Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 28 October 2021
Sec. Eating Behavior

Translation and Validation of a State-Measure of Body Image Satisfaction: The Body Image State Scale

\r\nLuc Bardi,*Luc Bardi1,2*Claire ArnaudClaire Arnaud1Cline BagsCéline Bagès1Frdric LangloisFrédéric Langlois2Amelie Rousseau,Amelie Rousseau1,3
  • 1Université de Lille, ULR 4072 – PSITEC – Psychologie: Interactions, Temps, Emotions, Cognition, Lille, France
  • 2Laboratoire sur l’anxiété et le Perfectionnisme, Department of Psychology, Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières, Trois-Rivières, QC, Canada
  • 3Centre d’Etudes et Recherches en Psychopathologie et Psychologie de la Santé (CERPPS), Université de Toulouse (UT2J), Toulouse, France

The aim of the present study is to test the validity and reliability of the French Body Image State Scale (F-BISS). The scale was translated using a back-translation technique, with discrepancies being settled through consensus. Three hundred and twelve female participants were recruited. Convergent validity was assessed using eating disorder evaluation and social comparison. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses were also conducted. The translated Body Image State Scale (BISS) demonstrated good psychometric properties, with good internal consistency (α = 0.83), and adequate goodness-of-fit. The translated BISS presented a unifactorial structure, with one factor explaining 56% of the variance. The exploratory factor analysis led to the removal of a single item due to insufficient factor loading (<0.45). Its convergent validity seems consistent with previous literature. Discriminant analyses showed a significant difference in F-BISS score between participants relative to eating disorder symptomatology (t = 11.65; p < 0.001). This translation could prove useful in both research and clinical settings to assess state body satisfaction in French populations.

Introduction

A growing body of research warns of the public health issue represented by body dissatisfaction, or negative body assessment (Bucchianeri and Neumark-Sztainer, 2014; Griffiths et al., 2017; Bornioli et al., 2019). Developing reliable measurement tools or validating existing ones in multiple languages in this context allows researchers to better understand the onset of eating disorder development. Indeed, body dissatisfaction is a risk factor for eating disorders (Stice, 2002; Stice et al., 2011). In western countries, women are pressured to achieve a thin body-ideal and western sociocultural influences have been proven to be a risk factor for general population body dissatisfaction (Holmqvist and Frisén, 2010). In France, men’s body dissatisfaction differs from women’s, with men being more preoccupied about their muscularity and gaining weight (Pope et al., 2000) and women being more concerned about being thin and toned (Girard et al., 2018). In this study, we will focus on state-measurement tools, as they have raised criticism in the way some were developed and validated in the past (Cash et al., 2002; Bateson et al., 2007). More precisely, we will focus on the validation of a positive body assessment, or body satisfaction, state scale: the Body Image State Scale (BISS) (Cash et al., 2002).

Body dissatisfaction or satisfaction are indeed variably conceptualized in two ways, “state,” and “trait,” meaning it can be viewed as an immediate state of being, or a stable personality trait (Cash et al., 2002). Multiple trait body-dissatisfaction scales have been translated in French, such as the Body Shape Questionnaire (BSQ; Cooper et al., 1987; Rousseau et al., 2005), the body-dissatisfaction subscale of the EDI (Garner et al., 1983; Archinard et al., 1996), the Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire’s (EDE-Q’s) shape and weight concern subscales (Fairburn and Beglin, 1994; Fairburn, 2008; Carrard et al., 2015), or the Male Body Dissatisfaction Scale (Ochner et al., 2009; Rousseau et al., 2014). The use of trait-scales lie in evaluating body-dissatisfaction in a punctual fashion, for research or clinical purposes such as determining if a patient is suitable for therapy. Moreover, trait-measures are often retrospective, and do not seem much suited for assessing change over a shorter period of time. Therefore, life-changes that may harm body image such as receiving a surgery (Sarwer et al., 2010) could only be assessed after a period of time when using a trait-measure. Since negative body image is linked to pathology, having to wait to correctly assess it could hinder patients’ treatment. Cash et al. (2002) emphasized the fact that most measures of body dissatisfaction or satisfaction are trait measures, such as the BSQ (Cooper et al., 1987). To the research team’s knowledge this statement holds true today in France with only one figure or contour-drawing based state scale being validated in French (Moussally et al., 2017) and another Contour-Drawing Rating Scale (CDRS; Thompson and Gray, 1995) being used in some research with French-speaking samples (Duchesne et al., 2017; Rivière et al., 2018) with seemingly no prior validation. However, this type of scale has been criticized for not being representative enough of female body diversity (Bateson et al., 2007). When searching for the keywords “body-dissatisfaction” and “state scale” and “French,” no relevant results are found on the PsycNet or ScienceDirect databases.

Cash et al. (2002) also underlined the issue that tools used to measure state body satisfaction in previous research have often been developed from trait-scales, or constructed without prior validation. As said earlier, these scales may lack sensitivity. For this purpose, Cash et al. (2002) developed the Body-Image State Scale. This 6-item state-measure of body satisfaction is non-specific, exclusively text-based and originally available in English. Its 9-point scale with specific body-related statements to choose from should allow participants to give a precise answer. It is less prone to the unrealistic body representation associated with contour-drawing based scales, as participants are asked about their own body, without being shown one. The BISS has been widely used in previous literature (i.e., Etu and Gray, 2010; Walker et al., 2012; Boersma and Jarry, 2013). Currently, Spanish, Italian, and Dutch versions have been developed, with only the Spanish version being the subject of a validation study (Carraro et al., 2010; Alleva et al., 2014; Mebarak Chams et al., 2019). Convergent validity has been tested using the BSQ (Cooper et al., 1987), and the EDE-Q (Fairburn and Beglin, 1994). Validation studies have shown very good psychometric qualities (Cash et al., 2002; Mebarak Chams et al., 2019). The BISS appears to have a single-factor structure, with every item loading on the main factor at a >0.75 coefficient (Mebarak Chams et al., 2019). Convergent validity across studies also appears coherent with literature: the BISS had a negative correlation with body-dissatisfaction (r = −0.52, p < 0.001; Cash et al., 2002; r = −0.58, p < 0.001; Mebarak Chams et al., 2019), eating disorder symptomatology (r = −0.79, p < 0.01; Alleva et al., 2014) and general psychopathology (r = −0.33, p < 0.001; Mebarak Chams et al., 2019). Discriminant analyses also showed a significant difference between Body Mass Index (BMI) groups with participants having a higher BMI being less satisfied than those with a lower BMI (Mebarak Chams et al., 2019). However, no study has yet replicated a long-term test–retest procedure like in the original study. Indeed, the only test–retest data available comes from Carraro et al. (2010), who assessed test–retest fidelity only an hour after an experimental task (r = 0.87, p < 0.01). The BISS appears to have been neither translated nor validated into French.

Validation of the BISS in French would be of use for a variety of professions. State scales as said are indeed very useful in research and clinical settings. In a research setting, they may be used to assess the immediate impact of an exposure task such as the ones used in the study of social media’s impact on body image (Tiggemann and Zaccardo, 2015; Cohen et al., 2019). In a clinical setting, they could be used by the therapist as a self-evaluation tool for patients to better understand what causes body image-related distress, and assess differences over time. The BISS is even more relevant for patients’ use, as it is a short and easy scale to use.

To validate the BISS, it seemed important to take notice of the differences in body dissatisfaction expression between men and women’s body image. Indeed, research has shown men tend to express dissatisfaction related to their muscle mass (Karazsia et al., 2017). As mentioned earlier, this finding seems to hold true in a French sample, with men wanting to gain weight and muscle mass (Pope et al., 2000). French women, on the other hand, seem to be pressured to reach a more thin and toned body-shape (Girard et al., 2018). Moreover, 70% of French women want to lose weight (Valls et al., 2013) and 34.6% state that their self-opinion depends on their weight (Lachaud et al., 2004). While those results underline the importance of validating a state-measure in French to adequately assess immediate body-satisfaction, this would also mean we would need different measures of convergent validity for each gender. Therefore, we wished to focus at first on a female population.

Secondly, elements in research made us consider validating the BISS in a younger population. Indeed, research showed that cut-off scores to the EDE-Q (Fairburn and Beglin, 1994; Fairburn, 2008) varied between age groups, being higher in a younger population (Rø et al., 2015). Moreover, older women tend to display a lesser desire, or drive, to be thinner (Pruis and Janowsky, 2010); this would be a bias since tools available for this study were mainly validated in a younger population and addressed thinness-related preoccupations.

The main objective of this study is therefore to translate and validate the BISS (Cash et al., 2002) in an 18–25-year-old female French population.

It was expected here that a French version of the BISS would negatively correlate with measures of body dissatisfaction, social comparison, and eating symptomatology, and would present a single-factor structure, as demonstrated in relevant research (Mebarak Chams et al., 2019). Moreover, we wished to replicate differences between BMI groups and eating disorder symptomatology groups found in the Mebarak Chams (2019) study; it is expected that higher BMI categories will have a lesser French Body Image State Scale (F-BISS) score than lower BMI groups. It is also expected that participants with a clinically significant score to an eating disorder symptomatology scale will have a lower score than participants with a clinically non-significant score (Mebarak Chams et al., 2019).

Materials and Methods

Participants

The study included 312 participants. Seventy-eight participants completed the test two times at a 2-week interval (NTime 1 = 312; NTime 2 = 78). Participants were 18–25-year-old female students (M = 21.07, SD = 1.82). Self-reported values of height and weight were used to derive BMI (M = 22.88, SD = 4.48). Participants did not receive compensation. For sample size, Everitt’s (1975) recommendations of a subject to item ratio of 10 were used.

Procedure

This study received approval by the University’s Board of Ethics. Recommendations for translation from Cha et al. (2007) were followed. The scale was translated into French by several members of the research team, one of whom was fluent in English (C1 level; Council of Europe, n.d.). Due to a lack of independent translators, the translation was then back-translated by another English-fluent team member and reviewed by the research team. Minor discrepancies were settled through consensus. An advertisement for a study on body image was posted on various French-speaking student Facebook groups, with a message stating the research team’s intent to recruit 18–25-year-old female participants. Willing participants had to fill out the Body Shape Questionnaire 8-item (BSQ-8C), the EDE-Q, the Physical Appearance Comparison Scale-4 (PACS-4), the BISS, and a sociodemographic data questionnaire, in that order. When ending the questionnaire, participants were asked to create a code (enabling anonymous test–retest analyses) consisting of the two last digits of participants’ phone number, the first two letters of their first name and the last two digits of their birth year. Participants were then asked if they wished to take part in the study’s second phase. If they did, they had to input their participant code again and provide their email address. Willing participants received an email 2 weeks later with a link to another questionnaire. To replicate the original study, we chose to respect the same 2-week delay between questionnaire answering (Cash et al., 2002). A 2-week interval also seems to be considered the highest appropriate interval for retest of a state-scale (Polit, 2014). The questionnaire contained the French BISS, as well as another 4-item questionnaire to be validated in another study. Participants were again prompted to input their code. Finally, a written debrief was sent via email.

Study Material

Body Image State Scale

This 6-item self-reported scale (Cash et al., 2002) is rated on a 9-point Likert scale. Each item begins with the sentence “Right now, I feel” [for instance: “Right now, I feel (Extremely dissatisfied to Extremely satisfied) with my physical appearance”]. Every rating’s phrasing is different. For instance, item 1 ranges from Extremely dissatisfied (1) to Extremely satisfied (9), while item 4 ranges from Extremely physically attractive (9) to Extremely physically unattractive (1). Score is a mean of every item. Higher scores denote higher body satisfaction. Half items have reverse rating (2, 4, 6). Internal consistency calculated by Cronbach’s alpha in the original study is 0.77 in women, and 0.83 in this study’s sample.

Body Shape Questionnaire 8-Item

This self-reported questionnaire was developed by Cooper et al. (1987), and abbreviated by Evans and Dolan (1993). This abridged version has been validated in French by Lentillon-Kaestner et al. (2014). This 8-item questionnaire measures body dissatisfaction over the past 4 weeks [item example: “Has thinking about your shape interfered with your ability to concentrate (e.g., while watching television, reading, listening to conversations?”)]. A total score of trait body dissatisfaction is obtained by adding the score for each Likert-scale item (1–6; maximum score of 48). A higher score denotes higher body dissatisfaction. In this study’s sample, internal consistency measured by Cronbach’s alpha is 0.93, and scores range from 8 to 48, with a mean score of 25.35 (SD = 10.47).

Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire

This self-reported questionnaire (Fairburn and Beglin, 1994; Fairburn, 2008), validated in French by Carrard et al. (2015), assesses eating disorder symptomatology. Items include questions such as “Have you gone for long periods of time (8 waking hours or more) without eating anything at all in order to influence your shape or weight?” Answers are on a 7-point Likert scale. While the questionnaire was originally designed with four subscales in mind, it is advised to only use the overall score, as a single-factor structure is thought to be more robust (Friborg et al., 2013). The four subscales are restraint (items 1, 2, 3, 4, 5; α = 0.84), eating concerns (items 7, 9, 19, 21, 20; α = 0.80), shape concerns (items 6, 8, 23, 10, 26, 27, 28, 11; α = 0.91), and weight concerns (items 22, 24, 8, 25, 12; α = 0.89). Cut-off scores from Rø et al. (2015) study were used. While Rø et al. (2015) evaluated Norwegian adults, and warned about eventual cross-cultural differences, a lack of similar data in the French validation article (Carrard et al., 2015) forced us to use a near cultural equivalent for the same scale. For an underweight BMI, cut-off was 1.62. For a normal BMI, cut-off was 2.51. For an overweight BMI, cut-off was 3.15. Finally, for an obese BMI, cut-off was 3.26. A score superior to these values was considered clinically significant. In this study’s sample, internal consistency measured by Cronbach’s alpha is 0.91, and scores range from 0 to 5.57, with a mean score of 1.95 (SD = 1.40).

The Physical Appearance Comparison Scale-4

This self-reported 4-item questionnaire (Thompson et al., 1991; Dany and Urdapilleta, 2012) measures the general tendency of individuals to compare themselves with others in social situations (item example: “At parties or other social events, I compare my physical appearance to the physical appearance of others.”). Participants are presented a Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). Overall score is calculated by adding each item’s individual score. The higher the score (maximum score of 20), the more likely the individual is to use social comparison. In this study’s sample, internal consistency measured by Cronbach’s alpha is 0.84, and scores range from 4 to 20, with a mean score of 11.92 (SD = 3.85).

Sociodemographic Data Questionnaire

This questionnaire was used to report on participants’ age, education level, and area of study. As some parts of this questionnaire were explicitly meant for female participants (EDEQ items on menstruation and contraceptive pill usage), and since the recruitment campaign was explicitly directed at cisgendered women 18–25 years old, no questions about gender were added.

Statistical Analyses

Construct validity was assessed using exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses, as well as inter-item correlations. The exploratory factor analysis was conducted on SPSS 25, while the confirmatory factor analysis was carried out on RStudio using the lavaan package (Rosseel, 2012). Both used a maximum likelihood estimation method. For the factor analyses, a “fair” item loading cut-off (0.45) was set (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2007). A 0.2 cut-off was set for communalities (Child, 2006). The Kaiser criterion was used, meaning valid factors should have an eigenvalue greater than one (Costello and Osborne, 2005). For the exploratory factor analysis, examination of the scree-plot and a parallel analysis were also used to corroborate factor solutions. Data from one random split-half of the sample (n = 156) was selected for the exploratory factor analysis. Data from the other random split-half of the sample (n = 156) was selected for the confirmatory factor analysis. While no clear recommendation exists on what constitutes a satisfactory percentage of explained variance, values between 50 and 60% were chosen to be retained (Peterson, 2000). Goodness-of-fit indices were established prior to testing (Hooper et al., 2008). RMSEA values close to 0.06 were considered indicators of good fit. SRMR values under 0.05 were retained, as well-fitting models tend to obtain similar values. Finally, as it is recommended that a CFI index should be > 0.95, it was decided to keep that value for this testing. Model improvement was assessed through modification index values (MI), implemented in R by the lavaan package. Higher MI values indicate a better fit of the corresponding model, using the LaGrange multiplier.

For inter-item correlations, moderate correlations (0.3–0.7) were considered the lowest acceptable degree of correlation. Internal consistency was evaluated using Cronbach’s alpha and McDonald’s omega. Test–retest fidelity was controlled using absolute agreement intra-class correlations between BISS scores at times 1 and 2, with a two-way mixed model. Convergent validity was determined using Pearson’s correlations between BSQ-8C, EDE-Q, PACS-4 scores and BISS scores. Concurrent validity was assessed using ANOVAs between BMI groups and t-tests between eating disorder symptomatology groups. Univariate normality was assessed for t-tests. A cut-off interval of [−2; 2] was used as per George and Mallery’s (2010) recommendations. For clinically significant participants, kurtosis was 0.33 and skewness was −0.43. For non-clinically significant participants, kurtosis was −0.61 and skewness was 0.36.

Results

Item-Analysis and Construct Validity

The exploratory factor analysis was computed using a maximum likeliness extraction method. KMO index was satisfactory (KMO = 0.82), and Bartlett’s test of sphericity produced a significant result (p < 0.001). Only one component reached eigenvalue >1.00 (eigenvalue = 3.35) (see Figure 1). A parallel analysis confirmed a one-factor solution (eigenvalue of second factor: 0.92; parallel analysis eigenvalue for a two-factor solution: 1.14). According to the chosen cut-off, item 2 does not adequately load on the main factor (see Table 1). When assessing communalities, item 2 was the only one under our chosen cut-off point (see Table 2). Therefore, item 2 was removed from the rest of the analyses.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scree-plot for the F-BISS scale. Dashed line represents the Kaiser criterion.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Exploratory factor analysis of the F-BISS.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Communalities for the exploratory factor analysis of the F-BISS.

The confirmatory analysis also returned a single factor solution (see Figure 2). The MI function on RStudio indicated higher MI values when allowing correlation between items 4 and 6 (MI = 37.86). When allowing correlation between items 4 and 6, goodness-offit indices were adequate for the second model (see Table 3). All items had high item-total correlation coefficients at time 1, ranging from r = 0.65 (p < 0.001) to r = 0.85 (p < 0.001; see Table 4).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Confirmatory factor analysis of the BISS (model 2). Sts, main factor. One-way arrows represent factor loading. Two-way arrows represent correlations between items.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Confirmatory factorial analysis, and goodness-of-fit indices of the F-BISS.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. French Body Image State Scale items inter-correlations and item-total correlations (ITC).

Reliability

Cronbach’s alpha and McDonald’s omega were used to define the F-BISS’s internal consistency. At time 1 and time 2, internal consistency without item 2 was satisfactory, with α = 0.83 at both time 1 and time 2. Using McDonald’s omega, internal consistency remained satisfactory, with ω = 0.85 at time 1, and ω = 0.86.

The test–retest reliability of the F-BISS was calculated without item 2 over a 2-week period. The coefficient of correlation was r = 0.86 (p < 0.001) for single measures.

Convergent Validity

Pearson’s correlations were used to establish links between the F-BISS and BMI, trait measures of body image, measures of social comparison (PACS-4) and measures of eating disorder symptomatology (EDE-Q). F-BISS scores without item 2 strongly and negatively correlated with body dissatisfaction (BSQ; r = −0.74, p < 0.001), eating disorder symptomatology (EDE-Q; r = −0.72, p < 0.001), and moderately correlated with BMI (r = −0.36, p < 0.001), and comparison to others (PACS-4; r = −0.43, p < 0.001).

Differences between BMI categories were significant (F = 13.85; p < 0.001), with Bonferroni post hoc testing revealing no significant differences between underweight and normal categories (p = 0.86), no significant differences between overweight and obese categories (p = 1.00), but significant differences between underweight/normal and overweight/obese categories (p < 0.001).

Finally, differences between women with clinically significant and non-clinically significant symptomatology for eating disorders were significant (t = 11.65; p < 0.001). The clinically non-significant group had significantly higher satisfaction.

Discussion

The results yielded showed the French version of the BISS (F-BISS) to have good psychometric qualities. Items 1, 3, 4, 5, and 6 were adequately loaded on a main factor (>0.45), moderately to strongly intercorrelated, and internal consistency was good after removing item 2. A one-factor solution also showed adequate fit after removal of item 2, and allowing for correlation between items 4 and 6.

The F-BISS was negatively correlated to overall EDE-Q score. Moreover, participants with a clinically significant symptomatology were less satisfied than other participants. This is consistent with eating disorder literature, as a lower F-BISS score indicated lower body satisfaction, and thus greater dissatisfaction, a risk factor in eating disorder development (Stice et al., 2011). As with the Spanish validation of the scale (Mebarak Chams et al., 2019) the F-BISS was negatively correlated to a trait measure of body dissatisfaction (BSQ). Finally, the F-BISS was negatively correlated with social comparison. Again, this is consistent with research (Rodgers et al., 2011) as the original BISS, and thus the F-BISS, measure body satisfaction (Cash et al., 2002). BMI’s correlation to the F-BISS was somewhat consistent with the Spanish validation (r = −0.28; Mebarak Chams et al., 2019) but not with the original validation (r = −0.53; Cash et al., 2002). Furthermore, an ANOVA showed that significant differences in F-BISS scores between BMI groups lie between “clusters” formed by the underweight and normal groups, and the overweight and obese groups. These findings could indicate either that BMI underestimates obesity prevalence by categorizing obese people as overweight (Shah and Braverman, 2012), or that BMIs in the original study displayed stronger differences between groups, perhaps explained by a possibly higher prevalence of female obesity in the United States than in France (Diouf et al., 2010; An, 2014). In that case, it would be beneficial to assess differences in overall BISS score between American and French samples; even if western countries are supposed to display similar rates of body dissatisfaction (Holmqvist and Frisén, 2010), a higher obesity prevalence could be linked to higher body dissatisfaction in one population over another (Weinberger et al., 2016). Finally, results showing a significant difference between participants with a clinically significant eating disorders symptomatology score are coherent with research establishing body dissatisfaction as a risk-factor of eating disorder development (Stice, 2002; Stice et al., 2011).

Test–retest reliability was also slightly higher than the original scale with a 2-week interval (r = 0.69; Cash et al., 2002), and comparable to the first test–retest coefficient from the Italian validation with an hour interval (r = 0.87; Carraro et al., 2010). This may imply that this translation is more stable than expected. In both our study and the original study, test–retest reliability was assessed in a neutral “questionnaire-filling” context. However, in our study, participants had to fill the questionnaire at home using a computer, while they had to come to a laboratory in the original study. Perhaps the presence of the research team in the original study could have induced social comparison of appearance before the experiment, causing a bias as social comparison of appearance is linked to lesser body satisfaction (Myers and Crowther, 2009). State-mood, a variable we have not controlled, has been shown to have an effect on state body dissatisfaction; when an individual feels better, their body dissatisfaction tends to be lower (Colautti et al., 2011). With this in mind, it could be more stable in a familiar environment like the one in our study than in a laboratory context. Moreover, we have not verified if scores would remain stable after subjecting participants to different contexts, like in the original study (Cash et al., 2002). Finally, perhaps testing the scale with shorter retest intervals could be appropriate, to reduce eventual lability (Polit, 2014).

The research conducted presents some limitations. Firstly, it has not been tested in a male population, unlike every other validation study. This poses a concern with regard to generalization. As mentioned, the reason for men’s exclusion is that a meta-analysis has shown that men present higher dissatisfaction about muscularity (Karazsia et al., 2017). This study would have required other measures of convergent validity for men, as the scales used were focused on thin appearance and weight loss, and were validated in a female population (Dany and Urdapilleta, 2012; Lentillon-Kaestner et al., 2014; Carrard et al., 2015). Secondly, the research has never been tested within a broader age group. This should be corrected in future studies by using appropriate scales. Indeed, older women are also affected by body-image issues (Marshall et al., 2012), despite being less thinness-driven than younger women (Pruis and Janowsky, 2010). Thirdly, contrary to the Spanish translation (Mebarak Chams et al., 2019), item 2 had to be deleted due to insufficient factor loading and low inter-item correlations. This item made reference to body size and shape. It was surmised after the study that “body size and shape” should have simply been translated as “silhouette” and not “taille et forme du corps.” The latter translation was thought to be more literal, and closer to the original English phrasing. However, this item’s removal does not seem detrimental to the overall validity of the scale, as borne out in section “Results,” making corrections probably unnecessary. Finally, EDE-Q cut-off scores used in this study were validated in a Norwegian adult sample, not a French sample. While results are coherent with literature, it would be needed to establish psychometric norms in a French population to provide more reliable cut-off scores. Moreover, a clinically significant EDE-Q score cannot be considered an eating disorder diagnosis. Therefore, it would be needed in future research to assess F-BISS scores in women with and without clinically established eating disorders.

Future studies should focus on replicating the original study’s protocol, and test the F-BISS in different situational contexts, such as a day on the beach, or reading a fashion magazine alone. Indeed, the F-BISS was only tested in a neutral (questionnaire filling) situation without any physical intervention of the research team, which could explain its relative stability. When the situational context is positive for body image, F-BISS scores should be higher, indicating body satisfaction (Cash et al., 2002). When the situational context is negative or threatening for body image, F-BISS scores should be lower. Other measures of convergent validity should also be used, such as indicators of thin-ideal internalization. The SATAQ-4R (Schaefer et al., 2017) or the DKB-35 (Zohar et al., 2017; Lev-Ari et al., 2020), another trait-measure of body image, would be adequate choices. Another measure of convergent validity that should be used is state-mood. As said earlier, state-mood is positively linked to state body-dissatisfaction (Colautti et al., 2011). Conducting further analyses in other ethnic groups, age groups, genders, or other French-speaking samples, such as Belgian or French-Canadian samples, could prove worthwhile for generalization. Finally, validation in an eating disorder diagnosed population and comparison with a general population would allow verification of our t-test results in a clinical setting.

In a research setting, this scale could be used to identify, for instance, the effect of exposure to body image-threatening content in 18–25-year-old females. In a clinical setting, such as eating disorder treatment, its ability to measure state body image satisfaction would allow for enhanced monitoring of the patient’s state, and a more comprehensive understanding of the daily life situations that increase or decrease body satisfaction.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Université de Lille ethical review board (ref: 2019-345-S71). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

LB: original draft, methodology, recruitment, conceptualization, and reviewing and editing. CA: reviewing and editing, recruitment, methodology, and conceptualization. CB and FL: reviewing and editing and formal analysis. AR: data supervision and curation, project administration, reviewing and editing, and formal analysis. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by the PSITEC laboratory (which typography and referral is Univ. Lille, ULR 4072 – PSITEC – Psychologie: Interactions, Temps, Emotions, Cognition, F3 59000 Lille, France) and the Direction d’Appui à la Recherche of the University of Lille (file number: 4839).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank everybody who volunteered to take part in our study, as well as our undergraduate students for participating in data collection. We also wish to thank Brian Stacy for proofreading this manuscript.

References

Alleva, J. M., Martijn, C., Jansen, A., and Nederkoorn, C. (2014). Body language: affecting body satisfaction by describing the body in functionality terms. Psychol. Women Q. 38, 181–196. doi: 10.1177/0361684313507897

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

An, R. (2014). Prevalence and trends of adult obesity in the US, 1999–2012. ISRN Obes. 2014:185132. doi: 10.1155/2014/185132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Archinard, M., Rouget, P., Painot, D., and Liengne, C. (1996). “Inventaire des troubles du comportement alimentaire 2,” in Protocoles et Échelles D’évaluation en Psychiatrie et en Psychologie, 1st Edn, eds M. Bouvard and J. Cottraux (Issy-les-Moulineaux: Masson), 275–276.

Google Scholar

Bateson, M., Cornelissen, P. L., and Tovée, M. J. (2007). “Methodological issues in studies of female attractiveness,” in The Body Beautiful, eds V. Swami and A. Furnham (London: Palgrave Macmillan), 46–62.

Google Scholar

Boersma, K. E., and Jarry, J. L. (2013). The paradoxical moderating effect of body image investment on the impact of weight-based derogatory media. Body Image 10, 200–209. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bornioli, A., Lewis-Smith, H., Smith, A., Slater, A., and Bray, I. (2019). Adolescent body dissatisfaction and disordered eating: predictors of later risky health behaviours. Soc. Sci. Med. 238:112458. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.112458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bucchianeri, M. M., and Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2014). Body dissatisfaction: an overlooked public health concern. J. Public Ment. Health 13, 64–69. doi: 10.1108/JPMH-11-2013-0071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrard, I., Rebetez, M. M., Mobbs, O., and Van der Linden, M. (2015). Factor structure of a French version of the eating disorder examination-questionnaire among women with and without binge eating disorder symptoms. Eat. Weight Disord. 20, 137–144. doi: 10.1007/s40519-014-0148-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carraro, A., Nart, A., and Scarpa, S. (2010). Effects of a single session of physical exercise on body state image. Rev. Bras. Cienc. Esporte 32, 173–184. doi: 10.1590/S0101-32892010000400012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cash, T. F., Fleming, E. C., Alindogan, J., Steadman, L., and Whitehead, A. (2002). Beyond body image as a trait: the development and validation of the Body image States Scale. Eat. Disord. 10, 103–113. doi: 10.1080/10640260290081678

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cha, E. S., Kim, K. H., and Erlen, J. A. (2007). Translation of scales in cross−cultural research: issues and techniques. J. Adv. Nurs 58, 386–395. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2007.04242.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Child, D. (2006). The Essentials of Factor Analysis, 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Continuum.

Google Scholar

Cohen, R., Fardouly, J., Newton-John, T., and Slater, A. (2019). # BoPo on Instagram: an experimental investigation of the effects of viewing body positive content on young women’s mood and body image. New Med. Soc. 21, 1546–1564. doi: 10.1177/1461444819826530

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colautti, L. A., Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M., Skouteris, H., McCabe, M., Blackburn, S., and Wyett, E. (2011). Accounting for fluctuations in body dissatisfaction. Body Image 8, 315–321. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, P. J., Taylor, M. J., Cooper, Z., and Fairburn, C. G. (1987). The development and validation of the body shape questionnaire. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 6, 485–494.

Google Scholar

Costello, A. B., and Osborne, J. (2005). Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Pract. Assess. Res. Eval. 10, 1–9. doi: 10.7275/jyj1-4868

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Council of Council of Europe (n.d.).The CEFR Levels. Available Online at: https://www.coe.int/en/web/common-european-framework-reference-languages/level-descriptions (accessed September 5, 2020).

Google Scholar

Dany, L., and Urdapilleta, I. (2012). Validation of a French measure of body comparison: the physical appearance comparison scale. Rev. Int. Psychol. Soc. 25, 97–112.

Google Scholar

Diouf, I., Charles, M. A., Ducimetière, P., Basdevant, A., Eschwege, E., and Heude, B. (2010). Evolution of obesity prevalence in France: an age-period-cohort analysis. Epidemiology 21:360. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181d5bff5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duchesne, A. P., Dion, J., Lalande, D., Bégin, C., Émond, C., Lalande, G., et al. (2017). Body dissatisfaction and psychological distress in adolescents: is self-esteem a mediator? J. Health Psychol. 22, 1563–1569. doi: 10.1177/1359105316631196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Etu, S. F., and Gray, J. J. (2010). A preliminary investigation of the relationship between induced rumination and state body image dissatisfaction and anxiety. Body Image 7, 82–85. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, C., and Dolan, B. (1993). Body shape questionnaire: derivation of shortened “alternate forms”. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 13, 315–321.

Google Scholar

Everitt, B. S. (1975). Multivariate analysis: the need for data, and other problems. Br. J. Psychiatry 126, 237–240. doi: 10.1192/bjp.126.3.237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairburn, C. G. (2008). Cognitive Behavior Therapy and Eating Disorders. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Fairburn, C. G., and Beglin, S. J. (1994). Assessment of eating disorders: interview or self−report questionnaire? Int. J. Eat. Disord. 16, 363–370.

Google Scholar

Friborg, O., Reas, D. L., Rosenvinge, J. H., and Rø, Ø (2013). Core pathology of eating disorders as measured by the eating disorder examination questionnaire (EDE-Q): the EATING DISORDERS 11 predictive role of a nested general (g) and primary factors. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 22, 1–10. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garner, D. M., Olmstead, M. P., and Polivy, J. (1983). Development and validation of a multidimensional eating disorder inventory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2, 15–34.

Google Scholar

George, D., and Mallery, M. (2010). SPSS for Windows Step by Step: A Simple Guide and Reference, 17.0 update, 10th Edn. London: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Girard, M., Rodgers, R. F., and Chabrol, H. (2018). Prospective predictors of body dissatisfaction, drive for thinness, and muscularity concerns among young women in France: a sociocultural model. Body Image 26, 103–110. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, S., Murray, S. B., Bentley, C., Gratwick-Sarll, K., Harrison, C., and Mond, J. M. (2017). Sex differences in quality of life impairment associated with body dissatisfaction in adolescents. J. Adolesc. Health 61, 77–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2017.01.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmqvist, K., and Frisén, A. (2010). Body dissatisfaction across cultures: findings and research problems. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 18, 133–146. doi: 10.1002/erv.965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., and Mullen, M. R. (2008). Structural equation modelling: guidelines for determining model fit. Electronic J. Bus. Res. Methods 6, 53–60.

Google Scholar

Karazsia, B. T., Murnen, S. K., and Tylka, T. L. (2017). Is body dissatisfaction changing across time? A cross-temporal meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 143, 293–320. doi: 10.1037/bul0000081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lachaud, M., Miquel, V., Rousseau, A., and Chabrol, H. (2004). Pratiques alimentaires et préoccupations corporelles chez les jeunes femmes. J. Ther. Comportementale Cogn. 14, 131–134. doi: 10.1016/S1155-1704(04)97460-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lentillon-Kaestner, V., Berchtold, A., Rousseau, A., and Ferrand, C. (2014). Validity and reliability of the French versions of the body shape questionnaire. J. Person. Assess. 96, 471–477. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2013.843537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lev-Ari, L., Zohar, A. H., and Bachner-Melman, R. (2020). Enjoying your body: the psychometric properties of an English version of the Dresden Body Image Questionnaire. Aust. J. Psychol. 72, 267–275.

Google Scholar

Marshall, C., Lengyel, C., and Utioh, A. (2012). Body dissatisfaction: among middle-aged and older women. Can. J. Diet. Pract. Res. 73, 241–247. doi: 10.3148/73.2.2012.e241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mebarak Chams, M. R., Tinoco, L., Mejia-Rodriguez, D., Martinez-Banfi, M. L., Preuss, H., Hammerle, F., et al. (2019). The spanish body image state scale: factor structure, reliability and validity in a colombian population. Front. Psychol. 10:2553. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moussally, J. M., Grynberg, D., Goffinet, S., Simon, Y., and Van der Linden, M. (2017). Novel assessment of own and ideal body perception among women: validation of the computer-generated figure rating scale. Cogn. Ther. Res. 41, 632–644. doi: 10.1007/s10608-016-9827-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myers, T. A., and Crowther, J. H. (2009). Social comparison as a predictor of body dissatisfaction: a meta-analytic review. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 118, 683–698. doi: 10.1037/a0016763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochner, C. N., Gray, J. A., and Brickner, K. (2009). The development and initial validation of a new measure of male body dissatisfaction. Eat. Behav. 10, 197–201. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2009.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, R. A. (2000). A meta-analysis of variance accounted for and factor loadings in exploratory factor analysis. Mark. Lett. 11, 261–275. doi: 10.1023/A:1008191211004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polit, D. F. (2014). Getting serious about test–retest reliability: a critique of retest research and some recommendations. Qual. Life Res. 23, 1713–1720. doi: 10.1007/s11136-014-0632-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pope, H. G. Jr., Gruber, A. J., Mangweth, B., Bureau, B., Decol, C., Jouvent, R., et al. (2000). Body image perception among men in three countries. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 1297–1301. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.8.1297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pruis, T. A., and Janowsky, J. S. (2010). Assessment of body image in younger and older women. J. Gen. Psychol. 137, 225–238. doi: 10.1080/00221309.2010.484446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivière, J., Rousseau, A., and Douilliez, C. (2018). Effects of induced rumination on body dissatisfaction: is there any difference between men and women? J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 61, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rø, Ø, Reas, D. L., and Stedal, K. (2015). Eating disorder examination questionnaire (EDE−Q) in Norwegian adults: discrimination between female controls and eating disorder patients. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 23, 408–412. doi: 10.1002/erv.2372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodgers, R., Chabrol, H., and Paxton, S. J. (2011). An exploration of the tripartite influence model of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating among Australian and French college women. Body Image 8, 208–215. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling and more. Version 0.5–12 (BETA). J. Stat. Softw. 48, 1–36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rousseau, A., Denieul, M., Lentillon, V., and Valls, M. (2014). French validation of the male body dissatisfaction scale in a sample of young men. J. Ther. Comportementale Cogn. 24, 122–129. doi: 10.1016/j.jtcc.2014.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rousseau, A., Knotter, R. M., Barbe, R. M., Raich, R. M., and Chabrol, H. (2005). Étude de validation de la version française du body shape questionnaire. Encephale 31, 162–173. doi: 10.1016/S0013-7006(05)82383-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarwer, D. B., Wadden, T. A., Moore, R. H., Eisenberg, M. H., Raper, S. E., and Williams, N. N. (2010). Changes in quality of life and body image after gastric bypass surgery. Surg. Obes. Relat. Dis. 6, 608–614. doi: 10.1016/j.soard.2010.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaefer, L. M., Harriger, J. A., Heinberg, L. J., Soderberg, T., and Kevin Thompson, J. (2017). Development and validation of the sociocultural attitudes towards appearance questionnaire−4−revised (SATAQ−4R). Int. J. Eat. Disord. 50, 104–117. doi: 10.1002/eat.22590

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shah, N. R., and Braverman, E. R. (2012). Measuring adiposity in patients: the utility of body mass index (BMI), percent body fat, and leptin. PLoS One 7:e33308. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0033308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E. (2002). Risk and maintenance factors for eating pathology: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 128, 825–848. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.5.825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stice, E., Marti, C. N., and Durant, S. (2011). Risk factors for onset of eating disorders: evidence of multiple risk pathways from an 8-year prospective study. Behav. Res. Ther. 49, 622–627. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2011.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using Multivariate Statistics, 5th Edn. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Google Scholar

Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L., and Tantleff-Dunn, S. (1991). The physical appearance comparison scale. Behav. Therap. 14:174.

Google Scholar

Thompson, M. A., and Gray, J. J. (1995). Development and validation of a new body-image assessment scale. J. Person. Assess. 64, 258–269. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6402_6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiggemann, M., and Zaccardo, M. (2015). « Exercise to be fit, not skinny »: the effect of fitspiration imagery on women’s body image. Body Image 15, 61-67. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valls, M., Rousseau, A., and Chabrol, H. (2013). Influence des médias, insatisfaction envers le poids et l’apparence et troubles alimentaires selon le genre. Psychol. Fr. 58, 229–240. doi: 10.1016/j.psfr.2013.06.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, D. C., Murray, A. D., Lavender, J. M., and Anderson, D. A. (2012). The direct effects of manipulating body checking in men. Body Image 9, 462–468. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinberger, N. A., Kersting, A., Riedel-Heller, S. G., and Luck-Sikorski, C. (2016). Body dissatisfaction in individuals with obesity compared to normal-weight individuals: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes. Facts 9, 424–441. doi: 10.1159/000454837

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zohar, A. H., Lev-Ari, L., Bachner-Melman, R., and Kreitler, S. (2017). The well rounded body image: the dresdner körperbildfragebogen DKB-35. Isr. J. Psychiatry Relat. Sci. 54, 22–27.

Google Scholar

Keywords: body satisfaction, state measure, validation, French, body image

Citation: Bardi L, Arnaud C, Bagès C, Langlois F and Rousseau A (2021) Translation and Validation of a State-Measure of Body Image Satisfaction: The Body Image State Scale. Front. Psychol. 12:724710. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.724710

Received: 14 June 2021; Accepted: 05 October 2021;
Published: 28 October 2021.

Edited by:

Michail Mantzios, Birmingham City University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Maya Libben, The University of British Columbia Okanagan Campus, Canada
Lilac Lev-Ari, Ruppin Academic Center, Israel

Copyright © 2021 Bardi, Arnaud, Bagès, Langlois and Rousseau. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Luc Bardi, bHVjLmJhcmRpQHVxdHIuY2E=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.