Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 17 December 2021
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

The Moderating Role of Anticipated Regret and Product Involvement on Online Impulsive Buying Behavior

\r\nBin Li,,*Bin Li1,2,3*Minqi HuMinqi Hu2Xiaoxi Chen*Xiaoxi Chen2*Yongxin Lei\r\nYongxin Lei2
  • 1The Institute of Enterprise Development, Jinan University, Guangzhou, China
  • 2Management School, Jinan University, Guangzhou, China
  • 3Research Institute on Brand Innovation and Development of Guangzhou, Guangzhou, China

Online impulsive buying behavior has drawn an increasing amount of attention from researchers and marketers as well; however, little research has explored how cognitive aspect and emotional aspect effect online impulsive buying together. The study examines the role of product involvement (cognitive aspect) and anticipated regret (emotional aspect) on the online impulsive buying behavior of the consumer. The results indicate that consumers who experienced downward anticipated regret showed more online impulsive buying behavior than those who experienced upward anticipated regret. Moreover, anticipated regret moderates the relationship between product involvement and online impulsive buying behavior, for participants who experienced downward anticipated regret showing more online impulsive buying behavior than those who experienced upward anticipated regret in the low product involvement group, but there is no differential between downward and upward anticipated regret in the high involvement product group. These findings suggest that anticipated regret helps consumers make more deliberative online shopping choices. The implications for both future research and online consumers are discussed.

Introduction

People today enjoy convenient services provided by shopping websites. Reports from Internet Retailer (2019) indicated that Alibaba and Amazon jointly created a huge sales volume of $1.13 billion in 2018. During the COVID-19, the online store of Amazon achieved a 29% increase in sales (Davis, 2020). Online impulsive buying behavior makes a negative influence on consumers. People make their purchases online based on pictures and description from sellers. However, not all online information from sellers is believable. Consumers may experience negative affect due to online impulsive buying behavior (Ahn and Kahlor, 2020). Although consumers know how passive the situation is, they still engage in online impulsive buying behavior.

Online impulsive buying behavior is prevalent nowadays. Impulsive buying tendency urges consumers to buy the product immediately without hesitation (Chan et al., 2017). Research on impulsive buying behavior has concentrated their attention on external and internal factors. External factors help create atmosphere to urge the impulsive emotions, like shopping festival, quality of shopping website, and so on, of the consumers (Parboteeah et al., 2009; Liao et al., 2016; Guo et al., 2017; Hashmi et al., 2019; Ahn and Kahlor, 2020; Chen and Ku, 2021). Rather than being touched by arranged facilities and wrapped products, internal factors always relate to personal conditions and traits. Young consumers show higher impulsive buying tendency (Styvén et al., 2017). Online impulsive buying tendency is positively related to pressure (Moran and Kwak, 2015). When consumers feel time pressure, online impulsive buying behavior would become a carrier of their negative emotions (Sohn and Lee, 2017). More perceived relevance would promote the online impulsive buying tendency of the consumers (Dodoo and Wu, 2019). Under the situation of online shopping, it seems that people are more likely to shop without consideration of consequences.

It seems that emotion takes a leading position in online impulsive buying behavior. However, impulsive buying behavior also has its cognitive part. The cognitive function of emotion and the co-existence of cognition and emotion in the online shopping experience are supported as well (Nussbaum, 2003; Izogo and Jayawardhena, 2018). Product involvement links to the cognitive aspect of online impulsive buying behavior (Sohn and Lee, 2017). Danish Habib and Qayyum (2018) found the subsequence between cognitive aspect and emotional aspect in online impulsive buying behavior. This article agrees that cognition and emotion work mutually in online impulsive buying behavior. Emotion could link cognitive consequences, and reversely cognition could link emotional processes (Danish Habib and Qayyum, 2018; Chen et al., 2020; Chen and Ku, 2021).

Lesser studies press on affective aspects and cognitive aspects simultaneously. The decisions of online impulsive buying behavior depend on a combination of affective system and cognitive system. This study is conducted to shed more light on the mental process behind online impulsive buying behavior, especially how the cognitive aspect affects the emotional aspect. College students from China were invited to a simulated scenario to test product involvement, anticipated regret, and online impulsive buying behavior. The result was supposed to tell online impulsive buying behavior under the interaction of product involvement and anticipated regret.

Hypothesis Development

Theoretical Background

Easily accessible online shopping makes online impulsive buying closer to consumers. Impulsive buying behavior refers to the tendency of the consumers to buy spontaneously, unreflectively, immediately, and kinetically (Rook, 1987; Rook and Fisher, 1995). Features of impulsive buying behavior are the lack of information and insufficient evaluation of choices (Lim and Rashad, 2015; Xu et al., 2020). Control over self when faced with an online stimulus is also important in online impulsive buying behavior (Parboteeah et al., 2009). Most researchers use stimulus-organism-response theory and the theory of planned behavior to define impulsive buying behavior (Changa et al., 2011; Bilal Ahmad et al., 2019; Vazquez et al., 2020; Wu et al., 2020). Emotion state of mind is a significant mediator in stimulus-organism-response theory (Bilal Ahmad et al., 2019). Impulsiveness further facilitates the formation of unplanned impulsive buying behavior. To distinguish online impulsive buying from conventional online impulsive buying, Madhavaram and Laverie (2004) further defined online impulsive buying as the immediate reaction of consumers to external stimuli, especially stimuli of sensory information online stores.

In the extant literature, research has examined how website quality, review, social factor, and other factors influence online impulsive buying (Chang et al., 2012; Zhang and Zhang, 2015; Hashmi et al., 2019; Lin and Liu, 2019; Zhao et al., 2019). Product-related information could equip online consumers well when faced with potential risk on online shopping and post-purchase regret (Izah and Iskandar, 2019; Wu et al., 2020). Searching information could also help alleviate uncertain feeling in online shopping (Friedrich et al., 2019). Adequate information is indispensable for forming objective and effective evaluation. The lack of attention and evaluation brings much likelihood of online impulsive buying behavior (Drossos et al., 2014; Chan et al., 2017). Although cognitive resources and capacity of consumers are limited, they sometimes show a reluctant attitude to make more efforts on searching information for cognitive processes. This may involve specific classification of products.

Consumers are likely to let off their negative emotions by online shopping behavior. Researchers believed that impulsive buying behavior has become a form of emotional regulation (Fenton-O’Creevy et al., 2018; Sundström et al., 2019). When doing shopping, consumers choose different strategies that change from rational to affective (Peng et al., 2019). Time pressure under online shopping would influence the rational evaluation of consumers to low-involvement products (Zhao et al., 2019). Positive emotions like pleasure could increase purchase intention (Wakefield and Baker, 1998). Lin and Liu (2019) found that web pages could increase the online impulsive buying intention by color display since chromatic web page color displays lead to more aroused and stronger positive emotions. Writing reviews with emotional contents would increase impulsivity (Motyka et al., 2018). Negative emotions like regret also have an impact on online impulsive buying behavior. Izah and Iskandar (2019) proposed that the relationship between online impulsive buying and post-purchase regret is direct. During online shopping, impulsive consumers may also wonder necessity of this deal. No one can assure consumers the best time to buy something. Consumers may be afraid of possible regret from emotionally unplanned online shopping.

Product Involvement and Online Impulsive Buying Behavior

Product involvement is a cognitive factor that affects the decision-making behavior of consumers. Zaichkowsky (1994) believed that involvement is the perceived relevance of an individual to internal needs and interests. High involvement means high product-personal relevance (Greenwald and Leavitt, 1984). Product involvement is subjective. Product value perceived by an individual, category of product, and correlation between an individual and a product affect the level of product involvement (Jones et al., 2003; Hong, 2015; Han and Kim, 2017). The subjective perception of consumers to products is crucial to product involvement. People under high product involvement would process information through the central route. People under low product involvement tend to process information through the peripheral route (Petty and Cacioppo, 1984). The affective part of product involvement presses on affective motive. The cognitive part of product involvement makes consumers focus on the utilitarian value of products (Drossos et al., 2014; Chan et al., 2017). Findings supported the relationship between high product involvement and positive emotional associations (Jaeger et al., 2018). High product involvement presses on the formation of affection and consumers would think before feeling. Low product involvement represents an affective need that influences cognition and consumers would feel before thinking (Belanche et al., 2017; Han and Kim, 2017; Verhagen and Bloemers, 2018). Low product involvement brings more online impulsive buying behavior, and high product involvement guides consumers to shop thoughtfully (Lloyd, 2014; Habib et al., 2021). Research could pay more attention to the relationship between product involvement and online impulsive buying behavior from an affective and rational perspective.

Product involvement affects the cognition and behavior of consumers. Product involvement contains affective and cognitive dimensions. The affective dimension of product involvement describes the feelings of consumers on the product. The cognitive dimension of product involvement describes information processing methods and the knowledge on products (Sandhe, 2020). Product involvement and cognitive ability have been proved to be related (Laaksonen, 1994; Marshall and Bell, 2004; Hong, 2015; Liu et al., 2020). High product involvement is accompanied by higher cognitive levels (Saqib et al., 2010). Impulsive buying is the cognitive response of consumers (Xiang et al., 2016; Kamboj et al., 2018; Vazquez et al., 2020). The cognitive dimension of product involvement and impulsiveness has a direct relationship with purchase intention (Drossos et al., 2014). Consumers will suffer from mistake shopping on high-involvement products since these products are important and expensive, but consequences from wrong shopping on low-involvement products are not unbearable (Jiang et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2020). When consumers are aware of the importance of goods, they will spend more time evaluating goods, and the possibility of online impulsive buying behavior thus decreases. Under low product involvement, the product is not highly relevant to consumers. Cognitive resources invested in collecting information will decrease accordingly. Consumers under low product involvement are more susceptible to marketing stimuli and are more likely to engage in online impulsive buying behavior.

H1. Product involvement has a significant and negative impact on online impulsive buying behavior, and participants do more online impulsive buying behavior when presented with low product involvement than with high product involvement.

Anticipated Regret as a Moderator

Anticipated regret refers to the anxiety caused by the individual worrying about possible loss before making a decision, which can cause hesitation and doubt (Ritov and Baron, 1995). Counterfactual thinking before decision-making can lead to anticipated regrets. Conditional propositions like “what if” or “if only” are typical conceptualized expressions of counterfactual thoughts, which contain both an antecedent and a consequent (Roese, 1994). Directions of counterfactual thoughts tell the difference between alternatives and what happened. Counterfactual thoughts describe alternatives better than what happened, known as upward counterfactual thoughts; counterfactual thoughts describe what happened better than alternatives, known as downward counterfactual thoughts (Roese, 1994; Markman and McMullen, 2003; Epstude and Roese, 2008). Sandberg et al. (2016) explained anticipated regret stems from action regret for the commission of a behavior or inaction regret for the omission of a behavior. Participants who took advices felt more anticipated regret than participants who ignored them (Tzini and Jain, 2018).

Regret related more to cognitive consequence than merely reaction to stimuli (Zeelenberg and Pieters, 2007). Anticipated regret is a cognitive expectation about emotion and an emotionally inert (Robinson and Clore, 2002; Chun et al., 2019). Anticipated regret comes from personal assumption, but not the experience and reaction from anticipated regret is actually a virtual emotion (Chun et al., 2019). Research found that people under anticipated regret would make their decision more prudently (Hamilton et al., 2017; Verkijika, 2018; Ahn and Kahlor, 2020). Result from Hayashi et al. (2019) supported that anticipated regret is one of the antecedents of impulsive decision-making.

Emotion is an important antecedent that affects decision-making behavior. People tend to regret and they will try hard to prevent future regrets and avoid current regrets (Zeelenberg et al., 2006). People can use counterfactual thinking to anticipate the emotional consequences of imagined decision-making. Anticipated regrets in different directions will have different effects on the online impulsive buying behavior of consumers. When the direction of anticipated regret is upward, consumers believe that the price of the product will reduce and the current buying is a loss. People will abandon online impulsive buying behavior to avoid regret caused by the loss. When the direction of anticipated regret is downward, consumers believe that the future price of the product will be higher. They will feel regret if they miss the current price. At this time, the possibility of online impulsive buying behavior increases.

H2. The direction of anticipated regret has a significant and negative impact on online impulsive buying behavior, for participants who experienced downward anticipated regret showing more online impulsive buying behavior than those who experienced upward anticipated regret.

Anticipated regret relates to the cognitive process of online impulsive buying behavior. Anticipated regret not only assumes emotionally driven function but also conveys information to consumers and affects their cognitive style. This is not rejected by product involvement. The relationship between the chain of cognition and emotion and online impulsive buying behavior exists. Danish Habib and Qayyum (2018) found that low perceived risk and high perceived trust enhance the positive emotions of consumers when shopping online. Consumers with high positive emotions will spend more time browsing shopping websites. Online impulsive buying behavior will increase as a result. The model of Baumeister et al. (2007) supports the function of emotion to guide behavior through cognitive processes. In the formation process of online impulsive buying behavior, anticipated regret can directly drive emotions, and it can also act on the cognitive process to adjust the relationship between product involvement and online impulsive buying behavior.

H3. The direction of anticipated regret moderates the relationship between product involvement and online impulsive buying behavior.

Methodology

Sample

A total of 188 Chinese volunteers were recruited from a university in Wuhan city, China, and were randomly assigned to the four treatment groups. Researchers recruited volunteers, and bonus prizes were offered for participants. At first, participants were required to report their online shopping experience. This is the inclusion criterion on participants. Participants without online shopping experience and participants without complete response were excluded. The effective number of participants was 163 (46% male). The average age of the subjects was 21.07 + 2.07. This research also required participants to report basic information related to their online shopping experience.

In addition to the following measures, gender, age, length of experience on online shopping, and frequency of online shopping were controlled. Over 90% of participants reported their monthly income as less than 1,500 yuan. More than half of the participants had been shopping online for 1–3 years. Most participants would shop online 1–2 times a month. To better understand the mechanism of online impulsive buying behavior, we also included online shopping attitude and impulsive buying trait as control variables.

Measures

Under the guidance of researchers, participants reported their basic information on online shopping experience first. After the manipulation test on product involvement, online shopping attitude and impulsive buying trait were tested. Then, participants were asked to finish the simulated scenario task. The whole process of the experiment was provided at an online platform called Wenjuanxing.

Simulated Scenario

The scenario task was revised from the former version (Rook and Fisher, 1995; Hetts et al., 2000). The revised simulated scenario task was based on shopping habit and actual expenditure of Chinese college students. At first, we designed different conditions for high product involvement (laptop) versus low product involvement (camera) based on the pretest. At the beginning of experiment, participants were invited to read the description on consumption decision of a college student. The product planned to buy online was a portable hard drive for school, and they found another product (camera vs. laptop) at a discount, which they yearn for a long time but didn’t plan to buy now. Product involvement was manipulated. Under high product involvement condition, the unplanned product was a laptop. Under low product involvement condition, the unplanned product was a camera. Under the situation of upward anticipated regret, participants were asked to imagine that the college student bought a laptop at discounted price and found the shopping website provided a lower price a week later. Participants were asked to think for a minute about how regret they were for buying the product. Under the situation of downward anticipated regret, participants were asked to imagine that the college student decided not to buy the camera and found it back to the original price. No other shopping websites provided lower price for the camera. After that, a chance of re-choosing was provided for participants. Participants were asked to think for a minute about how report for not buying the product and their intention on online impulsive buying.

Product Involvement

Personal Involvement Inventory (Zaichkowsky, 1994) was used to measure the product involvement of participants. The items of product involvement are as follows: important-unimportant, relevant-irrelevant, means nothing-means a lot to me, worthless-valuable, involving-uninvolving, and not needed-needed. Participants responded on a 7-point Likert scale with a higher score representing a higher level of product involvement. Cronbach’s α for pretest was 0.92.

Laptop and camera were chosen for manipulation on product involvement. An online pretest was conducted to measure the product involvement of laptop, cell phone, and camera, which were alternative material; 45 participants (55.6% male) were invited to an online pretest. Personal product inventory of Zaichkowsky (1994) was used. There was a significant difference among product involvement of laptop, cell phone, and camera, F(88, 2) = 37.56, p < 0.001, SS = 6,008.95, MS = 3,004.47, Mmobile = 54.40, Mcamera = 44.58, Mlaptop = 60.80. In pretest, the α for cell phone, camera, and laptop were 0.88, 0.97, and 0.92, respectively.

Impulsive Buying Trait

Impulsive Buying Trait Inventory (Zhang, 2010) was performed using the 7-point semantic differential scale. Twelve items were included in this scale, such as “When shopping, I like to buy it first and don’t care if I have enough money” and “As long as you like it, you should buy it immediately.” Items were scored on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree. Cronbach’s α was 0.83.

Online Shopping Attitude

The online shopping attitude measure was revised by two pre-validated scales. This scale measured the perceived trust and risk on online shopping. The former three items were used to measure the perceived trust of consumers in online shopping, drawn from Han and Liu (2009), like “I think most shopping websites is trustworthy.” Items were scored on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree. The latter four items were used to measure the perceived risk of consumers in online shopping, drawn from Li (2010), like “I think online shopping has product performance risks (fake, etc.).”Cronbach’s α was 0.75.

Online Impulsive Buying Behavior

Online impulsive buying behavior was measured by a single item. Participants were asked to re-choose after simulated scenario material. Under the situation of re-choosing, participants could decide whether to buy and which to buy.

Results

Descriptive Statistics and Correlations

The correlation matrix is reported in Table 1. Monthly income of participants (p < 0.05), impulsive buying trait (p < 0.01), and anticipated regret (p < 0.01) were significantly correlated with online impulsive buying behavior.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The correlation matrix.

Hypothesis Testing

Common Method Bias

A Harman single-factor test (Podsakoff and Organ, 1986) was conducted and found that the explained variance of the first principal component was 24.85%. Being below the cut-off value of 50% (Podsakoff et al., 2003), we concluded that common method bias was not a serious problem.

Manipulation Check on Product Category

Participants in the laptop condition reported higher involvement than those in the camera condition, t(161) = –3.76, p < 0.0001, Mcamera = 4.58, Mlaptop = 5.32. The results confirmed that the manipulation of a product category was successful.

To test the hypothesis, we conducted a 2 (product involvement: high vs. low) × 2 (anticipated regret: upward vs. downward) ANOVA on online impulsive buying behavior while controlling for gender, age, and income. The analysis revealed that the main effect of product involvement on online impulsive buying behavior was significant, and participants with low product involvement (M = 2.98) showed more online impulsive buying behavior than participants with high product involvement (M = 2.65), F(1, 163) = 4.64, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.03. Thus, H1 was supported.

The result also showed that the main effect of anticipated regret on online impulsive buying behavior was significant, and participants who experienced downward anticipated regret (M = 3.13) showed more online impulsive buying behavior than those experienced upward anticipated regret (M = 2.48), F(1, 163) = 12.57, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.08. Therefore, H2 was supported.

More important, the interaction effect between product involvement and anticipated regret on online impulsive buying behavior was significant, F(1, 163) = 4.11, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.02. Under the condition of high product involvement, participants who experienced downward anticipated regret (M = 2.81) showed a non-significant difference in online impulsive buying behavior compared with participants who experienced upward anticipated regret (M = 2.50), F(1, 163) = 1.27, df = 1, p > 0.05. In the low product involvement condition, those who experienced downward anticipated regret (M = 3.46) acted significantly more online impulsive buying behavior than those who experienced upward anticipated regret (M = 2.46), F (1,163) = 13.25, df = 1, p < 0.01 (see Figure 1). Thus, H3 was supported.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Moderating effect of anticipated regert. *p < 0.05. ns = Non-significant.

Discussion

This study investigated the interaction between anticipated regret and product involvement on online impulsive buying behavior. The main effect of product involvement and anticipated regret was supported. Anticipated regret significantly moderated the relationship between anticipated regret and product involvement.

Different product involvements have significantly different effects on online impulsive buying behavior. In the online shopping environment, product involvement affects online impulse shopping behaviors through the perception of consumers of products. Under the condition of higher product involvement, consumers are more willing to spend time and energy to collect information and evaluate products. While under the condition of lower product involvement, consumers lack motivation to engage information collection and product evaluation, and thus, their understanding of products is one-sided.

Consumers always pursue maximized utility in sales. In buying and selling transactions, merchants use product advantages to gain more, while consumers use product defects to pay less (Saqib et al., 2010). Online shopping magnifies this phenomenon. If consumers feel high perceived value, they will obtain much information about target products through the Internet. Thus, it is common that consumers abandon online impulsive buying behavior after receiving a bad evaluation on the product. Consumers with lower product involvement pay less cognitive effort to evaluate products. At this time, they may decide to shop immediately. Conclusions from this study provide supporting evidence for the relationship between product involvement and online impulsive buying behavior (Liang, 2012).

Product involvement reveals an emotional aspect in online impulsive buying behavior. Product involvement means engaged emotion (Gu et al., 2012). Kahneman (2011) asserted that emotion makes more impact on the decision than cognition. Low product involvement urges people make decision through affect heuristic, which means consumers would rely more on intuition and make decision more emotionally (Kahneman, 2011). When they do with low-involvement product, they spend less time or energy on searching information, and thus, they prefer to pay less for low-involvement product (Traylor, 1981; Ghasemaghaei and Hassanein, 2015). Similarly, consumers would take more factors into consideration for high involvement product (Stephen and Galak, 2012; Li, 2020). However, people may unexpectedly fail in online impulsive buying under low product involvement. People would persuade themselves to accept the shoddy product to avoid cognitive dissonance (Saqib et al., 2010). Time and energy people spend on information search largely might help mitigate potential risks they may suffer from. Objective and sufficient information is the basis of cognitive decision, which also influences the emotional aspect.

The relationship between anticipated regret directions and online impulsive buying behavior reflects regret aversion. When anticipated regret direction is downward, the current choice is better than the future plan. Consumers expect that choosing future plan will bring regret. To avoid future regret, consumers are more likely to choose current one and engage online impulsive buying behavior. The important influence of downward anticipated regret on online impulsive buying behavior has been supported. Positive expectations on future results can enlarge the possibility of online impulsive buying (Li et al., 2019). When the direction of anticipated regret is upward, the future choice is better than the current choice. Consumers under this situation are likely to choose future choice. In other words, upward anticipated regret makes it easier for consumers to give up online impulsive buying behavior.

This article is consistent with cognitive function of emotional factors. Results support the interaction between anticipated regret direction and product involvement. Previous studies have shown the combined effect of cognition and emotion on online impulsive buying behavior (Danish Habib and Qayyum, 2018; Fenton-O’Creevy et al., 2018). Choosing one from two always remind consumers of possibly consequent regret. Inaction inertia could help people avoid such regret (Tykocinski and Pittman, 1998; van Putten et al., 2013a,b).

The second alternative would remind people of the missed alternative. The fact on missed alternative causes people feel regret. Since anticipated regret exists, no matter how large attractiveness the second alternatives have, people still choose to omit it. People show reluctance to the second chance when they have missed the first chance in the same action domain, and this is inaction inertia (Tykocinski et al., 1995; Tykocinski and Ortmann, 2011). Researchers suggested that regret is an effective predictor of inaction inertia (Arkes et al., 2002; Sevdalis et al., 2006; van Putten et al., 2013b). Difference in attractiveness between two alternatives is an important condition of inaction inertia (Tykocinski et al., 2004; Zeelenberg et al., 2006). Just as Tykocinski et al. (1995) have proved in their experiment, the larger the difference in the attractiveness of the two chances exists, the larger the possibility of inaction inertia is.

Inaction inertia tends to happen in situations with anticipated regret (Butler and Highhouse, 2000; Tykocinski and Pittman, 2001). Upward anticipated regret in this research created a missed subsequent chance with larger attractiveness. In the initial situation, participants were assumed to have missed the chance to buy laptop or camera at a lower price. Upward anticipated regret comes from the counterfactual thinking on buying at discounted price or a better discounted price. When participants were asked to choose again, results show that they did not show clear preference on online impulsive buying. The reported indicator of online impulsive buying behavior is close to not buying; in other words, it is inaction. This inaction could be found in both high product involvement and low product involvement under upward anticipated regret. Missed subsequent attractive deals make people feel regret and keep inaction when faced with the second chance (van Putten et al., 2013a; Chen et al., 2021). Liu and Chou (2019) further discussed the performance of inaction inertia under different promotion strategies.

Downward anticipated regret comes from the comparison between missed alternative and inferior alternatives. In this study, participants did not take the first chance to have online impulsive buying on both camera and laptop. The situation under upward anticipated regret is that the latter alternative is better; condition under downward anticipated regret is that the current alternative is better (McConnell et al., 2000). Downward anticipated regret in this research comes from missed an attractive alternative. Participants under high product involvement are likely to take second alternative to have online impulsive buying behavior. The high product involvement means more cognitive effort and objective evaluation. Downward anticipated regret reminded participants of the current chance but evaluated potential risk stressed careful action (Dholakia, 2001; Sandhe, 2020). On the contrary, participants under low product involvement show a clear tend to have online impulsive buying behavior. Partially due to less engagement in efforts and spent time, consumers under low product involvement may care less about the risk of mispurchase (Kim, 2005). Trivial attributes could help mitigate inaction inertia in some extent, and products with low product involvement are probably considered a trivial product since people attach little importance to them (Kumar, 2019).

This study sheds light on existing literature on cognitive aspect and emotional aspect of online impulsive buying behavior. Discussion on cognition and emotion has been for decades (Gray, 1990; Clore and Palmer, 2009; Kahneman, 2011), and perspective combining emotion and cognition has been developed (Hasking et al., 2017; Raschle et al., 2017). This study provides more supporting evidence for the interaction between cognition and emotion. Product involvement involves activeness of cognitive resource, thus playing a cognitive part in online impulsive buying behavior (Sohn and Lee, 2017). Subsequence between cognitive aspect and emotional aspect in online impulsive buying behavior helps better understand the mental mechanism of consumers. Result from Danish Habib and Qayyum (2018) supported that the cognitive aspect could lead toward the emotion aspect in online impulsive buying behavior. Anticipated regret urges people to reconsider their decision rationally based on experience (Zeelenberg, 1999). Alternatively, cognition triggers emotional changes as well. Low product involvement is likely to stimulate impulsive emotions. People under high product involvement states tend to collect information actively, which can ease impulsive emotions. It can be seen that the two-way chain of cognition and emotion is particularly important in online impulsive buying behavior.

Second, the study highlighted the general application of regret theory. As negative emotion, people always try to avoid experiencing regret in their decision (Mourali et al., 2018). Online shopping is full of discount activities. Discounted products can easily trigger anticipated regret and a hotbed of inaction inertia (van Putten et al., 2013a; Chen et al., 2021). Inaction inertia helps better understand how online impulsive buying consumers react to product involvement and anticipated regret under irregular discounts. Moreover, based on the former research, this study further proposed the relationship between the direction of anticipated regret and inaction inertia, which enriches the current theoretical mechanism of inaction inertia (Sevdalis et al., 2006; Su et al., 2013).

Consumers should actively collect product information and take advantage of regret. Online sellers often use low-price gimmicks to attract consumers to focus entirely on the low prices of goods. Online impulsive buying behavior thus happens. Consumers are supposed to search more information about products and remind themselves of anticipated regret to mitigate impulsiveness. This can help reduce unnecessary online impulsive buying behavior. Since low involvement product is likely to attract the online impulsive buying behavior of the consumer, corporation related to such product should try to promote quality to reach higher consumer satisfaction. This would bring more returned customers to corporation. Meanwhile, government could provide technical guidance for corporations to provide low-involvement product. More supervision on corporation is needed for pushing more rational online shopping rather than impulsive online buying.

Limitations and Future Research

The limitations of this study are as follows. First, this study only used scales and text descriptions to simulate online impulsive buying scenarios. The actual online shopping environment is different from this. Offering a simulation environment only by text description is an insufficient measure. Simulated materials used in this study were revised based on actual online shopping experience and consumption preference of the target sample. And the results also showed that no serious concern on common method bias. A deliberate behavioral lab could have helped this study receive better response, for example, a simulated shopping website. Future research should create more life-like simulation environment for better observing online impulsive buying behavior. Second, results based on student sample are limited. Student sample is one of the limitations of this study. Consumers engaged in online impulsive buying are available over all age groups and all professions. This study controlled age and monthly income to reduce unexpected influence. Future study should enlarge a range of sample rather than only focusing on specific group. Future research is supposed to use a larger sample source to expand the scope of application of the conclusions. Third, this study did not take income type into consideration. The privacy of online shopping can promote online impulsive buying behavior (Chih et al., 2012). However, the living expenses of Chinese college students mean “controlled” consumption, which represents the loss of privacy on online impulsive buying behavior. Future research could fully address the relationship between income types and online impulsive buying behavior.

Conclusion

Regret always accompanies with decision in daily life. Anticipated regret helps consumers adjust current decision to avoid possible loss and future regret. This rule also works with different product types. This research is expected to help consumers understand the relationship between emotion and reason. Regret could buffer impulsive feelings in some extent. Consumers are supposed to establish healthy online shopping style by the take better advantage of their emotion.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Jinan University Management School Research Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

BL and XC designed the study. XC and YL collected the data. BL and MH analyzed the data and draft the manuscript. MH, XC, and YL participated in the interpretation of the data. BL, MH, and XC revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by National Natural Science Fund of China (NSFC) (Grant Numbers: 71601084 and 71701080), Foundation of Institute for Enterprise Development, Jinan University, Guangdong Province (Grant Number: 2021MYZD01), Jinan University Management School Funding Program (Grant Number: GY21011), and Foundation of Research Institute on Brand Innovation and Development of Guangzhou (2021CR03).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.732459/full#supplementary-material

References

Ahn, J., and Kahlor, L. A. (2020). No regrets when it comes to your health: anticipated regret, subjective norms, information insufficiency and intent to seek health information from multiple sources. Health Commun. 35, 1295–1302. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2019.1626535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arkes, H. R., Kung, Y. H., and Hutzel, L. (2002). Regret, valuation, and inaction inertia. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 87, 371–385. doi: 10.1006/obhd.2001.2978

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., DeWall, C. N., and Zhang, L. (2007). How emotion shapes behavior: feedback, anticipation, and reflection, rather than direct causation. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 11, 167–203. doi: 10.1177/1088868307301033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belanche, D., Flavián, C., and Pérez-Rueda, A. (2017). Understanding interactive online advertising: congruence and product involvement in highly and lowly arousing, skippable video ads. J. Interactive Market. 37, 75–88.

Google Scholar

Bilal Ahmad, M., Fawad Ali, H., Sabir Malik, M., Humayun, A. A., and Ahmad, S. (2019). Factors affecting impulsive buying behavior with mediating role of positive mood: an empirical study. Eur. Online J. Nat. Soc. Sci. 8, 17–35.

Google Scholar

Butler, A., and Highhouse, S. (2000). Deciding to sell: the effect of prior inaction and offer source. J. Econ. Psychol. 21, 223–232. doi: 10.1016/s0167-4870(00)00002-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, T. K. H., Cheung, C. M. K., and Lee, Z. W. Y. (2017). The state of online impulse-buying research: a literature analysis. Inform. Manag. 54, 204–217.

Google Scholar

Chang, Y. P., Xiao, W. F., Qin, W., and Yan, J. (2012). The influence mechanism of third-party product review (TRPs) on impulsive buying intention within the internet environment: by product category and commentators rank for regulation variables [in Chinese]. Acta Psychol. Sin. 44, 1244–1264.

Google Scholar

Changa, H. J., Eckmanb, M., and Yanb, R. N. (2011). Application of the stimulus-organism-response model to the retail environment: the role of hedonic motivation in impulse buying behavior. Int. Rev. Retail Distrib. Consum. Res. 21, 233–249. doi: 10.1080/09593969.2011.578798

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, C. D., and Ku, E. C. S. (2021). Diversified online oeview websites as accelerators for online impulsive buying: the moderating effect of price dispersion. J. Internet Commerce 20, 113–135. doi: 10.1080/15332861.2020.1868227

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J., Hui, L. S., Yu, T., Feldman, G., Zeng, S., Ching, T. L., et al. (2021). Foregone opportunities and choosing not to act: replications of inaction inertia effect. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 12, 333–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0528.1997.tb00908.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, W. K., Lin, Y. L., Pan, H. S., and Chen, C. K. (2020). “Research on online impulsive buying and post-purchase dissonance,” in Proceedings 2020 IEEE 21st International Conference on Information Reuse and Integration for Data Science, IRI 2020, (Piscataway, NJ: IEEE).

Google Scholar

Chih, W., Wu, C. H., and Li, H. (2012). The antecedents of consumer online buying impulsiveness on a travel website: individual Internal factor perspectives. J. Travel Market. 29, 430–443.

Google Scholar

Chun, H. H., Park, J., and Thomas, M. (2019). Cold anticipated regret versus hot experienced regret: why consumers fail to regret unhealthy consumption. J. Assoc. Consum. Res. 4, 125–135.

Google Scholar

Clore, G. L., and Palmer, J. (2009). Affective guidance of intelligent agents: how emotion controls cognition. Cogn. Syst. Res. 10, 21–30. doi: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2008.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danish Habib, M., and Qayyum, A. (2018). Cognitive emotion theory and emotion-action tendency in online impulsive buying behavior. J. Manag. Sci. 5, 86–99.

Google Scholar

Davis, D. (2020). Amazon’s North America Sales Surge 29% in Q1 as Coronavirus Drives Demand. Digital Commerce 360. Available online at: https://www.digitalcommerce360.com/article/amazon-sales/ (accessed October 10, 2020).

Google Scholar

Dholakia, U. M. (2001). A motivational process model of product involvement and consumer risk perception. Eur. J. Mark. 35, 1340–1362.

Google Scholar

Dodoo, N. A., and Wu, L. (2019). Exploring the anteceding impact of personalised social media advertising on online impulse buying tendency. Int. J. Internet Mark. Adv. 13, 73–95. doi: 10.1504/ijima.2019.10019167

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drossos, D. A., Kokkinaki, F., Giaglis, G. M., and Fouskas, K. G. (2014). The effects of product involvement and impulse buying on purchase intentions in mobile text advertising. Electron. Commer. Res. Appl. 13, 423–430. doi: 10.1016/j.elerap.2014.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epstude, K., and Roese, N. J. (2008). The functional theory of counterfactual thinking. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 12, 168–192.

Google Scholar

Fenton-O’Creevy, M., Dibb, S., and Furnham, A. (2018). Antecedents and consequences of chronic impulsive buying: can impulsive buying be understood as dysfunctional self-regulation? Psychol. Mark. 35, 175–188. doi: 10.1002/mar.21078

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedrich, T., Schlauderer, S., and Overhage, S. (2019). The impact of social commerce feature richness on website stickiness through cognitive and affective factors: an experimental study. Electron. Commer. Res. Appl. 36:100861. doi: 10.1016/j.elerap.2019.100861

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghasemaghaei, M., and Hassanein, K. (2015). Online information quality and consumer satisfaction: the moderating roles of contextual factors –a meta-analysis. Inform. Manag. 52, 965–981. doi: 10.1016/j.im.2015.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, J. A. (1990). Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition. Cogn. Emot. 4, 269–288. doi: 10.1080/02699939008410799

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., and Leavitt, C. (1984). Audience involvement in advertising: four levels. J. Consum. Res. 11, 581–592. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(99)00062-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gu, B., Park, J., and Konana, P. (2012). Research note—the impact of external word-of-mouth sources on retailer sales of high-involvement products. Inform. Syst. Res. 23, 182–196. doi: 10.1287/isre.1100.0343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, J., Xin, L., and Wu, Y. (2017). “Arousal or not? the effects of scarcity messages on online impulsive purchase,” in Proceedings of the International Conference on HCI in Business, Government, and Organizations, (Cham: Springer).

Google Scholar

Habib, S., Hamadneh, N. N., and Alsubie, A. (2021). Modeling advertising practices for product involvement and consumer impulsivity in branded apparel: a case study of Indian consumers. Sustainability 13, 1–14.

Google Scholar

Hamilton, K., Kirkpatrick, A., Rebar, A., White, K. M., and Hagger, M. S. (2017). Protecting young children against skin cancer: parental beliefs, roles, and regret. Psychooncology 26, 2135–2141. doi: 10.1002/pon.4434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, H., and Liu, X. W. (2009). Research on the relationship between trust, perceived risk and online shopping intention [in Chinese]. Econ. Forum 16, 94–97. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00890

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, M. C., and Kim, Y. (2017). Why consumers hesitate to shop online: perceived risk and product involvement on taobao.com. J. Promotion Manag. 23, 24–44. doi: 10.1080/10496491.2016.1251530

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hashmi, H., Attiq, S., and Rasheed, F. (2019). Factors affecting online impulsive buying behavior: a stimulus organism response model approach. Mark. Forces 14, 19–42.

Google Scholar

Hasking, P., Whitlock, J., Voon, D., and Rose, A. (2017). A cognitive-emotional model of NSSI: using emotion regulation and cognitive processes to explain why people self-injure. Cogn. Emot. 31, 1543–1556. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1241219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayashi, Y., Foreman, A. M., Friedel, J. E., and Wirth, O. (2019). Threat appeals reduce impulsive decision making associated with texting while driving: a behavioral economic approach. PLoS One 14:e0213453. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0213453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hetts, J. J., Boninger, D. S., Armor, D. A., Gleicher, F., and Nathanson, A. (2000). The influence of anticipated counterfactual regret on behavior. Psychol. Mark. 17, 345–368. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1520-6793(200004)17:4<345::aid-mar5>3.0.co;2-m

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, I. B. (2015). Understanding the consumer’s online merchant selection process: the roles of product involvement, perceived risk, and trust expectation. Int. J. Inform. Manag. 35, 322–336. doi: 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2015.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Internet Retailer (2019). 2019 Online Marketplaces Report. Available online at: https://www.channeladvisor.com/resources/library-webinars/internetretailer-2019-online-marketplaces-report/?show_confirmation=1 (accessed October 10, 2020).

Google Scholar

Izah, V. N., and Iskandar, B. P. (2019). “I see it, I buy it, I regret it: a study of online post-purchase regret phenomena of fashion products,” in Paper Presented at the International Conference on Management in Emerging Markets, (Indonesia).

Google Scholar

Izogo, E. E., and Jayawardhena, C. (2018). Online shopping experience in an emerging e-retailing market: towards a conceptual model. J. Consumer Behav. 17, 379–392. doi: 10.1002/cb.1715

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaeger, S. R., Lee, P. Y., and Ares, G. (2018). Product involvement and consumer food-elicited emotional associations: insights from emoji questionnaires. Food Res. Int. 106, 999–1011. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2018.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, C., Duan, R., Jain, H. K., Liu, S., and Liang, K. (2015). Hybrid collaborative filtering for high-involvement products: a solution to opinion sparsity and dynamics. Decis. Support Syst. 79, 195–208. doi: 10.1016/j.dss.2015.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, M. A., Reynolds, K. E., Weun, S., and Beatty, S. E. (2003). The product-specific nature of impulse buying tendency. J. Bus. Res. 56, 505–511. doi: 10.1016/s0148-2963(01)00250-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking Fast and Slow. London: Penguin.

Google Scholar

Kamboj, S., Sarmah, B., Gupta, S., and Dwivedi, Y. (2018). Examining branding co-creation in brand communities on social media: applying the paradigm of stimulus-organism-response. Int. J. Inform. Manag. 39, 169–185. doi: 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2017.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H. S. (2005). Consumer profiles of apparel product involvement and values. J. Fashion Mark. Manag. Int. J. 9, 207–220. doi: 10.1108/13612020510599358

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, P. (2019). The impact of trivial attributes on inaction inertia. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 25, 733–743. doi: 10.1037/xap0000229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laaksonen, P. (1994). Consumer Involvement: Concepts Ad Research. London, UK: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Li, J. (2010). Research on gender differences in perceived risk of online shopping [in Chinese]. Econ. Forum 6, 134–137. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2021.635859

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J., Yang, R., Cui, J., and Guo, Y. (2019). Imagination matters when you shop online: the moderating role of mental simulation between materialism and online impulsive buying. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 12, 1071–1079. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S227403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y. (2020). Consumers’ perceived usefulness of online reviews: effects of emotional certainty and product involvement. Soc. Behav. Pers. 47, 1–16. doi: 10.2224/sbp.8403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, Y.-P. (2012). The relationship between consumer product involvement, product knowledge and impulsive buying behavior. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 57, 325–330. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.09.1193

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liao, C., To, P., Wong, Y., Palvia, P., and Kakhki, M. D. (2016). The impact of presentation mode and product type on online impulsive buying decisions. J. Electron. Commer. Res. 17, 153–168.

Google Scholar

Lim, P. L., and Rashad, Y. (2015). What Internal and External Factors Influence Impulsive Buying Behavior in Online Shopping? Type: Double Blind Peer Reviewed International Researc. USA: Global Journals Inc.

Google Scholar

Lin, C., and Liu, T. (2019). The webmosphirics effects on shopping behavior: the influences of web page color displays on online impulse purchasing intention. J. Bus. Econ. 10, 24–32. doi: 10.15341/jbe(2155-7950)/01.10.2019/003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, H. H., and Chou, H. Y. (2019). The effects of promotional package frames and price strategies on inaction inertia. Psychol. Mark. 36, 214–228. doi: 10.1002/mar.21173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Q., Zhang, X., Huang, S., Zhang, L., and Zhao, Y. (2020). Exploring consumers’ buying behavior in a large online promotion activity: the role of psychological distance and involvement. J. Theor. Appl. Electron. Commerce Res. 15, 66–80.

Google Scholar

Lloyd, N. (2014). Exploring the role of product involvement in shaping impulsive buying tendencies in online retail environments. J. Promot. Commun. 2, 87–112.

Google Scholar

Madhavaram, S. R., and Laverie, D. A. (2004). Exploring impulse purchasing on the internet. Adv. Consum. Res. 31, 59–66.

Google Scholar

Markman, K. D., and McMullen, M. N. (2003). A reflection and evaluation model of comparative thinking. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 7, 244–267. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0703_04

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, D., and Bell, R. (2004). Relating the food involvement scale to demographic variables, food choice and other constructs. Food Qual. Prefer. 15, 871–879. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2004.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McConnell, A. R., Niedermeier, K. E., Leibold, J. M., El-Alayli, A. G., Chin, P. P., and Kuiper, N. M. (2000). What if I find it cheaper someplace else?: role of prefactual thinking and anticipated regret in consumer behavior. Psychol. Mark. 17, 281–298. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1520-6793(200004)17:4<281::aid-mar2>3.0.co;2-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moran, B., and Kwak, L. (2015). Effect of stress, materialism and external stimuli on online impulse buying. J. Res. Consum. 27, 26–51.

Google Scholar

Motyka, S., Grewal, D., Aguirre, E., Mahr, D., de Ruyter, K., and Wetzels, M. (2018). The emotional review-reward effect: how do reviews increase impulsivity? J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 46, 1032–1051. doi: 10.1007/s11747-018-0585-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mourali, M., Yang, Z., Pons, F., and Hassay, D. (2018). Consumer power and choice deferral: the role of anticipated regret. Int. J. Res. Mark. 35, 81–99. doi: 10.1016/j.ijresmar.2017.09.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nussbaum, M. C. (2003). Upheavals of Thought: the Intelligence of Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Parboteeah, D. V., Valacich, J. S., and Wells, J. D. (2009). The influence of website characteristics on a consumer’s urge to buy impulsively. Inform. Syst. Res. 20, 60–78. doi: 10.1287/isre.1070.0157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, L., Zhang, W., Wang, X., and Liang, S. (2019). Moderating effects of time pressure on the relationship between perceived value and purchase intention in social e-commerce sales promotion: considering the impact of product involvement. Inform. Manag. 56, 317–328. doi: 10.1016/j.im.2018.11.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petty, R. E., and Cacioppo, J. T. (1984). The effects of involvement on responses to argument quantity and quality: central and peripheral routes to persuasion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 46, 69–81. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.46.1.69

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Podsakoff, P. M., and Organ, D. W. (1986). Self-reports in organizational research: problems and prospects. J. Manag. 12, 531–544. doi: 10.1177/014920638601200408

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raschle, N. M., Fehlbaum, L. V., Menks, W. M., Euler, F., Sterzer, P., and Stadler, C. (2017). Investigating the neural correlates of emotion–cognition interaction using an affective stroop task. Front. Psychol. 8:1489. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ritov, I., and Baron, J. (1995). Outcome knowledge, regret, and omission bias. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 64, 119–127. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1995.1094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, M. D., and Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: evidence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychol. Bull. 128, 934–960. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roese, N. J. (1994). The functional basis of counterfactual thinking. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 66, 805–818. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.805

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rook, D. W. (1987). The buying impulse. J. Consum. Res. 14, 189–199.

Google Scholar

Rook, D. W., and Fisher, R. J. (1995). Normative influences on impulsive buying behavior. J. Consum. Res. 22, 305–313. doi: 10.1086/209452

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandberg, T., Hutter, R., Richetin, J., and Conner, M. (2016). Testing the role of action and inaction anticipated regret on intentions and behaviour. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 55, 407–425. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandhe, A. A. (2020). Testing and validating a tool to measure product involvement from its antecedents. ITEGAM-JETIA 6, 27–31.

Google Scholar

Saqib, N. U., Frohlich, N., and Bruning, E. (2010). The influence of involvement on the endowment effect: the moveable value function. J. Consum. Psychol. 20, 355–368. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2010.06.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sevdalis, N., Harvey, N., and Yip, M. (2006). Regret triggers inaction inertia - but which regret and how? Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 45, 839–853. doi: 10.1348/014466605x84790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sohn, H. K., and Lee, T. J. (2017). Tourists’ impulse buying behavior at duty-free shops: the moderating effects of time pressure and shopping involvement. J. Travel Tour. Mark. 34, 341–356. doi: 10.1080/10548408.2016.1170650

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephen, A. T., and Galak, J. (2012). The effects of traditional and social earned media on sales: a study of a microlending marketplace. J. Mark. Res. 49, 624–639. doi: 10.1509/jmr.09.0401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Styvén, M. E., Foster, T., and Wallström, Å. (2017). Impulse buying tendencies among online shoppers in Sweden. J. Res. Interact. Mark. 11, 416–431. doi: 10.1108/JRIM-05-2016-0054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Su, S., Chen, R., and Huang, J. S. (2013). The regulation of inaction inertia: from the forward-looking perspective of multiple reference points [in Chinese]. Acta Psychol. Sin. 45, 1393–1409. doi: 10.3724/sp.j.1041.2013.01393

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sundström, M., Hjelm-Lidholm, S., and Radon, A. (2019). Clicking the boredom away - exploring impulse fashion buying behavior online. J. Retailing Consum. Serv. 47, 150–156. doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2018.11.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Traylor, M. B. (1981). Product involvement and brand commitment. J. Adv. Res. 21, 51–56.

Google Scholar

Tykocinski, O. E., and Ortmann, A. (2011). The lingering effects of our past experiences: the sunk-cost fallacy and the inaction-inertia effect. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 5, 653–664. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00369.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tykocinski, O. E., and Pittman, T. S. (1998). The consequences of doing nothing: inaction inertia as avoidance of anticipated counterfactual regret. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 75, 607–616. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tykocinski, O. E., and Pittman, T. S. (2001). Product aversion following a missed opportunity: price contrast or avoidance of anticipated regret? Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 23, 149–156.

Google Scholar

Tykocinski, O. E., Pittman, T. S., and Tuttle, E. E. (1995). Inaction inertia: foregoing future benefits as a result of an initial failure to act. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68, 793–803. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.793

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tykocinski, O., Israel, R., and Pittman, T. S. (2004). Inaction inertia in the stock market. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 34, 1166–1175. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb02001.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzini, K., and Jain, K. (2018). The role of anticipated regret in advice taking. J. Behav. Decis. Making 31, 74–86. doi: 10.1002/bdm.2048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Putten, M., Zeelenberg, M., and van Dijk, E. (2013a). How consumers deal with missed discounts: transaction decoupling, action orientation and inaction inertia. J. Econ. Psychol. 38, 104–110.

Google Scholar

van Putten, M., Zeelenberg, M., van Dijk, E., and Tykocinski, O. E. (2013b). Inaction inertia. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 24, 123–159.

Google Scholar

Vazquez, D., Wu, X., Nguyen, B., Kent, A., Gutierrez, A., and Chen, T. (2020). Investigating narrative involvement, parasocial interactions, and impulse buying behaviours within a second screen social commerce context. Int. J. Inform. Manag. 53:102135. doi: 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2020.102135

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhagen, T., and Bloemers, D. (2018). Exploring the cognitive and affective bases of online purchase intentions: a hierarchical test across product types. Electron. Commerce Res. 18, 537–561. doi: 10.1007/s10660-017-9270-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verkijika, S. F. (2018). Understanding smartphone security behaviors: an extension of the protection motivation theory with anticipated regret. Comput. Secur. 77, 860–870. doi: 10.1016/j.cose.2018.03.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wakefield, K. L., and Baker, J. (1998). Excitement at the mall: determinants and effects on shopping response. J. Retail. 74, 515–539. doi: 10.1016/s0022-4359(99)80106-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, L., Chiu, M. L., and Chen, K. W. (2020). Defining the determinants of online impulse buying through a shopping process of integrating perceived risk, expectation-confirmation model, and flow theory issues. Int. J. Inf. Manag. 52:102099. doi: 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2020.102099

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiang, L., Zheng, X., Lee, M. K., and Zhao, D. (2016). Exploring consumers’ impulse buying behavior on social commerce platform: the role of parasocial interaction. Int. J. Inform. Manag. 36, 333–347. doi: 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2015.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, H., Zhang, K. Z., and Zhao, S. J. (2020). A dual systems model of online impulse buying. Ind. Manag. Data Syst. 120, 845–861. doi: 10.1108/IMDS-04-2019-0214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaichkowsky, J. L. (1994). The personal involvement inventory: reduction, revision, and application to advertising. J. Adv. 23, 59–70. doi: 10.1080/00913367.1943.10673459

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeelenberg, M. (1999). The use of crying over spilled milk: a note on the rationality and functionality of regret. Philos. Psychol. 12, 325–340. doi: 10.1080/095150899105800

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeelenberg, M., and Pieters, R. (2007). A theory of regret regulation 1.0. J. Consum. Psychol. 17, 3–18. doi: 10.1207/s15327663jcp1701_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeelenberg, M., Nijstad, B. A., van Putten, M., and van Dijk, E. (2006). Inaction inertia, regret, and valuation: a closer look. Organ. Behav. Hum. Dec. Proces. 101, 89–104. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.11.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J. F. (2010). Research on the Influencing Factors of Potential Consumers’ Online Group Buying Intention and Decision [in Chinese]. Unpublished master’s thesis. China: Central China Normal University.

Google Scholar

Zhang, M., and Zhang, Z. (2015). The influence of word of mouth on impulsive buying under the networked environment [in Chinese]. Soft Sci. 21, 110–114.

Google Scholar

Zhao, Z., Du, X., Liang, F., and Zhu, X. (2019). Effect of product type and time pressure on consumers’ online impulse buying intention. J. Contemp. Mark. Sci. 2, 137–154. doi: 10.1108/JCMARS-01-2019-0012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: anticipated regret, product involvement, online impulsive buying behavior, cognitive aspect, emotional aspect

Citation: Li B, Hu M, Chen X and Lei Y (2021) The Moderating Role of Anticipated Regret and Product Involvement on Online Impulsive Buying Behavior. Front. Psychol. 12:732459. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.732459

Received: 29 June 2021; Accepted: 28 October 2021;
Published: 17 December 2021.

Edited by:

Jie Li, Xi’an Jiaotong-Liverpool University, China

Reviewed by:

Anshul Verma, S. P. Jain Institute of Management and Research (SPJIMR), India
Amit Kumar Srivastava, Indian Institute of Technology Roorkee, India

Copyright © 2021 Li, Hu, Chen and Lei. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Bin Li, bingoli@jnu.edu.cn; Xiaoxi Chen, txiaoxi@jnu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.