Skip to main content

CLINICAL TRIAL article

Front. Psychol., 24 December 2021
Sec. Neuropsychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Psychotic Experiences, Social Cognition and Pragmatic Communication in the Psychosis Continuum View all 12 articles

Voices 2: Improving Prosodic Recognition in Schizophrenia With an Online Rehabilitation Program

\r\nMaría Lado-Codesido,María Lado-Codesido1,2Rosa María Rey VarelaRosa María Rey Varela3Marina Larios QuionesMarina Larios Quiñones4Luis Martínez AgulleiroLuis Martínez Agulleiro5Julieta Ossa BasanesJulieta Ossa Basanes2María Martínez QuerolMaría Martínez Querol2Raimundo MateosRaimundo Mateos6Carlos SpuchCarlos Spuch7Alejandro García-Caballero,*\r\nAlejandro García-Caballero7,8*
  • 1University of Santiago de Compostela, Santiago de Compostela, Spain
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Donostia University Hospital, San Sebastián, Spain
  • 3Department of Psychology, National University of Distance Education, Madrid, Spain
  • 4Centro de Rehabilitación Laboral “Nueva Vida,” Red Pública de Atención Social a Personas con Enfermedad Mental Grave y Duradera, Consejería de Políticas Sociales, Familias, Igualdad y Natalidad, Asociación Psiquiatría y Vida, Madrid, Spain
  • 5Department of Psychiatry, Complexo Hospitalario Universitario de Ferrol, Ferrol, Spain
  • 6Department of Psychiatry, University of Santiago de Compostela, Santiago de Compostela, Spain
  • 7Translational Neuroscience Research Group, Galicia Sur Health Research Institute, University of Vigo, CIBERSAM, Vigo, Spain
  • 8Department of Psychiatry, University Hospital Complex of Ourense, Ourense, Spain

Introduction: Emotion recognition of voices may play an important role in interpersonal communication and patients with schizophrenia present alterations in this regard. Several on-line rehabilitation tools have been developed for treatment in this area. Voices is an on-line prosodic recognition program consisting of identifying different emotional tones in neutral phrases, in different sessions of gradually increasing difficulty. This training tool has previously reported benefits, and a new version has been created called Voices 2. The main aim of this study is to test the capacity of the Voices 2 program to improve emotion recognition through prosody for adults with schizophrenia. Secondly, it seeks to observe durability effects 1 month after intervention.

Method: A randomized, single-blind, multicenter clinical trial was conducted with 44 outpatients diagnosed with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. The intervention group (also called Voices) was treated with Voices 2, whereas the control group was treated with auditory training that was not related to emotions. Sociodemographic and clinical data, clinical state (PANSS), Intelligence Quotient and prosodic recognition (RMV-SV) were measured at baseline. After intervention, RMV-SV and PANSS were assessed. One month later, the RMV-SV measure was repeated.

Results: The control group (n = 19) and the Voices group (n = 22) did not differ on χ2, t or U tests in sociodemographic, clinical and psychometric variables at baseline or post-intervention (all p-values > 0.05). In the Voices group, statistically significant differences were observed in the RMV-SV scale applied post-intervention vs. that applied pre-intervention (Z = 2.47, p = 0.013). Similar results were observed in the 1-month follow-up RMV-SV vs. the pre-intervention RMV-SV (Z = 1.97, p = 0.049). PANSS scale was also assessed with no significant differences between pre vs. post measures in both groups. Lastly, Voices 2 was rated relatively higher, based on its ease of understanding, entertainment value, usefulness and the appropriateness of use of its emotional glossary.

Discussion: Improvements were observed in prosodic recognition following intervention with Voices 2 in the Voices group. Although these results are similar to other clinical trial rehabilitation programs, specific research on the matter remains scarce. Certain aspects, such as the durability of effects or adherence should be thoroughly studied and clarified.

Clinical Trial Registration: [https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/G95C4].

Introduction

Schizophrenia is a severe and chronic mental disorder that affects 20 million people across the world (James et al., 2018). There are three types of main symptoms: positive symptoms (such as delusions, hallucinations, or disorganized speech); negative symptoms (affective flattening, alogia, or avolition) and cognitive symptoms (dysfunctions in working memory or processing speed, deficits in reasoning and abstract thinking, among others). Positive symptoms are manifested in acute episodes of the illness, while negative and cognitive symptoms are present during inter-episode phases, and they represent a significant factor in the limitation of the quality of life of patients. People with schizophrenia may present a high degree of social, occupational and academic dysfunction throughout their life (Ventura et al., 2009; Halverson et al., 2019).

The cognitive symptoms of the illness include deficits related to detection, processing and the use of social information, which enables integration and communication with other people. These tasks are part of Social Cognition (SC), and SC deficits are significantly related to deteriorating functionality in schizophrenia (Fett et al., 2011; Pinkham, 2014).

One of the bases of SC, which enables correct social interaction, is emotional processing. Emotional processing alterations do not improve with antipsychotic treatment (Penn et al., 2009), and they are related to the severity of the illness (Irani et al., 2012). Facial emotion processing deficits have been widely documented (Kohler et al., 2010; Gao et al., 2021). In studies related to other sensory channels, such as the auditory pathway, results are similar to those of facial recognition, although they are collected less frequently (Lin et al., 2018). In the auditory channel, emotional recognition is carried out through tone of voice, also called affective prosody.

Affective prosody is an important issue in the field of schizophrenia because it helps to clarify meanings and resolve ambiguity in human speech through the auditory channel, when no other channel is available (e.g., a phone call) (Paulmann and Pell, 2011). Moreover, prosodic recognition is integrated as part of the multisensory channels needed for emotion communication (composed of facial and linguistic expressions and paralinguistic inputs), and it is essential for interpersonal relations. If any of these channels fail to integrate emotional signals, interpersonal conflicts could arise (Lin et al., 2020).

Deficits in prosodic recognition are associated with negative and cognitive symptoms (Thomas et al., 2017). Basic auditory skills and auditory emotion processing are impaired in schizophrenia patients with cognitive disturbances (Kraus et al., 2019). The presence of negative symptomatology has been associated with worsening prosodic recognition (Leitman et al., 2005; Castagna et al., 2013). One of the hypotheses considered by Castagna et al. (2013) is that both the cognitive state and the negative symptoms that are altered in schizophrenia could bias the evaluation and discrimination of the auditory stimuli from the environment, making them less competitive, and therefore, more deficient than in non-pathological conditions.

Moreover, it has been shown that deficits in prosodic perception have a crucial effect on global functionality (Leitman et al., 2005, 2007). These prosodic deficits also have an impact on social skills, resulting in alterations, misunderstandings, and inappropriate social responses (Kee et al., 2003; Pinkham and Penn, 2006). Primary auditory processing has been directly related to social functioning, independently of cognition (Medalia et al., 2019).

Due to the impact of emotional processing on cognitive and social dysfunction, it is vitally important to study its treatment. Several research avenues have been developed in the treatment of prosodic recognition. Pharmacology, the use of oxytocin and neurostimulation are some of the possibilities under development, although sufficient evidence is still unavailable (Tan et al., 2018). Currently, the most effective treatment is cognitive rehabilitation (Horan and Green, 2019). According to the Cognitive Remediation Experts Workshop (Florence, Italy, April 2010), cognitive rehabilitation is defined as an intervention based on behavioral training, aimed at improving cognitive processes with the aim of achieving a durable and generalized improvement. The results of these objectives in the studies conducted are also varied. The generalized improvements observed after applying an intervention program, with regards to untrained cognitive functions, psychosocial functioning and symptomatology, have been verified in certain studies (McGurk et al., 2007; Kurtz and Richardson, 2012), although reviews point to a lack of attention to this objective (Fiszdon and Reddy, 2012). As for the durability of post-intervention effects, their short-term benefits have been demonstrated (Wykes et al., 2011; Fiszdon and Reddy, 2012; Revell et al., 2015), although research on the maintenance of these improvements in the long term continues to be limited.

Furthermore, among the cognitive rehabilitation programs published, the conditions that must be met to achieve greater effectiveness are not clear. While certain reviews support the use of computerized methods compared to other types (Byrne et al., 2015), others maintain that the type of program does not necessarily entail differences in the results (Morin and Franck, 2017). Likewise, there does not appear to be significant association between a specific intervention based on a particular cognitive function and a global intervention based on several cognitive aspects when designing an intervention program (Grant et al., 2017). Studies agree that the number of tools is scarce and the methodological quality of studies is modest (Wykes et al., 2011; Grant et al., 2017). It is necessary to further examine the design of cognitive rehabilitation tools, by conducting studies with greater methodological rigor, making it possible to compare tools and draw valid conclusions.

At present, social cognition rehabilitation programs are continuously emerging. Emotion recognition is the subdomain that presents a greater number of developed tools for rehabilitation (Fiszdon and Reddy, 2012). One of the main effective SC programs in schizophrenia is called Training Affect Recognition (TAR) (Frommann et al., 2003), which focuses on facial expression recognition of basic emotions. Another program reviewed, called Social Cognition Interaction Training (SCIT) (Penn et al., 2005; Combs et al., 2007), includes various treatment modalities and is based on emotional perception, theory of mind and social perception. The final type of intervention, named Cognitive Enhancement Treatment (CET) (Eack et al., 2007), combines aspects of SC with neurocognition training and includes a prosodic rehabilitation module.

Despite this, literature on prosodic recognition rehabilitation remains scarce. Certain programs have been designed, such as Cognitive Pragmatic Treatment (Bosco et al., 2016), which is administered based on a group therapy model, in 20 half-hour sessions. Training is based on linguistics, paralinguistics and extra-linguistics, theory of mind and other cognitive functions. The results of this study did not provide affective prosodic measurements, and the sample was too small (n = 17). In another type of program created by Bechi et al. (2018), based on auditory and visual training and comprising of 8 h long sessions, the results showed an improvement in auditory abilities with training and an extended benefit in the auditory channel upon completion of the visual training. Finally, SocialVille (Nahum et al., 2014) is another relevant program in prosodic recognition rehabilitation. This program combines 40 working sessions of SC exercises, including auditory perception of basic emotion training, visual affect perception, social cue perception, theory of mind, self-referential style and empathy. The results showed an improvement in SC scales and functioning (Miley et al., 2020).

In this context, the web platform www.e-motionaltraining.com was developed, which includes different rehabilitation games in SC and social skills for schizophrenia and other mental illnesses (Vázquez-Campo et al., 2016; Maroño Souto et al., 2018). A specific intervention was created on this web platform, focused on affective prosody recognition, called Voices. The Voices program consists of eight sessions of varying levels in the form of a game with 15 different trials. In each trial, sentences of speech with neutral lexical content (e.g., “I brought what you asked for”) are reproduced in different emotional valences (e.g., happy, angry, surprised…). These sentences are recorded by professional actors and actresses and uploaded to the program in an mp3 file. For each trial, two, three or four response options with different emotions are shown, with only one being the correct answer. Once the participant has chosen their answer, the program returns feedback regarding the correct/incorrect response. Trial difficulty increases gradually over the course of the sessions. A randomized, multicenter clinical trial was conducted, comparing the intervention with Voices in clinically stable patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, compared to patients with the same diagnosis who regularly attended their routine treatment at psychosocial rehabilitation centers. The result of the prosodic recognition scale applied to evaluate the participants pre- and post-intervention was favorable for the Voices program, compared to conventional rehabilitation (p < 0.05). The participants following the Voices treatment filled out satisfaction surveys on the program, in which 80% of users considered the tool to be easy to use and entertaining (Lado-Codesido et al., 2019). Some of the limitations of the study include the scarcity of similar studies, hindering comparison between intervention programs, the minimal number of effective sessions and the lack of evaluation of other cognitive measures to ascertain the generalized effect of rehabilitation.

Following the initial results of the Voices program, the tool was optimized and adapted, creating the Voices 2 program. New sentences were added, recorded by new voluntary actors and actresses, equipping the tool with greater diversity. A reference glossary was also created to understand certain complex emotional terms (called “emotional glossary”), which can be used by users independently. The study methodology was improved, adding an active control group and a more comprehensive post-intervention evaluation, in order to observe the durability of improvements in the short term. The Voices 2 tool, like all the other programs on the platform www.e-motionaltraining.com, was developed in Spanish.

The main goal of this study was to assess improvements in prosodic recognition with a new version of a training tool called Voices 2, through a prosodic recognition scale, in clinically stable patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. The second aim was to observe the maintenance of the benefits in a short period after intervention (1 month is proposed). The final objective was to evaluate the usability of Voices 2 tasks from the participant’s point of view.

Materials and Methods

A randomized, single-blind, multicenter clinical trial was conducted with 44 outpatients diagnosed with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. The patients were recruited from four psychosocial rehabilitation centers in A Coruña, Madrid and Guipúzcoa. All patients attended the centers as outpatients. The selected patients were randomized by a computer-generated randomization list. The allocation sequence was randomly assigned and concealed from the research team. Three of the recruited patients were lost to follow-up, 2 in the control group (one of them refused to continue, the other moved house), and 1 in the intervention group (who refused to continue), also called the Voices group (Control n = 19, Voices n = 22). Therefore, the final sample was composed of 41 patients (Figure 1). The attrition rate of the intervention was 6.7%.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. CONSORT Flowchart diagram.

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

We included patients who voluntarily agreed to participate in the study, who were between 18 to 65 years of age, had a diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition), were clinically stable and followed up by the Department of Psychiatry, presented an intelligence quotient (IQ) within the normal range (> 70), who can read and write, and had no comorbidity with other psychiatric, neurological or severe auditory diseases or current substance abuse (except nicotine). We excluded patients with legal disability by reason of mental disability. During the informed consent process, the researchers confirmed that participants understood the voluntariness of their participation and the randomization strategy. Written informed consent was therefore obtained by researchers not pertaining to the clinical staff, to minimize social desirability biases. Recruitment was planned for March 2020, but it was postponed due to the COVID-19 pandemic situation from August 2020 until April 2021.

Initial Procedures and Characterization of the Sample

Before the intervention, participants were recruited at each center. An evaluation was conducted by the research team, consisting of the collection of personal, sociodemographic and clinical data, psychometric evaluation for IQ, measured by the K-BIT test, clinical state assessed by the PANSS scale, and prosodic recognition state by the RMV-SV scale (for further information on patient evaluation tools see point 2.5). The mean age of the sample was 43.22 (SD = 1.3), most of the participants were men, and were diagnosed with schizophrenia. For more information, see Table 2.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Description of procedure of the clinical trial.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample and by subgroups.

After these initial procedures, participants were randomized into the Voices and control group. The researchers were blind to the assignment.

Intervention Description

The control group participated using a free computerized music program called Lyrics Training1. This music program entails participants listening to different songs and guessing hidden words from the lyrics that are displayed on the screen while the song is playing. Each correct answer adds points to a total score that is shown at the end of the song. The difficulty of the program can be increased and adapted to the user’s ability. This program can be used autonomously, with the support of the research team where necessary. Although prosody could be considered closely connected to music, this program is focused on semantic aspects of the lyrics, and is not related to social cognitive rehabilitation, or to emotion recognition training (the real aim of the Lyrics Training program is language learning). Moreover, this program is not specific to mental illness.

The Voices group participated in sessions using Voices 2, which is the optimized version of the Voices program (Lado-Codesido et al., 2019). Eighty-two new trials have been added to the sixty-three initial trials, with each sentence recorded by professional actors and actresses, while new sessions of progressive difficulty have also been designed. At the start of the game, the program automatically plays a sentence with neutral lexical content, which was produced with a specific emotion, and several response options are shown with different simple and complex emotions, as explained above. Feedback is given after each trial, and when each game has been completed, the final score is displayed. For each training session, participants can play the same game level as many times as they want, and the scores of each game will be compared on the final screen. In each game, different trials appear randomly. For more specific details about the Voices tool, see Lado-Codesido et al. (2019).

Intervention with Lyrics Training and Voices 2 was composed of a total of eight sessions over the course of a month. These sessions were divided into 2 weekly sessions lasting approximately 30 min. The participants attended their reference center to undergo training. A common data collection protocol was established for all centers involved in the study. Training was conducted with a personal computer or tablet for each participant, in a quiet room, with the support of trained personnel from the research team to show the participant how to use the application. Both the control and the Voices groups underwent treatment as usual (including drug therapy, case management and individual and group psychotherapy).

Natural Semantic Metalanguage

One of the problems arising from our first Voices clinical trial was that users showed difficulties in comprehending the lexicon referring to complex emotions. However, explaining these complex terms was not easy and dictionary definitions were, on many occasions, circular, defining one complex term with a synonym of comparable difficulty. To resolve this issue, it was necessary to seek an operative definition model avoiding circularity, i.e., a “universal” language that could disambiguate complex emotions without recurring to synonyms. From a linguistic perspective, this solution was delivered by means of the so-called “Natural Semantic Metalanguage” (NSM). The NSM is an approach to explain human emotions that allows comprehension across different cultures, previously used in other pathologies such as autism (Mullan et al., 2020) and in other fields of medicine, to facilitate effective communication with patients (Peeters and Marini, 2018). It is a “mini-language” expressed through grammar and a reduced group of “primitive concepts,” which represent the same meaning in every language (Wierzbicka, 1999). These “primitive concepts” are chosen and combined to construct a definition of emotional terms to facilitate the understanding of people with greater difficulties, such as those with psychosis. Further information can be found on the NSM webpage: https://nsm-approach.net/.

Specialists in NSM voluntarily collaborated in the preparation of a consultation tool called “emotional glossary.” Through NSM, they defined complex emotional terms that were used in the game, and which could pose greater difficulties to participants. This consultation tool was then made accessible from the program’s main website and participants could check it autonomously throughout the game and as many times as needed (Figure 2). Unfortunately, only two of the four centers could use the emotional glossary. The assessment questionnaire that was administered at the end of Voices 2 intervention also asked participants to assess this tool and its ease of understanding.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Instructions for the emotional glossary attached to the Voices 2 program. (B) Example of a complex emotional term in NSM. Reprinted from www.e-motionaltraining.com under a CC BY license, with permission from Fundación Biomédica Galicia Sur, original copyright 2018.

Post-testing Procedures

At the end of the intervention with Voices 2 and Lyrics Training, both groups were retested with RMV-SV and PANSS. The Voices group also completed a 5-min user experience questionnaire. One month after the post-intervention test, RMV-SV was tested once more in both groups (Table 1).

Evaluation Tools

Several tools were employed to assess the benefits of performing prosodic training, and to improve Voices 2 tasks.

Patient Evaluation

All patients were assessed by the instruments described below. The results for all the scales mentioned below are provided in raw scores, except for IQ scores that are provided in standard scores:

• Customized datasheet designed by the authors for recording demographic and clinical data, including sex, age, occupation, educational level, marital status, current cohabitation, diagnosis and associated diagnoses, illness duration, handedness, and equivalence of antipsychotic treatment to chlorpromazine. The initial database characterization was obtained from this demographic data. This information was collected once at baseline, during a conventional clinical interview with the patient and based on their electronic medical history.

Reading the Mind in the Voices—Spanish Version, RMV-SV (Sánchez-Reales et al., 2019). Validation of the Reading the Mind in the Voices—Test Revised (RMV-TR) scale (Golan et al., 2007), which includes 33 segments translated and adapted from English and recorded by professional actors, with four response options, with simple and complex emotions. This test was administered before, after and 1 month after finishing the intervention to both groups.

Positive and Negative Symptom Scale (PANSS) (Kay et al., 1987). This scale assesses the positive and negative symptom severity. It was applied before and after intervention to both groups.

Kauffman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT) (Kaufman and Kaufman, 2011). This test includes the measure of verbal and non-verbal intelligence in adults. It was applied before the intervention.

• User experience questionnaire, created for the evaluation of the Voices tool and re-adapted to the Voices 2 tool. The questionnaire included 11 questions on a Likert-type scale (ranging from 1 = total disagreement, to 5 = total agreement) to assess different aspects of the intervention (frequency of PC or Internet use; ease of connection, ease of understanding, entertainment value, autonomy of use and usefulness of Voices 2; subjective perception of the benefits obtained after using the program in the usual environment, new relationships, work environment and self-esteem; duration of the intervention and assessment of the emotional glossary). It was applied post-intervention to the Voices group.

Ethical Aspects

This study has been designed respecting the rules of good clinical practice and the ethical principles for medical research of the World Medical Association, which are reflected in the Declaration of Helsinki and its subsequent amendments. Likewise, European and state regulations regarding medical research are respected, particularly Organic Law 15/1999 of December 13 on the protection of personal data. All patients included were adequately informed about the purpose of the study and were asked to sign an informed consent. This study was approved by the local ethics committee (Clinical Research Ethics Committee of Galicia, Registration code: 2019/530, Euskadi, Registration code: PI2019192).

The authors confirm that all ongoing and related trials of this intervention are registered and anonymized. This study has been registered in an international registry of clinical trials, with Clinical Trial Registry Number: https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/G95C4. Under no circumstances will personal information be published or disclosed to persons outside the investigation or to the Ethics Committee for Clinical Research.

The study complies with current regulations on Intellectual Property, in accordance with Legislative Royal Decree 1/1996 of April 12. The ability of patients to understand the voluntariness of the study was assessed by researchers, by employing a clinical interview.

Statistical Analyses

Statistical analyses were performed, using SPSS version 25.0 software. The accepted α risk was 0.05. The following tests were applied:

• A descriptive analysis was performed. The quantitative variables are presented as means (M) and standard deviation (SD) or medians (Med) and ranges. The qualitative variables are presented as frequencies and percentages.

• To compare characteristics at baseline between the control and Voices groups, normality of data was tested, using the Shapiro–Wilk test. All variables presented a normal distribution except for the following variables: equivalence to chlorpromazine, Total IQ, PANSS positive, general psychopathology and PANSS total at baseline. To compare two qualitative variables, the chi-squared test was used. To compare quantitative variables, Student’s t-test for independent samples (in Gaussian distribution) and U-Mann–Whitney test (in non-Gaussian distribution) were applied.

• To assess significant differences in temporal changes in RMV-SV and PANSS pre-post intervention in each group, normality was again tested with Shapiro–Wilk test. In this case, distribution was non-normal in all variables. For our purpose, Wilcoxon’s signed-rank non-parametric test was applied.

• Lastly, Cohens d statistic was obtained to estimate the effect size in the control and Voices groups.

Results

A total of 44 participants were recruited and met the selected criteria. A single blinded, randomized assignation was carried out to select participants for the control group or for the Voices group. Three participants dropped out of the study. Finally, the statistical analysis was performed over a sample of 41 participants. All the descriptive analyses of the main variables of the sample are described in Table 2.

Baseline Characteristics and Analysis Between Subgroups

No significant differences were found between groups in χ2, t or U test (all p-values > 0.05) in any sociodemographic or clinical measure at baseline (Table 2). RMV-SV raw mean was 19.91 (SD = 4.8) in the control group, and 21.11 (SD = 4.1) in the Voices group, with no statistical differences observed between groups (t = 0.839, p = 0.406). In the IQ test, no differences were observed in terms of the standard scores for total IQ (U = 0.445, p = 0.657), verbal IQ (t = 0.132, p = 0.925), or non-verbal IQ (t = 0.662, p = 0.514) between groups. Nor were any differences found in the raw PANSS scores between subgroups, confirming the homogeneity between subgroups.

Assessment of Changes in RMV-SV Before, After and 1 Month After Intervention in Each Group

Raw RMV-SV scores at baseline, after intervention and 1 month after intervention in both groups are reported in Figure 3. RMV-SV scores after intervention are progressively higher compared to the previous scores, for both subgroups (control RMV-SV post-intervention M = 21.89, SD = 3.60, Voices M = 21.68, SD = 4.68). Similarly, at the 1-month follow-up, the maintenance of scores can be observed (RMV-SV 1 month follow-up control M = 21.44, SD = 4.01 vs. Voices M = 21.48, SD = 4.42). In the comparison between groups, no significant differences were observed in RMV-SV post-intervention (t = 0.161, p = 0.873) and in RMV 1-month follow-up (t = −0.23, p = 0.982).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. This box plot shows raw RMV-SV scores in the control and Voices groups before (in blue), after (in orange) and 1 month after intervention (in gray). Inside each box, “X” represents the raw RMV-SV mean, and the horizontal line shows the median of each group. Error bars represent the maximum and minimum scores of this test in each subgroup.

Time (pre-post) interaction effects were investigated for raw RMV-SV scores and raw PANSS scores (Table 3). Statistically significant differences were observed in the post-intervention RMV-SV scale vs. the pre-intervention RMV-SV (Z = 2.47, p = 0.013) in the Voices group. Likewise, statistically significant differences were observed between the 1-month follow-up RMV-SV, compared to the pre-intervention RMV-SV (Z = 1.97, p = 0.049) in the Voices group. However, there were no significant differences between the post-intervention and 1-month follow-up measures in the Voices group. In the case of the control group, there were no significant differences when contrasting the three raw RMV-SV means. The effect size of RMV-SV between the groups was calculated (d = 0.26, r = 0.13). Effect sizes for the pre-post intervention comparisons are included in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. RMV-SV analysis before, after and 1-month post-intervention.

Furthermore, raw PANSS total and PANSS subscales scores were also assessed before and after intervention to confirm clinical stability. No significant differences were found between pre- and post-intervention measures in both groups (Table 3).

In conclusion, significant improvements were observed in RMV-SV scores after Voices 2 intervention and these improvements were maintained 1 month later. These differences were not reported in the control group. In the case of the clinical variable PANSS, there were no temporal significant differences despite the use of the Voices 2 program.

User Experience Scale

All participants in the Voices group completed a 5-min self-reported questionnaire to describe their perception after using the Voices 2 program. Medians and interquartile ranges can be observed in Figure 4. It should be noted that in the scores referring to the frequencies of PC or Internet use (from “not frequent” to “very frequent”), users obtained lower medians (2.5 for PC use and four for Internet use), with wider interquartile ranges, so they are not highly familiar with this type of tool. Despite the fact that this could be an obstacle for the proper use of Voices 2, users rated the ease of understanding of the program (Med = 4), entertainment value (Med = 4.5), the utility of the program (Med = 4) and the convenience of the emotional glossary (Med = 4) highly.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. User experience scale. Medians and interquartile ranges (in brackets) are represented in each bar.

Discussion

In this randomized clinical study, a prosodic recognition program called Voices 2 has been tested in patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. A pre- and post-intervention assessment was conducted with the prosodic recognition scale RMV-SV and, in the case of clinical state, with the PANSS scale in each group. When comparing post-intervention RMV-SV between groups, no significant differences were observed. Despite this fact, the results confirmed significant differences in the post-intervention RMV-SV scale compared to the baseline values in the Voices group. In the control group, whose training entailed an auditory program that was not related to emotions, these results were not observed. The scores on this prosodic recognition scale also confirmed the significant differences observed 1 month after finalizing intervention in the Voices group, which were not observed in the control group. There were no significant differences in the clinical state of patients pre- and post-intervention in any of the subgroups. Users of the Voices 2 program considered the tool as globally attractive and efficient. These results suggest that Voices 2 could be useful as a specific intervention program for the rehabilitation of emotional prosody in patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder.

The results obtained in the scores of the RMV-SV are similar to the first version of the Voices study (Lado-Codesido et al., 2019). In the current study, it is worth noting that the improvements designed for Voices 2 did not achieve better results compared to the first version of Voices (e.g., the emotional glossary, or the addition of new trials to the game). As explained above, the emotional glossary was applied in two of the four centers and, given that it was used autonomously, its impact was not strictly measured. Although participants found this tool appropriate, further research is needed to clarify its contribution. Likewise, the addition of new trials in Voices 2 did not represent a greater improvement in results, but this variability could help avoid the repetition of phrases, thus reducing the memorization bias. The addition of an active control group in the present study implies increased methodological quality and gives the results greater rigor (Kurtz et al., 2016).

These findings are also in line with similar rehabilitation programs. For example, the SocialVille program, which we consider to be the most comparable with Voices 2, is based on PC games and includes specific auditory training and an active control group (Rose et al., 2015). Although significant results in the SC measures were not obtained in the initial trial (Nahum et al., 2014), when the study was repeated with a larger sample (n = 55), favorable results were observed for SC scales, including a prosodic identification scale (Nahum et al., 2021). At present, programs that include prosodic recognition measures, which would enable us to compare our results with other programs, continue to be extremely limited (Fiszdon and Reddy, 2012; Grant et al., 2017; Tan et al., 2018).

In this study, Voices 2 shows the durability of the effects achieved 1 month after intervention, with results that are consistent with other rehabilitation programs. For example, SocialVille reported positive results in prosodic recognition after 6 months (Miley et al., 2020). In the review carried out by Fiszdon and Reddy (2012), 50 studies on SC treatments were reported. Six of these studies evaluate the short-term maintenance of effects (between 1 day to 6 weeks), with favorable results. Similar results can also be observed in other meta-analyses, with a low number of studies that include this variable (Grynszpan et al., 2011; Wykes et al., 2011). Other studies evaluate maintenance in the longer term (one and 2 years), such as the cognitive rehabilitation program Cognitive Enhancement Therapy (Hogarty et al., 2004; Eack et al., 2011). In a recent meta-analysis carried out by Nijman et al. (2020), it is concluded that improvements are maintained in the follow-up of studies evaluating this variable, but with lower values compared to post-treatment. These conclusions are consistent with the results of Voices 2. Given the significance of the durability of these improvements, as the main objective of cognitive rehabilitation, it is crucial to continue to investigate the effects in the long term, as well as the underlying neoplastic changes brought about by cognitive rehabilitation.

The scores of the satisfaction survey on Voices 2 are also similar to those of the first version of the program, confirming the perception of this tool as accessible and attractive. Other rehabilitation programs also include this type of subjective user evaluation (Nahum et al., 2014; Rus-Calafell et al., 2014; Palumbo et al., 2017). The positive evaluation of the tool, individual motivation, the subjective perception of improvements and feedback given to users during rehabilitation could all positively contribute to treatment response and program adherence (Fiszdon et al., 2020). These points were considered when designing the Voices 2 program.

With regards to program adherence, it is worth noting the low dropout rate of 6.7%, compared to other studies reviewed, with rates of between 23–37% (Fernandez-Gonzalo et al., 2015; Fisher et al., 2017; Grant et al., 2017; Nahum et al., 2021). This could be due to the use of Voices 2 at the regular care centers of participants, who take part in more extensive psychiatric rehabilitation. Furthermore, Voices 2 seeks to represent situations that are similar to the real life of patients. These two factors could be key for treatment adherence (McGurk et al., 2007, 2012; Wykes et al., 2011; Parker et al., 2013). The duration of Voices 2 (approximately 4 weeks, compared to 8–16 weeks for the aforementioned programs), could also be associated with the differences in dropout rates.

Finally, it is worth noting the innovative creation of the emotional glossary as an exploratory tool, with the use of NSM for the first time in a rehabilitation program. In the same way as the work carried out in autism, we find this approach to be very interesting, as a new avenue for the lexical understanding of emotional terms in populations that present deficits in emotional processing. Furthermore, such tools facilitate the autonomous use of the Voices 2 program. Despite the fact that it has not been possible to use the glossary at all centers, users defined it as appropriate (Med = 4). After this first experience with NSM, we feel it would be interesting to expand on and further the study of the emotional glossary for future versions of the program.

Limitations

Certain limitations have been observed in this study. It is necessary to give cognitive rehabilitation studies sufficient methodological quality to enable comparison between studies. According to the criteria of the Clinical Trial Assessment Measure (CTAM), a larger clinical sample would be necessary. The small effect size is also a limitation. Moreover, repeating the RMV-SV scale on three different occasions could represent a learning bias, although this is not in line with the differences observed between subgroups. The repeated use of the RMV-SV is related to the lack of similar measures in Spanish, as well as its ease of application, without the need for prior training. In addition, the use of actors and actresses for play-acting vocal emotions could be considered an artificial way of expressing emotions (Jürgens et al., 2011), and it does not represent natural conditions. Furthermore, following the objectives of cognitive rehabilitation, the inclusion of measures of other cognitive or psychosocial functioning aspects in this study could make it possible to clarify whether the effects of Voices 2 are generalized, an objective that has not been investigated in this study.

Conclusion

Voices 2, the on-line prosodic recognition rehabilitation program, could be effective in improving prosodic recognition in patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, as well as being described as attractive and efficient from the user’s perspective. The durability of the improvements observed is maintained 1 month after intervention with Voices 2. These results support the use of Voices 2 and promote the progress of aspects of psychiatric rehabilitation that still need to be developed. This new tool could provide benefits in terms of the interpersonal communication of patients with schizophrenia, and changes in prosodic recognition in the short term. Further studies could be conducted to continue to examine knowledge of prosodic rehabilitation, improving the quality of studies on emotion recognition intervention and promoting the personalization of rehabilitation treatment for schizophrenia.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/G95C4.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Clinical Research Ethics Committee of Galicia, Registration code: 2019/530, Euskadi, Registration code: PI2019192. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

AG-C and ML-C created the Voices 2 program, selected the participants, applied the intervention, extracted data, and supervised the study. AG-C, ML-C, and LM designed the study. ML-C, ML, RR, LM, JO, and MM participated in patient selection and obtained and extracted data. ML-C, AG-C, and RM wrote and revised the manuscript. All authors have had full access to the study data, have personally reviewed the manuscript and have given their final approval to the version attached.

Funding

Eighty percent of the project was originally funded by the Galician Department of Health with European Regional Development Funds through the PRIS program (Program for the Precommercial Development of Research Results of the public health system of Galicia). The project also received the 8th Edition of the Siota Grant of the College of Psychologists of Galicia, the 2013 Biannual Grant of the Medical-Surgical Academy of Ourense, the 2nd Spanish Innova-eVia award for 2015 and the National Award for the Best Health Videogame at the e-Health Spanish Congress in Madrid 2016. e-Motional training received an unrestricted grant for extending its free use in Spain by Janssen laboratories during the 2018–2019 period. The program is currently funded by the 2020/54 grant of the National Plan on Drugs, Secretary of State of Health of Spain, and freely available on request.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank participants for their collaboration as well as all the professional workers of the following associations: APEM Carballo (A Coruña), Saúde Mental Ferrol, Eume e Ortegal (A Coruña), Agifes (Guipúzcoa), and Centro de Rehabilitacion Psicosocial Nueva Vida (Madrid). We would also like to thank the indispensable contribution of Bert Peeters for his help and María Auxiliadora Barrios for her contribution to the emotional glossary. Lastly, we would like to thank Daniel González Vázquez and Gloria Cámara for their help in the creation of Voices 2.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.lyricstrainning.com

References

Bechi, M., Bosia, M., Spangaro, M., Pigoni, A., Buonocore, M., Scrofani, D., et al. (2018). Visual and audio emotion processing training for outpatients with schizophrenia: an integrated multisensory approach. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 28, 1131–1144. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2016.1240698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosco, F. M., Gabbatore, I., Gastaldo, L., and Sacco, K. (2016). Communicative-Pragmatic Treatment in Schizophrenia: A Pilot Study. Front. Psychol. 7:1–12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, L. K., Pan, L., Mccabe, M., Mellor, D., and Xu, Y. (2015). Assessment of a six-week computer-based remediation program for social cognition in chronic schizophrenia. Shanghai Arch. Psychiatry 27, 296–306. doi: 10.11919/j.issn.1002-0829.215095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castagna, F., Montemagni, C., Maria Milani, A., Rocca, G., Rocca, P., Casacchia, M., et al. (2013). Prosody recognition and audiovisual emotion matching in schizophrenia: The contribution of cognition and psychopathology. Psychiatry Res. 205, 192–198. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.08.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Combs, D. R., Adams, S. D., Penn, D. L., Roberts, D., Tiegreen, J., and Stem, P. (2007). Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) for inpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: Preliminary findings. Schizophr. Res. 91, 112–116. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eack, S. M., Greenwald, D., Hogarty, S. S., and Keshavan, M. S. (2011). One-Year Durability of the Effects of Cognitive Enhancement Therapy on Functional Outcome in Early Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 120, 210–216. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.03.042.One-Year

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eack, S. M., Hogarty, G. E., Greenwald, D. P., Hogarty, S. S., and Keshavan, M. S. (2007). Cognitive enhancement therapy improves emotional intelligence in early course schizophrenia: Preliminary effects. Schizophr. Res. 89, 308–311. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandez-Gonzalo, S., Turon, M., Jodar, M., Pousa, E., Hernandez Rambla, C., García, R., et al. (2015). A new computerized cognitive and social cognition training specifically designed for patients with schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder in early stages of illness: A pilot study. Psychiatry Res. 228, 501–509. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fett, A.-K. J., Viechtbauer, W., Dominguez, M.-G., Penn, D. L., van Os, J., and Krabbendam, L. (2011). The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 573–588. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, M., Nahum, M., Howard, E., Rowlands, A., Brandrett, B., Kermott, A., et al. (2017). Supplementing intensive targeted computerized cognitive training with social cognitive exercises for people with schizophrenia: An interim report. Psychiatr. Rehabil. J. 40, 21–32. doi: 10.1037/prj0000244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiszdon, J. M., and Reddy, L. F. (2012). Review of social cognitive treatments for psychosis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 32, 724–740. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiszdon, J. M., Kurtz, M. M., Parente, L., Choi, J., Connecticut, V. A., System, H., et al. (2020). What variables predict cognitive remediation associated improvement in individuals with psychosis? Schizophr. Res. Cogn. 19:100148. doi: 10.1016/j.scog.2019.100148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frommann, N., Streit, M., and Wölwer, W. (2003). Remediation of facial affect recognition impairments in patients with schizophrenia: a new training program. Psychiatry Res. 117, 281–284. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(03)00039-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Z., Zhao, W., Liu, S., Liu, Z., Yang, C., and Xu, Y. (2021). Facial Emotion Recognition in Schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry 12:1–10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.633717

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., Hill, J. J., and Rutherford, M. D. (2007). The ‘Reading the Mind in the Voice’ Test-Revised: A Study of Complex Emotion Recognition in Adults with and Without Autism Spectrum Conditions. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 1096–1106. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0252-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, N., Lawrence, M., Preti, A., Wykes, T., and Cella, M. (2017). Social cognition interventions for people with schizophrenia: a systematic review focussing on methodological quality and intervention modality. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 56, 55–64. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grynszpan, O., Perbal, S., Pelissolo, A., Fossati, P., Jouvent, R., Dubal, S., et al. (2011). Efficacy and specificity of computer-assisted cognitive remediation in schizophrenia: A meta-analytical study. Psychol. Med. 41, 163–173. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710000607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halverson, T. F., Orleans-Pobee, M., Merritt, C., Sheeran, P., Fett, A., and Penn, D. L. (2019). Pathways to functional outcomes in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: Meta-analysis of social cognitive and neurocognitive predictors. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 105, 212–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hogarty, G. E., Flesher, S., Ulrich, R., Carter, M., Greenwald, D., Pogue-Geile, M., et al. (2004). Cognitive Enhancement Therapy for Schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 61, 866–876.

Google Scholar

Horan, W. P., and Green, M. F. (2019). Treatment of social cognition in schizophrenia: Current status and future directions. Schizophr. Res. 203, 3–11. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irani, F., Seligman, S., Kamath, V., Kohler, C., and Gur, R. C. (2012). A Meta-Analysis of Emotion Perception and Functional Outcomes in Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 137, 203–211. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.01.023.A

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, S. L., Abate, D., Abate, K. H., Abay, S. M., Abbafati, C., Abbasi, N., et al. (2018). Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 354 diseases and injuries for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2017: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017. Lancet 392, 1789–1858. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)32279-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jürgens, R., Hammerschmidt, K., and Fischer, J. (2011). Authentic and Play-Acted Vocal Emotion Expressions Reveal Acoustic Differences. Front. Psychol. 2:1–11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufman, A., and Kaufman, N. (2011). K-BIT: Test Breve de Inteligencia de Kaufman. London: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Kay, S. R., Fiszbein, A., and Opfer, L. A. (1987). The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 13, 261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kee, K. S., Green, M. F., Mintz, J., and Brekke, J. S. (2003). Is emotion processing a predictor of functional outcome in schizophrenia? Schizophr. Bull. 29, 487–497. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohler, C. G., Walker, J. B., Martin, E. A., Healey, K. M., and Moberg, P. J. (2010). Facial Emotion Perception in Schizophrenia: A Meta-analytic Review. Schizophr. Bull. 36, 1009–1019. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraus, M. S., Walker, T. M., Jarskog, L. F., Millet, R. A., and Keefe, R. S. E. (2019). Basic auditory processing de fi cits and their association with auditory emotion recognition in schizophrenia ✩. Schizophr. Res. 204, 155–161. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2018.08.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurtz, M. M., and Richardson, C. L. (2012). Social cognitive training for schizophrenia: A meta-analytic investigation of controlled research. Schizophr. Bull. 38, 1092–1104. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurtz, M. M., Gagen, E., Rocha, N. B. F., Machado, S., and Penn, D. L. (2016). Comprehensive treatments for social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: A critical review and effect-size analysis of controlled studies. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 43, 80–89. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lado-Codesido, M., Méndez Pérez, C., Mateos, R., Olivares, J. M., and García Caballero, A. (2019). Improving emotion recognition in schizophrenia with “VOICES”: An on-line prosodic self-training. PLoS One 14:e0210816. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leitman, D. I., Foxe, J. J., Butler, P. D., Saperstein, A., Revheim, N., and Javitt, D. C. (2005). Sensory contributions to impaired prosodic processing in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 58, 56–61. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leitman, D. I., Hoptman, M. J., Foxe, J. J., Saccente, E., Wylie, G. R., Nierenberg, J., et al. (2007). The neural substrates of impaired prosodic detection in schizophrenia and its sensorial antecedents. Am. J. Psychiatry 164, 474–482. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.164.3.474

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, Y., Ding, H., and Zhang, Y. (2018). Emotional prosody processing in schizophrenic patients: A selective review and meta-analysis. J. Clin. Med. 7:363. doi: 10.3390/jcm7100363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, Y., Ding, H., and Zhang, Y. (2020). Multisensory Integration of Emotion in Schizophrenic Patients. Multisens. Res. 33, 865–901. doi: 10.1163/22134808-bja10016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maroño Souto, Y., Vázquez Campo, M., Díaz Llenderrozas, F., Rodríguez, Álvarez, M., Mateos, R., et al. (2018). Randomized Clinical Trial with e-MotionalTraining® 1.0 for Social Cognition Rehabilitation in Schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry 9:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGurk, S. R., Eack, S. M., Kurtz, M., and Mueser, K. T. (2012). Cognitive Remediation and Psychosocial Rehabilitation for Individuals with Severe Mental Illness. Rehabil. Res. Pract. 2012, 1–2. doi: 10.1155/2012/283602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGurk, S. R., Twamley, E. W., Sitzer, D. I., McHugo, G. J., and Mueser, K. T. (2007). Reviews and Overviews A Meta-Analysis of Cognitive Remediation in Schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 164, 1791–1802.

Google Scholar

Medalia, A., Saperstein, A. M., Qian, M., and Javitt, D. C. (2019). Impact of baseline early auditory processing on response to cognitive remediation for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 208, 397–405. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miley, K., Fisher, M., Nahum, M., Howard, E., Rowlands, A., Brandrett, B., et al. (2020). Six month durability of targeted cognitive training supplemented with social cognition exercises in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. Cogn. 20:100171. doi: 10.1016/j.scog.2019.100171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morin, L., and Franck, N. (2017). Rehabilitation interventions to promote recovery from schizophrenia: A systematic review. Front. Psychiatry 8:100. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mullan, K., Peeters, B., and Sadow, L. (2020). “Using Minimal English to Model a Parental Understanding of Autism,” in Studies in Ethnopragmatics, Cultural Semantics, and Intercultural Communication: Ethnopragmatics and Semantic Analysis, eds K. Mullan, B. Peeters, and L. Sadow (Singapore: Springer Singapore), 1–256. doi: 10.1007/978-981-32-9983-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nahum, M., Fisher, M., Loewy, R., Poelke, G., Ventura, J., Nuechterlein, K. H., et al. (2014). A novel, online social cognitive training program for young adults with schizophrenia: A pilot study. Schizophr. Res. Cogn. 1, e11–e19. doi: 10.1016/j.scog.2014.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nahum, M., Lee, H., Fisher, M., Green, M. F., Hooker, C. I., Ventura, J., et al. (2021). Online Social Cognition Training in Schizophrenia: A Double-Blind, Randomized, Controlled Multi-Site Clinical Trial. Schizophr. Bull. 47, 108–117. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbaa085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nijman, S. A., Veling, W., van der Stouwe, E. C. D., and Pijnenborg, G. H. M. (2020). Social Cognition Training for People With a Psychotic Disorder: A Network Meta-analysis. Schizophr. Bull. 46, 1086–1103. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbaa023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palumbo, D., Mucci, A., Piegari, G., D’Alise, V., Mazza, A., and Galderisi, S. (2017). SoCIAL – training cognition in schizophrenia: A pilot study. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 13, 1947–1956. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S136732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, S., Foley, S., Walker, P., and Dark, F. (2013). Improving the social cognitive deficits of schizophrenia: a community trial of Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT). Australas. Psychiatry 21, 346–351. doi: 10.1177/1039856213486305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulmann, S., and Pell, M. D. (2011). Is there an advantage for recognizing multi-modal emotional stimuli? Motiv. Emot. 35, 192–201. doi: 10.1007/s11031-011-9206-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peeters, B., and Marini, M. (2018). “Narrative Medicine Across Languages and Cultures: Using Minimal English for Increased Comparability of Patients’ Narratives,” in Minimal English for a Global World: Improved Communication Using Fewer Words, ed. C. Goddard (London: Palgrave Macmillan), 259–286. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-62512-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penn, D. L., Keefe, R. S. E., Davis, S. M., Meyer, P. S., Perkins, D. O., Losardo, D., et al. (2009). The effects of antipsychotic medications on emotion perception in patients with chronic schizophrenia in the CATIE trial. Schizophr. Res. 115, 17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penn, D., Roberts, D. L., Munt, E. D., Silverstein, E., Jones, N., and Sheitman, B. (2005). A pilot study of social cognition and interaction training (SCIT) for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 80, 357–359. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinkham, A. E. (2014). Social Cognition in Schizophrenia. J. Clin. Psychiatry 75, 14–19. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13065su1.04

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinkham, A. E., and Penn, D. L. (2006). Neurocognitive and social cognitive predictors of interpersonal skill in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 143, 167–178. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Revell, E. R., Neill, J. C., Harte, M., Khan, Z., and Drake, R. J. (2015). A systematic review and meta-analysis of cognitive remediation in early schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 168, 213–222. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rose, A., Vinogradov, S., Fisher, M., Green, M. F., Ventura, J., Hooker, C., et al. (2015). Randomized controlled trial of computer-based treatment of social cognition in schizophrenia: the TRuSST trial protocol. BMC Psychiatry 15:142. doi: 10.1186/s12888-015-0510-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rus-Calafell, M., Gutiérrez-Maldonado, J., and Ribas-Sabaté, J. (2014). A virtual reality-integrated program for improving social skills in patients with schizophrenia: A pilot study. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 45, 81–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2013.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sánchez-Reales, S., Caballero-Peláez, C., Prado-Abril, J., Inchausti, F., Lado-Codesido, M., García-Caballero, A., et al. (2019). Spanish validation of the “Reading the Mind in the Voice” task: A study of complex emotion recognition in adults with autism spectrum conditions. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 67:101421. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2019.101421

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, B., Lee, S., and Lee, J. (2018). Social cognitive interventions for people with schizophrenia: A systematic review. Asian J. Psychiatr. 35, 115–131. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2016.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, M. L., Green, M. F., Hellemann, G., Sugar, C. A., Tarasenko, M., Calkins, M. E., et al. (2017). Modeling Deficits From Early Auditory Information Processing to Psychosocial Functioning in Schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry 74:37. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.2980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vázquez-Campo, M., Maroño, Y., Lahera, G., Mateos, R., and García-Caballero, A. (2016). e-Motional Training® : Pilot study on a novel online training program on social cognition for patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. Cogn. 4, 10–17. doi: 10.1016/j.scog.2015.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ventura, J., Hellemann, G. S., Thames, A. D., Koellner, V., and Nuechterlein, K. H. (2009). Symptoms as mediators of the relationship between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Schizophr. Res. 113, 189–199. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.03.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wierzbicka, A. (1999). Emotions across Languages and Cultures: Diversity and Universals, 1st Edn. Cambrigde: Cambrigde University Press.

Google Scholar

Wykes, T., Huddy, V., Cellard, C., McGurk, S. R., and Czobor, P. (2011). A Meta-Analysis of Cognitive Remediation for Schizophrenia: Methodology and Effect Sizes. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 472–485. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10060855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: prosodic recognition, emotion recognition, social cognition, natural semantic metalanguage, online cognitive training, computer based cognitive training, schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder

Citation: Lado-Codesido M, Rey Varela RM, Larios Quiñones M, Martínez Agulleiro L, Ossa Basanes J, Martínez Querol M, Mateos R, Spuch C and García-Caballero A (2021) Voices 2: Improving Prosodic Recognition in Schizophrenia With an Online Rehabilitation Program. Front. Psychol. 12:739252. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.739252

Received: 10 July 2021; Accepted: 22 November 2021;
Published: 24 December 2021.

Edited by:

Guillermo Soto, University of Chile, Chile

Reviewed by:

Giulia Agostoni, Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Italy
Kara Hawthorne, Gallaudet University, United States

Copyright © 2021 Lado-Codesido, Rey Varela, Larios Quiñones, Martínez Agulleiro, Ossa Basanes, Martínez Querol, Mateos, Spuch and García-Caballero. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alejandro García-Caballero, alejandro.alberto.garcia.caballero@sergas.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.