Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 05 July 2021
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Common Threats and Cooperation: Recent Perspectives and Avenues Forward View all 3 articles

Perceived Level of Threat and Cooperation

  • 1Coller Scool of Management, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
  • 2Department of Economics, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva, Israel

1. Introduction

The evolution of parochial altruism goes hand in hand with intergroup conflict. Helping other group members is evolutionary stable in the presence of an outside threat (Bowles et al., 2003; Guzmán et al., 2007), and hostility toward other groups can evolve together with parochial altruism (Choi and Bowles, 2007; Bowles, 2008). Furthermore, group reputation fosters cooperation with fellow group members in times of conflict—even in an environment that does not foster cooperation in times of peace (Hugh-Jones and Zultan, 2013).

Social scientists have long documented that intergroup conflict increases intragroup cooperation (Sumner, 1906; Williams, 1947; Simmel, 1955; Coser, 1956). Over a century ago, Sumner (1906) wrote that “the exigencies of war with outsiders are what makes peace inside.” Indeed, prosocial behaviors, such as volunteering and blood donations, increase during times of war or exposure to terror attacks (Schmiedeberg, 1942; Janis, 1951; Glynn et al., 2003; Penner et al., 2005; Steinberg and Rooney, 2005; Gneezy and Fessler, 2012; Berrebi and Yonah, 2016). This phenomenon can be reproduced in experimental settings, either under experimentally induced external threat to the group (Wright, 1943; Feshbach and Singer, 1957; Sherif, 1961, 1966; Burnstein and McRae, 1962; Hargreaves-Heap and Varoufakis, 2002), or in times of interstate conflict (Gneezy and Fessler, 2012). Democratic leaders are also apparently aware of this phenomenon, often dubbed the common enemy effect, as they are more likely to initiate interstate conflict at times of internal unrest or threatened leadership (Sirin, 2011).

In this paper, we argue that outside threat has the capacity to both increase and decrease intragroup cooperation. We propose that the crucial psychological variable that determines the response to outside threat is the level at which threat is perceived and construed. Outside threat increases cooperation only if it menaces the group as a whole (Williams, 1947). As intergroup conflict poses a threat both to the group as a whole and to individual group members, the same threat can trigger different—and even opposing—responses, depending on how it is perceived. These perceptions are sensitive to the duration and intensity of the conflict, media coverage, and the salience of various aspects of intergroup conflict.

This Perceived Target of Threat principle can be summed thus: individuals who perceive the group to be under threat help the group, whereas an individual who perceives himself to be under threat helps himself (Weisel and Zultan, 2016). In the following, we review empirical support for this principle and discuss its psychological antecedents.

2. Mobilization in Intergroup Conflict

The most direct effect of outside threat on intra-group cooperation is on mobilization in the context of the intergroup conflict. The perception of the group being attacked leads to the well-known “rally around the flag” effect, namely an increase in support for political incumbents and military action (Mueller, 1970, 1973; Brody, 1991; Baker and Oneal, 2001; Newman and Forcehimes, 2010).

Paradoxically, however, while attacks on the group generate support for military action, with time, ingroup casualties lead people to withdraw support (Mueller, 1973; Gartner and Segura, 1998; Kuijpers, 2019). This effect is often interpreted as reflecting a rational cost-benefit calculation, with ingroup casualties seen as costs of war (Larson, 1996; Gartner, 2008a; Gelpi et al., 2009). In this view, casualties oppose, rather than moderate, the “rally around the flag” effect.

Public opinion, however, mainly reacts to local—rather than national—casualties (Gartner et al., 1997; Kriner and Shen, 2012, 2014). Furthermore, the effect of casualties decays rapidly, and is strongest in individuals who are less attentive to national and local news—where a possible explanation is that such individuals are more likely to experience the casualties information in a more personal way (Althaus et al., 2012). Most illustrative is the finding that while the events of 9/11 lead to public support for president Bush and enabled the war in Iraq, personal ties to 9/11 casualties were negatively correlated with presidential support (Gartner, 2008b). Thus—notwithstanding the acknowledged merit of the cost-benefit view—the reaction to casualties is more visceral than a cold weighing of costs.

The perceived target of threat principle provides a unifying explanation for the apparent contradiction between the “rally around the flag” and the casualties effect. We suggest that causalities shift perceptions from the group-level to the individual-level threat, thus fostering opposition to the conflict. Put in context, if an individual perceives her group to be under attack, as did most Americans during the 9/11 attacks, she will display parochial altruism and be mobilized to participate in and support the conflict effort. On the other hand, a more personal perception of the attack as threatening oneself—which evolves over time and with personal experience of casualties—is associated with a feeling of insecurity and leads the individual to withdraw support from group efforts (Gartner, 2008a,b).

3. Natural Threats

Some evolutionary arguments for cooperation in conflict do not extend to natural disasters (Hugh-Jones and Zultan, 2013; De Jaegher and Hoyer, 2016). When Barclay and Benard (2020) introduced external threat to a public goods game, cooperation was higher when the threat was framed as social (vs. asocial). Nonetheless, empirical studies suggest that—similar to intergroup conflict—natural disasters increase shared group identity, trust, and willingness to help (Pena et al., 2014; Vezzali et al., 2015; Drury et al., 2016; Maki et al., 2019; Lee, 2020, 2021). Post-disaster social reactions typically follow two distinct paths. Initial responses to disaster are characterized by mobilization of social support as communities cooperate in mutual help and protection. With time, social responses shift to disillusionment, and social support deteriorates (Kaniasty and Norris, 2004; Kaniasty, 2020).

We suggest that these dynamics can be viewed to go hand in hand with a shift in the perceived level of threat. A number of observations are in line with this interpretation. Tilcsik and Marquis (2013) found that, whereas small-scale disasters have a positive effect on local firms' philanthropic spending, major disasters have a negative effect. Vardy and Atkinson (2019) found, using laboratory dictator games, that exposure to people harmed by a cyclone increased prosociality, while incurring personal property damage had a negative effect on prosociality. These effects are consistent with the view that proximity and scale increase the likelihood of being affected personally by the disaster and shifting the perception of threat to the individual level, thereby reducing social cohesiveness in the community.

4. Experimental Team Games

Field research on mobilization in conflict and on responses to natural threats provides suggestive evidence in support of the principle of perceived level of threat. The observed behavioral patterns, however, are correlational in nature and open to different interpretations and multiple explanations. In order to isolate the perceived level of threat, we conducted laboratory experiments using team games, where the perceived level of threat can be directly manipulated in order to establish its causal effect (Weisel and Zultan,, in press; Weisel and Zultan, 2016).

Experimental team games provide a laboratory model for studying intergroup conflict (Bornstein, 2003). Bornstein and Ben-Yossef (1994) provided experimental support for increased cooperation in conflict using the Intergroup Prisoner's Dilemma (IPD) game introduced by (Bornstein, 1992). As in the Prisoner's dilemma (PD) game, players can cooperate with the group by contributing to help their ingroup members. Cooperation, however, is modeled after mobilization in intergroup conflict in that it inflicts a loss on outgroup members. Figure 1 depicts the 3-player PD and the 6-player (two groups) IPD.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The IPD and PD games. Contributions are made to a group account in the PD and to a conflict account in the IPD. Panel (A) presents the payoff to the player based on the total contributions (columns) and her own contribution (rows). Panel (B) presents the same games as the change in payoff based on all players' choices between contributing and investing in a private account compared to a base endowment of 14. The player faces a choice between a private account and a group account. In the IPD, the choice is between a private account and a conflict account. The labels “Group A” and “Group B” refer to the asymmetric game that combines features of the PD and the IPD.

Because the intragroup structure is identical in the PD and the IPD, the comparison of cooperation levels in the two games provides a clean test for the effect of intergroup conflict on intragroup cooperation. Bornstein and Ben-Yossef (1994) found higher cooperation levels in the IPD (54.7%) compared to the PD (27.2%). This result has been replicated in several studies, and in some was moderated by personality type (Probst et al., 1999; Baron, 2001; Weisel and Zultan, 2016).

The studies that compared cooperation in the IPD and in the PD, such as Bornstein and Ben-Yossef (1994), framed the effects of contributions in the outgroup as a threat to the group as a whole. The typical instructions present the players' payoffs as a result of an explicit comparison of the sum of contributions in each of the two groups, hence the groups compete for a common group resource (see Figure 1A). Weisel and Zultan (2016) altered the instructions to shift the threat to the individual. A player's payoff was no longer based on a comparison of the total contributions made by the two groups, but instead increased with each contribution in the ingroup and decreased with each contribution in the outgroup (see Figure 1B). The actions of the outgroup now pose a threat to the individual, detached from the parallel effect on the other ingroup members. This variation in experimental instructions thus manipulates the perception of level of threat.

As in previous studies with the standard instructions, conflict increased cooperation. The effect was small overall, but strong and significant among pro-socials, with 55% contributions in the IPD compared to 33% in the PD. In the individual level condition, cooperation levels decreased with conflict from 61 to 31%. Note that the manipulation alters not only perception of conflict as collective threat, but also as collective action in the competition for resources with the outgroup. The experimental design addressed this issue by considering an asymmetric game, in which contributions in Group B harmed Group A members, but not vice versa (cf. Figure 1). Consequently, the manipulation affects the collective threat aspect in Group A and the collective action aspect in Group B. The manipulation in the instructions affected behavior in Group A as it did behavior in the symmetric IPD while having no significant effect on Group B, leading to the conclusion that the perception of collective threat—and not collective action—drives behavior (cf. De Dreu et al., 2016; De Dreu and Gross, 2019).

In the IPD game of Weisel and Zultan, (in press), participants allocated 10 tokens between private and conflict accounts. Similar to the binary version of the game, cooperation level increased from 38% under individual threat to 48% under group threat. Half of the groups communicated by electronic chat before making decisions. Independent judges rated whether cooperating in conflict, as reflected in the chats, was due to (a) competing with the outgroup; (b) defending the ingroup; (c) helping the ingroup; or (d) harming the outgroup.

We regressed cooperation levels (sum of tokens allocated to the conflict account by all group members) on the four reasons interacted with level of threat. In both conditions, discussions of helping the ingroup are strongly correlated with cooperation levels, whereas discussions of harming or competing with the outgroup do not correlate with cooperation. As could be expected, in the group threat condition, discussions of defending the ingroup are correlated with high cooperation levels. This pattern reverses under individual level threat. Here, groups who cooperated more tended to discuss defending the ingroup less than groups who cooperated less. The differing patterns between the two conditions clearly establish that changing the perceived level of threat fundamentally shifts the way in which people think of cooperation in conflict.

5. Discussion

Several observations suggest that a major factor in determining whether outside threat promotes or hinders ingroup cooperation is the extent of direct exposure to threat. More direct relation to casualties reverses the rally around the flag effect (Gartner, 2008b), and negative reactions to disaster are observed in proximity to inflicted harm (Tilcsik and Marquis, 2013). Berrebi and Yonah (2021) found that charitable contributions in the US generally increase in the aftermath of mass shootings, but decrease in the directly affected localities. Mass shootings are local secluded events not associated with concrete threat to people other than the immediate victims. Observed effects on philanthropic activity—which generally does not target victims of mass shootings—is attributable to psychological reactions rather than to responses to actual needs.

Theoretical accounts of reduced cooperation in face of threat often invoke conservation of resources theory, which states that “when confronted with stress [defined as the threat of a net loss of resources], individuals are predicted…to strive to minimize net loss of resources” (Hobfoll, 1989). We suggest that applying the distinction made in social identity theory between interpersonal and intergroup behavior to conservation of resources theory provides an underlying psychological mechanism for the seemingly opposing reactions to threat. Social identity theory posits that situational factors determine whether group affiliation—rather than personal perspective—governs the social behavior of individuals. In particular, intergroup conflict leads individuals to “behave toward each other as a function of their respective group memberships, rather than in terms of their individual characteristics or interindividual relationships” (Tajfel and Turner, 1979). When people construe conflict as a potential threat to their resources, they strive to maintain and protect their resources, reducing costly cooperation. When conflict is construed as an intergroup process, focus shifts to the threat to the group resources. Such perceptions of a common threat also strengthen group identity, leading people to view group and self resources as one, motivating them to maintain and protect group resources—increasing ingroup cooperation as a result.

According to our interpretation of the field studies discussed above, perceived level of threat varies with proximity—spatial, temporal, and social—to the victims. Proximity is naturally associated with myriad variables that guide cooperative behavior, making it difficult to establish a pure psychological effect of conflict on cooperation. Results from the laboratory experiments reviewed above provide clean evidence for such psychological effects. Framing conflict at the individual or group level manipulates perceived level of conflict while fixing the material effects of conflict. Thus, we can attribute any differences in behavior and its correlates between individual and group frames of conflict to the mediating effects of psychological perceptions.

Author Contributions

OW and RZ contributed to all aspects of the research. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Althaus, S. L., Bramlett, B. H., and Gimpel, J. G. (2012). When war hits home the geography of military losses and support for war in time and space. J. Conflict Resol. 56, 382–412. doi: 10.1177/0022002711422340

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, W. D., and Oneal, J. R. (2001). Patriotism or opinion leadership? The nature and origins of the “rally round the flag” effect. J. Conflict Resol. 45, 661–687. doi: 10.1177/0022002701045005006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barclay, P., and Benard, S. (2020). The effects of social vs. asocial threats on group cooperation and manipulation of perceived threats. Evol. Hum. Sci. 2:E54. doi: 10.1017/ehs.2020.48

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron, J. (2001). Confusion of group interest and self-interest in parochial cooperation on behalf of a group. J. Conflict Resol. 45, 283–296. doi: 10.1177/0022002701045003002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berrebi, C., and Yonah, H. (2016). Terrorism and philanthropy: the effect of terror attacks on the scope of giving by individuals and households. Public Choice 169, 171–194. doi: 10.1007/s11127-016-0375-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berrebi, C., and Yonah, H. (2021). Crime and philanthropy: prosocial and antisocial responses to mass shootings. Victims Offenders 16, 99–125. doi: 10.1080/15564886.2020.1787281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bornstein, G. (1992). The free-rider problem in intergroup conflicts over step-level and continuous public goods. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 62, 597–606. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.62.4.597

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bornstein, G. (2003). Intergroup conflict: individual, group, and collective interests. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 7, 129–145. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0702_129-145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bornstein, G., and Ben-Yossef, M. (1994). Cooperation in intergroup and single-group social dilemmas. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 30, 52–67. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1994.1003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowles, S. (2008). Conflict: altruism's midwife. Nature 456, 326–327. doi: 10.1038/456326a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowles, S., Choi, J.-K., and Hopfensitz, A. (2003). The co-evolution of individual behaviors and social institutions. J. Theoret. Biol. 223, 135–147. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5193(03)00060-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brody, R. A. (1991). Assessing the President: The Media, Elite Opinion, and Public Support. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Google Scholar

Burnstein, E., and McRae, A. V. (1962). Some effects of shared threat and prejudice in racially mixed groups. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 64, 257–263. doi: 10.1037/h0046022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, J.-K., and Bowles, S. (2007). The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Science 318, 636–640. doi: 10.1126/science.1144237

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coser, L. A. (1956). The Function of Social Conflict. Glenco, IL: Free Press.

Google Scholar

De Dreu, C., and Gross, J. (2019). Revisiting the form and function of conflict: Neurobiological, psychological, and cultural mechanisms for attack and defense within and between groups. Behav. Brain Sci. 42:E116. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X18002170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Dreu, C. K. W., Gross, J., Méder, Z., Giffin, M., Prochazkova, E., Krikeb, J., et al. (2016). In-group defense, out-group aggression, and coordination failures in intergroup conflict. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 10524–10529. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1605115113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Jaegher, K., and Hoyer, B. (2016). Collective action and the common enemy effect. Defence Peace Econ. 27, 644–664. doi: 10.1080/10242694.2014.925676

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drury, J., Brown, R., González, R., and Miranda, D. (2016). Emergent social identity and observing social support predict social support provided by survivors in a disaster: solidarity in the 2010 chile earthquake. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 46, 209–223. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feshbach, S., and Singer, R. (1957). The effects of personal and shared threats upon social prejudice. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 54, 411–416. doi: 10.1037/h0040958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gartner, S. S. (2008a). The multiple effects of casualties on public support for war: an experimental approach. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 102, 95–106. doi: 10.1017/S0003055408080027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gartner, S. S. (2008b). Ties to the dead: connections to Iraq War and 9/11 casualties and disapproval of the president. Am. Sociol. Rev. 73, 690–695. doi: 10.1177/000312240807300408

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gartner, S. S., and Segura, G. M. (1998). War, casualties, and public opinion. J. Conflict Resol. 42, 278–300. doi: 10.1177/0022002798042003004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gartner, S. S., Segura, G. M., and Wilkening, M. (1997). All politics are local losses and individual attitudes toward the Vietnam war. J. Conflict Resol. 41, 669–694. doi: 10.1177/0022002797041005004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelpi, C., Feaver, P. D., and Reifler, J. (2009). Paying the Human Costs of War: American Public Opinion and Casualties in Military Conflicts. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. doi: 10.1515/9781400830091

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glynn, S. A., Busch, M. P., Schreiber, G. B., Murphy, E. L., Wright, D. J., Tu, Y., et al. (2003). Effect of a national disaster on blood supply and safety: the September 11 experience. JAMA 289, 2246–2253. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.17.2246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gneezy, A., and Fessler, D. M. T. (2012). Conflict, sticks and carrots: war increases prosocial punishments and rewards. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 279, 219–223. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.0805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guzmán, R. A., Rodriguez-Sickert, C., and Rowthorn, R. (2007). When in Rome, do as the Romans do: the coevolution of altruistic punishment, conformist learning, and cooperation. Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, 112–117. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hargreaves-Heap, S., and Varoufakis, Y. (2002). Some experimental evidence on the evolution of discrimination, co-operation and perceptions of fairness. Econ. J. 112, 679–703. doi: 10.1111/1468-0297.00735

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources: a new attempt at conceptualizing stress. Am. Psychol. 44:513. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.3.513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hugh-Jones, D., and Zultan, R. (2013). Reputation and cooperation in defense. J. Conflict Resol. 57, 327–355. doi: 10.1177/0022002712445745

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janis, I. L. (1951). Air War and Emotional Stress: Psychological Studies of Bombing and Civilian Defense. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. doi: 10.1037/13956-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaniasty, K. (2020). Social support, interpersonal, and community dynamics following disasters caused by natural hazards. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 32, 105–109. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.07.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaniasty, K., and Norris, F. H. (2004). “Social support in the aftermath of disasters, catastrophes, and acts of terrorism: altruistic, overwhelmed, uncertain, antagonistic, and patriotic communities,” in Bioterrorism: Psychological and Public Health Interventions, eds R. J. Ursano, A. E. Norwood, and C. S. Fullerton (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 200–229.

Google Scholar

Kriner, D., and Shen, F. (2014). Responding to war on Capitol Hill: battlefield casualties, congressional response, and public support for the war in Iraq. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 58, 157–174. doi: 10.1111/ajps.12055

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kriner, D. L., and Shen, F. X. (2012). How citizens respond to combat casualties: the differential impact of local casualties on support for the war in Afghanistan. Public Opin. Q. 76, 761–770. doi: 10.1093/poq/nfs048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuijpers, D. (2019). Rally around all the flags: the effect of military casualties on incumbent popularity in ten countries 1990-2014. Foreign Policy Anal. 15, 392–412. doi: 10.1093/fpa/orz014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larson, E. V. (1996). Casualties and Consensus: The Historical Role of Casualties in Domestic Support for U.S. Military Operations. Technical report, Rand, Santa Monica, CA.

Google Scholar

Lee, J. (2020). Post-disaster trust in Japan: the social impact of the experiences and perceived risks of natural hazards. Environ. Hazards 19, 171–186. doi: 10.1080/17477891.2019.1664380

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. (2021). The social impact of natural hazards: a multi-level analysis of disasters and forms of trust in mainland China. Disasters 45, 158–179. doi: 10.1111/disa.12410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maki, A., Dwyer, P. C., Blazek, S., Snyder, M., González, R., and Lay, S. (2019). Responding to natural disasters: examining identity and prosociality in the context of a major earthquake. Brit. J. Soc. Psychol. 58, 66–87. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mueller, J. (1970). Presidential popularity from Truman to Johnson. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 64, 18–34. doi: 10.2307/1955610

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mueller, J. E. (1973). War, Presidents, and Public Opinion. New York, NY: John Wiley.

Google Scholar

Newman, B., and Forcehimes, A. (2010). “Rally round the flag” events for presidential approval research. Elect. Stud. 29, 144–154. doi: 10.1016/j.electstud.2009.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pena, A. A., Zahran, S., Underwood, A., and Weiler, S. (2014). Effect of natural disasters on local nonprofit activity. Growth Change 45, 590–610. doi: 10.1111/grow.12056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penner, L., Brannick, M. T., Webb, S., and Connell, P. (2005). Effects on volunteering of the September 11, 2001, attacks: an archival analysis. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 35, 1333–1360. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2005.tb02173.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Probst, T. M., Carnevale, P. J., and Triandis, H. C. (1999). Cultural values in intergroup and single-group social dilemmas. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 77, 171–191. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1999.2822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmiedeberg, M. (1942). Some observations on individual reactions to air raids. Int. J. Psychoanal. 23, 146–175.

Google Scholar

Sherif, M. (1961). Intergroup Conflict and Cooperation: The Robbers Cave Experiment. Norman, OK: University of Oklahoma Press.

Google Scholar

Sherif, M. (1966). In Common Predicament: Social Psychology of Intergroup Conflict and Cooperation. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Google Scholar

Simmel, G. (1955). Conflict and the Web of Group Affiliations. New York, NY: Free Press.

Google Scholar

Sirin, C. V. (2011). Is it cohesion or diversion? Domestic instability and the use of force in international crises. Int. Polit. Sci. Rev. 32, 303–321. doi: 10.1177/0192512110380554

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinberg, K. S., and Rooney, P. M. (2005). America gives: a survey of Americans' generosity after September 11. Nonprofit Vol. Sec. Q. 34, 110–135. doi: 10.1177/0899764004269738

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sumner, W. G. (1906). Folkways: A Study of the Sociological Importance of Usages, Manners, Customs, Mores, and Morals. Boston, MA: Ginn. doi: 10.2307/1412602

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tajfel, H., and Turner, J. C. (1979). “An integrative theory of intergroup conflict,” in The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations, eds W. G. Austin and S. Worchel (Monterey, CA: Brookes/Coole), 33–47.

Google Scholar

Tilcsik, A., and Marquis, C. (2013). Punctuated generosity: how mega-events and natural disasters affect corporate philanthropy in US communities. Adm. Sci. Q. 58, 111–148. doi: 10.1177/0001839213475800

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vardy, T., and Atkinson, Q. D. (2019). Property damage and exposure to other people in distress differentially predict prosocial behavior after a natural disaster. Psychol. Sci. 30, 563–575. doi: 10.1177/0956797619826972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vezzali, L., Cadamuro, A., Versari, A., Giovannini, D., and Trifiletti, E. (2015). Feeling like a group after a natural disaster: common ingroup identity and relations with outgroup victims among majority and minority young children. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 54, 519–538. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisel, O., and Zultan, R. (2016). Social motives in intergroup conflict: group identity and perceived target of threat. Eur. Econ. Rev. 90, 122–133. doi: 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2016.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisel, O., and Zultan, R. (in press). Perceptions of conflict: parochial cooperation outgroup spite revisited. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 10.1016/j.obhdp.2021.04.001

Google Scholar

Williams, R. M. Jr. (1947). The reduction of intergroup tensions: a survey of research on problems of ethnic, racial, and religious group relations. Soc. Sci. Res. Council Bull. 57:153.

Google Scholar

Wright, M. E. (1943). The influence of frustration upon the social relations of young children. J. Pers. 12, 111–122. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1943.tb01951.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: intergroup conflict, cooperation, threat, framing, common enemy, social support, conservation of resources

Citation: Weisel O and Zultan R (2021) Perceived Level of Threat and Cooperation. Front. Psychol. 12:704338. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.704338

Received: 02 May 2021; Accepted: 17 May 2021;
Published: 05 July 2021.

Edited by:

Kris De Jaegher, Utrecht University, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Nobuhiro Mifune, Kochi University of Technology, Japan

Copyright © 2021 Weisel and Zultan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ro'i Zultan, zultan@bgu.ac.il

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.