- Developmental Ethology and Cognitive Psychology Laboratory, Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l’Alimentation, Inrae, AgroSup Dijon, CNRS (UMR 6265), Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, Dijon, France
A recent body of research has emerged regarding the interactions between olfaction and other sensory channels to process social information. The current review examines the influence of body odors on face perception, a core component of human social cognition. First, we review studies reporting how body odors interact with the perception of invariant facial information (i.e., identity, sex, attractiveness, trustworthiness, and dominance). Although we mainly focus on the influence of body odors based on axillary odor, we also review findings about specific steroids present in axillary sweat (i.e., androstenone, androstenol, androstadienone, and estratetraenol). We next survey the literature showing body odor influences on the perception of transient face properties, notably in discussing the role of body odors in facilitating or hindering the perception of emotional facial expression, in relation to competing frameworks of emotions. Finally, we discuss the developmental origins of these olfaction-to-vision influences, as an emerging literature indicates that odor cues strongly influence face perception in infants. Body odors with a high social relevance such as the odor emanating from the mother have a widespread influence on various aspects of face perception in infancy, including categorization of faces among other objects, face scanning behavior, or facial expression perception. We conclude by suggesting that the weight of olfaction might be especially strong in infancy, shaping social perception, especially in slow-maturing senses such as vision, and that this early tutoring function of olfaction spans all developmental stages to disambiguate a complex social environment by conveying key information for social interactions until adulthood.
Introduction
Humans have long been considered by academia as “microsmatic” mammals, with the underlying idea that the sense of smell plays only a minor role in human behavior and cognition. This flawed conception may be traced back to the 19th-Century writings of the comparative neuroanatomist Paul Broca (notably Broca, 1888; Schaal and Porter, 1991; cf. McGann, 2017). But Broca had illustrious contemporaries who shared similar ideas [e.g., Darwin who affirmed that, to Man, “the sense of smell is of extremely slight service, if any” (Darwin, 1871/1992)], and numerous were the followers who forced this line of reasoning in propagating an explicit conceptual glide from Broca’s initial structural observations into unfounded functional inferences (e.g., Freud, 1930/1962). This “microsmaty fallacy” (Lundström and Olsson, 2010) long pervaded scientific psychology for a variety of reasons (Sela and Sobel, 2010; McGann, 2017), including the consideration that the human vomeronasal organ is vestigial (Doty, 2001; Stowers and Spehr, 2015), the yet unsuccessful search for human pheromones (Doty, 2010; Wyatt, 2015, 2017), and the fact that humans had no obvious scent glands (Stoddart, 1990; Schaal and Porter, 1991). Recently, however, a growing amount of studies in biology, psychology, and anthropology stressed the overlooked role of olfaction in human behavior, and in particular in social cognition (e.g., Stoddart, 1990; Schaal and Porter, 1991; Classen et al., 2002; Stevenson, 2010; de Groot et al., 2017; Pause, 2017). As recent reviews have excellently covered the effects on face perception of arbitrary odors selected for their hedonic valence (Syrjänen et al., 2021, see also Spence, 2021), the present essay will survey investigations on how natural human body odors, or isolated compounds therein, can modulate the perception and emotional appraisal of faces. Our analysis of the current literature will exploit the lens of face processing under various olfactory influences in following evolutionary and developmental perspectives. Note that the current review is narrative and includes various papers selected without systematic methodology. However, the target articles included in the review (i.e., those involving specifically body odors influence on face perception) have been selected from a screening of databases (PubMed, Medline, and Web of Science) and the use of search engines (Google Scholar, Google) using the following keywords and their combinations: “body odor,” “face,” “multisensory,” “infants.” Papers related to non-human populations, and/or artificial odors were not included. Studies cited in the selected papers were also reviewed, and included whenever relevant to the topic.
The Social Nose
Both face and voice convey a wealth of visual and acoustic information upon which humans rely ubiquitously to organize their everyday social interactions (e.g., Latinus and Belin, 2011; Quinn and Macrae, 2011; Pascalis et al., 2014; Schirmer and Adolphs, 2017). But, aside from vision and audition, olfaction increasingly appears as another effective means to sample social information from conspecifics, especially through the perception of volatile compounds emitted from multiple bodily sites (Schaal and Porter, 1991; Havlicek and Roberts, 2009; Pause, 2012; Lübke and Pause, 2015; de Groot et al., 2017). Olfactory communication is advantageous for several reasons (Wyatt, 2003). First, it can function when vision and audition are restricted or absent (e.g., in noisy or dark environment: Doty, 1986; in privation of vision: e.g., Cuevas et al., 2009). Second, odorants disperse in space and persist over time, allowing to communicate about past events (e.g., long after the sender’s departure can low volatile molecules inform about his/her state at the place of release; Pause, 2012), and making odors good candidates to support social communication (Wysocki and Preti, 2004; Doty, 2010; de Groot et al., 2017). However, an apparent pitfall of olfaction is that odor experience is notoriously hard to translate into words (Engen, 1987; Yeshurun and Sobel, 2010), which undermines the usefulness of self-reports. This led some researchers to consider olfaction as a “muted sense” (Olofsson and Gottfried, 2015). Humans as a species appear, however, surprisingly good at detecting and discriminating odors, although they struggle to identify them, especially in decontextualized laboratory experiments studying participants pertaining to WEIRD (western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic) and ODD (old, deodorized, and desensitized) societies (Henrich et al., 2010; Roberts et al., 2020). However, ethnographic and ethnolinguistic studies report elaborated ways to conceptualize and lexicalize everyday odor impressions in traditional societies, including those related to the human body, (e.g., Majid and Burenhult, 2014; Barkat-defradas and Motte-florac, 2016; Majid et al., 2018), showing that the human sense of smell is far from being muted, especially in cultures where it bears everyday survival value (Majid and Kruspe, 2018; Majid, 2021). Nevertheless, the olfaction-language conundrum remains persistent among experimentalists, inducing numerous laboratory studies that think to circumvent it in relying on implicit measurements (e.g., brain or autonomous nervous system activity), especially since individuals typically respond to odors without awareness (Pause et al., 1998; Pause, 2012), and/or in following indirect approached, for instance examining how olfaction impacts other sensory modalities, such as vision.
Various levels of social information can be conveyed through body odors, including individuality (Hold and Schleidt, 1977; Mallet and Schaal, 1998; Platek et al., 2001; Lenochova and Havlicek, 2008), self (Hold and Schleidt, 1977; Lord and Kasprzak, 1989; Mallet and Schaal, 1998; Platek et al., 2001), kin (Porter and Moore, 1981; Porter et al., 1983, 1985, 1986; Porter, 1998; Schaal and Marlier, 1998; Weisfeld et al., 2003; Lundström et al., 2009; Schäfer et al., 2020), age (Haze et al., 2001; Yamazaki et al., 2010; Mitro et al., 2012), sex (Russell, 1976; Doty et al., 1978; Schleidt, 1980), and personality (McBurney et al., 1976; Sorokowska et al., 2012, 2016; Sorokowska, 2013a,b). Body odor can also inform about transient states such as illness (Moshkin et al., 2012; Olsson et al., 2014; Newman and Buesching, 2019), and emotions (e.g., Chen and Haviland-Jones, 1999, 2000; Prehn et al., 2006; de Groot et al., 2012, 2020; Zheng et al., 2018; for reviews, see Pause, 2012; Calvi et al., 2020; Kontaris et al., 2020). Beyond the mere consideration of the ability of olfaction to cue different levels of social information, several questions emerge on the potential interactions between the sensory channels processing this social information.
To date, the influences of vision on olfaction have been extensively investigated, with for instance the report of a facilitated perception of odors presented simultaneously with semantically congruent objects (Gottfried and Dolan, 2003), or the striking result of oenology students providing an olfactory description of white wine artificially colored red (with an odorless dye) as a red wine (Morrot et al., 2001; for other studies linking color and olfactory perception see Zellner et al., 1991; Österbauer et al., 2005). Overall, these vision-to-olfaction influences were mostly studied in relation to the food domain (for a review, see Verhagen and Engelen, 2006). By contrast, the converse direction of modulation - visual perception by olfactory stimuli - is only beginning to be scrutinized (e.g., Kuang and Zhang, 2014). Olfaction may however prove especially useful to hone our interpretation of social interactions. Real-life facial cues are often ambiguous, and may hide or fake true intentions and feelings, especially compared with typical lab settings endorsing the use of stereotypical-posed faces (Aviezer et al., 2017; Barrett et al., 2019). Natural body odor cues are more difficult for an individual to manipulate than facial cues, and even though one may know what facial movements are expected in a particular context, these olfactory cues are however involuntarily emitted, actually sometimes unbeknownst to the individual himself. In sum, olfactory-visual face perception may assuage the consequences of social uncertainty. In the following subsections, we present studies examining the influence of human body odors on different aspects of face processing, from the perception of invariant aspect of faces (e.g., identity, sex, and attractiveness) to the processing of transient facial expression of emotions (for a summary of included papers see Supplementary Tables 1–4).
The Influence of Body Odors on the Processing of Invariant Face Characteristics
What makes a face attractive has been widely analyzed, and several determinants have now been identified including (but not limited to) symmetry, sexual dimorphism, averageness, and recently, facial adiposity and color (for reviews see Rhodes, 2006; Little et al., 2011; de Jager et al., 2018; Thorstenson, 2018). Under an evolutionary account that conceives facial attractiveness as an adaptation to solve the problem of mate choice, these components may serve as indicators of mate quality, such as health, resistance to parasites, or fertility (Thornhill and Gangestad, 1993; Little et al., 2011; Foo et al., 2017). However, and despite the popular maxim that “beauty is in the eye of the beholder,” face and person attractiveness is “more than meet the eyes” and is largely multimodal (Puts et al., 2012; Groyecka et al., 2017; but see Roth et al., 2021). With regard to mate selection, multisensory cues may well provide a better overall indication of mate quality than unisensory information. Indeed, even though one of these cues might not “honestly” advertises mate value (Zahavi, 1975), the simultaneous occurrence of fake quality cues on different sensory channels is unlikely (Rikowski and Grammer, 1999) – although nowadays that is why perfume and make-up are used (see Gaby and Zayas, 2017). Body odors may convey such a honest information as their saliency has been highlighted in mate selection (Havlicek et al., 2008; Lübke and Pause, 2015; White and Cunningham, 2017), and in the establishment of romantic relationships (for review, see Mahmut and Croy, 2019). Body odor pleasantness and facial attractiveness correlate weakly but significantly in both men (axillary odor: Roberts et al., 2011; Carrito et al., 2017; Mahmut et al., 2019; whole torso odor conveyed on a t-shirt: Gangestad and Thornhill, 1998; Rikowski and Grammer, 1999; Thornhill and Gangestad, 1999) and women (whole torso odor: Rikowski and Grammer, 1999; Thornhill and Gangestad, 1999; Thornhill et al., 2003), even though when it comes to mate choice, women are reportedly more prone than men to rely on olfaction (Herz and Cahill, 1997; Herz and Inzlicht, 2002; Havlicek et al., 2008; White and Cunningham, 2017; but see Foster, 2008).
Direct influence of body odors on the perception of face attractiveness has been reported, as implicit exposure to a male axillary odor increases attractiveness judgments of women toward male faces (Thorne et al., 2002), but not of males exposed to a female axillary odor and judging the attractiveness of female faces (Habel et al., 2021). The influence of body odors expands beyond attractiveness per se, following the “beautiful-is-good stereotype” (Dion et al., 1972; see also the “halo effect”; Nisbett and Wilson, 1977), according to which desirable traits are attributed to individuals considered as beautiful. Translated to olfactory issues, exposure to pleasant body odors (male axillary sweat) induced trustworthiness attribution in a Trust Game (Lobmaier et al., 2020), whereby participants gave more money to a trustee during the game. Similar findings were reported in women following exposure to an isolated component of human body effluvium (i.e., hexanal; van Nieuwenburg et al., 2019). Implicit exposure to hexanal not only affected interpersonal attribution of trust in a Trust Game, but also increased face trustworthiness ratings (van Nieuwenburg et al., 2019). The influence of the odorant on face trustworthiness ratings was particularly evident when presented outside of conscious awareness, thus ruling out an effect merely based on differences in odor intensity or pleasantness.
Recent findings have also highlighted the importance of the context and relationship status on these effects, as attractiveness judgments of highly attractive female faces decrease for pair-bonded, but not single men, implicitly exposed to the axillary odor of fertile (i.e., ovulating) women, interpreted as a defense against potential threats to the stability of their current relationship (Oren and Shamay-Tsoory, 2017). By contrast, women engaged in stable relationships showed a preference for the axillary odor of dominant men during their fertile phase, whereas single women did not (Havlicek et al., 2005; for similar results with face preference, see Little and Jones, 2012). Regarding dominance, although it is possible to make relatively accurate (i.e., above chance level) judgments of dominance based on body odor alone (worn t-shirt odor, Sorokowska et al., 2012; axillary odor, Sorokowska, 2013a,b; Sorokowska et al., 2016), the influence of body odor on the perception of facial dominance have not been formally examined so far.
Human body odors of various sources (e.g., axillae, skin, and breath) are discriminable by sex (e.g., Russell, 1976; Doty et al., 1978; Schleidt, 1980; Mutic et al., 2016), an ability thought to be mostly mediated by odor intensity (male axillary odors being rated as more intense than female axillary odors, Doty et al., 1978; Schleidt, 1980; Chen and Haviland-Jones, 1999), and fueled by the stereotypical overgeneralization that average men smell worse and more intensely than average women (Schleidt, 1980; Mutic et al., 2016; Carrito et al., 2017). Hence, body odors have been claimed to modulate face sex perception (Mutic et al., 2016). More precisely, androgynous faces (i.e., computer-generated faces constituted of equivalent female/male characteristic ratio) were rated as more feminine following exposure to a body odor (i.e., male or female axillary sweat) compared to a no-odor condition in female, but not male participants, which was interpreted as a femininity bias (Mutic et al., 2016). It is worth noting that both male and female participants displayed biased estimation of the androgynous faces in the absence of body odor, those faces being rated as slightly feminine and masculine by male and female participants, respectively. Thus, it could be considered that the body odor actually sharpened the perception of sex-ambiguous faces toward a more objective rating (i.e., closer to a sex-neutral estimation), rather than creating a true femininity bias.
Reminiscent of faces and fingerprints, body odors are idiosyncratic (Nicolaides, 1974; Penn et al., 2007), and their composition is largely under genetic control (Kuhn and Natsch, 2009; Natsch and Emter, 2020) producing a stable “odorprint,” albeit transiently modifiable, for instance through diet, emotion or hygiene (for review see Havlicek and Roberts, 2009). Even trained dogs are unable to discriminate between the body odors sampled on worn t-shirts of identical twins, as long as they follow the same diet (Hepper, 1988). This unique personal odorprint yields strong multisensory facilitation of self-face recognition (axillary odor, Platek et al., 2004), shortening reaction time for self-face recognition compared to other odors priming conditions (i.e., male or female axillary odor, androstenone, phenylethanol, or no odor), and suggesting that our own body odor is integral to the cognitive representation of our self. A similar multisensory facilitation would seem plausible for the processing of familiar individuals, such as relatives, mates, or friends, although the latter study did not find any priming effect of the body odor of participants’ co-workers/friends on the recognition of their face (Platek et al., 2004). However, it has been pointed out that these null findings likely stem from a lack of power due to the small samples (i.e., from 9 to 12 per experiment) used in the study rather than reflecting true absence of effect (Brédart, 2004). Being underpowered is especially problematic when comparing two conditions that may differ in effect sizes, and because we are “experts of ourselves” the magnitude of multisensory facilitation is likely much stronger for self-face recognition than for the recognition of other familiar faces (Brédart, 2004). A cautious conclusion would therefore be that the facilitation of self-face recognition through self-odor priming can be observed under conditions that, in contrast, do not allow facilitation of another familiar face recognition. For these reasons, it is actually unclear whether the influence of body odor on face identity recognition extends beyond self-face recognition and further investigations are needed.
The Case of Androstenes
Among the large variety of compounds that constitute human body odor (de Lacy Costello et al., 2014; Ferdenzi et al., 2020), some androstenes present in axillary secretions received much attention in the last two decades, 5α-androst-16-en-3-one (androstenone), 5α-androst-16-en-3α-ol (androstenol), 4,16-androstadien-3-one (androstadienone), and to a lesser extent, estra-1,3,5(10),16-tetraen-3-ol (estratetraenol) (for a comprehensive review, see Havlicek et al., 2010). This focus is based on a widespread – although highly controversial – view that these compounds could constitute human sex pheromones (Rikowski and Grammer, 1999; Thornhill and Gangestad, 1999; Savic et al., 2001, 2005; Berglund et al., 2006, 2008; Zhou et al., 2014; Ye et al., 2019; Chen et al., 2021; for in-depth discussions of why these molecules should not be designated as pheromones, see Wysocki and Preti, 2004; Doty, 2010; Wyatt, 2015). Resolving this issue is beyond the scope of the current review, and we will hereafter follow a cautious approach considering androstenes as body odorants rather than “putative human pheromones”.
Face Sex Perception
The production of androstenes is sexually dimorphic, with women emitting fewer quantities of androstenone than men (Gower and Ruparelia, 1993), and estratetraenol being mainly produced by pregnant women (Thysen et al., 1968). For this reason, androstenone (along with androstadienone and androstenol) was typically associated with males, and estratetraenol with females (e.g., Kovács et al., 2004; Zhou et al., 2014; Chen et al., 2021). These compounds were furthermore thought to convey information about masculinity and femininity, and to facilitate face sex perception in a multisensory context (Kovács et al., 2004). Initial findings in a sample of male participants have shown that androstadienone facilitated the perception of masculinity in female-to-male morphed faces (i.e., computer-generated linear continuum of faces with varying female/male ratio), while estratetraenol has no influence on the perception of facial femininity (Kovács et al., 2004). However, subsequent studies failed to replicate this effect, in both male and female participants (Ferdenzi et al., 2016; Hare et al., 2017), casting doubt on the potency of these compounds to influence face sex perception (Wyatt, 2015; Hare et al., 2017).
Mate Selection and Attractiveness
Androstenes were further proposed to be related to mate selection and sexual behavior (Gower and Ruparelia, 1993; Thornhill and Gangestad, 1999; Savic et al., 2001), for instance by modulating judgments of attractiveness in the face of potential mates (e.g., Saxton et al., 2008; Niu and Zheng, 2020). Supporting this view, a number of studies reported that exposure to androstenes increase perceived attractiveness in faces (i.e., androstadienone: Saxton et al., 2008; Ferdenzi et al., 2016; androstenol: Kirk-Smith et al., 1978; Filsinger et al., 1985). However, some studies reported an effect on same-sex faces only, for male participants rating male faces (androstenol: Filsinger et al., 1985), whereas others reported an effect for opposite-sex faces, for female participants (fertile woman) rating male faces (androstadienone: Saxton et al., 2008; Ferdenzi et al., 2016), or for ratings of female but not male faces by both male and female participants (androstenol: Kirk-Smith et al., 1978). By contrast, other studies failed to find any effect on facial attractiveness judgments (i.e., androstenol: Black and Biron, 1982; androstadienone and estratetraenol: Hare et al., 2017; androstadienone: Lundström and Olsson, 2005, see also Frey et al., 2012) or even found decreased attractiveness ratings following exposure to the compounds (androstenone: Filsinger et al., 1985; Kirk-Smith and Booth, 1990; androstadienone: Parma et al., 2012). In sum, the evidence is mixed as to whether androstenes reliably influence the appraisal of attractiveness in faces. However, before drawing any conclusions regarding the evolutionary role of androstenes in mate choice scenarios, it is important to note that the effects of these compounds are highly context-dependent (e.g., of the sex of the experimenter, Jacob et al., 2001; Lundström and Olsson, 2005), and might need to be embedded within a relevant social context, including for instance realistic male–female interactions (e.g., real dating situations, Saxton et al., 2008).
General Social Cognition
Another possibility is that, beyond mate selection, androstenes plays a role in more general aspects of social cognition, such as person perception. Behavioral evidence supports this view, as indicated by reports of increased generosity in female, but not male, participants (Perrotta et al., 2016), or increased cooperation in male participants (Huoviala and Rantala, 2013) following exposure to androstadienone during economic games (i.e., dictator game: Kahneman et al., 1986). More directly related to face processing, exposure to androstadienone fastens reaction speed toward schematic angry faces compared to happy faces (Frey et al., 2012), but also increases the judgment of dominance in faces (Banner and Shamay-tsoory, 2018), and triggers gaze avoidance (Banner et al., 2019) in men with high social anxiety. These two latter results however suggest that the signaling properties of androstadienone, if any, are very subtle as the effects were only observed for men with social anxiety, a population known for being hypersensitive to non-verbal social cues (Gilboa-Schechtman and Shachar-Lavie, 2013). Furthermore, a visual priming study using neutral faces reported no influence of androstadienone on attentional prioritization of social over non-social stimuli (see study 3 in Hummer and McClintock, 2009). Besides, several studies using the modified emotional Stroop task (e.g., faces with fearful and happy expressions presented with the words “happy” or “fear” written across them, Etkin et al., 2006) either found small effects on reduction of error rates, but not reaction times (Hornung et al., 2018b), reported findings limited to angry faces and to male participants (Hornung et al., 2017), or failed to find effects of androstadienone (Hornung et al., 2018a).
Androstenes, Body Odors, and Face Perception
Overall, it appears that many results about the effects of androstenes on face perception are fairly contentious, with multiple research groups reporting conflicting findings. The very choice to specifically investigate the signaling value of androstenes among the myriad of other constituents of human body odors (e.g., de Lacy Costello et al., 2014) have been found somewhat arbitrary, and their biological relevance is yet to be demonstrated (Havlicek et al., 2010; Wyatt, 2015; Hare et al., 2017). Moreover, their use in studies at physiologically implausible concentrations (e.g., for concentrations of several order of magnitude above natural quantities, see Savic et al., 2001, 2005; Berglund et al., 2006), dampened the strength of the conclusions regarding their physiological and behavioral effects. Thus, provided that the focus is put on cognitive mechanisms or effects rather than on the molecules, the use of axillary extracts in their entirety seems a safe way to cope with these pitfalls (Wyatt, 2015).
The Influence of Body Odor on the Perception of Transient Face Characteristics
Aside from conveying invariant social information, body odors also communicate more dynamic information related to emotional states, and humans have been found able to discriminate such “emotional body odors” (e.g., Chen and Haviland-Jones, 2000). Humans reliably discriminate between body odors collected on participants who have experienced events that induce fear or happy states (Ackerl et al., 2002; Zhou and Chen, 2011; Haviland-Jones et al., 2016a). These observations laid the ground for research on the chemical communication of emotions in humans, with a particular focus on fear/stress chemosensory cues (de Groot and Smeets, 2017; reviewed in Schaal, 2013) probably in relation with similar processes noted since decades in other vertebrates (e.g., Verheggen et al., 2010).
Anxiety odor (i.e., collected from the axillae just before an academic examination in male students) was found to disrupt the emotional priming of a happy face on a neutral face in a two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) task (Pause et al., 2004). In typical unisensory (i.e., visual) priming paradigms (Murphy and Zajonc, 1993; Cameron et al., 2012), neutral target faces tend to be found more pleasant when primed by happy compared to fear or sad faces. Exposure to an anxiety odor thus decreased the pleasantness of neutral faces primed by happy faces, although in female participants only. By contrast, exposure to a control body odor (i.e., collected during mere physical exercise) did not affect the visual priming, as neutral faces were judged more positively when primed by happy faces rather than by negative facial expressions (Pause et al., 2004). From these results, it is difficult to determine whether the anxiety odor decreased the positive value of the happy faces, of the neutral faces, or both. In any case, these findings indicate that anxiety odor cues can alter the perceived valence of expressive faces. These results were however limited to judgment of neutral faces primed by happy faces, as priming with fearful or sad faces did not make neutral faces more unpleasant following exposure to anxiety odor, presumably because participants’ response reached ceiling (Pause et al., 2004). Tentative interpretations were a dominance of chemosensory stimuli when sensory channels convey incongruent social cues, or a processing priority of anxiety-related information over positive and neutral information (for experimental support of the latter view, see de Groot et al., 2014b).
Further evidence that emotional chemosensory cues can influence face processing were provided by a psychophysical study showing that exposure to fear body odor (collected in axillary sweat after watching a 20-min clip excerpted from horror movies) biased female participants toward interpreting ambiguous fear-to-happy morphed facial expressions as more fearful in a 2-AFC task (Zhou and Chen, 2009). These findings indicate that visual and olfactory stimuli were integrated in an emotion-congruent manner (i.e., based on the redundant fear information), resulting in a face percept biased toward fear. Similar findings were recently reported using fear-to-disgust morphed faces, with “disgust” faces being perceived as more “fearful” by female participants after sniffing fear axillary sweat (de Groot et al., 2021). Fear axillary odor was also found to shorten the time interval that fear faces, but not disgust or neutral faces, took to reach visual awareness using a breaking Continuous Flash Suppression technique (i.e., b-CFS, the dichoptic presentation of dynamic noise to the dominant eye and a target stimulus to the other, which momentarily suppresses the target stimulus from visual awareness), again in a sample of female participants (Silvia et al., 2020). The influence of axillary fear sweat on the perception of emotional faces extends to male participants, biasing ratings of a continuum of neutral-to-happy morphed faces as less happy (Zernecke et al., 2011), and biasing ratings of neutral-to-fearful morphed faces as more fearful (Wudarczyk et al., 2016), in the context of a fear odor compared to an exercise odor. It is worth mentioning that in the aforementioned studies the effect was mainly evident for the most emotionally ambiguous faces or during challenging stimulation parameters hampering visual perception (e.g., the b-CFS technique). This suggests that the relative effectiveness of olfaction to communicate emotion in a multisensory context increases to the extent that perception in the other sensory modality (here, vision) is ambiguous. This is in keeping with the basic multisensory integration principle of “inverse effectiveness” according to which multisensory integration is more effective when the single modalities evoke relatively weak responses when presented in isolation (Meredith and Stein, 1983). In some respects, these findings also relate to the multisensory “reliability” rule holding that more weight is given to the modality providing the more precise (or more appropriate, see Welch and Warren, 1980) perceptual estimate depending on the task at hand (Ernst and Bülthoff, 2004), following a statistical optimal scheme (i.e., Maximum Likelihood Estimation, see Ernst and Banks, 2002). Further supporting this view, fear axillary odor has been found to quicken the recognition of fearful faces (but not other negative-valenced facial expressions such as anger and disgust) during the presentation of a stream of blurred face images gradually becoming clearer (Kamiloğlu et al., 2018).
Adding yet another layer of complexity, receivers sometimes fail to display emotion-specificity in their response to chemosensory cues conveyed in emotional axillary sweat odor. For instance, fear-related odor cues from the axillae have been found to boost the classification speed of emotional faces, regardless of the emotion depicted (i.e., fear, disgust, happy, and neutral faces: de Groot et al., 2015b), and to slightly increase the classification accuracy of dynamic facial expressions of both angry and happy faces (Rocha et al., 2018). Fear axillary odor also increased the readiness to detect both happy and fear faces using b-CFS (de Groot et al., 2018, but see Silvia et al., 2020). In a psychophysical task akin to that used in Zhou and Chen’s (2009), stress odors (i.e., collected from the axillae during skydiving) were found to sharpen visual discrimination of angry faces (Mujica-Parodi et al., 2009), refining the judgments of whether or not morphed faces (neutral-to-angry) were more neutral or more threatening. Thresholds of judgments between neutral and angry faces were similar for stress and control (physical exercise) odor conditions (i.e., the inflection points in the psychometric curves were similar for both odor conditions). In other words, the axillary stress odor increased accuracy in the assessment of angry and neutral faces rather than increasing the attribution of threat to neutral stimuli (Mujica-Parodi et al., 2009). The authors pointed out that an axillary stress odor may not be a consistent property to be associated with an angry face compared to a fear face, and might rather signal a nearby conspecific reacting to the angry individual (Mujica-Parodi et al., 2009). This odor cue may thus act as an alert signal increasing sensory vigilance overall (Mujica-Parodi et al., 2009; Rubin et al., 2012; de Groot et al., 2015b), inducing a general state of sensory acquisition (Susskind et al., 2008).
In fact, a number of studies have reported such increase in sensory vigilance following exposure to axillary stress odor (e.g., Prehn et al., 2006; de Groot et al., 2012; de Groot et al., 2015b). This phenomenon might partly account for the readiness to associate face and chemosensory stimuli in a non-emotionally specific manner in some studies (e.g., de Groot et al., 2015b, 2018; Rocha et al., 2018), even though the reasons for unsystematic increase of sensory vigilance are actually unclear. At the neural level, fear odor enhanced two early event-related-potentials (ERPs) components related to attention and face-processing (i.e., N1 and N170, respectively) during the presentation of anxious facial expressions (Adolph et al., 2013), consistent with an enhanced allocation of attention toward faces as expected under sensory vigilance. Note, however, that although the enhancement of N1 and N170 were observed when comparing a fear odor to a no odor condition, these ERP components were not different in amplitude when comparing the axillary fear odor to an exercise sweat condition (control odor), leaving open the question of whether the effect might stem from the exposure to any human body odor.
Such heightened vigilance effect seems restricted to fear/anxiety odor, as axillary sweat collected after other emotion-related challenges (happiness or disgust) have not been found to generate such an overall increase in vigilance (e.g., de Groot et al., 2018; Silvia et al., 2020). More generally, there is a paucity of data examining olfactory-visual interactions apart from fear/anxiety odors, among which results are mixed. Regarding the influence of a happy odor (collected from armpit), one study reported an increase of the unconscious readiness to detect happy faces in b-CFS (de Groot et al., 2018), whereas another study failed to find an influence of the happy odor on the perception of ambiguous fear-to-happy morphed facial expressions (Zhou and Chen, 2009). Disgust axillary odor (collected after participants watched disgusting movie excerpts) similarly failed to influence the processing of emotional faces (i.e., fear, disgust, and neutral faces) in b-CFS (Silvia et al., 2020). Using a non-emotional transient property, a study found that the axillary odor of sickness (i.e., an inflammatory response experimentally induced following injection of an endotoxin) negatively influenced the pleasantness ratings of faces of donors taken two hours after endotoxin (sick) or placebo (healthy) treatment (Regenbogen et al., 2017). The sickness odor decreased face pleasantness ratings compared to a control odor (i.e., an unused sampling pad), although not significantly when compared to the axillary odor of healthy individuals (Regenbogen et al., 2017; see also Sarolidou et al., 2020). Finally, although the most commonly studied carrier of human chemosensory cues is sweat, emotional tears (collected from female donors watching sad films) were also found to modulate face perception, whereby male participants rated female faces less sexually attractive after sniffing tears than after sniffing a saline solution (Gelstein et al., 2011, for discussion see, Gračanin et al., 2017; Sobel, 2017).
Examining the Signaling Value of Emotional Body Odor: Competing Frameworks
Even though it stands to reason that emotional chemosensory cues do communicate information, these outcomes led to question the actual signaling value of emotional body odors (de Groot et al., 2015b). For instance, with regards to fear odor, an outstanding issue is whether the odor emulated the precise emotional state of the sender in the receiver (de Groot et al., 2012), or alternatively whether it generated a non-specific state of high arousal and negative valence unrelated to fear, upon which the fear emotion category is attributed depending on contextual factors (Hess and Fischer, 2013; Barrett, 2017). The former view relates to a widespread conception in psychology considering that emotions are psychologically and biologically basic, discrete, and shared within and across cultures (Ekman and Friesen, 1971; Ekman, 1992a; Scollon et al., 2004; Izard, 2007; Ekman and Cordaro, 2011). Each emotional state is associated with a specific neurophysiological pattern (a definable brain circuit or affect program) producing particular expressive behaviors (e.g., facial expressions), and distinct autonomic correlates (Ekman, 1992a,b; Izard, 1993; Panksepp, 1998; Buck, 1999; Cacioppo et al., 2000; Cosmides and Tooby, 2000; LeDoux, 2000). According to this perspective, emotional chemosensory cues in senders should induce discrete emotions (de Groot et al., 2017), which should translate in receivers displaying emotion-specificity in their response to the chemosensory stimulation (e.g., Zhou and Chen, 2009; Zernecke et al., 2011; Kamiloğlu et al., 2018; de Groot et al., 2021).
However, reports that emotional odors can affect the perception of facial expressions beyond the emotion category of the chemosensory stimuli (e.g., Mujica-Parodi et al., 2009; de Groot et al., 2015b; Rocha et al., 2018) led to nuance the claim that emotional odors bear a one-to-one correspondence to specific emotions. These latter findings rather dovetail with an alternative framework holding that emotions are grounded in core affects, and originate from the integral blend of at least two fluctuating properties – valence and arousal – thought of as psychological primitives or building blocks of emotions (Russell, 2003; Barrett, 2006b, 2017; Wilson-Mendenhall et al., 2013; Siegel et al., 2018). In this view, emotions are not “natural kind” (Barrett, 2006a), but emerge from these core affects in association with bodily feelings, conceptual knowledge, prior experience, and expectations (Barrett, 2017). Regarding chemosensory perception, this questions what meaning is chemically communicated from the sender to the receiver, and whether the message provides enough clear-cut information (e.g., a “discrete emotion package,” or a chemosensory cue with some valence and arousal properties) to trigger a partial simulation of a previously experienced emotional episode in the receiver (de Groot et al., 2015b).
A recent integrative framework (de Groot et al., 2017) pointed out that a chemosensory cue can acquire signal property over time by associative learning, provided that some statistical regularities can be extracted from the chemical components carried by body odor emitted when individuals experience an emotional episode. For example, the emotional impact of androstenone is positively correlated with the amount of exposure to it during sexual experience (Knaapila et al., 2012), and recent evidence suggests that patterns of chemical volatiles emitted by the body correlate with emotional states (Smeets et al., 2020). If reliable associations can be shaped by learning chemical profiles and co-occurring circumstances, chemosensory cues will be able to communicate emotional information with behavioral consequences indistinguishable from what could be expected under a discrete account of emotion (de Groot et al., 2020). Therefore, after exposure to correlated input signals, the visual system could use olfactory signals to build more reliable percepts of a social scene (Kuang and Zhang, 2014). Such a perspective implies that our brains relies on statistical regularities from past experiences, suggesting that a developmental investigation of the early influence of olfaction on face perception would provide valuable insights on the origins of these olfaction-to-vision influences. Although limited, an emerging literature is beginning to unravel how human chemosensory stimuli influence, and even mediate, face perception during the first months of aerial life.
Developmental Aspects: Olfaction-To-Vision Influences in Infancy
Olfaction is a sensory channel benefiting from an early start as nasal chemosensation is functionally operative from the periphery to the brain by the end of the second gestational trimester (Schaal, 1988; Schaal et al., 2004; Browne, 2008). The partial overlap between the chemical profile of colostrum/milk secretions and that of the amniotic fluid allows for an olfactorily-smoothed natal transition continuity between intra-uterine and extra-uterine life (Schaal et al., 2020), notably through the exposure to colostrum, mother’s breast and body odor (Schaal et al., 2004; Marlier and Schaal, 2005; Klaey-Tassone et al., 2020). Such chemosensory continuity is essential in the adaptive development of other mammalian neonates, and is thought of as laying down the foundation for optimal social-emotional growth and cognitive development in humans as well (Schaal et al., 2020). Supporting this view, exposure to the odor of mother’s breast increases eye-opening in newborns (Doucet et al., 2007), favoring the infants’ first visual investigations of the maternal face. At later age, exposure to maternal body odor (presented using t-shirts worn during prior 3 nights) drives 4-month-old infants to allocate their attention toward social (i.e., faces) over non-social (i.e., cars) pictures presented pairwise from a screen, and especially to increase their active scanning of the eye area of the face (Durand et al., 2013). Moreover, the mother’s body odor shapes the visual categorization of faces (but not cars) presented embedded in a fast 6-Hz visual stream of widely variable natural images, enhancing a face-selective neural response in 4-month-olds (Leleu et al., 2020; Rekow et al., 2020). In addition, when using visual stimuli that infants could hardly categorize as faces (i.e., objects with face like configurations eliciting face pareidolia in adults), exposure to mother’s body odor boosts a selective response to these objects over brain regions typically responding to human faces in 4-month-olds (Rekow et al., 2021b).
Beyond the generic response to faces per se, maternal body odor also influences the processing of facial information, as indicated by the fact that 4-month-olds engage more their attention to their mother’s face than a stranger’s face in the context of maternal odor than they do in the context of a control (unworn t-shirt) odor (Durand et al., 2020). Rather unexpectedly, this face–odor association was further found to be category-specific rather than individual-specific, since body odors of other postparturient women also bolstered the visual attention of 4-month-old infants to mother’s versus stranger’s faces (Durand et al., 2020). Because other studies demonstrated that infants can discriminate their mother’s body odor from that of a stranger at a few days to weeks of age (Macfarlane, 1975; Cernoch and Porter, 1985; Porter and Winberg, 1999), it is unlikely that this pattern of findings reflects a confusion between mother’s and stranger’s body odor. Although speculative, a possibility is that the psychobiological state of postpartum leads to the release of a common pattern of chemical compounds in the body odor of postparturient women, to which infants would have responded categorically. In any case, a more specific response to the mother’s body odor was reported in older infants during the processing of emotional facial expressions (Jessen, 2020). Compared to the body odor of another mother (or a control odor), the exposure to their own-mother’s body odor reduced the typical neural response for fear faces in 7-month-olds, suggesting that maternal body odor can impact emotional learning in infancy (Jessen, 2020), although the underpinning mechanisms remain unclear.
Overall, these findings suggest that human body odor (represented here exclusively by the global maternal odor, data based on paternal odor being so far virtually inexistent) is processed as a social cue from early on, enhancing or facilitating the perception of faces (e.g., Durand et al., 2013; Leleu et al., 2020; Rekow et al., 2021b). This is consistent with the suggestion that olfaction is scaffolding the infant cognitive development, and helps to cope with the relative immaturity of the visual system in infancy (Schaal and Durand, 2012; Schaal et al., 2020).
Underlying this view is that albeit immature, infants have a cognitive integrity of their own (Bjorklund, 1997). In an evolutionary, ecological conception of development, the cognitive ability of infants should not be considered poorer than those of the child or adult organisms, but as fully fit to their ontogenetic stage, allowing to respond adaptively to whatever challenges their ecological niche poses to them (Spear, 1984). In other words, infancy should not be seen as an unfinished form of adult functioning, and some particular maturational constraint may in fact afford the infant some temporary advantage (Altmann, 2002; Kawai et al., 2012; Bjorklund, 1997). From this perspective, the immaturity of the visual system might prove advantageous inasmuch as this would entail a stronger weighting of olfaction in early learning (building on multisensory integration rules), fostering associative learning between chemical profiles emitted in conspecifics’ body odors and social information provided through other sensory channels, at a time where building a social brain is critical for the organism (Atzil et al., 2018). If so, the younger they are infants might prove more prone to olfaction-vision associative learning, and the influence of olfaction on face processing might reveal negatively associated with the development of visual perceptual skills in infancy. Indirect evidence suggests such a decrease of the weight of olfaction in adulthood compared to early infancy, since in adults the implicit exposure of human body odor no longer enhances the neural categorization response of faces as it does in infants (Leleu et al., 2020; Rekow et al., 2021a). Interestingly, an effect of body odor is however found on the adults’ brain response when visual perception is made more challenging (i.e., by using ambiguous face-like objects as stimuli presented for brief durations with forward- and backward-masking, Rekow et al., 2021a).
Final Considerations and Future Directions
Body odors have been found to modulate the visual processing of both invariant (i.e., sex, attractiveness, and identity), and transient (i.e., emotional expressions and health) face properties. Spontaneously processed as carrying social information (Pause, 2012), body odors easily bind with face-related visual cues in a multisensory percept (Cecchetto et al., 2020), and overall, social smells bear on how we look at faces. However, large discrepancies emerge across studies in the way chemosensory information is integrated with faces, ranging from the multisensory integration of what seems redundant properties (e.g., identity: Platek et al., 2004; emotion: Zhou and Chen, 2009; Kamiloğlu et al., 2018; de Groot et al., 2021) to a more loosely matched correspondence with a rather unspecific influence of body odors (e.g., sex: Mutic et al., 2016; emotion: Mujica-Parodi et al., 2009; de Groot et al., 2015b; Rocha et al., 2018; health: Regenbogen et al., 2017).
Causes of Variations of the Body Odor Influence
The puzzling versatility of the influence of body odors on face perception probably results from a variety of causes and may partially reflect difficulties in controlling the chemosensory information conveyed by natural odorants emitted by the body. For instance, the collection of fear odor includes a wide diversity of methods that markedly differ (e.g., first-time tandem skydiving, high rope course, academic examination, watching film clips excerpted from horror movies), which probably induce emotional states of variable intensity and quality (Lübke and Pause, 2015), which in turn may have consequences on the intensity and/or the quality of released odor stimuli (e.g., de Groot et al., 2020). Extreme physiological and psychological stress resulting from skydiving or high rope course likely activate different autonomic responses than watching horror movies while sitting in an armchair, the former inducing a strong physiological arousal potentially mixing various emotions (e.g., fear, relief, joy, and disgust), whereas the latter may evoke more specific emotions but with a weaker intensity (Pause, 2012; Lübke and Pause, 2015). Consequently, the pattern of volatile compounds released in both cases may differ, as may their influence on visual cognition in receivers. Note that labeling all these emotional body odors interchangeably as fear, stress, or anxiety odors may thus be somehow arbitrary and confusing.
Another source of variation may stem from the fact that the multisensory integration of two co-occurring sensory stimulations is not mandatory, and the brain has to determine which cues are derived from the same event before integrating them (i.e., the “correspondence problem”; Ernst and Bülthoff, 2004). Multisensory integration is thus both parameter and situation dependent (Munoz and Blumstein, 2020); one input being possibly discarded if too discrepant with the other source, as the benefits of ignoring a stimulus sometimes outweigh integrating two stimuli (Munoz and Blumstein, 2012). Inferring causation from correlation might be less straightforward for olfactory-visual than audio-visual integration, because olfactory perception is neither temporally nor spatially structured like visual perception (e.g., Sela and Sobel, 2010). Consequently, the embedding context along with the task at hand might crucially affect whether and to what extent olfactory cues will bind with visual information, which may contribute to the relative inconsistency of the influence of body odors on face perception in the reviewed studies.
It should be noted, however, that these theoretical considerations only hold as long as research findings are reliable, which sadly may turn out to be a too strong assumption considering the “reproducibility crisis”, as fewer than half of the findings have been estimated reproducible (Open Science Collaboration, 2015), and chemosensory research is no exception (Wyatt, 2020; Syrjänen et al., 2021). Various threats to reproducible science have been put forward (e.g., p-hacking, analytical flexibility, low statistical power, publication bias, Munafò et al., 2017; Nelson et al., 2018), and placed in the context of a competition-based model of science that values sheer output over good research (e.g., base evaluation of researcher on impact factor to determine hiring or promotion, Moher et al., 2018). This situation probably contributed to the publication of false positives resulting in the overall disparate outcomes reviewed here. This rather bleak picture should not hide recent improvements in practices (i.e., the “Psychology’s Renaissance”, Nelson et al., 2018), since means to increase the integrity of our discipline like pre-registered studies are beginning to emerge (e.g., de Groot et al., 2018; Silvia et al., 2020; Roth et al., 2021), and data disclosure is now widely encouraged (Morey et al., 2016; Wyatt, 2020).
Potential Developments in Emotional Body Odors Research
Notwithstanding aforementioned remarks, based on the current body of research, there is now little doubt that body odors can communicate emotional information (de Groot et al., 2017). Nevertheless, it appears that not all emotions are equal when it comes to the transmission ability of body odor, as indicated by the overwhelming preponderance of findings related to fear/anxiety/stress odors compared to other emotional body odors such as happy or disgust body odors. This bias in the literature may stem from the greater potency of fear odors to trigger behavioral responses compared to other emotional body odors (Silvia et al., 2020), since, from an evolutionary perspective, failure to detect fear cues might prove more detrimental than missing happy cues (Öhman, 2009; Zhou and Chen, 2009). On the other hand, this bias may also reflect a lack of research on other body odors (Haviland-Jones et al., 2016b), perhaps precisely because other emotional odors may have been considered as carrying less evolutionary salience than fear/threatening cues (de Groot et al., 2015a). These two possibilities are not mutually exclusive, and future studies should further examine to what extent emotional body odors – aside from fear, stress, or anxiety odors – hold the ability to influence social perception based on vision (or audition, or touch). Recent promising findings suggest that this area of research is beginning to be scrutinized, with emerging investigations of body odors related to aggression (Pause et al., 2020), sexual arousal (Wisman and Shrira, 2020), dominance/competition (Fialová et al., 2020), or happiness (Smeets et al., 2020). Another line of research could involve investigations on how anosmic individuals process facial cues in comparison to normosmic individuals in both visual only and olfactory-visual contexts. While anosmic individuals are at obvious disadvantage to integrate olfactory and visual cues into multisensory percepts, they have been found to exhibit a specific expertise in tracking low-intensity visual cues of disgust or fear in faces (but not of happiness, sadness, anger, or surprise), showing better accuracy than normosmic participants in classifying these two facial emotions in a unisensory visual context (Lemogne et al., 2015). Interestingly, the expertise of anosmic participants in decoding facial emotion was correlated with the duration of their anosmia. Being unable to predict hazards based on olfactory cues, anosmic individuals may have been induced to attend more frequently and more deeply to others’ facial reactions elicited by olfaction and in detecting, at low-expressive intensity, related facial actions that reflect sensing of dangerous/disgusting items and express withdrawal. This increased accuracy in detecting facial disgust/fear might help permanently anosmic individuals to level with normosmic participants in the visual recognition of some facial emotions. Although speculative, a possibility is that in cases of transient anosmia, such as those caused by viral infections (e.g., Kollndorfer et al., 2015; Lechien et al., 2020; Petrocelli et al., 2021), the recuperation of olfaction might increase the detection of facially-expressed disgust or fear in prior anosmics, who may then surpass normosmics in olfactory-visual contexts. This remains to be formally examined, however. Furthermore, a comprehensive social cognition framework should also account for the developmental aspects of the multisensory social processing, and mechanisms underpinning early chemosensory influences on face processing in infancy and childhood may be important to understand for this reason. So far, however, only a few studies investigated how body odors influence face perception in infancy, and none has yet examined the response of infants to emotional body odors. Similarly, the developmental trajectory of olfaction-to-vision body odor influences in childhood is largely unknown, thus calling for more research to address these gaps in knowledge.
Methodological Considerations: Sex Differences
A somewhat insidious blind spot that develops in olfactory research is related to sex differences. Typically females were shown to outperform males, notably by showing lower olfactory thresholds (Brand and Millot, 2001; Sergeant, 2010; Stevenson, 2010; for a meta-analysis see Sorokowski et al., 2019). These olfactory differences combined with findings reporting that women are also slightly better in processing social-emotional information (Proverbio et al., 2008; Proverbio, 2017) logically led to the observed male-female asymmetry in processing emotional chemosensory stimuli (de Groot et al., 2014a). Hence, to maximize the likelihood of finding an effect, many studies tested only female receivers exposed to body odor from male senders (e.g., de Groot et al., 2015b, 2021), because men possess larger apocrine sweat glands producing more intense body odor and are not subject to menstrual fluctuations of emission (Chen and Haviland-Jones, 1999; Sergeant, 2010). The downside of such experimental setting is the questionable generalizability of the results to the male population of odor receivers on the one hand, and to the female population as odor emitters on the other hand. Moreover, the effect sizes of olfactory sex differences are notably small (Sorokowski et al., 2019), suggesting to qualify this female advantage. In addition, interindividual variability in emotional body odor detection may prove to have a larger influence than sex differences, at least for the perception of happy and fear body odors (Haviland-Jones et al., 2016a). Strikingly, analyses of individual differences in emotional body odor perception indicated that almost 18% of individuals fail to discriminate happy and fear axillary odors from each other, and from a sterile gauze pad (Haviland-Jones et al., 2016a). At first glance, this could question the validity and utility of these odors as chemosensory cues in natural social situations. However, it should be kept in mind that the explicit detection of an odorant is far from a prerequisite for assessing an influence on the behavior of the receivers (Pause, 2012), even though such influence is denied by the participants themselves (for discussions, see, Haviland-Jones and Wilson, 2008; Haviland-Jones et al., 2016b). For these reasons, inclusion of both male and female participants should be fostered, and ideally, similar sex-clustered sampling should also be considered for the collection of olfactory stimuli.
Ecological Considerations: The Use of Artificial Fragrances
Although the focus of the current review was put on human body odors, the widespread use of artificial fragrances (e.g., Roberts et al., 2010), however, requires to acknowledge their influence on everyday social communication (Allen et al., 2019; Spence, 2021), and regarding this latter point in particular, the interaction between extraneous fragrances and natural body odors should be carefully examined. Perfumes for instance may be used to exalt some particular invariant components of body odor (e.g., masculinity/femininity, personality traits, see Allen et al., 2019, for review) or perhaps hide others (e.g., emotional or illness cues), which may have influences on both receiver and sender sides in modulating person and face perception (e.g., Roberts et al., 2009; for review see Spence, 2021; Syrjänen et al., 2021). The nature and extent of interactions between artificial fragrances and body odor components related to transient states such as emotion or illness, however, are relatively unexplored, as are their impacts on social cognition. Note that skin exposure to scented products goes well beyond perfumes (e.g., deodorants, creams, soaps, and shampoos), and their influence on social perception can probably be traced back even to very early parent-infant social interactions, during which the premises of conditioning and associative learning between body odor and artificial fragrance may have been established. A complete understanding of olfactory communication through body odors must extend to ecologically valid conditions, thus artificial fragrances should be incorporated in future research aiming to better understand how artificial fragrances and natural body odor cues intertwine, and modulate social perception.
Conclusion
The overarching goal of all cognitive processes is the control of adaptive action (Semin et al., 2012), which requires a perceptual system that produces reliable and robust percepts to cope with the basic uncertainty of the environment, including its social component. To do so, all brains must face the ill-posed problem of perception: multiple overt actions or covert attitudes are often possible based on a same unisensory stimulation, which can even have a low signal-to-noise ratio. In this endeavor, relying on multisensory perception to disambiguate unisensory inputs is highly effective, by combining percept estimates across sensory channels using a weighting process proportional to the reliability of each modality (Ernst and Banks, 2002), and by biasing perception away from the least certain modalities. Accordingly, because olfaction may be seen as a “weak sense” compared to the “dominant” visual system in adult humans, olfaction-to-vision interactions are usually reported in situations where quality of visual signals is not optimal (e.g., in early development) or degraded (e.g., Zhou et al., 2010; Kuang and Zhang, 2014), thereby making the reliability of visual and olfactory estimates comparable. Overall, the findings reviewed here nonetheless indicate that human chemosensory stimuli can have a substantial influence on the visual processing of faces, impacting situations of visual ambiguity (e.g., Zhou and Chen, 2009; Zernecke et al., 2011; Kamiloğlu et al., 2018; de Groot et al., 2021), though not limited to them (e.g., de Groot et al., 2015b; Wudarczyk et al., 2016). This discrete but pervasive influence of olfaction on visual-social processing may appear surprising when considering that, in many cases, the visual perceptual estimates were not especially compromised. However, sensory uncertainty may not only be characterized by weak stimulus signal-to-noise ratio or intensity, but also by variation in the predictive value of the stimulus (Munoz and Blumstein, 2012). It has been argued that social cognition is intrinsically marked by uncertainty because thoughts and intentions are largely hidden (FeldmanHall and Shenhav, 2019), and even emotional facial displays are not without ambiguities (Barrett et al., 2019). Seen from this vantage, the typical imbalance between vision and olfaction sensory estimates may thus be reduced in the social context, hereby offering more weight to chemosensory signals. In other words, during social encounters in our uncertain world, the nose could sometimes turn out to be as reliable as the eyes for an adaptive behavior.
Author Contributions
FD, NM, BS, and AL ensured literature monitoring. FD wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors revised critically, read, and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This research was supported by the French “Investissements d’Avenir” program, project ISITE-BFC (contract ANR-15-IDEX-0003) attributed to FD, and by the French National Research Agency (contract ANR-19-CE28-0009) attributed to AL.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.750944/full#supplementary-material
References
Ackerl, K., Atzmueller, M., and Grammer, K. (2002). The scent of fear. Neuroendocrinol. Lett. 23, 79–84.
Adolph, D., Meister, L., and Pause, B. M. (2013). Context counts! social anxiety modulates the processing of fearful faces in the context of chemosensory anxiety signals. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:283. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00283
Allen, C., Havlíček, J., and Roberts, S. C. (2019). “The effects of artificial fragrances on human olfactory communication,” in Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 14, Vol. 14, ed. C. Buesching (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 107–117. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-17616-7_9
Altmann, G. T. M. (2002). Learning and development in neural networks – the importance of prior experience. Cognition 85, 71–99. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00106-3
Atzil, S., Gao, W., Fradkin, I., and Barrett, L. F. (2018). Growing a social brain. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 624–636. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0384-6
Aviezer, H., Ensenberg, N., and Hassin, R. R. (2017). The inherently contextualized nature of facial emotion perception. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 17, 47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.06.006
Banner, A., Gabay, S., and Shamay-Tsoory, S. (2019). Androstadienone, a putative chemosignal of dominance, increases gaze avoidance among men with high social anxiety. Psychoneuroendocrinology 102, 9–15. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.11.025
Banner, A., and Shamay-tsoory, S. (2018). Effects of androstadienone on dominance perception in males with low and high social anxiety. Psychoneuroendocrinology 95, 138–144. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.05.032
Barkat-defradas, M., and Motte-florac, M. E. (2016). Words for Odours?: Language Skills and Cultural Insights, eds M. Barkat-Defradas and M. E. Motte-florac (Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing).
Barrett, L. F. (2006a). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 1, 28–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00003.x
Barrett, L. F. (2006b). Solving the emotion paradox: categorization and the experience of emotion. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 10, 20–46. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2
Barrett, L. F. (2017). The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 1–23. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw154
Barrett, L. F., Adolphs, R., Marsella, S., Martinez, A. M., and Pollak, S. D. (2019). Emotional expressions reconsidered: challenges to inferring emotion from human facial movements. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 20, 1–68. doi: 10.1177/1529100619832930
Berglund, H., Lindstrom, P., Dhejne-Helmy, C., and Savic, I. (2008). Male-to-female transsexuals show sex-atypical hypothalamus activation when smelling odorous steroids. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1900–1908. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm216
Berglund, H., Lindstrom, P., and Savic, I. (2006). Brain response to putative pheromones in lesbian women. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 8269–8274. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0600331103
Bjorklund, D. F. (1997). The role of immaturity in human development. Psychol. Bull. 122, 153–169. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.122.2.153
Black, S. L., and Biron, C. (1982). Androstenol as a human pheromone: no effect on perceived physical attractiveness. Behav. Neural Biol. 34, 326–330. doi: 10.1016/S0163-1047(82)91711-3
Brand, G., and Millot, J. L. (2001). Sex differences in human olfaction: between evidence and enigma. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 54, 259–270. doi: 10.1080/02724990143000045
Brédart, S. (2004). Cross-modal facilitation is not specific to self-face recognition. Conscious. Cogn. 13, 610–612. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2004.04.001
Browne, J. V. (2008). Chemosensory development in the fetus and newborn. Newborn Infant Nurs. Rev. 8, 180–186. doi: 10.1053/j.nainr.2008.10.009
Buck, R. (1999). The biological affects: a typology. Psychol. Rev. 106, 301–336. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.2.301
Cacioppo, J. T., Bernston, G. G., Larsen, J. T., Poehlmann, K. M., and Ito, T. A. (2000). “The psychophysiology of emotions,” in Handbook of Emotion, eds M. Lewis and J. M. Haviland-Jones (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 173–191. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-08-100508-8.00004-7
Calvi, E., Quassolo, U., Massaia, M., Scandurra, A., D’Aniello, B., and D’Amelio, P. (2020). The scent of emotions: a systematic review of human intra- and interspecific chemical communication of emotions. Brain Behav. 10:e01585. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1585
Cameron, C. D., Brown-Iannuzzi, J. L., and Payne, B. K. (2012). Sequential priming measures of implicit social cognition: a meta-analysis of associations with behavior and explicit attitudes. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 16, 330–350. doi: 10.1177/1088868312440047
Carrito, M. L., Santos, I. M., Alho, L., Ferreira, J., Soares, S. C., Bem-Haja, P., et al. (2017). Do masculine men smell better? An association between skin color masculinity and female preferences for body odor. Chem. Senses 42, 269–275. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjx004
Cecchetto, C., Fischmeister, F. P. S., Gorkiewicz, S., Schuehly, W., Bagga, D., Parma, V., et al. (2020). Human body odor increases familiarity for faces during encoding−retrieval task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 1904–1919. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24920
Cernoch, J. M., and Porter, R. H. (1985). Recognition of maternal axillary odors by infants. Child Dev. 56, 1593–1598. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1985.tb00224.x
Chen, D., and Haviland-Jones, J. (1999). Rapid mood change and human odors. Physiol. Behav. 68, 241–250. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(99)00147-X
Chen, D., and Haviland-Jones, J. (2000). Human olfactory communication of emotion. Percept. Mot. Skills 91, 771–781. doi: 10.2466/pms.2000.91.3.771
Chen, K., Ye, Y., Troje, N. F., and Zhou, W. (2021). Oxytocin modulates human chemosensory decoding of sex in a dose-dependent manner. eLife 10:e59376. doi: 10.7554/ELIFE.59376
Classen, C., Howes, D., and Synnott, A. (2002). Aroma: The Cultural History of Smell. (London: Routledge). doi: 10.4324/9780203428887
Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (2000). “Evolutionary psychology and the emotions,” in Handbook of Emotions, eds M. Lewis and J. M. Haviland-Jones (New York, NY: Guilford), 91–115.
Cuevas, I., Plaza, P., Rombaux, P., De Volder, A. G., and Renier, L. (2009). Odour discrimination and identification are improved in early blindness. Neuropsychologia 47, 3079–3083. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.004
Darwin, C. (1871/1992). “The evidence of the descent of man from some lower form,” in The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex, Vol. 1, (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press), 9–33. doi: 10.1037/12293-001 (Original work published 1871)
de Groot, J. H. B., Kirk, P. A., and Gottfried, J. A. (2020). Encoding fear intensity in human sweat. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 375:20190271. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0271
de Groot, J. H. B., Kirk, P. A., and Gottfried, J. A. (2021). Titrating the smell of fear: initial evidence for dose-invariant behavioral, physiological, and neural responses. Psychol. Sci. 32, 558–572. doi: 10.1177/0956797620970548
de Groot, J. H. B., Semin, G. R., and Smeets, M. A. M. (2014a). Chemical communication of fear: a case of male–female asymmetry. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 1515–1525. doi: 10.1037/a0035950
de Groot, J. H. B., Semin, G. R., and Smeets, M. A. M. (2014b). I can see, hear, and smell your fear: comparing olfactory and audiovisual media in fear communication. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 825–834. doi: 10.1037/a0033731
de Groot, J. H. B., Semin, G. R., and Smeets, M. A. M. (2017). On the communicative function of body odors: a theoretical integration and review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 306–324. doi: 10.4135/9781446247631.n8
de Groot, J. H. B., and Smeets, M. A. M. (2017). Human fear chemosignaling: evidence from a meta-analysis. Chem. Senses 42, 663–673. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjx049
de Groot, J. H. B., Smeets, M. A. M., Kaldewaij, A., Duijndam, M. J. A., and Semin, G. R. (2012). Chemosignals communicate human emotions. Psychol. Sci. 23, 1417–1424. doi: 10.1177/0956797612445317
de Groot, J. H. B., Smeets, M. A. M., Rowson, M. J., Bulsing, P. J., Blonk, C. G., Wilkinson, J. E., et al. (2015a). A sniff of happiness. Psychol. Sci. 26, 684–700. doi: 10.1177/0956797614566318
de Groot, J. H. B., Smeets, M. A. M., and Semin, G. R. (2015b). Rapid stress system drives chemical transfer of fear from sender to receiver. PLoS One 10:e0118211. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118211
de Groot, J. H. B., van Houtum, L. A. E. M., Gortemaker, I., Ye, Y., Chen, W., Zhou, W., et al. (2018). Beyond the west: Chemosignaling of emotions transcends ethno-cultural boundaries. Psychoneuroendocrinology 98, 177–185. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.08.005
de Jager, S., Coetzee, N., and Coetzee, V. (2018). Facial adiposity, attractiveness, and health: a review. Front. Psychol. 9:2562. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02562
de Lacy Costello, B., Amann, A., Al-Kateb, H., Flynn, C., Filipiak, W., Khalid, T., et al. (2014). A review of the volatiles from the healthy human body. J. Breath Res. 8:014001. doi: 10.1088/1752-7155/8/1/014001
Dion, K., Berscheid, E., and Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 24, 285–290. doi: 10.1037/h0033731
Doty, R. L. (1986). Odour-guided behaviour in mammals. Experientia 42, 257–271. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.04.052
Doty, R. L., Orndorff, M. M., Leyden, J., and Kligman, A. (1978). Communication of gender from human axillary odors: relationship to perceived intensity and hedonicity. Behav. Biol. 23, 373–380. doi: 10.1016/S0091-6773(78)91393-7
Doucet, S., Soussignan, R., Sagot, P., and Schaal, B. (2007). The “smellscape” of mother’s breast: effects of odor masking and selective unmasking on neonatal arousal, oral, and visual responses. Dev. Psychobiol. 49, 129–138. doi: 10.1002/dev.20210
Durand, K., Baudouin, J.-Y., Lewkowicz, D. J., Goubet, N., and Schaal, B. (2013). Eye-catching odors: olfaction elicits sustained gazing to faces and eyes in 4-month-old infants. PLoS One 8:e70677. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0070677
Durand, K., Schaal, B., Goubet, N., Lewkowicz, D. J., and Baudouin, J.-Y. (2020). Does any mother’s body odor stimulate interest in mother’s face in 4-month-old infants? Infancy 25, 151–164. doi: 10.1111/infa.12322
Ekman, P. (1992a). An argument for basic emotions. Cogn. Emot. 6, 169–200. doi: 10.1080/02699939208411068
Ekman, P. (1992b). Are there basic emotions? Psychol. Rev. 99, 550–553. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.99.3.550
Ekman, P., and Cordaro, D. (2011). What is meant by calling emotions basic. Emot. Rev. 3, 364–370. doi: 10.1177/1754073911410740
Ekman, P., and Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 17, 124–129. doi: 10.1037/h0030377
Ernst, M. O., and Banks, M. S. (2002). Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415, 429–433. doi: 10.1038/415429a
Ernst, M. O., and Bülthoff, H. H. (2004). Merging the senses into a robust percept. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 162–169. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.002
Etkin, A., Egner, T., Peraza, D. M., Kandel, E. R., and Hirsch, J. (2006). Resolving emotional conflict: a role for the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in modulating activity in the amygdala. Neuron 51, 871–882. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.07.029
FeldmanHall, O., and Shenhav, A. (2019). Resolving uncertainty in a social world. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3, 426–435. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0590-x
Ferdenzi, C., Delplanque, S., Atanassova, R., and Sander, D. (2016). Androstadienone’s influence on the perception of facial and vocal attractiveness is not sex specific. Psychoneuroendocrinology 66, 166–175. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.01.016
Ferdenzi, C., Richard Ortegón, S., Delplanque, S., Baldovini, N., and Bensafi, M. (2020). Interdisciplinary challenges for elucidating human olfactory attractiveness. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 375:20190268. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0268
Fialová, J., Třebický, V., Kuba, R., Stella, D., Binter, J., and Havlíček, J. (2020). Losing stinks! The effect of competition outcome on body odour quality. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 375:20190267. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0267
Filsinger, E. E., Braun, J. J., and Monte, W. C. (1985). An examination of the effects of putative pheromones on human judgments. Ethol. Sociobiol. 6, 227–236. doi: 10.1016/0162-3095(85)90015-9
Foo, Y. Z., Simmons, L. W., and Rhodes, G. (2017). Predictors of facial attractiveness and health in humans. Sci. Rep. 7:39731. doi: 10.1038/srep39731
Foster, J. D. (2008). Beauty is mostly in the eye of the beholder: olfactory versus visual cues of attractiveness. J. Soc. Psychol. 148, 765–774. doi: 10.3200/SOCP.148.6.765-774
Freud, S. (1930/1962). Civilization and Its Discontents. New York, NY: W. W. Norton. (Original work published 1930)
Frey, M. C. M., Weyers, P., Pauli, P., and Mühlberger, A. (2012). Androstadienone in motor reactions of men and women toward angry faces. Percept. Mot. Skills 114, 807–825. doi: 10.2466/07.16.22.28.PMS.114.3.807-825
Gaby, J. M., and Zayas, V. (2017). Smelling is telling: human olfactory cues influence social judgments in semi-realistic interactions. Chem. Senses 42, 405–418. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjx012
Gangestad, S. W., and Thornhill, R. (1998). Menstrual cycle variation in women’s preferences for the scent of symmetrical men. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 265, 927–933. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1998.0380
Gelstein, S., Yeshurun, Y., Rozenkrantz, L., Shushan, S., Frumin, I., Roth, Y., et al. (2011). Human tears contain a chemosignal. Science 331, 226–230. doi: 10.1126/science.1198331
Gilboa-Schechtman, E., and Shachar-Lavie, I. (2013). More than a face: a unified theoretical perspective on nonverbal social cue processing in social anxiety. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:904. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00904
Gottfried, J. A., and Dolan, R. J. (2003). The nose smells what the eye sees: crossmodal visual facilitation of human olfactory perception. Neuron 39, 375–386. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00392-1
Gower, D. B., and Ruparelia, B. A. (1993). Olfaction in humans with special reference to odorous 16-androstenes: their occurrence, perception and possible social, psychological and sexual impact. J. Endocrinol. 137, 167–187. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.1370167
Gračanin, A., van Assen, M. A. L. M., Omrčen, V., Koraj, I., and Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M. (2017). Chemosignalling effects of human tears revisited: does exposure to female tears decrease males’ perception of female sexual attractiveness? Cogn. Emot. 31, 139–150. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1151402
Groyecka, A., Pisanski, K., Sorokowska, A., Havlícek, J., Karwowski, M., Puts, D. A., et al. (2017). Attractiveness is multimodal: beauty is also in the nose and ear of the beholder. Front. Psychol. 8:778. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00778
Habel, U., Regenbogen, C., Kammann, C., Stickel, S., and Chechko, N. (2021). Male brain processing of the body odor of ovulating women compared to that of pregnant women. Neuroimage 229:117733. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117733
Hare, R. M., Schlatter, S., Rhodes, G., and Simmons, L. W. (2017). Putative sex-specific human pheromones do not affect gender perception, attractiveness ratings or unfaithfulness judgements of opposite sex faces. R. Soc. Open Sci. 4:160831. doi: 10.1098/rsos.160831
Haviland-Jones, J. M., McGuire, T. R., and Wilson, P. (2016a). Testing for individual differences in the identification of chemosignals for fear and happy: phenotypic super-detectors, detectors and non-detectors. PLoS One 11:e0154495. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0154495
Haviland-Jones, J. M., Wilson, P., and Freyberg, R. (2016b). “Olfaction: explicit and implicit emotional processing,” in Handbook of Emotions, eds M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, and L. F. Barrett (New York, NY: Guilford Press). 196–218.
Haviland-Jones, J. M., and Wilson, P. J. (2008). “A “nose” for emotion: emotional information and challenges in odors and semiochemicals,” in Handbook of Emotions, eds M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, and L. F. Barrett (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 235–248.
Havlicek, J., Murray, A. K., Saxton, T. K., and Roberts, S. C. (2010). Current issues in the study of androstenes in human chemosignaling. Vitam. Horm. 83, 47–81. doi: 10.1016/S0083-6729(10)83003-1
Havlicek, J., and Roberts, S. C. (2009). MHC-correlated mate choice in humans: a review. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 497–512. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.007
Havlicek, J., Roberts, S. C., and Flegr, J. (2005). Women’s preference for dominant male odour: effects of menstrual cycle and relationship status. Biol. Lett. 1, 256–259. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2005.0332
Havlicek, J., Saxton, T. K., Roberts, S. C., Jozifkova, E., Lhota, S., Valentova, J., et al. (2008). He sees, she smells? Male and female reports of sensory reliance in mate choice and non-mate choice contexts. Pers. Individ. Differ. 45, 565–570. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.06.019
Haze, S., Gozu, Y., Nakamura, S., Kohno, Y., Sawano, K., Ohta, H., et al. (2001). 2-nonenal newly found in human body odor tends to increase with aging. J. Invest. Dermatol. 116, 520–524. doi: 10.1046/j.0022-202x.2001.01287.x
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 61–83. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
Hepper, P. G. (1988). The discrimination of human odour by the dog. Perception 17, 549–554. doi: 10.1068/p170549
Herz, R. S., and Cahill, E. D. (1997). Differential use of sensory information in sexual behavior as a function of gender. Hum. Nat. 8, 275–286. doi: 10.1007/BF02912495
Herz, R. S., and Inzlicht, M. (2002). Sex differences in response to physical and social factors involved in human mate selection. The importance of smell for women. Evol. Hum. Behav. 23, 359–364. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(02)00095-8
Hess, U., and Fischer, A. (2013). Emotional mimicry as social regulation. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 17, 142–157. doi: 10.1177/1088868312472607
Hold, B., and Schleidt, M. (1977). The importance of human odour in non−verbal communication. Z. Tierpsychol. 43, 225–238. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1977.tb00072.x
Hornung, J., Kogler, L., Erb, M., Freiherr, J., and Derntl, B. (2018a). The human body odor compound androstadienone increases neural conflict coupled to higher behavioral costs during an emotional Stroop task. Neuroimage 171, 364–375. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.027
Hornung, J., Kogler, L., Wolpert, S., Freiherr, J., and Derntl, B. (2017). The human body odor compound androstadienone leads to anger-dependent effects in an emotional Stroop but not dot-probe task using human faces. PLoS One 12:e0175055. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0175055
Hornung, J., Noack, H., Thomas, M., Farger, G., Nieratschker, V., Freiherr, J., et al. (2018b). Bayesian informed evidence against modulation of androstadienone-effects by genotypic receptor variants and participant sex: a study assessing Stroop interference control, mood and olfaction. Horm. Behav. 98, 45–54. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.12.003
Hummer, T. A., and McClintock, M. K. (2009). Putative human pheromone androstadienone attunes the mind specifically to emotional information. Horm. Behav. 55, 548–559. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.01.002
Huoviala, P., and Rantala, M. J. (2013). A putative human pheromone, androstadienone, increases cooperation between men. PLoS One 8:e62499. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062499
Izard, C. E. (1993). Four systems for emotion activation: cognitive and noncognitive processes. Psychol. Rev. 100, 68–90. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.100.1.68
Izard, C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2, 260–280. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00044.x
Jacob, S., Kinnunen, L. H., Metz, J., Cooper, M., and McClintock, M. K. (2001). Sustained human chemosignal unconsciously alters brain function. Neuroreport 12, 2391–2394. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200108080-00021
Jessen, S. (2020). Maternal odor reduces the neural response to fearful faces in human infants. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 45:100858. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100858
Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., and Thaler, R. H. (1986). Fairness and the assumptions of economics. J. Bus. 59, S285–S300. doi: 10.1086/296367
Kamiloğlu, R. G., Smeets, M. A. M., de Groot, J., and Semin, G. R. (2018). Fear odor facilitates the detection of fear expressions over other negative expressions. Chem. Senses 43, 419–426. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjy029
Kawai, Y., Nagai, Y., and Asada, M. (2012). “Perceptual development triggered by its self-organization in cognitive learning,” in Proceedings of the IEEE International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems, Vilamoura-Algarve, 5159–5164. doi: 10.1109/IROS.2012.6386102
Kirk-Smith, M. D., and Booth, D. A. (1990). “The effect of five odorants on mood and assessment of other people,” in Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 5, eds D. W. Macdonald, D. Muller-Schwartz, and S. Natynczuk (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 48–54.
Kirk-Smith, M. D., Booth, D. A., Carroll, D., and Davies, P. (1978). Human social attitudes affected by androstenol. Res. Commun. Psychol. Psychiatry Behav. 3, 379–384.
Klaey-Tassone, M., Durand, K., Damon, F., Heyers, K., Mezrai, N., Patris, B., et al. (2020). Human neonates prefer colostrum to mature milk: evidence for an olfactory bias toward the “initial milk”? Am. J. Hum. Biol. 33:e23521. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.23521
Knaapila, A., Tuorila, H., Vuoksimaa, E., Keskitalo-Vuokko, K., Rose, R. J., Kaprio, J., et al. (2012). Pleasantness of the odor of androstenone as a function of sexual intercourse experience in women and men. Arch. Sex. Behav. 41, 1403–1408. doi: 10.1007/s10508-011-9804-7
Kollndorfer, K., Fischmeister, F. P. S., Kowalczyk, K., Hoche, E., Mueller, C. A., Trattnig, S., et al. (2015). Olfactory training induces changes in regional functional connectivity in patients with long-term smell loss. Neuroimage Clin. 9, 401–410. doi: 10.1016/J.NICL.2015.09.004
Kontaris, I., East, B. S., and Wilson, D. A. (2020). Behavioral and neurobiological convergence of odor, mood and emotion: a review. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 14:35. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00035
Kovács, G., Gulyás, B., Savic, I., Perrett, D. I., Cornwell, R. E., Little, A. C., et al. (2004). Smelling human sex hormone-like compounds affects face gender judgment of men. Neuroreport 15, 1275–1277. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000130234.51411.0e
Kuang, S., and Zhang, T. (2014). Smelling directions: olfaction modulates ambiguous visual motion perception. Sci. Rep. 4:5796. doi: 10.1038/srep05796
Kuhn, F., and Natsch, A. (2009). Body odour of monozygotic human twins: a common pattern of odorant carboxylic acids released by a bacterial aminoacylase from axilla secretions contributing to an inherited body odour type. J. R. Soc. Interface 6, 377–392. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2008.0223
Latinus, M., and Belin, P. (2011). Human voice perception. Curr. Biol. 21, R143–R145. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.12.033
Lechien, J. R., Journe, F., Hans, S., Chiesa-Estomba, C. M., Mustin, V., Beckers, E., et al. (2020). Severity of anosmia as an early symptom of COVID-19 infection may predict lasting loss of smell. Front. Med. 7:582802. doi: 10.3389/FMED.2020.582802
LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 155–184. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155
Leleu, A., Rekow, D., Poncet, F., Schaal, B., Durand, K., Rossion, B., et al. (2020). Maternal odor shapes rapid face categorization in the infant brain. Dev. Sci. 23, e12877. doi: 10.1111/desc.12877
Lemogne, C., Smadja, J., Zerdazi, E. H., Soudry, Y., Robin, M., Berthoz, S., et al. (2015). Congenital anosmia and emotion recognition: a case-control study. Neuropsychologia 72, 52–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.04.028
Lenochova, P., and Havlicek, J. (2008). “Human body odour individuality,” in Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 11, eds J. L. Hurst, R. J. Beynon, S. C. Roberts, and T. D. Wyatt (New York, NY: Springer), 189–198. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-73945-8_18
Little, A. C., and Jones, B. C. (2012). Variation in facial masculinity and symmetry preferences across the menstrual cycle is moderated by relationship context. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 999–1008. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.11.007
Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., and DeBruine, L. M. (2011). Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 366, 1638–1659. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0404
Lobmaier, J. S., Probst, F., Fischbacher, U., Wirthmüller, U., and Knoch, D. (2020). Pleasant body odours, but not genetic similarity, influence trustworthiness in a modified trust game. Sci. Rep. 10:3388. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-60407-6
Lord, T., and Kasprzak, M. (1989). Identification of self through olfaction. Percept. Mot. Skills 69, 219–224. doi: 10.2466/pms.1989.69.1.219
Lübke, K. T., and Pause, B. M. (2015). Always follow your nose: the functional significance of social chemosignals in human reproduction and survival. Horm. Behav. 68, 134–144. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.10.001
Lundström, J. N., Boyle, J. A., Zatorre, R. J., and Jones-Gotman, M. (2009). The neuronal substrates of human olfactory based kin recognition. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2571–2580. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20686
Lundström, J. N., and Olsson, M. J. (2005). Subthreshold amounts of social odorant affect mood, but not behavior, in heterosexual women when tested by a male, but not a female, experimenter. Biol. Psychol. 70, 197–204. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.01.008
Lundström, J. N., and Olsson, M. J. (2010). Functional neuronal processing of human body odors. Vitam. Horm. 83, 1–23. doi: 10.1016/S0083-6729(10)83001-8
Macfarlane, A. (1975). “Olfaction in the development of social preferences in the human neonate,” in Proceedings of the Ciba Foundation Symposium, eds R. Porter and M. O’Connor (New York, NY: Elsevier), 103–117. doi: 10.1002/9780470720158.CH7
Mahmut, M. K., and Croy, I. (2019). The role of body odors and olfactory ability in the initiation, maintenance and breakdown of romantic relationships – a review. Physiol. Behav. 207, 179–184. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.05.003
Mahmut, M. K., Stevenson, R. J., and Stephen, I. (2019). Do women love their partner’s smell? Exploring women’s preferences for and identification of male partner and non-partner body odor. Physiol. Behav. 10:112517. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.04.006
Majid, A. (2021). Human olfaction at the intersection of language, culture, and biology. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 111–123. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.11.005
Majid, A., and Burenhult, N. (2014). Odors are expressible in language, as long as you speak the right language. Cognition 130, 266–270. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.004
Majid, A., Burenhult, N., Stensmyr, M., De Valk, J., and Hansson, B. S. (2018). Olfactory language and abstraction across cultures. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 373:20170139. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0139
Majid, A., and Kruspe, N. (2018). Hunter-gatherer olfaction is special. Curr. Biol. 28, 409–413. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.014
Mallet, P., and Schaal, B. (1998). Rating and recognition of peers’ personal odors by 9-year-old children: an exploratory study. J. Gen. Psychol. 125, 47–64. doi: 10.1080/00221309809595576
Marlier, L., and Schaal, B. (2005). Human newborns prefer human milk: conspecific milk odor is attractive without postnatal exposure. Child Dev. 76, 155–168. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00836.x
McBurney, D. H., Levine, J. M., and Cavanaugh, P. H. (1976). Psychophysical and social ratings of human body odor. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 3, 135–138. doi: 10.1177/014616727600300126
McGann, J. P. (2017). Poor human olfaction is a 19th-century myth. Science 356:eaam7263. doi: 10.1126/science.aam7263
Meredith, M., and Stein, B. (1983). Interactions among converging sensory inputs in the superior colliculus. Science 221, 389–391. doi: 10.1126/science.6867718
Mitro, S., Gordon, A. R., Olsson, M. J., and Lundström, J. N. (2012). The smell of age: perception and discrimination of body odors of different ages. PLoS One 7:e38110. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038110
Moher, D., Naudet, F., Cristea, I. A., Miedema, F., Ioannidis, J. P. A., and Goodman, S. N. (2018). Assessing scientists for hiring, promotion, and tenure. PLoS Biol. 16:e2004089. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2004089
Morey, R. D., Chambers, C. D., Etchells, P. J., Harris, C. R., Hoekstra, R., Lakens, D., et al. (2016). The peer reviewers’ openness initiative: incentivizing open research practices through peer review. R. Soc. Open Sci. 3:150547. doi: 10.1098/rsos.150547
Morrot, G., Brochet, F., and Dubourdieu, D. (2001). The color of odors. Brain Lang. 79, 309–320. doi: 10.1006/brln.2001.2493
Moshkin, M., Litvinova, N., Litvinova, E. A., Bedareva, A., Lutsyuk, A., and Gerlinskaya, L. (2012). Scent recognition of infected status in humans. J. Sex. Med. 9, 3211–3218. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2011.02562.x
Mujica-Parodi, L. R., Strey, H. H., Frederick, B., Savoy, R., Cox, D., Botanov, Y., et al. (2009). Chemosensory cues to conspecific emotional stress activate amygdala in humans. PLoS One 4:e6415. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006415
Munafò, M. R., Nosek, B. A., Bishop, D. V. M., Button, K. S., Chambers, C. D., Percie Du Sert, N., et al. (2017). A manifesto for reproducible science. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:0021. doi: 10.1038/s41562-016-0021
Munoz, N. E., and Blumstein, D. T. (2012). Multisensory perception in uncertain environments. Behav. Ecol. 23, 457–462. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arr220
Munoz, N. E., and Blumstein, D. T. (2020). Optimal multisensory integration. Behav. Ecol. 31, 184–193. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arz175
Murphy, S. T., and Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 64, 723–739. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.5.723
Mutic, S., Moellers, E. M., Wiesmann, M., and Freiherr, J. (2016). Chemosensory communication of gender information: masculinity bias in body odor perception and femininity bias introduced by chemosignals during social perception. Front. Psychol. 6:1980. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01980
Natsch, A., and Emter, R. (2020). The specific biochemistry of human axilla odour formation viewed in an evolutionary context. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 375:20190269. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0269
Nelson, L. D., Simmons, J., and Simonsohn, U. (2018). Psychology’s Renaissance. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 69, 511–534. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011836
Newman, C., and Buesching, C. D. (2019). “Detecting the smell of disease and injury: scoping evolutionary and ecological implications,” in Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 14, ed. C. D. Buesching (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 238–250. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-17616-7_17
Nicolaides, N. (1974). Skin lipids: their biochemical uniqueness. Science 186, 19–26. doi: 10.1126/science.186.4158.19
Nisbett, R. E., and Wilson, T. D. (1977). The halo effect: evidence for unconscious alteration of judgments. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 35, 250–256. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.35.4.250
Niu, C., and Zheng, L. (2020). Androstadienone resulted in a rightward shift of women’s preference for sexually dimorphic male faces across the continuum of femininity-masculinity. Horm. Behav. 118:104635. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.104635
Öhman, A. (2009). Of snakes and faces: an evolutionary perspective on the psychology of fear. Scand. J. Psychol. 50, 543–552. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00784.x
Olofsson, J. K., and Gottfried, J. A. (2015). The muted sense: neurocognitive limitations of olfactory language. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 314–321. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.04.007
Olsson, M. J., Lundström, J. N., Kimball, B. A., Gordon, A. R., Karshikoff, B., Hosseini, N., et al. (2014). The scent of disease: human body odor contains an early chemosensory cue of sickness. Psychol. Sci. 25, 817–823. doi: 10.1177/0956797613515681
Open Science Collaboration (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science 349:aac4716. doi: 10.1126/science.aac4716
Oren, C., and Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2017). Preliminary evidence of olfactory signals of women’s fertility increasing social avoidance behavior towards women in pair-bonded men. Sci. Rep. 7:11056. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-11356-0
Österbauer, R. A., Matthews, P. M., Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C. F., Hansen, P. C., and Calvert, G. A. (2005). Color of scents: chromatic stimuli modulate odor responses in the human brain. J. Neurophysiol. 93, 3434–3441. doi: 10.1152/jn.00555.2004
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions. New York, NY: Oxford university Press.
Parma, V., Tirindelli, R., Bisazza, A., Massaccesi, S., and Castiello, U. (2012). Subliminally perceived odours modulate female intrasexual competition: an eye movement study. PLoS One 7:e30645. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030645
Pascalis, O., Loevenbruck, H., Quinn, P. C., Kandel, S., Tanaka, J. W., and Lee, K. (2014). On the links among face processing, language processing, and narrowing during development. Child Dev. Perspect. 8, 65–70. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12064
Pause, B. M. (2012). Processing of body odor signals by the human brain. Chemosens. Percept. 5, 55–63. doi: 10.1007/s12078-011-9108-2
Pause, B. M. (2017). “Human chemosensory communication,” in Springer Handbook of Odor, ed. A. Buettner (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 129–130. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-26932-0_52
Pause, B. M., Krauel, K., Sojka, B., and Ferstl, R. (1998). Body odor evoked potentials: a new method to study the chemosensory perception of self and non-self in humans. Genetica 104, 285–294. doi: 10.1023/A:1026462701154
Pause, B. M., Ohrt, A., Prehn, A., and Ferstl, R. (2004). Positive emotional priming of facial affect perception in females is diminished by chemosensory anxiety signals. Chem. Senses 29, 797–805. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjh245
Pause, B. M., Storch, D., and Lübke, K. T. (2020). Chemosensory communication of aggression: women’s fine-tuned neural processing of male aggression signals. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 375:20190270. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0270
Penn, D. J., Oberzaucher, E., Grammer, K., Fischer, G., Soini, H. A., Wiesler, D., et al. (2007). Individual and gender fingerprints in human body odour. J. R. Soc. Interface 4, 331–340. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2006.0182
Perrotta, V., Graffeo, M., Bonini, N., and Gottfried, J. A. (2016). The putative chemosignal androstadienone makes women more generous. J. Neurosci. Psychol. Econ. 9, 89–99. doi: 10.1037/npe0000055
Petrocelli, M., Cutrupi, S., Salzano, G., Maglitto, F., Salzano, F. A., Lechien, J. R., et al. (2021). Six-month smell and taste recovery rates in coronavirus disease 2019 patients: a prospective psychophysical study. J. Laryngol. Otol. 135, 436–441. doi: 10.1017/S002221512100116X
Platek, S. M., Burch, R. L., and Gallup, G. G. (2001). Sex differences in olfactory self-recognition. Physiol. Behav. 73, 635–640. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00539-X
Platek, S. M., Thomson, J. W., and Gallup, G. G. (2004). Cross-modal self-recognition: the role of visual, auditory, and olfactory primes. Conscious. Cogn. 13, 197–210. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2003.10.001
Porter, R. H. (1998). Olfaction and human kin recognition. Genetica 104, 259–263. doi: 10.1023/A:1026404319384
Porter, R. H., Balogh, R. D., Cernoch, J. M., and Franchi, C. (1986). Recognition of kin through characteristic body odors. Chem. Senses 11, 389–395. doi: 10.1093/chemse/11.3.389
Porter, R. H., Cernoch, J. M., and Balogh, R. D. (1985). Odor signatures and kin recognition. Physiol. Behav. 34, 445–448. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(85)90210-0
Porter, R. H., Cernoch, J. M., and McLaughlin, F. J. (1983). Maternal recognition of neonates through olfactory cues. Physiol. Behav. 30, 151–154. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(83)90051-3
Porter, R. H., and Moore, J. D. (1981). Human kin recognition by olfactory cues. Physiol. Behav. 27, 493–495. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(81)90337-1
Porter, R. H., and Winberg, J. (1999). Unique salience of maternal breast odors for newborn infants. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, 439–449. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(98)00044-X
Prehn, A., Ohrt, A., Sojka, B., Ferstl, R., and Pause, B. M. (2006). Chemosensory anxiety signals augment the startle reflex in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 394, 127–130. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.10.012
Proverbio, A. M. (2017). Sex differences in social cognition: the case of face processing. J. Neurosci. Res. 95, 222–234. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23817
Proverbio, A. M., Zani, A., and Adorni, R. (2008). Neural markers of a greater female responsiveness to social stimuli. BMC Neurosci. 9:56. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-56
Puts, D. A., Jones, B. C., and Debruine, L. M. (2012). Sexual selection on human faces and voices. J. Sex Res. 49, 227–243. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2012.658924
Quinn, K. A., and Macrae, C. N. (2011). The face and person perception: insights from social cognition. Br. J. Psychol. 102, 849–867. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02030.x
Regenbogen, C., Axelsson, J., Lasselin, J., Porada, D. K., Sundelin, T., Peter, M. G., et al. (2017). Behavioral and neural correlates to multisensory detection of sick humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 6400–6405. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1617357114
Rekow, D., Baudouin, J.-Y., Durand, K., and Leleu, A. (2021a). Smell what you hardly see : odors assist categorization in the human visual cortex. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2021.05.25.445626
Rekow, D., Baudouin, J.-Y., Poncet, F., Damon, F., Durand, K., Schaal, B., et al. (2021b). Odor-driven face-like categorization in the human infant brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 118:e2014979118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2014979118
Rekow, D., Leleu, A., Poncet, F., Damon, F., Rossion, B., Durand, K., et al. (2020). Categorization of objects and faces in the infant brain and its sensitivity to maternal odor: further evidence for the role of intersensory congruency in perceptual development. Cogn. Dev. 55:100930. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2020.100930
Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 57, 199–226. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190208
Rikowski, A., and Grammer, K. (1999). Human body odour, symmetry and attractiveness. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 266, 869–874. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0717
Roberts, S. C., Havlíček, J., and Schaal, B. (2020). Human olfactory communication: current challenges and future prospects. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 375:20190258. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0258
Roberts, S. C., Kralevich, A., Ferdenzi, C., Saxton, T. K., Jones, B. C., Debruine, L. M., et al. (2011). Body odor quality predicts behavioral attractiveness in humans. Arch. Sex. Behav. 40, 1111–1117. doi: 10.1007/s10508-011-9803-8
Roberts, S. C., Little, A. C., Lyndon, A., Roberts, J., Havlicek, J., and Wright, R. L. (2009). Manipulation of body odour alters men’s self-confidence and judgements of their visual attractiveness by women. Int. J. Cosmet. Sci. 31, 47–54. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2494.2008.00477.x
Roberts, S. C., Miner, E. J., and Shackelford, T. K. (2010). The future of an applied evolutionary psychology for human partnerships. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 14, 318–329. doi: 10.1037/a0021253
Rocha, M., Parma, V., Lundström, J. N., and Soares, S. C. (2018). Anxiety body odors as context for dynamic faces: categorization and psychophysiological biases. Perception 47, 1054–1069. doi: 10.1177/0301006618797227
Roth, T. S., Samara, I., and Kret, M. E. (2021). Multimodal mate choice: exploring the effects of sight, sound, and scent on partner choice in a speed-date paradigm. Evol. Hum. Behav. 42, 461–468. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2021.04.004
Rubin, D., Botanov, Y., Hajcak, G., and Mujica-Parodi, L. R. (2012). Second-hand stress: inhalation of stress sweat enhances neural response to neutral faces. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 208–212. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq097
Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychol. Rev. 110, 145–172. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145
Sarolidou, G., Axelsson, J., Kimball, B. A., Sundelin, T., Regenbogen, C., Lundström, J. N., et al. (2020). People expressing olfactory and visual cues of disease are less liked. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 375:20190272. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0272
Savic, I., Berglund, H., Gulyas, B., and Roland, P. (2001). Smelling of odorous sex hormone-like compounds causes sex-differentiated hypothalamic activations in humans. Neuron 31, 661–668. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00390-7
Savic, I., Berglund, H., and Lindström, P. (2005). Brain response to putative pheromones in homosexual men. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 7356–7361. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0407998102
Saxton, T. K., Lyndon, A., Little, A. C., and Roberts, S. C. (2008). Evidence that androstadienone, a putative human chemosignal, modulates women’s attributions of men’s attractiveness. Horm. Behav. 54, 597–601. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.06.001
Schaal, B. (1988). Olfaction in infants and children: developmental and functional perspectives. Chem. Senses 13, 145–190. doi: 10.1093/chemse/13.2.145
Schaal, B. (2013). “Comment les émotions se transforment en odeurs? Vers l’hypothèse de la contagion émotionnelle olfactive,” in Odeurs et Emotions. Le Nez a ses Raisons, eds B. Schaal, C. Ferdenzi, and O. Wathelet (Dijon: Editions Universitaires de Dijon), 293–335.
Schaal, B., and Durand, K. (2012). “The role of olfaction in human multisensory development,” in Multisensory Development, eds A. Bremner, D. Lewkowicz, and C. Spence (Oxford: Oxford university Press), 29–62. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586059.003.0002
Schaal, B., Hummel, T., and Soussignan, R. (2004). Olfaction in the fetal and premature infant: functional status and clinical implications. Clin. Perinatol. 31, 261–285. doi: 10.1016/j.clp.2004.04.003
Schaal, B., and Marlier, L. (1998). Maternal and paternal perception of individual odor signatures in human amniotic fluid – Potential role in early bonding? Neonatology 74, 266–273. doi: 10.1159/000014033
Schaal, B., and Porter, R. H. (1991). “Microsmatic humans” revisited: the generation and perception of chemical signals. Adv. Study Behav. 20, 135–199. doi: 10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60321-6
Schaal, B., Saxton, T. K., Loos, H., Soussignan, R., and Durand, K. (2020). Olfaction scaffolds the developing human from neonate to adolescent and beyond. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 375:20190261. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0261
Schäfer, L., Sorokowska, A., Sauter, J., Schmidt, A. H., and Croy, I. (2020). Body odours as a chemosignal in the mother–child relationship: new insights based on an human leucocyte antigen-genotyped family cohort. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 375:20190266. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0266
Schirmer, A., and Adolphs, R. (2017). Emotion perception from face, voice, and touch: comparisons and convergence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 216–228. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.01.001
Schleidt, M. (1980). Personal odor and nonverbal communication. Ethol. Sociobiol. 1, 225–231. doi: 10.1016/0162-3095(80)90009-6
Scollon, C. N., Diener, E., Oishi, S., and Biswas-Diener, R. (2004). Emotions across cultures and methods. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 35, 304–326. doi: 10.1177/0022022104264124
Sela, L., and Sobel, N. (2010). Human olfaction: a constant state of change-blindness. Exp. Brain Res. 205, 13–29. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2348-6
Semin, G. R., Garrido, M. V., and Palma, T. A. (2012). “Socially situated cognition: recasting social cognition as an emergent phenomenon,” in The SAGE Handbook of Social Cognition, eds S. T. Fiske and C. N. Macrae (Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications), 138–164. doi: 10.4135/9781446247631.n8
Sergeant, M. J. T. (2010). Female perception of male body odor. Vitam. Horm. 83, 25–45. doi: 10.1016/S0083-6729(10)83002-X
Siegel, E. H., Sands, M. K., Van den Noortgate, W., Condon, P., Chang, Y., Dy, J., et al. (2018). Emotion fingerprints or emotion populations? A meta-analytic investigation of autonomic features of emotion categories. Psychol. Bull. 144, 343–393. doi: 10.1037/bul0000128
Silvia, F., Gomes, N., Korb, S., and Semin, G. R. (2020). Not all emotions are equal: fear chemosignals lower awareness thresholds only for fearful faces. Chem. Senses 45, 601–608. doi: 10.1080/2331186X.2019.1662162
Smeets, M. A. M., Rosing, E. A. E., Jacobs, D. M., van Velzen, E., Koek, J. H., Blonk, C., et al. (2020). Chemical fingerprints of emotional body odor. Metabolites 10:84. doi: 10.3390/metabo10030084
Sobel, N. (2017). Revisiting the revisit: added evidence for a social chemosignal in human emotional tears. Cogn. Emot. 31, 151–157. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1177488
Sorokowska, A. (2013a). Assessing personality using body odor: differences between children and adults. J. Nonverbal Behav. 37, 153–163. doi: 10.1007/s10919-013-0152-2
Sorokowska, A. (2013b). Seeing or smelling? Assessing personality on the basis of different stimuli. Pers. Individ. Differ. 55, 175–179. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.02.026
Sorokowska, A., Sorokowski, P., and Havlícek, J. (2016). Body odor based personality judgments: the effect of fragranced cosmetics. Front. Psychol. 7:530. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00530
Sorokowska, A., Sorokowski, P., and Szmajke, A. (2012). Does personality smell? Accuracy of personality assessments based on body odour. Eur. J. Pers. 26, 496–503. doi: 10.1002/per.848
Sorokowski, P., Karwowski, M., Misiak, M., Marczak, M. K., Dziekan, M., Hummel, T., et al. (2019). Sex differences in human olfaction: a meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 10:242. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00242
Spear, N. E. (1984). “Ecologically determined dispositions control the ontogeny of learning and memory,” in Comparative Perspectives on the Development of Memory, eds R. Kail and N. E. Spear (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 325–358.
Spence, C. (2021). The scent of attraction and the smell of success: crossmodal influences on person perception. Cogn. Res. 6:46. doi: 10.1186/s41235-021-00311-3
Stevenson, R. J. (2010). An initial evaluation of the functions of human olfaction. Chem. Senses 35, 3–20. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjp083
Stoddart, D. M. (1990). The Scented Ape: The Biology and Culture of Human Odour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Stowers, L., and Spehr, M. (2015). “The vomeronasal organ,” in Handbook of Olfaction and Gustation, ed. R. L. Doty (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc), 1113–1132. doi: 10.1002/9781118971758.ch51
Susskind, J. M., Lee, D. H., Cusi, A., Feiman, R., Grabski, W., and Anderson, A. K. (2008). Expressing fear enhances sensory acquisition. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 843–850. doi: 10.1038/nn.2138
Syrjänen, E., Fischer, H., Liuzza, M. T., Lindholm, T., and Olofsson, J. K. (2021). A review of the effects of valenced odors on face perception and evaluation. Iperception 12:20416695211009550. doi: 10.1177/20416695211009552
Thorne, F., Neave, N., Scholey, A., Moss, M., and Fink, B. (2002). Effects of putative male pheromones on female ratings of male attractiveness: influence of oral contraceptives and the menstrual cycle. Neuroendocrinol. Lett. 23, 291–297.
Thornhill, R., and Gangestad, S. W. (1993). Human facial beauty – averageness, symmetry, and parasite resistance. Hum. Nat. 4, 237–269. doi: 10.1007/BF02692201
Thornhill, R., and Gangestad, S. W. (1999). The scent of symmetry a human sex pheromone that signals fitness? Evol. Hum. Behav. 20, 175–201. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00005-7
Thornhill, R., Gangestad, S. W., Miller, R., Scheyd, G., McCollough, J. K., and Franklin, M. (2003). Major histocompatibility complex genes, symmetry, and body scent attractiveness in men and women. Behav. Ecol. 14, 668–678. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arg043
Thorstenson, C. A. (2018). The social psychophysics of human face color: review and recommendations. Soc. Cogn. 36, 247–273. doi: 10.1521/soco.2018.36.2.247
Thysen, B., Elliott, W. H., and Katzman, P. A. (1968). Identification of estra-1,3,5 (10),16-tetraen-3-ol (estratetraenol) from the urine of pregnant women (1). Steroids 11, 73–87. doi: 10.1016/S0039-128X(68)80052-2
van Nieuwenburg, D., de Groot, J. H. B., and Smeets, M. A. M. (2019). The subtle signaling strength of smells: a masked odor enhances interpersonal trust. Front. Psychol. 10:1890. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01890
Verhagen, J. V., and Engelen, L. (2006). The neurocognitive bases of human multimodal food perception: sensory integration. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 613–650. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.11.003
Verheggen, F. J., Haubruge, E., and Mescher, M. C. (2010). Alarm pheromones-chemical signaling in response to danger. Vitam. Horm. 83, 215–239. doi: 10.1016/S0083-6729(10)83009-2
Weisfeld, G. E., Czilli, T., Phillips, K. A., Gall, J. A., and Lichtman, C. M. (2003). Possible olfaction-based mechanisms in human kin recognition and inbreeding avoidance. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 85, 279–295. doi: 10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00061-4
Welch, R. B., and Warren, D. H. (1980). Immediate perceptual response to intersensory discrepancy. Psychol. Bull. 88, 638–667. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.88.3.638
White, T. L., and Cunningham, C. (2017). Sexual preference and the self-reported role of olfaction in mate selection. Chemosens. Percept. 10, 31–41. doi: 10.1007/s12078-017-9223-9
Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Barrett, L. F., and Barsalou, L. W. (2013). Neural evidence that human emotions share core affective properties. Psychol. Sci. 24, 947–956. doi: 10.1177/0956797612464242
Wisman, A., and Shrira, I. (2020). Sexual chemosignals: evidence that men process olfactory signals of women’s sexual arousal. Arch. Sex. Behav. 49, 1505–1516. doi: 10.1007/s10508-019-01588-8
Wudarczyk, O. A., Kohn, N., Bergs, R., Goerlich, K. S., Gur, R. E., Turetsky, B., et al. (2016). Chemosensory anxiety cues enhance the perception of fearful faces – an fMRI study. Neuroimage 143, 214–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.002
Wyatt, T. D. (2003). Pheromones and Animal Behaviour: Communication by Smell and Taste. Cambridge: Cambridge university press.
Wyatt, T. D. (2015). The search for human pheromones: the lost decades and the necessity of returning to first principles. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 282:20142994. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.2994
Wyatt, T. D. (2020). Reproducible research into human chemical communication by cues and pheromones: learning from psychology’s renaissance. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 375:20190262. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0262
Wysocki, C. J., and Preti, G. (2004). Facts, fallacies, fears, and frustrations with human pheromones. Anat. Rec. A Discov. Mol. Cell. Evol. Biol. 281, 1201–1211. doi: 10.1002/ar.a.20125
Yamazaki, S., Hoshino, K., and Kusuhara, M. (2010). Odor associated with aging. Anti Aging Med. 7, 60–65. doi: 10.3793/jaam.7.60
Ye, Y., Zhuang, Y., Smeets, M. A. M., and Zhou, W. (2019). Human chemosignals modulate emotional perception of biological motion in a sex-specific manner. Psychoneuroendocrinology 100, 246–253. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.10.014
Yeshurun, Y., and Sobel, N. (2010). An odor is not worth a thousand words: from multidimensional odors to unidimensional odor objects. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 61, 219–241. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163639
Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection – a selection for a handicap. J. Theor. Biol. 53, 205–214. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(75)90111-3
Zellner, D. A., Bartoli, A. M., and Eckard, R. (1991). Influence of color on odor identification and liking ratings. Am. J. Psychol. 104, 547–561. doi: 10.2307/1422940
Zernecke, R., Haegler, K., Kleemann, A. M., Albrecht, J., Frank, T., Linn, J., et al. (2011). Effects of male anxiety chemosignals on the evaluation of happy facial expressions. J. Psychophysiol. 25, 116–123. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000047
Zheng, Y., You, Y., Farias, A. R., Simon, J., Semin, G. R., Smeets, M. A., et al. (2018). Human chemosignals of disgust facilitate food judgment. Sci. Rep. 8:17006. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-35132-w
Zhou, W., and Chen, D. (2009). Fear-related chemosignals modulate recognition of fear in ambiguous facial expressions. Psychol. Sci. 20, 177–183. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02263.x
Zhou, W., and Chen, D. (2011). Entangled chemosensory emotion and identity: familiarity enhances detection of chemosensorily encoded emotion. Soc. Neurosci. 6, 270–276. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2010.523537
Zhou, W., Jiang, Y., He, S., and Chen, D. (2010). Olfaction modulates visual perception in binocular rivalry. Curr. Biol. 20, 1356–1358. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.05.059
Keywords: multisensory perception, olfaction, vision, body odor, face processing, emotion, adults, infants
Citation: Damon F, Mezrai N, Magnier L, Leleu A, Durand K and Schaal B (2021) Olfaction in the Multisensory Processing of Faces: A Narrative Review of the Influence of Human Body Odors. Front. Psychol. 12:750944. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.750944
Received: 31 July 2021; Accepted: 06 September 2021;
Published: 05 October 2021.
Edited by:
Sara Invitto, University of Salento, ItalyReviewed by:
Jonas K. Olofsson, Stockholm University, SwedenAnna Oleszkiewicz, University of Wrocław, Poland
Copyright © 2021 Damon, Mezrai, Magnier, Leleu, Durand and Schaal. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Fabrice Damon, fabrice.damon@u-bourgogne.fr