helped them better understand what people reflected in the transcripts.

Data Analysis

The current study utilized both inductive and deductive approaches. Regarding the inductive analysis, the lead researcher first read through all the transcripts to become intimately familiar with the data sets’ contents and made some preliminary notes on the initial insights relevant to the research questions. For deductive analysis, there were some preliminary concepts (Supplementary Material 2) regarding the potential impacts of the KBP project proposed by the key stakeholder, the MHC. The preliminary concepts were considered as codes while analyzing the qualitative data.

Codes relevant to the research questions were generated inductively and deductively based on the semantic meaning of the responses and the latent meaning or the interpretation of the contents. The codes could be modified iteratively throughout the coding process to accommodate new ideas. After the codes were created, a cluster of codes sharing unifying features were combined into a higher-level subtheme or theme depending on how well it described a coherent and meaningful pattern in the data. The codes arising from the transcripts and the theme framework were discussed with other researchers to achieve a consensus, which would enhance the trustworthiness and credibility of the results. Themes concerning the research questions were reviewed and adjusted to capture better the overall tone of the entire dataset. Finally, the patterns and relationships of the themes were interpreted. The participation level (full or partial) was also added as an attribute of classification to the participants. Commonalities and variations of the themes were compared between participants in different participation groups.

Results

Quantitative Results

There were 1239 participants who registered online to take part in the KBP card exchange, of which 709 had valid baseline measures, and 289 had both valid baseline and follow-up measures. Details regarding the number of individuals at each stage of the project are provided in Figure 1.

Descriptive Characteristics

For the participants in our study who completed both questionnaires, most people (N = 254, 88%) were aged between 18 and 60 years, with equal numbers (44%) in the 19–40 and 41–60 categories. Most participants were female (N = 271, 94%) and white (N = 278, 96%). The mean wellbeing score for these participants was 20.7 (SD = 3.48) at baseline and was 21.5 (SD = 3.86) at follow-up. Eighty one percent (N = 229) of participants sent and received a card, and 19% (N = 54) only sent but did not receive one. Hardly anybody (N = 6) received a card but did not send one. Compared to the people who only completed the before-questionnaire, participants completing both questionnaires were generally older and had a lower baseline sense of belonging. There was also weak evidence that more completers were female. There was no evidence of any other differences between completers and non-completers. Table 1 shows participants’ descriptive statistics summarized according to completer status.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and baseline measures of participants.

Analysis for Research Question 1

A simple linear regression provided strong evidence that participants had greater wellbeing after taking part in the program (estimated change from baseline to follow-up: 0.77; 95% CI: 0.44–1.10; p < 0.001; standardized effect size = 0.21). Baseline measures of our secondary outcomes were not collinear with wellbeing or each other (see Table 2), so we proceeded to analysis of our secondary outcomes: loneliness, hope and belongingness.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlations between baseline wellbeing, loneliness, sense of belonging and hope.

There was strong evidence that loneliness scores decreased from baseline to follow-up (estimated change: –0.28; 95% CI: –0.43 to –0.13; p < 0.001; standardized effect size = 0.15), and sense of belonging also improved (estimated change: 1.98; 95% CI: 1.44–2.52; p < 0.001; standardized effect size = 0.37). Although the majority of participants (n = 203, 70%) reported no change in hope, and the probability of no change was greater than three times the probability of experiencing a positive change in hope (relative risk ratio for positive change vs. no change: 0.32; 95% CI: 0.24–0.42; p < 0.001), there was also strong evidence that an increase in hope was three times more likely than a decrease (relative risk ratio: 3.10; 95% CI: 1.89–5.06; p < 0.001). Table 3 provides further details.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Estimated change in wellbeing, loneliness, belonging and hope and associations with change in wellbeing.

Analysis for Research Question 2

There was no evidence that baseline loneliness was associated with wellbeing improvements (p = 0.751) or that level of participation in the project was related to wellbeing changes (p = 0.587). There was no evidence that change in wellbeing differed across age groups (global p = 0.379). Please see Table 3 for further details.

Qualitative Results

Four overarching themes were identified with the 289 transcripts: motivators, potential mechanisms, project impacts, evaluations and suggestions for improvements (see Table 4). There were no additional codes or themes added after checking the other 99 transcripts.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Themes generated regarding how KBP was experienced by participants and how KBP achieved the benefits.

Theme 1. Motivators

The first theme captures the reasons why participants decided to take part in the project.

Sub-Theme 1a. Altruism

Participants perceived that the KBP project could help others, which motivated them to initiate the kindness behaviors and take part. They considered that their kindness of sending a card would benefit others; for example, ‘It is a brilliant way to show kindness and help uplift a stranger (ID32).’ Some participants also noted that taking part in the project gave them an opportunity to show care toward others, ‘I hoped the recipient knew someone was thinking about them (ID41).’

Sub-Theme 1b. Anticipate Receiving

Participants stated that taking part in this exchange program enabled them to look forward to receiving a handmade card from a strange. Responders wrote that ‘I looked forward to receiving the card all week and checked the post more often than I normally would (ID206).’

Sub-Theme 1c. Difficult Time

Some participants reflected that they faced mental difficulties, stress or low mood when the project was advertised. Therefore, they hoped to take part in this positive project with an expectation of feeling more encouraged. One responder commented that ‘Valentine’s Day was a sad day for me this year (ID187).’ Participants also felt lonely during Valentine’s Day, making them more willing to connect to the world, ‘As a single person, I guess I can feel a little left out on Valentine’s Day (ID179).’

Theme 2. Potential Mechanisms

Participants described four potential mechanisms that may influence their experience of the program.

Sub-Theme 2a. Pleasure in Making a Card

Many participants mentioned that they enjoyed the processes of making a card because they could slow down and spend time being creative and making artistic items. This process promoted self-care. Participants wrote that ‘I loved making the card and being creative (ID72); It made me think what would make me happy (ID164).’

Sub-Theme 2b. Pleasure in Sending a Card

Some people stated that they enjoyed giving something that others might find helpful, ‘I sent two cards, and both individually handmade by me, and if it brightened someone’s day, then I’m delighted (ID28).’

Sub-Theme 2c. Individual Fulfillment

Participants obtained personal fulfillment by taking part in such a meaningful and national-wide project. The sending experiences made them feel proud of themselves, ‘Sending someone a card of good wishes made me feel useful (ID35).’

Sub-Theme 2d. Appreciate Other’s Thoughts and Behaviors

A substantial number of participants commented that it was really nice to receive a card from a stranger, and they appreciated others making beautiful handmade cards with thoughtful messages; they felt cared for by others, ‘Really appreciated the words and effort (ID84).’

Theme 3. Project Impacts

This theme captures the perceived project impacts.

Sub-Theme 3a. Positive Affective Impacts

Participants had positive changes in their mood by taking part in the project. They felt joyful, excited, warmed and inspired after the card-exchange, ‘It gave me a lovely warm feeling for days afterward (ID144).’

Sub-Theme 3b. Feel the Self Is Special and Valued

Receiving a card and performing a highly meaningful task that benefited others made people feel valued and special to themselves and others, ‘Receiving it made me feel very special (ID88).’

Sub-Theme 3c. Connection

Participants reflected that the exchange program provided an opportunity to connect to others despite being strangers. Hence, they felt less lonely: ‘I feel connected to my ‘senders,’ even though I don’t know them (ID200); It made me feel less lonely in the world (ID12).’ Participants also found that the project restored their faith in humanity: ‘Restored some faith in the kindness of people (ID162).’

Sub-Theme 3d. Negative Experiences

There were only few negative experiences compared to positive impacts reported in the responses. Some people felt sad when they did not receive a card, ‘I found it hard not receiving a card. Felt disappointed and sad (ID136).’ Others felt disappointed getting an inappropriate card, “upon opening I got a little disheartened as the person clearly hadn’t put as much effort in (ID54).” Additionally, the stress felt when attempting to make a good card was also a negative experience for some, ‘I felt quite pressured to create something worthy of sending (ID68).’

Theme 4. Evaluations and Suggestions for Improvements

This theme describes participants’ appraisal of the project and participants’ advice for improving it in the future.

Sub-Theme 4a. Positive Project Evaluations

Participants commented that they loved the idea of the project, which was relatively simple in its procedures but was highly positive and spread kindness, ‘I love the idea of random acts of kindness (ID53)’; ‘Such a great movement (ID185).’

Sub-Theme 4b. Unpredictable

Participants noted many uncertainties in the project. For example, they were unsure about the recipient’s responses when receiving the card, ‘Weird to not know how they were received (ID3).’ Moreover, participants understood that there was no guarantee of receiving a card, which may be a risk for those who were vulnerable and did not receive a card, ‘It could devastate someone who is very lonely and depressed if they did not get one (ID72).’

Sub-Theme 4c. Suggestions for Improvements

Participants provided some suggestions for improving the project. For example, they considered that improving the project’s publicity and providing participants with confirmation that their card had been received, and that they would receive a back-up card if they requested one, could be useful. They suggested that the back-up system would raise a second expectation of receiving a card, but it might even be hurtful if the additional card was not received; thus, this back-up system should be further developed. Further details about the stranger, such as their age or more personalized information, might be helpful when making the cards.

Overall, few substantial differences were observed in participants’ program experiences with different participation levels (sent and received/sent but not received/not sent but received). However, among the six participants who did not send but did receive a card, none of them identified the project could be an altruistic action, and this group exclusively reported the code of guilt (see Supplementary Material 3). Moreover, compared to the others, participants who sent but did not receive a card responded more about the disappointment of not receiving a card and had less positive affective emotions. People who sent and received a card reported more individual fulfillments compared to the other two groups. Further details about the themes and codes can be found in Supplementary Material 3.

Discussion

Main Findings and Interpretations

The current study evaluated a nationwide participatory public mental health project and has provided preliminary indications that the KBP project may help improve people’s wellbeing, loneliness and sense of belonging. The 0.77-point increase on the SWEMWBS measure for the KBP only has a small standardized effect size just above 0.2; however, this meets established thresholds for a meaningful, non-negligible change (Shah et al., 2018). The results also suggest that, although this project might not affect any change in hope for most people, taking part in the project is more likely to result in increased hope for the future than a loss of hope. Contrary to the hypotheses, the results provided no evidence that the level of loneliness at baseline affected the impacts of GBV on participants’ wellbeing. There was also no evidence of differences in wellbeing outcomes between people who sent and received a card and those who only gave or only received a card.

Although there was, on average, a small change in wellbeing and loneliness found in the quantitative results, the experiences shared in the qualitative results suggested that the experience of taking part in the project could be joyous and warm, which had quite large and sustained affective impacts for some. Qualitative and quantitative results both suggested that sending the kindness cards in this project could improve wellbeing, which further supported the evidence in the previous literature that performing acts of kindness promotes wellbeing and affective emotions (Curry et al., 2018, p. 4). Furthermore, the qualitative results indicated that the process of making and sending was highly positive for people, with engaging in a creative act and helping others both being important to many participants. These observations may help explain the quantitative finding that improvements in wellbeing were not different for those who only gave a card from those who also received one.

The qualitative results also revealed some potential mechanisms explaining how the KBP project may have helped people improve wellbeing and loneliness. First, the participants perceived the project to be an altruistic action benefiting others, which motivated them to send cards to strangers. Participants could increase personal fulfillment by thinking that they were performing a significantly meaningful task to help others, increasing their self-esteem and happiness. This finding aligns with the previous literature noting that people could derive satisfaction and gain more resources from the kindness behaviors that help others, which makes them happy (Curry et al., 2018, p. 11).

The findings also illustrated that enjoyment in making cards allowed people respite from the pressures of life and spend time being creative, which promoted self-care and made them feel joyful. This observation agrees with some studies proposing that engaging in creative activities may enhance positive mood and make people flourish (Dunn et al., 2008; Forgeard and Eichner, 2014). The card-making processes enabled people to search for positive quotes, poems and goodwill messages to write kind words, and it also allowed them to make an artistic card creatively. Lomas (2016) suggested that art and literature integrate the essence of humanity, and such artistic expression and appreciation helps people make sense of their lives and enriches their experiences, both of which can substantially improve wellbeing. Ryan and Deci (2000) also proposed that engaging in creative activities could satisfy the need for autonomy, which may boost wellbeing. Creatively developing good ideas offers a sense that one could master a piece of work. This self-sufficiency might evoke the positive emotions of pleasure and pride (Amabile et al., 2005, p. 369), which are the key components in wellbeing enhancement.

Gratitude for others’ efforts for the kind messages and handmade cards was also shown to not only make people feel excited and warmed while receiving, but it also enabled them to feel special and cherished. Consequently, participants could establish a close connection with the sender and the world because they felt cared for and loved by others. The benefits of receiving kindness that have been evidenced were mainly about the positive affective impacts (Lyubomirsky and Layous, 2013; Pressman et al., 2015; Nadler, 2017) or one’s self-efficacy, feeling of fulfillment and closeness in intimate relationships (Gleason et al., 2003, p. 1042). The qualitative results in this study suggested that receiving kindness from the KBP could improve wellbeing, and it could also help with the collective aspects of loneliness that people felt more included in society. However, due to the positive impacts of receiving kindness, the experiences of not receiving a card might be a potential barrier to the project’s benefits for some people.

Strengths and Limitations

This study is novel in utilizing a mixed-methods design to understand how kindness acts in a public mental health project improve wellbeing and reduce loneliness for both the giver and the receiver. It provides insight into the potential mechanisms explaining which components in the kind acts could enhance wellbeing and reduce loneliness. It also has the significant advantage of using nationwide data to explore the effectiveness of a public program involving acts of kindness for promoting general wellbeing and social connection in a large social community.

Despite these strengths, there are still some limitations identified in this study. First, there was no control group in this pre–post study. As a result, it is not possible to draw strong inferences about the effectiveness of the KBP intervention (Fitz-Gibbon and Morris, 1987), i.e., it is unclear whether the improvements in wellbeing and loneliness were entirely attributed to the impacts of taking part in KBP, or people merely felt better after Valentine’s Day – for instance, because Valentine’s Day was over, or with the flourish of spring.

Regarding the study measures: to maintain a good response rate, the questionnaire was designed to be sufficiently brief to capture the four individual outcomes (Edwards et al., 2002). Thus, there were a limited number of questions extracted from the structured measures for belongingness and hope, which might potentially reduce the measures’ reliability (Goodman et al., 2015). This brief-measure issue was particularly prominent when analyzing hope. Merely including a single item made it less sensitive for discriminating the change in hope over time, which might explain why most participants reported no change in hope. Moreover, this study only used brief online free-text responses for the qualitative analysis. The content in these materials was not always clear or in-depth; hence, it might be difficult to capture a full understanding of the experiences of the project.

Regarding the participants of KBP, participants in the current datasets were mostly white and female, potentially because the project was called ‘Great British Valentines,’ which failed to attract some ethnic minority groups from distinct cultures or religions that do not celebrate Valentine’s Day. Therefore, the results might not generalize to ethnic minorities or to men. In addition, the insufficient number of people from non-White British ethnic groups does not allow us to explore whether this cultural homogeneity may enhance the program’s effects, if a card is received from someone with some shared cultural experience and perspectives, or conversely whether exchanges with people different from oneself are even more connecting and powerful. Furthermore, we lacked data about other characteristics of interest for participants, for instance their socio-economic or marital/partnership status, with which to describe our sample or explore potential moderators of the program’s effects. Regarding the data available to the researchers, participants who continued to complete the questionnaire at follow-up were generally older and had a lower level of belongingness than those who only completed the baseline measures. Therefore, there might be an attrition bias in the study data. Additionally, there were only six people who did not send but received a card. Therefore, this study might miss the experiences shared by this group, and whether the KBP helped them was unclear. Finally, the collection date for the ‘After questionnaire’ was only 1 week after the intervention. This study does not tell us whether the enhancement in wellbeing and decrease in loneliness due to this project would be maintained over the longer term.

Implications

Implications for Practice

The current study has shown that the KPB project has the potential to enhance wellbeing and reduce loneliness for the general population. This supports providing more KBP card exchanges in more contexts in the future, particularly during periods when people are vulnerable to mental or physical difficulties due to social isolation or natural disasters (Emerson et al., 2020), although sufficiently powered randomized controlled trials are required to provide more robust evidence on efficacy. It could be an inexpensive intervention to improve public wellbeing and reduce loneliness worldwide for people under the social isolation orders due to the COVID-19 pandemic (Grover et al., 2020; Clair et al., 2021). The mean baseline wellbeing score for the participants in the current study is below population norms (Women: M = 23.7, SD = 3.99; Men: M = 23.6, SD = 3.92) (Ng Fat et al., 2017), suggesting that KBP was reaching people in need of help with improving wellbeing, which supports the value of KBP as a beneficial public health project.

However, our study suggests it may be helpful to strengthen some procedures to maximize benefits and mitigate any negative experiences of the KBP project. First, our qualitative findings suggest that not receiving a card may reduce the likelihood of project benefits. Therefore, strengthening the back-up system to provide an additional card may be helpful, to ensure that everyone could receive a card. It may also be helpful to set up a way for participants to confirm online that they have sent a card; otherwise, they could receive a reminder. Additionally, it might be helpful for participants to know whether their card was received by the recipient in a direct or indirect feedback system. People may derive satisfaction from their kindness behaviors that are appreciated by others, potentially enhancing the project’s effectiveness (Ouweneel et al., 2014; Curry et al., 2018, p. 11).

Implications for Research

Most importantly, it is desirable to utilize a more robust design in future evaluations, such as randomized control trials that introduce a comparison group to obtain more robust evidence of the project’s effectiveness. To explore the generalizability of our results, further research could recruit more participants with various demographic characteristics and cultural backgrounds in other KBP projects (such as currently planned MHC projects aimed at Pentecostal Christian churches over Easter, a new trial of Ramadan KBP or one for elderly people in care homes) to obtain more evidence across a broader population. Comparisons between demographic subgroups could potentially explore the influence of cultural homogeneity on the effectiveness of the KBP. It is also of interest for future evaluations to include a wider range of measures including not only wellbeing, loneliness, hope and sense of belonging, but also fulfillment, self-esteem and the positive affective emotions that were the project’s impacts as reported in the current qualitative results. More in-depth qualitative interviews with participants are also necessary to help understand the mechanisms and experiences better.

Another intriguing direction for future research would be to analyze how long the positive outcomes are maintained after the kindness interventions. The creative processes in the kindness acts that promote wellbeing and affective emotions might last no more than 2 days (Amabile et al., 2005; Conner et al., 2018). Ouweneel et al. (2014) suggested that the effects of kindness acts could lessen over time, whereas Seligman et al. (2005) argued that such effects could last for several months. Therefore, future research could potentially involve a longitudinal study tracking the KBP project’s impacts. Additionally, the positive outcomes of this project were achieved with participants whose average wellbeing scores were nearly three quarters of a standard deviation below the population norm (Ng Fat et al., 2017). Therefore, future studies could investigate whether the KBP might also work well specifically for people with depression. Finally, researchers could also perform a cost-effectiveness study for the KBP project to determine whether the modest gains in wellbeing and loneliness found in this study represent good value for money.

Conclusion

This study showed preliminary evidence that the KBP project may enhance wellbeing and reduce loneliness. The sending process seems to play a crucial role in the main positive impacts of the project. Qualitative reports suggests that altruism motivates people to initiate kindness behaviors, through which people may obtain personal fulfillment, and this could potentially enhance wellbeing. Moreover, receiving kindness enhances self-esteem and enables participants to perceive a connection with the sender who provides kindness, even though they are a stranger and there is no ongoing contact. Thus, the social connection might reduce participants’ loneliness. Therefore, this study supports providing future KBP projects, as a new initiative that is not only simple and cheap but may also be powerful for wellbeing promotion and loneliness prevention in the community.

Data Availability Statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: The datasets analyzed during the current study are not publicly available due the protection of the confidentiality of research participants, but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to BL-E, Yi5sbG95ZC1ldmFuc0B1Y2wuYWMudWs=.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University College London Research Ethics Committee. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

CW performed the data analysis, drafted the ethical application forms and the research report. BL-E and EP helped proceed the ethical approval, revised the qualitative coding framework, and provided feedback to the report to improve the writing-up. RJ helped with the quantitative data analysis. All the authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

EP and BL-E are supported by funding from UK Research and Innovation, through the Loneliness and Social Isolation in Mental Health Research Network (Grant Number: ES/S004440/1).

Author Disclaimer

The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of UKRI.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank Amy Pollard, the Director of the Mental Health Collective and organizer of the ‘Kindness by Post’ project, for her support for this study, for providing access to the study data and for contributing to development of themes for the qualitative analysis. We also extend thanks to the participants for contributing their data to the current research.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.813432/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

KBP, Kindness by Post; GBV, Great British Valentines; SWEMWBS, Short Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Wellbeing Scale; UCLA, University of California at Los Angeles; GBS, General Belongingness Scale; UCL, University College London.

References

Amabile, T. M., Barsade, S. G., Mueller, J. S., and Staw, B. M. (2005). Affect and creativity at work. Adm. Sci. Q. 50, 367–403. doi: 10.2189/asqu.2005.50.3.367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Apaolaza, V., Hartmann, P., Medina, E., Barrutia, J. M., and Echebarria, C. (2013). The relationship between socializing on the Spanish online networking site Tuenti and teenagers’ subjective wellbeing: the roles of self-esteem and loneliness. Comput. Hum. Behav. 29, 1282–1289. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., and Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II). London, UK: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Braun, V., and Clarke, V. (2012). “Thematic analysis,” in APA Handbook of Research Methods in Psychology, Vol 2: Research Designs:Quantitative, Qualitative, Neuropsychological, and Biological, eds H. Cooper, P. M. Camic, D. L. Long, A. T. Panter, D. Rindskopf, and K. J. Sher (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 57–71. doi: 10.1037/13620-004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., and Hawkley, L. C. (2003). Social isolation and health, with an emphasis on underlying mechanisms. Perspect Biol. Med. 46, S39–S52.

Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., Ernst, J. M., Burleson, M. H., McClintock, M. K., Malarkey, W. B., Hawkley, L. C., et al. (2000). Lonely traits and concomitant physiological processes: the MacArthur social neuroscience studies. Inter. J. Psychophysiol. 35, 143–154. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(99)00049-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, S., Grippo, A. J., London, S., Goossens, L., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2015). Loneliness: clinical import and interventions. Perspect Psychol. Sci. 10, 238–249. doi: 10.1177/1745691615570616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clair, R., Gordon, M., Kroon, M., and Reilly, C. (2021). The effects of social isolation on well-being and life satisfaction during pandemic. Humanit. Soc. Sci. Commun. 1:28. doi: 10.1057/s41599-021-00710-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conner, T. S., DeYoung, C. G., and Silvia, P. J. (2018). Everyday creative activity as a path to flourishing. J. Posit. Psychol. 13, 181–189. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2016.1257049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curry, O. S., Rowland, L. A., Van Lissa, C. J., Zlotowitz, S., McAlaney, J., and Whitehouse, H. (2018). Happy to help? J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 76, 320–329. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2018.02.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Jong-Gierveld, J. (1998). A review of loneliness:concepts and definitions, determinants and consequences. Rev. Clin. Gerontol. 8, 73–80. doi: 10.1017/S0959259898008090

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dormann, C. F., Elith, J., Bacher, S., Buchmann, C., Carl, G., Carré, G., et al. (2013). Collinearity: a review of methods to deal with it and a simulation study evaluating their performance. Ecography 36, 27–46. doi: 10.1111/J.1600-0587.2012.07348.X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, E. W., Aknin, L. B., and Norton, M. I. (2008). Spending money on others promotes happiness. Science 319, 1687–1688. doi: 10.1126/science.1150952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, P., Roberts, I., Clarke, M., DiGuiseppi, C., Pratap, S., Wentz, R., et al. (2002). Increasing response rates to postal questionnaires: systematic review. BMJ 324:1183. doi: 10.1136/bmj.324.7347.1183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emerson, E., Fortune, N., Aitken, Z., Hatton, C., Stancliffe, R., and Llewellyn, G. (2020). The wellbeing of working-age adults with and without disability in the UK:associations with age, gender, ethnicity, partnership status, educational attainment and employment status. Disabil. Health J. 13:100889. doi: 10.1016/j.dhjo.2020.100889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filep, S., Macnaughton, J., Glover, T., Filep, S., Macnaughton, J., and Glover, T. (2017). Tourism and gratitude:valuing acts of kindness. Ann. Tour. Res. 66, 26–36. doi: 10.1016/J.ANNALS.2017.05.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitz-Gibbon, C. T., and Morris, L. L. (1987). How To Design a Program Evaluation. Program Evaluation Kit. Durham, UK: University of Durham.

Google Scholar

Forgeard, M. J. C., and Eichner, V. (2014). “Creativity as a Target and Tool for Positive Interventions,” in The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Positive Psychological Interventions, eds A. C. Parks and S. M. Schueller (New Jersey, US: Willey Blackwell), 135–154. doi: 10.1002/9781118315927.CH7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gleason, M. E., Iida, M., Bolger, N., and Shrout, P. E. (2003). Daily supportive equity in close relationships. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29, 1036–1045. doi: 10.1177/0146167203253473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, A., Wrigley, J., Silversides, K., and Venus-Balgobin, N. (2015). Measuring Your Impact on Loneliness in Later Life. London, UK: Campaign to End Loneliness.

Google Scholar

Grover, S., Sahoo, S., Mehra, A., Avasthi, A., Tripathi, A., Subramanyan, A., et al. (2020). Psychological impact of COVID-19 lockdown:an online survey from India. Indian J. Psychiatry 62, 354–362. doi: 10.4103/psychiatry.IndianJPsychiatry_427_20

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawkley, L. C., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: a theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Ann. Beh. Med. 40, 218–227. doi: 10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houghton, S., Hattie, J., Carroll, A., Wood, L., and Baffour, B. (2016). It Hurts To Be Lonely! Loneliness and Positive Mental Wellbeing in Australian Rural and Urban Adolescents. J. Psychol. Couns. Sch. 26, 52–67. doi: 10.1017/JGC.2016.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaffke, L. (2018). Can “Acts of Kindness “enhance mental well-being and flourishing in individuals from the general Dutch population?. Ph.D thesis, Netherlands: University of Twente.

Google Scholar

Layous, K., Lee, H., Choi, I., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2013). Culture matters when designing a successful happiness-increasing activity:acomparison of the United States and South Korea. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 44, 1294–1303. doi: 10.1177/0022022113487591

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Layous, K., Nelson, S. K., Kurtz, J. L., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2017). What triggers prosocial effort? A positive feedback loop between positive activities, kindness, and well-being. J. Posit. Psychol. 12, 385–398. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2016.1198924

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S. L., Pearce, E., Ajnakina, O., Johnson, S., Lewis, G., Mann, F., et al. (2021). The association between loneliness and depressive symptoms among adults aged 50 years and older: a 12-year population-based cohort study. Lancet Psychiatry 8, 48–57. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30383-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lomas, T. (2016). Positive art:artistic expression and appreciation as an exemplary vehicle for flourishing. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 20, 171–182. doi: 10.1037/gpr0000073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyubomirsky, S., and Layous, K. (2013). How do simple positive activities increase well-being? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22, 57–62. doi: 10.1177/0963721412469809

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., and Diener, E. (2005a). The benefits of frequent positive affect: does happiness lead to success? Psychol. Bull. 131, 803–855. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., and Schkade, D. (2005b). Pursuing happiness: The architecture of sustainable change. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 9, 111–131. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.9.2.111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malone, G. P., Pillow, D. R., and Osman, A. (2012). The general belongingness scale (GBS): assessing achieved belongingness. Person. Individ. Differ. 52, 311–316. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masi, C. M., Chen, H. Y., Hawkley, L. C., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2011). A meta-analysis of interventions to reduce loneliness. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 15, 219–266. doi: 10.1177/1088868310377394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murberg, T. A. (2004). Long-term effect of social relationships on mortality in patients with congestive heart failure. Int. J. Psychiatry Med. 34, 207–217. doi: 10.2190/gkj2-p8bd-v59x-mjnq

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nadler, A. (2017). The human essence in helping relations:belongingness, independence, and status. Oxford Handb. Hum. Essence 1, 123–134.

Google Scholar

Ng Fat, L., Scholes, S., Boniface, S., Mindell, J., and Stewart-Brown, S. (2017). Evaluating and establishing national norms for mental wellbeing using the short Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (SWEMWBS): findings from the Health Survey for England. Q. Life Res. 26, 1129–1144. doi: 10.1007/s11136-016-1454-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Brien, B. C., Harris, I. B., Beckman, T. J., Reed, D. A., and Cook, D. A. (2014). Standards for reporting qualitative research: a synthesis of recommendations. Acad. Med. 89, 1245–1251. doi: 10.1097/acm.0000000000000388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Otake, K., Shimai, S., Tanaka-Matsumi, J., Otsui, K., and Fredrickson, B. L. (2006). HAPPY PEOPLE BECOME HAPPIER THROUGH KINDNESS: A COUNTING KINDNESSES INTERVENTION. J. Happiness Stud. 7, 361–375. doi: 10.1007/s10902-005-3650-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Otnes, C., Ruth, J. A., and Milbourne, C. C. (1994). The Pleasure and Pain of Being Close: Men’s Mixed Feelings About Participation in Valentine’s Day Gift Exchange. North American: Advances in Consumer Research.

Google Scholar

Ouweneel, E., Le Blanc, P. M., and Schaufeli, W. B. (2014). On being grateful and kind: results of two randomized controlled trials on study-related emotions and academic engagement. J. Psychol. 148, 37–60. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2012.742854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perlman, D., and Peplau, L. A. (1981). “Personal Relationships: 3. Relationships in Disorder,” in The Toward a Social Psychology of Loneliness, eds R. Gilmour and S. Duck (London: Academic Press), 31–56.

Google Scholar

Pressman, S. D., Kraft, T. L., and Cross, M. P. (2015). It’s good to do good and receive good:the impact of a ‘pay it forward’style kindness intervention on giver and receiver well-being. J. Posit. Psychol. 10, 293–302. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2014.965269

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, D. W. (1996). UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): reliability, validity, and factor structure. J. Pers. Assess 66, 20–40. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6601_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am. Psychol. 55, 68–78.

Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52, 141–166. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 57, 1069–1081.

Google Scholar

Ryff, C. D. (2014). Psychological well-being revisited: advances in the science and practice of eudaimonia. Psychother. Psychosom 83, 10–28. doi: 10.1159/000353263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E., Steen, T. A., Park, N., and Peterson, C. (2005). Positive psychology progress: empirical validation of interventions. Am. Psychol. 60, 410–421. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.5.410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shah, N., Cader, M., Andrews, W. P., Wijesekera, D., and Stewart-Brown, S. L. (2018). Responsiveness of the Short Warwick Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (SWEMWBS): evaluation a clinical sample. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 16:239. doi: 10.1186/s12955-018-1060-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sin, N. L., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2009). Enhancing well-being and alleviating depressive symptoms with positive psychology interventions: a practice-friendly meta-analysis. J. Clin. Psychol. 65, 467–487. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart-Brown, S., Tennant, A., Tennant, R., Platt, S., Parkinson, J., and Weich, S. (2009). Internal construct validity of the Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (WEMWBS): a Rasch analysis using data from the Scottish Health Education Population Survey. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 7:15. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-7-15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valtorta, N., and Hanratty, B. (2012). Loneliness, isolation and the health of older adults: do we need a new research agenda? J. R. Soc. Med. 105, 518–522. doi: 10.1258/jrsm.2012.120128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Mann, F., Lloyd-Evans, B., Ma, R., and Johnson, S. (2018). Associations between loneliness and perceived social support and outcomes of mental health problems: a systematic review. BMC Psychiatry 18:156. doi: 10.1186/s12888-018-1736-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, L. A., Westerhof, G. J., and Bohlmeijer, E. T. (2016). Can We Increase Psychological Well-Being? The Effects of Interventions on Psychological Well-Being:a Meta-Analysis of Randomized Controlled Trials. PLoS one 11:e0158092. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0158092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, A. M., and Joseph, S. (2010). The absence of positive psychological (eudemonic) well-being as a risk factor for depression: a ten year cohort study. J. Affect. Disord. 122, 213–217. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.06.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: kindness, wellbeing, loneliness, belongingness, public health

Citation: Wang C, Pearce E, Jones R and Lloyd-Evans B (2022) ‘Kindness by Post’: A Mixed-Methods Evaluation of a Participatory Public Mental Health Project. Front. Psychol. 12:813432. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.813432

Received: 11 November 2021; Accepted: 31 December 2021;
Published: 07 February 2022.

Edited by:

Jingyi Wang, Fudan University, China

Reviewed by:

Na Wang, Fudan University, China
Lu Niu, Central South University, China

Copyright © 2022 Wang, Pearce, Jones and Lloyd-Evans. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Brynmor Lloyd-Evans, Yi5sbG95ZC1ldmFuc0B1Y2wuYWMudWs=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.