- 1Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, University of Maia (ISMAI), Maia, Portugal
- 2Unit I&D of the Justice and Governance Research Center of the Law School, University of Minho, Braga, Portugal
- 3University Fernando Pessoa, Porto, Portugal
- 4Laboratory of Neuropsychophysiology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
- 5Institute of Psychology and Neuropsychology of Porto – IPNP Health, Porto, Portugal
- 6Centro de Solidariedade de Braga/Projecto Homem, Braga, Portugal
In romantic relationships, individual differences are determinant factors for relational quality. Specifically, romantic attachment (RA) and difficulties in emotional regulation influence each other and may have predictive potential for the perceived dyadic adjustment (DA) level. This paper aims to identify the developmental parallel between behavioral patterns built since childhood and the construction of the emotional regulation skills that characterize them. Our analysis was based on the attachment theory and the concepts of romantic relationship and DA. In this way, we sought to further the understanding of relationship dynamics, beyond the usual focus on a single element and on associative relationships, and by exploring other effects among the different dimensions of relational functioning. In particular, we explored the predictive ability of emotional regulation patterns (more flexible individual characteristics) in discriminating between RA styles (more perennial influences), and their impact on the quality of romantic relationships, in the anticipation of dyadic adjustment variations.
Introduction
A romantic relationship is characterized by feelings of trust, as well as close and intense interaction (Regan et al., 1998; Muise et al., 2018), generally including an intimate sexual dimension as a distinctive element (Meltzer et al., 2017; Maxwell and McNulty, 2019). It is defined by having its own identity, specific rules, and a trajectory (Finkel et al., 2017; Porreca, 2019) which is often unpredictable (Eastwick et al., 2018).
A trusting relationship is expected to have a certain degree of predictability, with expectations being created about fulfilling each partner’s needs, and creating a balance between a necessary and sufficient degree of relational dependence and the autonomy of each partner (Coimbra de Matos, 2017; Finkel et al., 2017; Overall and Cross, 2019), contributing to stability, commitment, and satisfaction (Spielmann et al., 2013; Wang, 2019, Unpublished). However, these expectations can also generate insecurity when faced with doubts about alternatives to satisfy needs in the absence of the relationship (Attridge et al., 1998). Still, despite the associated risks, most people feel motivated and desire to find, engage in, and maintain a romantic relationship (Bookwala, 2015; Joel et al., 2019).
According to attachment theory, one of the most frequently applied and influential in the study of romantic relationships (Finkel et al., 2017), trust is a differentiating factor concerning the representations of relationship that each partner brings into the couple. This dimension underlies predispositions and expectations about the other and about the future of the relationship (Campbell and Stanton, 2019), in addition to playing a protective role against relational problems (Shallcross and Simpson, 2012).
On the one hand, regardless of their degree of commitment or type, relationships tend to be a source of well-being (Farero et al., 2019; Waddell et al., 2019), and are associated with better physical and mental health (Braithwaite and Holt-Lunstad, 2017; Slatcher and Selcuk, 2017; Wang et al., 2017), as well as to reports of greater happiness (Kawamichi et al., 2016) and satisfaction with life (Gustavson et al., 2016). The absence of a relationship, on the other hand, seems to be associated with the perception of greater loneliness and lower social support, with negative indirect effects on life satisfaction (Adamczyk and Segrin, 2015). In addition, relational disruptions cause multiple consequences and substantial losses in different areas of life, negatively impacting physical and mental health (Fox and Tokunaga, 2015; Fincham et al., 2018).
Many individuals aspire to have a stable and lasting romantic relationship, which has significant implications for quality of life, from the point of view of the individual, the couple, or even society itself, making it an important topic of study (Spanier, 1976). Thus, understanding which factors are involved in relationship functioning, and how they are articulated has been an important focus of researchers (Bookwala, 2015; Bertoni et al., 2020).
This paper stems from a description of the concepts of romantic relationship and dyadic adjustment (DA), which is a comprehensive indicator of the perception of relational quality. We aim to expand our focus beyond the assessment of satisfaction, which is a more frequent target in this research field.
Difficulties in adapting to a shared life (Costa et al., 2016; Cho et al., 2020) or the breaking of trust (Wang, 2019, Unpublished) often generate marital conflicts and/or emotional distress. These result from a variety of causes linked to individual characteristics, communication, the couple’s routines, and surrounding contexts (Costa et al., 2016; Doss and Rhoades, 2017), causing damage to the couple’s bond (Warach and Josephs, 2019). Attempts to resolve these difficulties prove to be more effective in couples with better relational skills, who are able to project and reflect about the future of the relationship, with mutual consideration, openness and security, ability to communicate and regulate emotions (Costa et al., 2016), as well as share tasks and responsibilities (Costa et al., 2016; Weiser and Weigel, 2016; Davila et al., 2017).
When targeting relational difficulties or conflicts, the benefits and effectiveness of therapeutic interventions (Snyder and Halford, 2012; Davila et al., 2017; Wiebe et al., 2017) are still insufficient to mitigate negative consequences, namely regarding dissolution of relationships, including divorce (Røsand et al., 2014; Pordata, 2020). This emphasizes the relevance of research in this field, to guide practice at the individual, family, and/or couple level, as well as help identify and characterize mechanisms and models that govern or allow to predict how relationships develop, evolve (Consoli et al., 2018; Farero et al., 2019), and are reconstructed (Limeira and Féres-Carneiro, 2019).
Despite some theoretical and conceptual dispersion, the findings from research on romantic relationships tend to be coherent (Rogge et al., 2017). Generally, they focus on concepts related to relational quality (Fowers et al., 2016), frequently quantified by DA – a concept of difficult interpretation – which describes the dynamic process of the romantic partnership interaction and relation (Gomez and Leal, 2008; Rosado and Wagner, 2015; Rosado et al., 2016).
Dyadic Adjustment
The DA, or coordination between the members of a romantic couple, emerges at the beginning, and continues throughout the relationship, in a process through which partners adapt to each other and to their roles and responsibilities in the relationship (Kendrick and Drentea, 2016). The adequacy achieved in the relationship can potentially discriminate its degree of viability (Montesino et al., 2013; Kendrick and Drentea, 2016). As a measure of adjustment, DA implies the creation and maintenance of a flexible, but reliable, agreement about what the relationship needs in order to function, taking into consideration mutual discovery without neglecting respect for individual development (Rao, 2017).
A better DA is observed in a greater tendency to function as a team, which requires cognitive, emotional, and social maturity, as well as altruism on both sides to accommodate the other’s satisfaction over an exclusive focus on the individual (Kendrick and Drentea, 2016; Rani et al., 2017; Rao, 2017). Therefore, it requires a commitment toward the partner and the relationship that generates cohesion, and a search for agreement on shared issues that allows for good communication, consensual decisions, and effective conflict management (Kendrick and Drentea, 2016).
Research often uses DA as a result variable to assess how the quality of romantic relationships can be impacted by factors of an individual, contextual, and relational nature (Rosado et al., 2016; Cho et al., 2020). For Spanier (1976), this individual subjective perception is obtained from three components: (1) satisfaction or happiness in the relationship; (2) the couple’s cohesion regarding shared activities; and (3) consensus on relationship-specific issues, including the expression of affection. The data gathered allows for a qualitative differentiation between relationships (Spanier, 1976; Farero et al., 2019), in a longitudinal dynamic perspective, as evolving processes (Spanier, 1976; Scorsolini-Comin and dos Santos, 2012).
Thus, the perceived relational quality is inseparable from how close relationships are experienced, including romantic ones. These experiences, the satisfaction they provide, and the ease or difficulty with which they are managed, vary substantially according to individual attachment characteristics that predispose people to adopt a certain set of behaviors in relationships (Fraley and Hudson, 2017; Fraley and Roisman, 2019), which dictate hopes and fears in selecting, constructing, experiencing, and maintaining relationships (Bowlby, 1969, 1982; Ainsworth, 1979; Ainsworth et al., 2015; Fraley and Hudson, 2017; Fraley, 2019; Fraley and Roisman, 2019).
Attachment and Romantic Relationships
The individual differences, evidenced in relational patterns throughout life (Fraley and Hudson, 2017; Simpson and Rholes, 2017; Theisen et al., 2018), can be explained using mental models that, according to attachment theory, are internalized within the first social interactions with primary caregivers. These experiences create representations about the self and others, which become the guiding framework for expectations and responses to future relationships (Bowlby, 1969, 1982; Ainsworth, 1979; Sroufe, 2016; Simpson and Rholes, 2017; Eppel, 2018).
Attachment can be understood as an innate motivational system, which has evolved to guide behavior toward survival and adaptation (Fraley and Hudson, 2017), by promoting a lasting emotional connection (Fraley, 2019) as a safe base and haven (Hazan and Shaver, 1994; Ainsworth et al., 2015; Gillath et al., 2016; Simpson and Rholes, 2017). Ainsworth et al. (2015) differentiated the internalized models according to the conduct of the primary attachment figure. When this figure is responsive and available, the model is organized in a safe pattern, within experiences of care, comfort, protection, and alleviation of negative emotions, thus fostering confidence in the accessibility of help (Bowlby, 1969; Pascuzzo et al., 2013). This is not possible for insensitive or inconsistent caregiving attitudes, which enhance insecure patterns that will be anxious or avoidant (Fraley and Hudson, 2017; Simpson and Rholes, 2017; Umemura et al., 2018). Only the safe model allows independence from real proximity and transforms the relationship into a goal-corrected partnership, creating the conditions for a confident and autonomous exploration, including the establishment of secondary bonds (Hazan and Selcuk, 2015; Freeman and Simons, 2017; Mota, 2018).
These internal models permeate personality development, but not as a static phenomena (Pinquart et al., 2013; Groh et al., 2014; Theisen et al., 2018). If, on the one hand, they create a tendency for peer selection, on the other hand, they show susceptibility to the effects of socialization in new relationships (McConnell and Moss, 2011; Fraley and Hudson, 2017; Simpson and Rholes, 2017; Siegel et al., 2018; Fraley, 2019; Fraley and Roisman, 2019; Galaviz, 2019; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2019; Sutton, 2019), with this mutability being driven by the cumulative effect of continuous relational experiences (Van Ryzin et al., 2011; Simpson et al., 2015; Taylor et al., 2015; Gillath et al., 2016; Fraley and Hudson, 2017; Arriaga et al., 2018).
The acquisition of autonomy, in early adolescence (Theisen et al., 2018), tends to center the main bond on a romantic partnership (Freeman and Simons, 2017; Umemura et al., 2018; Fraley, 2019), involving a sexual aspect which definitely contrasts with early attachment (Ávila et al., 2011; Zeifman and Hazan, 2016). In addition, adult relationships will also differ due to their reciprocal, bidirectional, and voluntary nature, with parity of roles (Gillath et al., 2016) and susceptibility to dissolution (Simpson et al., 2015). Adult social empowerment and the accumulation of relational experiences, romantic or otherwise, enhance the complexity of developing bonds, particularly in romantic attachment (RA), which highlights the importance of its characterization (Hazan and Selcuk, 2015; Merrill, 2018; Fraley and Roisman, 2019).
In the process of building primary attachment, the experience of dyadic emotion regulation (ER) is a central aspect of human growth and development (Cole, 2014), because it establishes the foundations for affective regulation mechanisms (Cassidy, 1994; Viddal et al., 2017), which can be generalized to future interactions, including romantic relationships (Bowlby, 1969; Cassidy, 1994; Brenning and Braet, 2013; Pascuzzo et al., 2013; Sroufe, 2016).
Emotion Regulation and the Quality of Romantic Relationships
Emotion regulation is a dynamic process that influences and is influenced by intra and interpersonal factors (Grandey and Melloy, 2017), neurologically materialized and inseparable from the quality of the primary caregiver relationship (Etkin et al., 2015; Eppel, 2018; Panksepp et al., 2019). The developed regulatory resources define the level of security or insecurity intrinsic to the internal relational models, differentiating them in terms of response to threatening stimuli (Brenning and Braet, 2013; Guzmán-González et al., 2016).
Insecure models manifest themselves between two extremes. One is anxiety/ambivalence, characterized by emotional hyperactivation and guided by fears of abandonment and lack of value, aiming to ensure and retain the availability of an unpredictable partnership (Brenning and Braet, 2013; Gillath et al., 2016; Winterheld, 2016). Another extreme is avoidance, which is characterized by hypoactivation/emotional deactivation, intending to inhibit negative emotions elicited by fear of rejection, in which discomfort with proximity and dependence promotes a refusal of support from the partner, who is seen as unreliable (Malik et al., 2015; Simpson and Rholes, 2017; Umemura et al., 2018; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2019).
In contrast, attachment security is characterized by low levels of anxiety and avoidance, and is considered a resilience resource when facing vulnerabilities and challenges, in which greater effectiveness in accepting, expressing, and using emotions improves the selection of regulatory strategies (Goodall, 2015; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2019) and lessens concerns with the responsiveness of others as sources of comfort and safety (Winterheld, 2016; Fraley and Hudson, 2017).
Hazan and Shaver (1987) were pioneers in expanding the theoretical attachment approach (Bowlby, 1969, 1982; Ainsworth, 1979) to romantic relationships. In this constantly evolving conceptualization (Siegel et al., 2018; Zeifman, 2019), romantic love is represented through flexible models, differentiated by the accumulation of relational experiences, in more or less safe patterns, that reverberate in future relationships (Godbout et al., 2017; Furman and Collibee, 2018; Girme et al., 2018; Fraley and Roisman, 2019).
Research on the impact of these differences has shown that secure patterns are characterized by partner support and acceptance, more effective emotional regulation and communication skills, the ability to identify and discuss negative experiences and resolve conflicts, with relationships being enjoyed with greater harmony, commitment, trust, and satisfaction (Gillath et al., 2016; Fraley, 2019). Insecure patterns, due to weak early development of coping resources, tend to welcome more negative perspectives and greater difficulties, concerns, and fears about intimacy (Mark et al., 2018; Birnbaum and Reis, 2019; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2019). The anxious/ambivalent extreme tends to be obsessive, emotional, and/or sexual, and characterized by jealousy or distrust, whereas individuals at the avoidant extreme exhibit discomfort with closeness and intimacy (Meyers and Landsberger, 2002; Simpson and Rholes, 2017).
Insecure attachments tend to have a negative impact on cognitive, emotional, and behavioral aspects of relational quality (Li and Chan, 2012), revealing a negative association with DA (Muraru and Turliuc, 2012). Couples in which both partners exhibit insecure RA tend to report lower DA, while the trend is reversed if both exhibit secure RA, with varying levels of DA being observed in couples with mixed attachment (Siegel et al., 2018). However, the mechanisms involved in the relationship between RA and DA remain relatively unknown (Martins et al., 2016). Thus, our objective is to contribute to their clarification.
Attachment, Difficulties in Emotion Regulation, and Relationship Quality
Attachment remains, throughout the life cycle (Cole, 2014; Zimmermann and Thompson, 2014), inseparable from ER patterns (Hazan and Selcuk, 2015). This renders the attachment system to be considered an emotion regulating system (Gillath et al., 2016), in which regulatory skills work as an extension of the internal models that guide behavior within relationships (Malik et al., 2015; Gardner et al., 2019).
Therefore, when establishing and developing RA, the regulation of emotions by each partner will depend on the pattern defined in their primary emotional connection and under potential influence of successive later relationships, which will have repercussions on each new connection under construction. Regulatory patterns were identified as mediators in the development of representations of romantic relationships in adulthood, in their degree of insecurity, with unsecure standards contributing to the development of less confident bonds (Ávila, et al., 2011; Pascuzzo et al., 2013; Espeleta et al., 2017). They are responsible for effects on relational quality, suggesting tendencies for lower relational adjustment, as well as complex links between RA, ER, and relationship quality, which have yet to be examined (Brandão et al., 2019).
Emotions, whose function is to direct action toward goals and the fulfillment of needs (Elliott, 2012), inevitably affect the interaction of couples (Veloso et al., 2011). They influence satisfaction, quality, and success (Cohen et al., 2012; Bloch et al., 2014; Waldinger and Schulz, 2016), which can become impaired when verbal or bodily emotional manifestations (Barrett and Gross, 2001; Gross, 2001; Peña-Sarrionandia et al., 2015; Dworkin et al., 2018) are inadequate in type, moment, intensity, or quality (Gross, 2015).
The maladaptive relevance of difficulties in emotion regulation (DER; Fernandez et al., 2016) boosted an integrative and transversal view (Vaz, 2018), covering a wide spectrum of aspects. These include the ability for awareness, clear understanding and acceptance of emotions, impulsivity control, goal-oriented behavior in the presence of negative emotions, as well as the selection and contextualized application of ER strategies (Gratz and Roemer, 2004). When regulatory impairment affects emotional flexibility (Brenning and Braet, 2013), well-being and adaptation are compromised (Veloso et al., 2011), and the implications are not limited to the individual level (Gross, 2001, 2015; Bjureberg et al., 2016).
In this sense, the adjustment of the emotional experience is fundamental (Gross, 1998, 2001; Peña-Sarrionandia et al., 2015). In a dyadic relationship, this adjustment is a bidirectional co-regulatory process (Haase, 2014; Gross, 2015; Horn and Maercker, 2016), positively associated with the couple’s emotional satisfaction and stability (Butler and Randall, 2013; Rusu et al., 2019). Moreover, it has effects on the protection of relationships and the strengthening of emotional connections (Dworkin et al., 2018). In fact, emotional intelligence (EI), which is the ability to perceive and discriminate one’s own and other’s emotions, is a factor of social effectiveness (Salovey and Mayer, 1990; Barrett and Gross, 2001; Mayer et al., 2016). It is also associated with better DA, predicting relational quality and explaining 48% of the variance in DA (Batool and Khalid, 2012).
Multiple studies have demonstrated significant associations (Dworkin et al., 2018; Rusu et al., 2019) and predictive effects of ER on relationship satisfaction (Bloch et al., 2014), highlighting the protective importance of individual ER capacity for relationship quality (Tani et al., 2015). In contrast, DER show significant negative associations with romantic relationship quality and satisfaction (Harrell, 2015; Rick et al., 2017), as well as with intimacy (Constant et al., 2018). Conversely, DER exhibit a positive association with the risk of rupture/divorce (Klein et al., 2016). Specifically, difficulties in acceptance, impulse, awareness, and selection of regulatory strategies reveal negative correlations with the satisfaction dimension of DA (Rick et al., 2017). Furthermore, DER predict fear of emotional involvement, dependency, and control, as well as impact the perception of partner availability, responsiveness, and commitment, making these skills a target of interest for competence training in therapeutic settings (Tani et al., 2015; Clark, 2018, Unpublished).
Final Reflection
Human beings are social beings, insofar as their humanity is built and fulfilled within relationships with other human beings. Romantic relationships establish a foundation for sharing a life project as a couple, combining “freedom, intimacy, and connection” (Coimbra de Matos, 2017, p. 5). Thus, participating in a romantic relationship is a deeply human act and a desire for most people (Bookwala, 2015), which makes it a topic of interest in psychology (Spanier, 1976). As such, this article intended to analyze variables involved in the functioning of romantic relationships, by expanding the focus beyond the assessment of satisfaction and trying to demonstrate how romantic attachment and the existence of DER are related to each other and to the perceived level of DA.
Research has shown that romantic relationships can be thought of from the perspective of the attachment theory (Hazan and Shaver, 1987). According to this theory, the individual relational predisposition can be seen as the result of a legacy that has been built since the first interactions, evolving with personal development (Bowlby, 1969; Pascuzzo et al., 2013). Attachment will, thus, be a relevant factor in the experience of relationships throughout life, in a cooperative parallel with the development of affective regulation processes (Cassidy, 1994; Viddal et al., 2017). Based on these assumptions, we sought to explore the relationships between the difficulties in these processes, RA patterns, and DA.
In this review, we attempted to organize current knowledge about the articulation between our target variables, starting by investigating the relevance of DA as an indicator of romantic relationship quality and viability (Montesino et al., 2013). Indeed, DA is a parameter that translates the coordination and adaptation work inherent to the evolution of a romantic relationship, as perceived by the person who lives it (Kendrick and Drentea, 2016), combining aspects of relationship satisfaction, partner consensus, and cohesion (Spanier, 1976). Therefore, an analysis of DA is a way to overcome the limitations imposed by more restricted focuses, such as the assessment of relationship satisfaction. The latter, although important for the success of the relationship (Bookwala, 2015), provides an individualistic perspective and ignores aspects of putting aside self-interest in favor of partner union. As such, relationship satisfaction represents a reduced (Fowers et al., 2016) and inadequate focus, given the complexity of relationship functioning (Roberson et al., 2018).
Thus, we tried to demonstrate that looking at romantic relationships through the perspective of their DA is an advantageous option, as it allows us to obtain a more comprehensive picture of their functioning (Anderson et al., 2014; Kendrick and Drentea, 2016; Farero et al., 2019). It also combines aspects of gratification and well-being with dimensions of difficulty, including tensions and anxieties that may interfere in partners’ coordinated articulation (Busby et al., 1995; Hernandez, 2008; Farero et al., 2019). This seems to be an essential point with regard to the study of romantic relationships, since any relationship is a permanent and dynamic exchange between partners, in an interaction that, among other facets, includes an important degree of emotional interdependence (Karan et al., 2019; Sels et al., 2020).
Therefore, it became important to explore the fundamental role of emotions in the functioning of romantic relationships, because not only do emotions emerge to signal needs and motivate individual behaviors (Elliott, 2012), but they also interfere, in a relevant way, in relationship dynamics (Haase, 2014; Rusu et al., 2019). The impact of emotional experience on relationships is not limited to the individual level, as it affects both partners (Gross, 2001, 2015; Butler and Randall, 2013; Bjureberg et al., 2016). This brings into focus the importance of understanding the mechanisms through which ER influences the DA of couples in romantic relationships.
In addition, ER is not merely an individual process, since it is often influenced by other processes, especially in romantic relationships (Thompson and Bolger, 1999; Niven et al., 2012; Butler and Randall, 2013). Thus, it is necessary to develop conceptual models that entwine the dynamic and dyadic nature of emotional responses within conjugal relationships. The capacity to identify emotions and to express them, as well as empathize and manage more defiant emotions, is essential for an adaptive and healthy relationship (Cordova et al., 2005), leading to increased emotional processing in both partners, as well as in the interaction of their emotional systems.
Indeed, research has shown that a more effective ER is associated with higher quality romantic relationships (Batool and Khalid, 2012; Dworkin et al., 2018; Rusu et al., 2019). Conversely, these relationships are negatively impacted by any regulatory difficulty (Harrell, 2015; Tani et al., 2015; Klein et al., 2016; Rick et al., 2017).
We were able to conclude that the set of conscious and unconscious processes that allow people to adjust their emotional experience organize patterns reflecting relevant individual differences in the functioning of romantic relationships (Fraley and Hudson, 2017; Fraley and Roisman, 2019). These processes, by dictating the way emotions are regulated (Barrett and Gross, 2001; Gross, 2001; Peña-Sarrionandia et al., 2015) and their expression is driven (Dworkin et al., 2018), determine the emotional responses that arise within relationships. Furthermore, these processes can be affected by multiple and diverse DER, related to awareness, understanding, and acceptance of emotions, as well as their management (Gratz and Roemer, 2004). These difficulties show a tendency to produce extreme results that can be harmful to the relationship. An example of this would be anxious hyperactivation, in which excessive demand of the partner’s attention motivates intense emotional reactions that tend to produce the opposite effect, driving the partner away (Estévez et al., 2018; Overall, 2019). An antipodal example is emotional deactivation, by avoiding the experience and expression of emotions (Brenning and Braet, 2013; Gillath et al., 2016; Winterheld, 2016; Simpson and Rholes, 2017; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2019).
In any event, these unregulated emotional manifestations suggest responses guided by relational representations in which trust is impaired, regarding not only the romantic partner, but also the person’s own role and their expectations concerning the relationship (Gillath et al., 2016; Fraley, 2019; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2019). This perception is in line with what is known about the parallel evolution of regulatory mechanisms and relational patterns, whose construction evolves in the common context of dependency harbored by the primary relationship (Cassidy, 1994; Cole, 2014; Viddal et al., 2017), reflecting the quality of care received as a space for internalization of trust in relationships (Bowlby, 1969; Pascuzzo et al., 2013). Research has been reinforcing the association between DER patterns and more insecure, anxious, or avoidant attachment patterns (Brenning and Braet, 2013; Malik et al., 2015; Gillath et al., 2016; Winterheld, 2016; Simpson and Rholes, 2017; Umemura et al., 2018; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2019), leading to reduced levels of relational quality (Li and Chan, 2012; Muraru and Turliuc, 2012; Rennebohm et al., 2017; Mark et al., 2018; Siegel et al., 2018; Birnbaum and Reis, 2019; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2019).
Hence, it is possible to conclude that these variables and their interactions are of fundamental importance in the understanding of romantic relationship functioning. However, there is still a large gap regarding the knowledge of the mechanisms that connect them (Martins et al., 2016). We understand that each individual enters a romantic relationship with a set of personal skills, expectations, and limitations that condition them, in a more or less voluntary way. These factors imprint trends in their options and reactions that impact the experience, quality, and viability of the romantic relationship. This reveals the importance of deepening the knowledge on these mechanisms, toward better informed development and planning of psychological interventions. The inseparability and interdependence shown between attachment styles and the regulatory dimension, in their joint evolution and articulation, and the consequences for the adjustment of romantic relationships, propel several issues that deserve further study. Namely, the possibility of bidirectional effects, which make each of these processes a potential predictor of the other, and open space for investigating these mutual influences. Moreover, considering the relative rigidness that characterizes attachment patterns, another point of focus could be the more constant functional aspects (Fraley, 2002), with DER standing out as factors of greater potential in the exploration of strategies with possible therapeutic utility (Clark, 2018, Unpublished).
Regarding the analysis of DER, this review was limited to general aspects and global effects. It did not explore particular characteristics, possible relationships between different DER, and the individual relationships of each DER with the different romantic attachment patterns. A detailed exploration of the literature is also absent, as to the specific influence of each DER on relationship quality, whether on general DA, or on the different dimensions of satisfaction, consensus, and cohesion. In addition, the coordination of factors is considered only from an individual perspective. Thus, the depth of aspects related to the couple’s dyadic relationship is open for further study. In addition, longitudinal studies can help to understand the growth and the variable effects during the relationship.
Despite these limitations, we present a summary of the current knowledge on the articulation of important relational variables, RA, DER, and DA, whose study has not been given due attention. This review sheds light on these issues, identifying a diverse set of new targets for future investigation in a domain so central to human functioning, as are romantic relationships.
Author Contributions
MF and VS-G contributed to conception and design of the study. LL wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors wrote sections of the manuscript and contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.
Funding
Financed by national funds of the National Agency for Science and Technology (FCT - Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia, I.P.), under the UID Financing/05749/2020.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Adamczyk, K., and Segrin, C. (2015). Direct and indirect effects of young adults’ relationship status on life satisfaction through loneliness and perceived social support. Psychol. Belg. 55, 196–211. doi: 10.5334/pb.bn
Ainsworth, M. S. (1979). Infant–mother attachment. Am. Psychol. 34, 932–937. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.932
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., and Wall, S. N. (2015). Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. New York: Psychology Press.
Anderson, S., Tambling, R., Huff, S., Heafner, J., Johnson, L., and Ketring, S. (2014). The development of a reliable change index and cutoff for the revised dyadic adjustment scale. J. Marital Fam. Ther. 40, 525–534. doi: 10.1111/jmft.12095
Arriaga, X., Kumashiro, M., Simpson, J., and Overall, N. (2018). Revising workingmodels across time: relationship situations that enhance attachment security. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 22, 71–96. doi: 10.1177/1088868317705257
Attridge, M., Berscheid, E., and Sprecher, S. (1998). Dependency and insecurity in romantic relationships: development and validation of two companion scales. Pers. Relat. 5, 31–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.1998.tb00158.x
Ávila, M., Cabral, J., and Matos, P. (2011). Parental attachment and romantic relationships in emerging adults: the role of emotional regulation processes. Fam. Sci. 2, 13–23. doi: 10.1080/19424620.2011.593342
Barrett, L., and Gross, J. (2001). “Emotional intelligence: a process model of emotion representation and regulation,” in Emotions: Current Issues and Future Directions. eds. T. Mayne and G. Bonanno (New York: Guilford Press), 286–311.
Batool, S., and Khalid, R. (2012). Emotional intelligence: a predictor of marital quality in Pakistani couples. Pak. J. Psychol. Res. 27, 65–88.
Bertoni, A., Iafrate, R., Donato, S., and Rapelli, G. (2020). “Marital adjustment,” in Encyclopedia of Quality of Life and Well-Being Research. ed. F. Maggino (Cham: Springer), 1–5.
Birnbaum, G., and Reis, H. (2019). Evolved to be connected: the dynamics of attachment and sex over the course of romantic relationships. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 25, 11–15. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.005
Bjureberg, J., Ljótsson, B., Tull, M., Hedman, E., Sahlin, H., Lundh, L., et al. (2016). Development and validation of a brief version of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale: the DERS-16. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 38, 284–296. doi: 10.1007/s10862-015-9514-x
Bloch, L., Haase, C., and Levenson, R. (2014). Emotion regulation predicts marital satisfaction: more than a wives’ tale. Emotion 14, 130–144. doi: 10.1037/a0034272
Bookwala, J. (2015). “Marital relationships and quality,” in The Encyclopedia of Adulthood and Aging (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc), 1–5.
Bowlby, J. (1982). Formação e Rompimento dos laços Afetivos [Development and Breaking of Affective Bonds]. São Paulo: Martins Fontes.
Braithwaite, S., and Holt-Lunstad, J. (2017). Romantic relationships and mental health. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 13, 120–125. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.04.001
Brandão, T., Matias, M., Ferreira, T., Vieira, J., Schulz, M., and Matos, P. (2019). Attachment, emotion regulation, and well-being in couples: intrapersonal and interpersonal associations. J. Pers. 88, 748–761. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12523
Brenning, K., and Braet, C. (2013). The emotion regulation model of attachment: an emotion-specific approach. Pers. Relat. 20, 107–123. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2012.01399.x
Busby, D., Christensen, C., Crane, D., and Larson, J. (1995). A revision of the dyadic adjustment scale for use with distressed and nondistressed couples: construct hierarchy and multidimensional scales. J. Marital Fam. Ther. 21, 289–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1752-0606.1995.tb00163.x
Butler, E., and Randall, A. (2013). Emotional coregulation in close relationships. Emot. Rev. 5, 202–210. doi: 10.1177/1754073912451630
Campbell, L., and Stanton, S. C. (2019). Adult attachment and trust in romantic relationships. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 25, 148–151. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.08.004
Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: influences of attachment relationships. J. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 59, 228–249. doi: 10.2307/1166148
Cho, M., Impett, E., Campos, B., Chen, S., and Keltner, D. (2020). Socioeconomic inequality undermines relationship quality in romantic relationships. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 37, 1722–1742. doi: 10.1177/0265407520907969
Cohen, S., Schulz, M., Weiss, E., and Waldinger, R. (2012). Eye of the beholder: the individual and dyadic contributions of empathic accuracy and perceived empathic effort to relationship satisfaction. J. Fam. Psychol. 26, 236–245. doi: 10.1037/a0027488
Cole, P. (2014). Moving ahead in the study of the development of emotion regulation. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 38, 203–207. doi: 10.1177/0165025414522170
Consoli, N., Bernardes, J., and Marin, A. (2018). Laços de afeto: as repercussões do estilo de apego primário e estabelecido entre casais no ajustamento conjugal [Ties of affection: the repercussions of the primary and that attachment style among couples on marital adjustment]. Avan. Psicol. Latin. 36, 315–329. doi: 10.12804/revistas.urosario.edu.co/apl/a.5409
Constant, E., Christophe, V., Bodenmann, G., and Nandrino, J. (2018). Attachment orientation and relational intimacy: the mediating role of emotional competences. Curr. Psychol. 40, 1374–1385. doi: 10.1007/s12144-018-0062-x
Cordova, J., Gee, C., and Warren, L. (2005). Emotional skilfulness in marriage: intimacy as a mediator of the relationship between emotional skilfulness and marital satisfaction. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 24, 218–235. doi: 10.1521/jscp.24.2.218.62270
Costa, C., Cenci, C., and Mosmann, C. (2016). Conflito conjugal e estratégias de resolução: uma revisão sistemática da literatura [Marital conflict and resolution strategies: a systematic literature review]. Tem. Psicol. 24, 325–338. doi: 10.9788/TP2016.1-22
Davila, J., Mattanah, J., Bhatia, V., Latack, J., Feinstein, B., Eaton, N., et al. (2017). Romantic competence, healthy relationship functioning, and well-being in emerging adults. Pers. Relat. 24, 162–184. doi: 10.1111/pere.12175
Doss, B., and Rhoades, G. (2017). The transition to parenthood: impact on couples’ romantic relationships. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 13, 25–28. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.04.003
Dworkin, J., Zimmerman, V., Waldinger, R., and Schulz, M. (2018). Capturing naturally occurring emotional suppression as it unfolds in couple interactions. Emotion 19, 1224–1235. doi: 10.1037/emo0000524
Eastwick, P., Keneski, E., Morgan, T., McDonald, M., and Huang, S. (2018). What do short-term and long-term relationships look like? Building the relationship coordination and strategic timing (ReCAST) model. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 147, 747–781. doi: 10.1037/xge0000428
Elliott, R. (2012). “Emotion-focused therapy,” in The Tribes of the Person-Centred Nation: An Introduction to the Schools of Therapy Related to the Person-Centred Approach. ed. P. Sanders (Ross-on-Wye: PCCS Books), 103–130.
Espeleta, H., Palasciano-Barton, S., and Messman-Moore, T. (2017). The impact of child abuse severity on adult attachment anxiety and avoidance in college women: the role of emotion dysregulation. J. Fam. Violence 32, 399–407. doi: 10.1007/s10896-016-9816-0
Estévez, A., Chávez-Vera, M., Momeñe, J., Olave, L., Vázquez, D., and Iruarrizaga, I. (2018). The role of emotional dependence in the relationship between attachment and impulsive behavior. Ann. Psychol. 34, 438–445. doi: 10.6018/analesps.34.3.313681
Etkin, A., Büchel, C., and Gross, J. (2015). The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 693–700. doi: 10.1038/nrn4044
Farero, A., Bowles, R., Blow, A., Ufer, L., Kees, M., and Guty, D. (2019). Rasch analysis of the revised dyadic adjustment scale (RDAS) with military couples. Contemp. Fam. Ther. 41, 125–134. doi: 10.1007/s10591-018-09486-2
Fernandez, K., Jazaieri, H., and Gross, J. (2016). Emotion regulation: a transdiagnostic perspective on a new RDoC domain. Cognit. Ther. Res. 40, 426–440. doi: 10.1007/s10608-016-9772-2
Fincham, F., Rogge, R., and Beach, S. (2018). “Relationship satisfaction,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Personal Relationships. eds. A. Vangelisti and D. Perlman (New York: Cambridge University Press), 422–436.
Finkel, E., Simpson, J., and Eastwick, P. (2017). The psychology of close relationships: fourteen core principles. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 68, 383–411. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044038
Fowers, B., Laurenceau, J., Penfield, R., Cohen, L., Lang, S., Owenz, M., et al. (2016). Enhancing relationship quality measurement: the development of the relationship flourishing scale. J. Fam. Psychol. 30, 997–1007. doi: 10.1037/fam0000263
Fox, J., and Tokunaga, R. (2015). Romantic partner monitoring after breakups: attachment, dependence, distress, and post-dissolution online surveillance via social networking sites. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 18, 491–498. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2015.0123
Fraley, R. (2002). Attachment stability from infancy to adulthood: meta-analysis and dynamic modeling of developmental mechanisms. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 6, 123–151. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0602_03
Fraley, R. (2019). Attachment in adulthood: recent developments, emerging debates, and future directions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 70, 401–422. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-102813
Fraley, R. C., and Hudson, N. W. (2017). “The development of attachment styles,” in Personality Development Across the Lifespan. Vol. 17. ed. J. Specht (London: Elsevier Academic Press), 275–292.
Fraley, R., and Roisman, G. (2019). The development of adult attachment styles: four lessons. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 25, 26–30. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.008
Freeman, H., and Simons, J. (2017). Attachment network structure as a predictor of romantic attachment formation and insecurity. Soc. Dev. 27, 201–220. doi: 10.1111/sode.12265
Furman, W., and Collibee, C. (2018). The past is present: representations of parents, friends, and romantic partners predict subsequent romantic representations. Child Dev. 89, 188–204. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12712
Galaviz, S. (2019). The Relation between Attachment Styles and Love Life Narratives. Master’s Capstone Project, University of California. eScholarship.
Gardner, A., Zimmer-Gembeck, M., and Campbell, S. (2019). Attachment and emotion regulation: a person-centred examination and relations with coping with rejection, friendship closeness, and emotional adjustment. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 38, 125–143. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12310
Gillath, O., Karantzas, G., and Fraley, R. (2016). Adult Attachment: A Concise Introduction to Theory and Research. Londres: Academic Press.
Girme, Y., Agnew, C., VanderDrift, L., Harvey, S., Rholes, W., and Simpson, J. (2018). The ebbs and flows of attachment: within-person variation in attachment undermine secure individuals’ relationship wellbeing across time. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 114, 397–421. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000115
Godbout, N., Daspe, M., Lussier, Y., Sabourin, S., Dutton, D., and Hébert, M. (2017). Early exposure to violence, relationship violence, and relationship satisfaction in adolescents and emerging adults: the role of romantic attachment. Psychol. Trauma 9, 127–137. doi: 10.1037/tra0000136
Gomez, R., and Leal, I. (2008). Ajustamento conjugal: características psicométricas da versão Portuguesa da dyadic adjustment scale [Marital adjustment: psychometric characteristics of the portuguese version of the dyadic adjustment scale]. Anál. Psicol. 26, 625–638. doi: 10.14417/ap.522
Goodall, K. (2015). Individual differences in the regulation of positive emotion: the role of attachment and self-esteem. Pers. Individ. Differ. 74, 208–213. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.10.033
Grandey, A., and Melloy, R. (2017). The state of the heart: emotional labor as emotion regulation reviewed and revised. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 22, 407–422. doi: 10.1037/ocp0000067
Gratz, K., and Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 26, 41–54. doi: 10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94
Groh, A., Roisman, G., Booth-LaForce, C., Fraley, R., Owen, M., Cox, M., et al. (2014). Stability of attachment security from infancy to late adolescence. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 79, 51–66. doi: 10.1111/mono.12113
Gross, J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 271–299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271
Gross, J. (2001). Emotion regulation in adulthood: timing is everything. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 10, 214–219. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00152
Gross, J. (2015). Emotion regulation: current status and future prospects. Psychol. Inq. 26, 1–26. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781
Gustavson, K., Røysamb, E., Borren, I., Torvik, F. A., and Karevold, E. (2016). Life satisfaction in close relationships: findings from a longitudinal study. J. Happiness Stud. 17, 1293–1311. doi: 10.1007/s10902-015-9643-7
Guzmán-González, M., Carrasco, N., Figueroa, P., Trabucco, C., and Vilca, D. (2016). Estilos de apego y dificultades de regulación emocional en estudiantes universitarios [Attachment styles and emotional regulation difficulties among university students]. Psykhe 25, 1–13. doi: 10.7764/psykhe.25.1.706
Harrell, M. (2015). The effects of deficits in emotional self-regulation on relationship satisfaction in young adults. Master’s thesis, University of South Carolina. USC Aiken Psychology Theses.
Hazan, C., and Selcuk, E. (2015). “Normative processes in romantic attachment: introduction and overview,” in Bases of Adult Attachment. eds. V. Zayas and C. Hazan (New York: Springer), 3–8.
Hazan, C., and Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52, 511–524. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.3.511
Hazan, C., and Shaver, P. (1994). Attachment as an organizational framework for research on close relationships. Psychol. Inq. 5, 1–22. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0501_1
Hernandez, J. (2008). Avaliação estrutural da escala de ajustamento diádico [Structural assessment of the dyadic adjustment scale]. Psicol. Estud. 13, 593–601. doi: 10.1590/S1413-73722008000300021
Horn, A., and Maercker, A. (2016). Intra- and interpersonal emotion regulation and adjustment symptoms in couples: the role of co-brooding and co-reappraisal. BMC Psychol. 4:51. doi: 10.1186/s40359-016-0159-7
Joel, S., Plaks, J., and MacDonald, G. (2019). Nothing ventured, nothing gained: people anticipate more regret from missed romantic opportunities than from rejection. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 36, 305–336. doi: 10.1177/0265407517729567
Karan, A., Rosenthal, R., and Robbins, M. (2019). Meta-analytic evidence that we-talk predicts relationship and personal functioning in romantic couples. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 36, 2624–2651. doi: 10.1177/0265407518795336
Kawamichi, H., Sugawara, S., Hamano, Y., Makita, K., Matsunaga, M., Tanabe, H., et al. (2016). Being in a romantic relationship is associated with reduced gray matter density in striatum and increased subjective happiness. Front. Psychol. 7:1763. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01763
Kendrick, H., and Drentea, P. (2016). “Marital adjustment,” in The Wiley Blackwell Encyclopedia of Family Studies (Hoboken, New Jersey: Wiley Blackwell), 1–2.
Klein, S., Renshaw, K., and Curby, T. (2016). Emotion regulation and perceptions of hostile and constructive criticism in romantic relationships. Behav. Ther. 47, 143–154. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2015.10.007
Li, T., and Chan, D. (2012). How anxious and avoidant attachment affect romantic relationship quality differently: a meta-analytic review. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 42, 406–419. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1842
Limeira, M., and Féres-Carneiro, T. (2019). O papel do perdão na reconciliação: Uma análise qualitativa das repercussões no recasamento com o ex-cônjuge [The role of forgiveness in reconciliation: a qualitative analysis of the repercussions on remarriage with the ex-spouse]. Cont. Clín. 12, 822–842. doi: 10.4013/ctc.2019.123.06
Malik, S., Wells, A., and Wittkowski, A. (2015). Emotion regulation as a mediator in the relationship between attachment and depressive symptomatology: a systematic review. J. Affect. Disord. 172, 428–444. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.10.007
Mark, K., Vowels, L., and Murray, S. (2018). The impact of attachment style on sexual satisfaction and sexual desire in a sexually diverse sample. J. Sex Marital Ther. 44, 450–458. doi: 10.1080/0092623X.2017.1405310
Martins, T., Canavarro, M., and Moreira, H. (2016). Adult attachment and dyadic adjustment: the mediating role of shame. J. Psychol. 150, 560–575. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2015.1114461
Maxwell, J., and McNulty, J. (2019). No longer in a dry spell: the developing understanding of how sex influences romantic relationships. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 28, 102–107. doi: 10.1177/0963721418806690
Mayer, J., Caruso, D., and Salovey, P. (2016). The ability model of emotional intelligence: principles and updates. Emot. Rev. 8, 290–300. doi: 10.1177/1754073916639667
McConnell, M., and Moss, E. (2011). Attachment across the life span: factors that contribute to stability and change. Aust. J. Educ. Dev. Psychol. 11, 60–77.
Meltzer, A., Makhanova, A., Hicks, L., French, J., McNulty, J., and Bradbury, T. (2017). Quantifying the sexual afterglow: the lingering benefits of sex and their implications for pair-bonded relationships. Psychol. Sci. 28, 587–598. doi: 10.1177/0956797617691361
Merrill, S. M. (2018). An exploration of the transition from romantic infatuation to adult attachment (Doctor’s Thesis). Cornell University: Nova Iorque, E.U.A.
Meyers, S., and Landsberger, S. (2002). Direct and indirect pathways between adult attachment style and marital satisfaction. Pers. Relat. 9, 159–172. doi: 10.1111/1475-6811.00010
Mikulincer, M., and Shaver, P. (2019). Attachment orientations and emotion regulation. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 25, 6–10. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.006
Montesino, M., Gómez, J., Fernández, M., and Rodríguez, J. (2013). Psychometric properties of the dyadic adjustment scale (DAS) in a community sample of couples. Psicothema 25, 536–541. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2013.85
Mota, S. (2018). Vinculação parental e vinculação amorosa: Um estudo com jovens adultos [Parental attachment and loving attachment: A study with young adults]. Master’s thesis, ISPA—Instituto Universitário. Repositório do ISPA.
Muise, A., Maxwell, J., and Impett, E. (2018). What theories and methods from relationship research can contribute to sex research. J. Sex Res. 55, 540–562. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2017.1421608
Muraru, A., and Turliuc, M. (2012). Family-of-origin, romantic attachment, and marital adjustment: a path analysis model. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 33, 90–94. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.01.089
Niven, K., Macdonald, I., and Holman, D. (2012). You spin me right round: cross-relationship variability in interpersonal emotion regulation. Front. Psychol. 3:394. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00394
Overall, N. (2019). Attachment insecurity and power regulation in intimate relationships. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 25, 53–58. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.03.004
Overall, N., and Cross, E. (2019). “Attachment insecurity and the regulation of power and dependence in intimate relationships,” in Power in Close Relationships. eds. C. Agnew and J. Harmon (New York: Cambridge University Press), 28–54.
Panksepp, J., Clarici, A., Vandekerckhove, M., and Yovell, Y. (2019). Neuro-evolutionary foundations of infant minds: from psychoanalytic visions of how primal emotions guide constructions of human minds toward affective neuroscientific understanding of emotions and their disorders. Psychoanal. Inq. 39, 36–51. doi: 10.1080/07351690.2019.1549910
Pascuzzo, K., Cyr, C., and Moss, E. (2013). Longitudinal association between adolescent attachment, adult romantic attachment, and emotion regulation strategies. Attach. Hum. Dev. 15, 83–103. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2013.745713
Peña-Sarrionandia, A., Mikolajczak, M., and Gross, J. (2015). Integrating emotion regulation and emotional intelligence traditions: a meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 6:160. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00160
Pinquart, M., Feußner, C., and Ahnert, L. (2013). Meta-analytic evidence for stability in attachments from infancy to early adulthood. Attach. Hum. Dev. 15, 189–218. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2013.746257
Pordata (2020). Divórcios. Available at: https://www.pordata.pt/Portugal/Divórcios-323 (Accessed June 1, 2021).
Porreca, W. (2019). Relação conjugal: desafios e possibilidades do “nós” [Marital relationship: challenges and possibilities of the “we”]. Psicol. Teor. Pesq. 35:e35nspe7. doi: 10.1590/0102.3772e35nspe7
Rani, R., Singh, L., and Jaiswal, A. (2017). Relationship between emotional maturity and marital adjustment among couples. Indian J. Health Wellbeing 8, 1020–1023.
Regan, P. C., Kocan, E. R., and Whitlock, T. (1998). Ain't love grand! A prototype analysis of the concept of romantic love. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 15, 411–420. doi: 10.1177/0265407598153006
Rennebohm, S. B., Seebeck, J., and Thoburn, J. W. (2017). Attachment, dyadic adjustment, and social interest: an indirect effects model. J. Individ. Psychol. 73, 208–224. doi: 10.1353/jip.2017.0017
Rick, J. L., Falconier, M. K., and Wittenborn, A. K. (2017). Emotion regulation dimensions and relationship satisfaction in clinical couples. Pers. Relat. 24, 790–803. doi: 10.1111/pere.12213
Roberson, P., Norona, J., Lenger, K., and Olmstead, S. (2018). How do relationship stability and quality affect wellbeing?: romantic relationship trajectories, depressive symptoms, and life satisfaction across 30 years. J. Child Fam. Stud. 27, 2171–2184. doi: 10.1007/s10826-018-1052-1
Rogge, R., Fincham, F., Crasta, D., and Maniaci, M. (2017). Positive and negative evaluation of relationships: development and validation of the positive–negative relationship quality (PN-RQ) scale. Psychol. Assess. 29, 1028–1043. doi: 10.1037/pas0000392
Rosado, J., Barbosa, P., and Wagner, A. (2016). Ajustamento conjugal: a função das características individuais, do casal e do contexto [Marital adjustment: The function of individual characteristics, the couple and the context.]. Rev.Psicol. Pesq. 10, 26–33. doi: 10.24879/201600100010044
Rosado, J., and Wagner, A. (2015). Qualidade, ajustamento e satisfação conjugal: revisão sistemática da literatura [Quality, adjustment and marital satisfaction: systematic literature review]. Pensando Fam. 19, 21–33.
Røsand, G., Slinning, K., Røysamb, E., and Tambs, K. (2014). Relationship dissatisfaction and other risk factors for future relationship dissolution: a population-based study of 18,523 couples. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 49, 109–119. doi: 10.1007/s00127-013-0681-3
Rusu, P., Bodenmann, G., and Kayser, K. (2019). Cognitive emotion regulation and positive dyadic outcomes in married couples. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 36, 359–376. doi: 10.1177/0265407517751664
Salovey, P., and Mayer, J. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagin. Cogn. Pers. 9, 185–211. doi: 10.2190/DUGG-P24E-52WK-6CDG
Scorsolini-Comin, F., and dos Santos, M. (2012). Ajustamento diádico e satisfação conjugal: correlações entre os domínios de duas escalas de avaliação da conjugalidad [Dyadic adjustment and marital satisfaction: correlations among the domains of two marital evaluation scales]. Psicol. Reflex. Crít. 24, 439–447. doi: 10.1590/S0102-79722011000300007
Sels, L., Cabrieto, J., Butler, E., Reis, H., Ceulemans, E., and Kuppens, P. (2020). The occurrence and correlates of emotional interdependence in romantic relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 119, 136–158. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000212
Shallcross, S., and Simpson, J. (2012). Trust and responsiveness in strain-test situations: a dyadic perspective. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 102, 1031–1044. doi: 10.1037/a0026829
Siegel, A., Levin, Y., and Solomon, Z. (2018). The role of attachment of each partner on marital adjustment. J. Fam. Issues 40, 415–434. doi: 10.1177/0192513X18812005
Simpson, J., Collins, W., Farrell, A., and Raby, K. (2015). “Attachment and relationships across time: an organizational-developmental perspective,” in Bases of Adult Attachment. eds. V. Zayas and C. Hazan (New York: Springer), 61–78.
Simpson, J., and Rholes, W. (2017). Adult attachment, stress, and romantic relationships. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 13, 19–24. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.04.006
Slatcher, R., and Selcuk, E. (2017). A social psychological perspective on the links between close relationships and health. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 26, 16–21. doi: 10.1177/0963721416667444
Snyder, D., and Halford, W. (2012). Evidence-based couple therapy: current status and future directions. J. Fam. Ther. 34, 229–249. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6427.2012.00599.x
Spanier, G. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: new scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. J. Marriage Fam. 38, 15–28. doi: 10.2307/350547
Spielmann, S., MacDonald, G., Maxwell, J., Joel, S., Peragine, D., Muise, A., et al. (2013). Settling for less out of fear of being single. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 105, 1049–1073. doi: 10.1037/a0034628
Sroufe, L. (2016). “The place of attachment in development,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. 3rd Edn. eds. J. Cassidy and P. Shaver (New York: The Guilford Press), 997–1011.
Sutton, T. (2019). Review of attachment theory: familial predictors, continuity and change, and intrapersonal and relational outcomes. Marriage Fam. Rev. 55, 1–22. doi: 10.1080/01494929.2018.1458001
Tani, F., Pascuzzi, D., and Raffagnino, R. (2015). Emotion regulation and quality of close relationship: the effects of emotion dysregulation processes on couple intimacy. Appl. Psychol. Bull. 272, 3–15.
Taylor, P., Rietzschel, J., Danquah, A., and Berry, K. (2015). Changes in attachment representations during psychological therapy. Psychother. Res. 25, 222–238. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2014.886791
Theisen, J., Fraley, R., Hankin, B., Young, J., and Chopik, W. (2018). How do attachment styles change from childhood through adolescence? Findings from an accelerated longitudinal cohort study. J. Res. Pers. 74, 141–146. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2018.04.001
Thompson, A., and Bolger, N. (1999). Emotional transmission in couples under stress. J. Marriage Fam. 61, 38–48. doi: 10.2307/353881
Umemura, T., Lacinová, L., Kotrčová, K., and Fraley, R. (2018). Similarities and differences regarding changes in attachment preferences and attachment styles in relation to romantic relationship length: longitudinal and concurrent analyses. Attach. Hum. Dev. 20, 135–159. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2017.1383488
Van Ryzin, M., Carlson, E., and Sroufe, L. (2011). Attachment discontinuity in a high-risk sample. Attach. Hum. Dev. 13, 381–401. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2011.584403
Vaz, F. (2018). Emotional processes in psychological adjustment and disorder: their characteristics and evolution during the therapeutic process. Doctoral dissertation, Universidade de Lisboa. Repositório da Universidade de Lisboa.
Veloso, M., Gouveia, J., and Dinis, A. (2011). Estudos de validação com a versão Portuguesa da escala de dificuldades na regulação emocional (EDRE) [Validation studies with the Portuguese version of the scale of difficulties in emotional regulation]. Psychol. Forsch. 54, 87–110. doi: 10.14195/1647-8606_54_4
Viddal, K., Berg-Nielsen, T., Belsky, J., and Wichstrøm, L. (2017). Change in attachment predicts change in emotion regulation particularly among 5-HTTLPR short-allele homozygotes. Dev. Psychol. 53, 1316–1329. doi: 10.1037/dev0000321
Waddell, N., Sibley, C., and Osborne, D. (2019). Better off alone? Ambivalent sexism moderates the association between relationship status and life satisfaction among heterosexual women and men. Sex Roles 80, 347–361. doi: 10.1007/s11199-018-0935-3
Waldinger, R., and Schulz, M. (2016). The long reach of nurturing family environments. Psychol. Sci. 27, 1443–1450. doi: 10.1177/0956797616661556
Wang, F., Edwards, K., and Hill, P. (2017). Humility as a relational virtue: establishing trust, empowering repair, and building marital well-being. J. Psychol. Christ. 36, 168–179.
Warach, B., and Josephs, L. (2019). The aftershocks of infidelity: a review of infidelity-based attachment trauma. Sex. Relat. Ther. 36, 68–90. doi: 10.1080/14681994.2019.1577961
Weiser, D., and Weigel, D. (2016). Self-efficacy in romantic relationships: direct and indirect effects on relationship maintenance and satisfaction. Pers. Individ. Differ. 89, 152–156. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.10.013
Wiebe, S., Johnson, S., Lafontaine, M., Moser, M. B., Dalgleish, T., and Tasca, G. (2017). Two-year follow-up outcomes in emotionally focused couple therapy: an investigation of relationship satisfaction and attachment trajectories. J. Marital Fam. Ther. 43, 227–244. doi: 10.1111/jmft.12206
Winterheld, H. (2016). Calibrating use of emotion regulation strategies to the relationship context: an attachment perspective. J. Pers. 84, 369–380. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12165
Zeifman, D. (2019). Attachment theory grows up: a developmental approach to pair bonds. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 25, 139–143. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.06.001
Zeifman, D., and Hazan, C. (2016). “Pair bonds as attachments: mounting evidence in support of Bowlby's hypothesis,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. 3rd Edn. eds. J. Cassidy and P. Shaver (New York: The Guilford Press), 416–434.
Keywords: romantic relationships, romantic attachment, difficulties in emotional regulation, dyadic adjustment, literature review
Citation: Fávero M, Lemos L, Moreira D, Ribeiro FN and Sousa-Gomes V (2021) Romantic Attachment and Difficulties in Emotion Regulation on Dyadic Adjustment: A Comprehensive Literature Review. Front. Psychol. 12:723823. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.723823
Edited by:
Luciana Karine de Souza, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, BrazilReviewed by:
Marina Zanella Delatorre, Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul, BrazilGiulia Radi, University of Perugia, Italy
Copyright © 2021 Fávero, Lemos, Moreira, Ribeiro and Sousa-Gomes. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Marisalva Fávero, bWZhdmVyb0Bpc21haS5wdA==