Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 22 July 2021
Sec. Human-Media Interaction
This article is part of the Research Topic Online Sensory Experiences: Consumer Reactions to Triggering Multiple Senses by Using Psychological Techniques and Sensory-enabling Technologies View all 7 articles

Toward Healthier Cookie Habits: Assessing the Role of Packaging Visual Appearance in the Expectations for Dietary Cookies in Digital Environments

  • 1Universidad de Los Andes School of Management, Bogotá, Colombia
  • 2Department of Economics, Management and Business Law, University of Bari, Bari, Italy
  • 3Independent Researcher, Rome, Italy
  • 4Centre for Multisensory Marketing, Department of Marketing, BI Norwegian Business School, Oslo, Norway

As we tend to consume more and more via e-commerce platforms, the digital version of a dietary product’s package can be one of the most important touchpoints that the consumer has with such product during the purchasing stage of the consumer’s journey. Hence, a dietary food/drink properly presented via its packaging in e-commerce is key, for example, to nudge consumers toward healthier purchase habits. In this study, we assessed the role of different configurations of visual cues commonly present in a product’s packaging (jar vs. bag, transparent vs. opaque, labeled vs. unlabeled) in the expectations associated with dietary cookies when presented in a digital environment. A between-participants study was conducted where eight different packages with different combinations of the three aforementioned features were digitally evaluated by the participants. The results suggest that the presence (vs. absence) of labeling triggered the highest ratings on most assessed dimensions (product quality, healthiness, lightness, sweetness, crumbliness, price, tastiness, greediness for product, product/packaging liking). Moreover, transparent (vs. opaque) packaging tends to yield higher expectations concerning this product’s quality (i.e., product liking, package liking, greediness), though it has an opposite effect on the expected healthiness for such cookies. Some particular interactions between these three visual cues were also observed and are discussed as part of the obtained results. In summary, our results point to how the visual appearance of packaging can be strategically used in order to potentially nudge consumers toward healthier cookie purchase habits.

Introduction

Packaging may be regarded as the “visiting card” of a food/drink product, since it is the first element that usually catch a consumer’s attention in, for example, a retail environment, acting as a key interface between brands and consumers. From the perspective of food marketing, the packaging of a food/drink is, in fact, an essential tool for communicating and promoting a product (Hawkes, 2010; Küster et al., 2019). Decades ago, Guss (1967) already suggested that “packaging is marketing.” That said, it is evident that packaging is not only about containing, protecting, and conserving foods and drinks, but it is a crucial element to be taken into consideration during the experience of such type of products (Krishna et al., 2017). Indeed, it has been suggested that packaging is a multisensory device capable of delivering specific consumer experiences (Velasco and Spence, 2019). The visual appearance of packaging, for instance, seems to be one of the most important elements during the formation of the expectations concerning the experience of the food/drink being contained in such packaging (Pinson, 1986; Selame and Koukos, 2002; Fenko et al., 2010; Spence and Velasco, 2018).

Indeed, it has been extensively shown that consumers’ decisions are significantly affected by the visual appearance of a product, including its packaging (Imram, 1999; Bloch et al., 2003; Crilly et al., 2004; Folkes and Matta, 2004; Zellner et al., 2004; Creusen and Schoormans, 2005; Schifferstein and Cleiren, 2005; Schifferstein, 2006; Schifferstein and Desmet, 2007; Vieira, 2010). Nowadays these reflections seem to grow in importance, as we tend to consume more and more via e-commerce platforms, and where the 2D, 3D, or augmented reality (AR) digital version of a product’s package is, most of the time, the single interaction that the consumer has with such product during the purchasing decision process (Petit et al., 2019, 2021). A food/drink properly presented via its packaging in e-commerce may be key, for example, to nudge consumers toward choosing new products. Packaging may also be critical for communicating health-related information (Vith et al., 2010; Miklavec et al., 2016; Hagmann and Siegrist, 2020), and where dietary food products may be important choices during the corresponding decision-making processes.

With the above being said, the general purpose of this study consisted of evaluating the role of a packaging’s type, label, and transparency on dietary products expectations. In particular, we aimed at looking for ways to customize the online experience of dietary cookies more accurately via its packaging. That said, it would be possible to nudge consumers more effectively toward healthier purchase choices, while at the same time allowing companies to better market dietary food products.

Theoretical Framework

The Choice of Food Stimuli

Dietary cookies were chosen as the product for this experiment. Cookies can be acquired in wide variety of types when it comes to flavor and production process (i.e., from fully crafted, till mass-produced), as well as concerning dietary properties (e.g., from very high-caloric till very specific choices in terms of dietary conditions). At present, cookies are, in fact, one of the most consumed products during breakfast in some European countries, such as Italy1. What is more, the particular market size of dietary cookies is expected to expand at 5.0% rate between 2019 and 2025, worldwide, and where the distribution via online channels is also expected to grow at 5.2% rate between 2019 and 2025 (Market Analysis Report, 2019). In summary, assessing the experience of dietary cookies, digitally, would certainly be in line with the demands of the present and future market.

Verbal Typologies in Packaging

When it comes to the particularities of the visual appearance of packaging, Silayoi and Speece (2004, 2007) suggested that one may classify visual elements of packaging into two main typologies: verbal (i.e., including health and nutritional claims), and non-verbal (as in color, type, shape, size, graphics).

Concerning verbal typologies, several studies have verified how particular sensory aspects of products, which may also be related to healthiness, can be influenced by messages present on food/beverage packaging (e.g., Tuorilla and Cardello, 2002; Verbeke et al., 2009; Carrillo et al., 2012; Lwin et al., 2014; Miraballes et al., 2014; Pinto et al., 2017; Steinhauser and Hamm, 2018; Steinhauser et al., 2019). Carrillo et al. (2012), for instance, studied consumers’ perceptions of low-calorie cookies packaging, alternating tasting/non-tasting sessions with presence vs. absence of information on packaging. They found that participants paid more attention to short nutritional information, while also reporting negative evaluations associated to excessive information on packaging. Meanwhile, three versions of a snack-bar were presented by Pinto et al. (2017) to Brazilian consumers (the first version with no information at all, the second with the product’s packaging, and the third with the product’s packaging including health-related information). The latter findings suggest that packaging attributes, along with price/flavor/health information, can influence consumers in their choice to purchase a snack bar, while also having a positive impact on consumers’ product acceptance. Efficacy on health claims have also been analyzed by Lwin et al. (2014). Their study shed light on the apparent low-impact of nutritional claims on unrestrained eaters, which contrasted with restrained eaters, whose opinion on not-so-healthy food improved when the corresponding package contained information relative to healthiness.

With the above being said, in this study, we decided to assess the effect of verbal typologies in packaging on the expectations of dietary cookies in a digital environment, by manipulating presence vs. absence of labeling.

Non-verbal Typologies in Packaging

When it comes to non-verbal typologies, existing evidence points toward crossmodal correspondences (that is, the associations that people make between features across the senses; Spence, 2011) as playing a crucial role on how the visual appearance of packaging can significantly affect our expectations, and consequent tasting experience of foods and drinks (Reinoso Carvalho et al., 2017a; Velasco and Spence, 2019). Based on crossmodal correspondences, different studies have revealed the role of a food/drink packaging and/or general container (i.e., cups), not only in terms of expectations, but also in actual taste/flavor perception (e.g., Raudenbush et al., 2002; Letona et al., 2014; van Rompay et al., 2014; Tu et al., 2015; Tijssen et al., 2017; Mead and Richerson, 2018; Spence and Carvalho, 2019).

For instance, shape of packaging has been shown to affect the perception of different food/drink (e.g., Ares and Deliza, 2010; Becker et al., 2011; Velasco et al., 2014, 2016; Spence and Wan, 2015; Simmonds et al., 2019). The impact of the shape of a yogurt’s packaging, for example, was analyzed by Becker et al. (2011), where participants sensitive to design perceived the yoghurt’s flavor as more intense when packed in an angular packaging, as compared to a rounder one. Concerning dietary food, packaging shape also seems to moderate perceived healthiness (e.g., Fenko et al., 2016; Festila and Chrysochou, 2018; Yarar et al., 2019; Ikonen et al., 2020). In particular, Yarar et al. (2019) showed that a slim humanoid-shaped type of package persuaded women with normal-to-high body-mass-index that such package contained healthier food, when compared to less slim packaging (cf. van Ooijen et al., 2017; Fenko, 2019). Koo and Suk (2016) also verified that food in taller packages tend to be perceived as having fewer calories than when contained in wider and less-tall packages. Marques da Rosa et al. (2018) also observed that healthiness of buttery cookies is deemed higher in case of a rounded, and red-to-yellow packages, compared to angular and blue-to-green ones.

Moreover, transparency (vs. opaqueness) of packaging is a critical attribute that has also been shown to influence food and drink expectations and experiences (Deng and Srinivasan, 2013; Adam and Ali, 2014; Tu et al., 2015; Simmonds and Spence, 2017; Simmonds et al., 2018; Sabri et al., 2020). For example, Tu et al. (2015) observed that cold tea tends to be perceived as sweeter when served in a transparent glass cup, as compared to when poured in an opaque paper/plastic receptacle. Adam and Ali (2014) also conducted a comparison of type of milk packaging. Here, they found a positive correlation between purchase intention and a transparent glass-milk container, and a negative correlation of the purchase intention associated to the opaque tetra-pak, and plastic milk containers. In this study, the presence of nutritional information on the package’s labeling also increased purchase intention.

In the present research, we focused on the role of packaging type and transparency on the expectations associated with dietary food products in a digital environment. Type and transparency are ubiquitous in packaging, while being both widely used to differentiate brands in the marketplace. In particular, here, we thought of the fact that, nowadays, most cookies are actually commercialized in angular-shaped, and opaque bag types (e.g., Robertson, 2011; Davidson, 2018). Nevertheless, we also recalled the most utilitarian cylindrical/round shaped transparent cookie jar types, which were often found as part of households in the United States, Canada, and United Kingdom (in the latter they are perhaps most commonly known as biscuit jars; Franklin, 1979; Allen, 2004). In brief, when it comes to non-verbal typologies in packaging, we were interested in comparing how such specific types of packaging (bag vs. jar, with both containing specific shapes), at contrasting levels of transparency, could impact the expectations of consumers for dietary cookies.

Research Gap

A recent study conducted by De Luca et al. (2019) served as principal inspiration for the present research. In this study it was assessed how consumers perceived aspects of dietary cookies when served in an unlabeled transparent-round-glass jar (as in a cookie jar), as compared to when served it its commercial/original packaging (in this case, an opaque bag which included labeling with branding and formulation; cf. Adam and Ali, 2014). These results, based on 31 European participants, suggested that such differences in packaging appearance may prompt different expectations on dietary cookies. For instance, this study reported that dietary cookies were thought as potentially lighter, but not necessarily healthier, when being offered in their original labeled packaging, as compared to when offered in the unlabeled transparent jar. Moreover, the participants rated the same cookies as potentially more caloric when presented in the unlabeled jar, as compared to the ratings under the influence of the original packaging. The obtained evidence also suggested that ‘lightness’ and ‘calories’ related wording were more effective message carriers for dietary cookies labeling, rather than ‘healthy’ type of wording, or wording related to flavor/hedonic sensations elicited by the cookies experience (i.e., crumbliness, greediness).

Based on De Luca et al.’s (2019) assessment, we decided to design this new study focused on carefully evaluating the effects of packaging transparency and type on expectations in an experiment resembling e-commerce. The latter is also aligned with the e-commerce growing trend for this food category (e.g., Market Analysis Report, 2019). As such, our new study would be principally about the expectations elicited by the digital visual appearance of packaging, where tasting the cookies and touching the packaging would not be part of our experimental scope (cf. Citrin et al., 2003; Overmars and Poels, 2015; Petit et al., 2019).

Here, we also saw the need to use customized brand-free labeling (something that was not considered in the aforesaid study). In fact, while designing our own experimental label for this new study, we would be able to optimize the amount and type of information present in such labeling (cf. Carrillo et al., 2012). The latter is relevant when assuming that the usual e-commerce platform does not always allow the consumer to properly read all the information that may be present in a dietary product’s label. Finally, we wanted to more accurately control the three dimensions of a packaging appearance that were explored in the latter study as well (jar vs. bag, transparency vs. opaqueness, unlabeled vs. labeled packaging). For this, we decided to compare such dimensions not only between two experimental conditions (as in De Luca et al., 2019), but across eight different versions of packaging.

The Present Study

The objective of this study, therefore, was to look for ways to customize the online experience of dietary cookies more accurately via its packaging. For this, we assessed how different combinations of three key aspects of a packaging appearance (with vs. without labeling, bag vs. jar, transparency vs. opaqueness) would affect the expectations of consumers while evaluating dietary cookies in a 2D digital context.

First, we hypothesized that an optimized label mainly communicating the dietary attributes on the packaging would be more strongly associated with healthier type of cookies, when compared to an unlabeled version of the same packaging. Second, we thought of comparing two packaging types (a cookie jar and a bag). Specifically, we thought that a jar usually has rounder shapes than most cookie bags (Robertson, 2011; Davidson, 2018). Based, for instance, on crossmodal correspondences research – which suggests that round shapes are more naturally associated with sweet/smooth sensations, when compared to more angular shapes (cf. Becker et al., 2011; see also Reinoso Carvalho et al., 2017b, for an example), one may expect that a jar may prompt higher expectations toward a sweeter, and/or smoother, type of cookie, when compared to a bag. Third, a transparent packaging would allow the cookies to be fully exposed, and where we hypothesized that this would have positive consequences on the expectations of consumers while evaluating the product (Petit et al., 2021; Simmonds and Spence, 2017; Simmonds et al., 2018). The latter may be even more relevant in a digital environment, where none of the senses, besides vision, tend to be significantly activated (at least not yet).

In brief, by contrasting the appearance of a packaging across such three dimensions, we tested the following main general hypothesis:

H1: Changes in the visual appearance of packaging, in terms of presence (vs. absence) of label, packaging type (jar vs. bag), and transparency (vs. opaqueness) will modulate the expectations for dietary cookies in digital environments.

The test of the general main hypothesis was carried out across the following three specific hypothesis, and where each of these particular hypotheses focused on the three specific aspects of the packaging being manipulated:

H1a: The presence vs. absence of labeling will principally modulate aspects related to the expected healthiness for this product (calories, healthiness, lightness).

H1b: A jar will most likely trigger sweeter and/or smoother sensations for the cookies, when compared to a bag type of packaging.

H1c: A transparent packaging will prompt higher expectations concerning specific sensory (e.g., sweetness, crumbliness) and qualitative (e.g., greediness, preference, price) aspects of cookies flavor.

In general, we assumed that the said modulation in expectations would be measurable in terms of the ratings related to healthiness (e.g., lighter, healthier, caloric content), differences in hedonic/qualitative ratings (e.g., tastiness, greediness – as in ‘these cookies look so good they make me hungry,’ price), and/or differences in sensory flavor ratings (e.g., sweetness, crumbliness).

Materials and Methods

Participants

In total, 4962 participants took part in the experiment (n = 496; 62.50% males, Mean of age 25.71 years SD = 7.91). The survey was programmed on Qualtrics software3 and the participants were recruited on Prolific Academic4, in exchange for GBP 0.63. The focus on this recruitment was to model the general population, so no specific filters were set concerning the attitudes that the sampled population might have toward cookies or dietary cookies. Here, most participants reported eating cookies (95.00%), loving cookies (86.10%), and usually paying attention to food/drink product’s labeling while shopping (73.80%). Only 31.50% of the participants reported actually consuming light and/or dietary type of cookies. Concerning cookies eating habits, the obtained sample appears as representative of an European population. For instance, previous evidence has reported that 96% of such consumers tend to buy into this category, and that cookies with certain dietary characteristics are being consumed by more or less a third of this kind of population (Harris Interactive Survey, 2019).

Stimuli

Eight different versions of packaging were produced for this experiment. They were carefully designed with respect to any unintended influences, for example, when it comes to the quality prompted by the packaging type, as well as while keeping away from resembling existing cookies brands. Such eight different versions of packaging included two variations of packaging type (jar vs. bag), two variations of packaging transparency (transparent vs. opaque), and two variations of labeling (labeled vs. unlabeled). Based on Carrillo et al. (2012) and De Luca et al. (2019), the label included the product’s title (in this case, ‘Light cookies’), along with highlights on the product’s formula (‘low in calories’; ‘palm-oil, milk, and eggs free’). Figure 1 shows the different versions of the packaging stimuli.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The eight experimental packages [labeled transparent jar (A), labeled transparent bag (B), labeled opaque jar (C), labeled opaque bag (D), unlabeled transparent jar (E), unlabeled transparent bag (F), unlabeled opaque jar (G), unlabeled opaque bag (H)]. The packages were presented scaled to match in size, and large enough in order for the participants be able to clearly see all the available information present in the label [the latter only when applicable, since (E–H) versions were unlabeled].

Design and Procedure

This study followed a 2 (packaging type: jar vs. bag) × 2 (labeling: label vs. no label) × 2 (transparency: opaque vs. transparent) between-participants experimental design. Each participant was randomly assigned to, and asked to evaluate, only one of the eight versions of the dietary cookies packaging (see Table 1). In particular, they were asked to evaluate the packages in terms of healthiness, quality, greediness, price, lightness, calories, crumbliness, sweetness, tastiness, cookies/packaging liking. The completion of the survey lasted for approximately 5 min in total.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. n, mean of age (with corresponding SD), and % of females, for each between-participants packaging sample.

The packages were presented scaled to match in size (1041 × 1476 pixels), and large enough in order for the participants be able to read all the available information present in the label (the latter only when applicable, since four of the eight versions of packaging were unlabeled; see Figure 1). Since the transparent packaging allowed the participants to actually see the cookies, and the opaque did not, it was decided to include mini replicas of the cookies as part of the label.

The answers concerning the expectations elicited by the packaging were all based on 5-point rating scales (anchored with 1 = ‘Not at all’, 5 = ‘Very much’). After answering these questions, the participants were also asked to provide some demographic information (age, gender), and their general cookies-consumption habits. Appendix Table A, shows a summary of the variables assessed in the survey. The order of the presentations of the survey’s questions, and multiple-choice answers, were fully randomized.

Data Analysis

A 2 × 2 × 2 multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted via SPSS 26, with type of packaging, labeling, and transparency as between-factors, and the rating-scales as the dependent variables (see Appendix Table A, for an overview on these dependent variables). Gender and age were included as controls (covariates).

Results

Table 2 (Means, SD) and Figure 2 (Means) show a descriptive summary of results across the eight different types of packaging.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of the dependent variable means and corresponding standard deviations (SD) as a function of the eight different packaging presentations.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Grayscale heat map representing the mean values of each dependent variables (X-axis) across the different experimental conditions (Y-axis). The lower mean values are being represented with the darkest colors, and vice-versa.

Table 3 shows the details on the general results of the multivariate test. In general, the multivariate tests show a main effect of packaging labeling and transparency (p ≤ 0.001 for both), as well as an interaction effect of packaging labelingtransparency (p ≤ 0.001), packaging typetransparency (p ≤ 0.001), and with all of the three packaging factors combined (p = 0.035). No interaction effects with age (p = 0.250) or gender (p = 0.210) were found.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary of the results of the multivariate test, considering all combinations of packaging factors (type – bag vs. jar, transparency – transparent vs. opaque, labeling – with vs. without label), and including controls for gender and age (only Pillai’s trace test being reported).

Table 4 shows the details of the results concerning the between-participants effects. Significant effects are being reported at 95% confidence (p ≤ 0.050). Such effects were found for all of the dependent variables concerning packaging labeling (healthiness, quality, greediness, expensive, lightness, high/low in calories, crumbliness, sweetness, tastiness, cookies and packaging liking); all of the dependent variables assessed via packaging transparency (except for expensive); all of the dependent variables when it comes to labelingtransparency interactions (except for healthiness, expensive, and lightness); all of the dependent variables assessed via packaging typetransparency interactions (except for healthiness, greediness, lightness, low/high in calories, and sweetness); and for healthiness, expensive, lightness, tastiness and cookies liking ratings, when it comes to packaging labelingtypetransparency interactions.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Summary of the main effects and interactions.

Main Effect of Packaging Labeling (With vs. Without Label)

Table 5 reports mean differences when comparing general presence vs. absence of label, and regardless of packaging type (jar vs. bag), and level of transparency (transparent vs. opaque). The variables being here considered are those which showed a significant difference in Table 4. This evidence points toward the presence of label in the packaging as triggering higher ratings in all of the assessed dimensions (except for high in calories, which had an opposite direction).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Mean differences for presence vs. absence of label and regardless of type (jar vs. bag) and level of transparency (transparent vs. opaque) of the packaging.

Main Effect of Packaging Transparency (Transparency vs. Opaqueness)

Table 6 shows the mean differences when comparing general transparency vs. opaqueness, and regardless of packaging type (jar vs. bag) and labeling (presence vs. absence). The variables being considered are those that prompted a significant difference in Table 4. Here, the evidence shows that a transparent packaging prompted higher ratings for quality, greediness, high in calories, crumbliness, sweetness, tastiness, cookies, and packaging liking ratings. On the other hand, an opaque packaging prompted higher ratings for healthiness, lightness, and low calories.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Mean differences for general transparency vs. opaqueness, and regardless of type (jar vs. bag) and labeling (presence vs. absence) of the packaging.

Interaction Between Labeling and Transparency

As shown in Tables 3, 4, there was evidence of a significant interaction between labeling and transparency on quality, greediness, high/low in calories, crumbliness, sweetness, tastiness, cookies, and packaging liking ratings (Figure 3). In particular, the ratings of quality, greediness, low in calories, and packaging liking, tend to decrease without labeling, and regardless of packaging transparency. Comparing to the latter, high in calories ratings have an opposite direction of effects. For crumbliness, tastiness, and cookies liking ratings, the ratings tend to decrease without labeling, but only for the opaque version of packaging. Concerning sweetness, evidence shows that the label tends to trigger similar effects on transparent and opaque packaging. However, the expected sweetness ratings tend to be enhanced for unlabeled transparent packaging and diminished in the unlabeled opaque one.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Interaction effects of Labeling*transparency in packaging, across the variables that prompted significant differences at 95% confidence in Table 4 [(A) Quality, (B) Greediness, (C) Low in calories, (D) High in calories, (E) Crumbliness, (F) Sweetness, (G) Tastiness, (H) Cookies liking, (I) Packaging liking]. Y-axis are the marginal means. X-axis are the labeling conditions. The black (darker) line corresponds to the transparent packaging values, whereas the gray (clearer) line the opaque ones. Error bars show confidence interval at 95%.

Interaction Between Type and Transparency

Tables 3, 4 show a significant interaction between type and transparency in packaging on quality, greediness, expensive, crumbliness, tastiness, cookies, and packaging liking ratings (see Figure 4). In particular, quality, greediness, crumbliness, tastiness, cookies, and packaging liking ratings tend to increase in the transparent, and decrease in the opaque, jar conditions. For the bag, however, ratings did not seem to be as significantly affected by changes in transparency, when compared to the jar. Concerning expensiveness ratings, people tend to expect the cookies as more expensive when presented in the transparent jar, as compared to the transparent bag. In addition, expensiveness ratings did not seem to be so significantly affected in the opaque version of both packaging.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Interaction effects of type*transparency in packaging, across the variables that prompted significant differences at 95% confidence in Table 4 [(A) Quality, (B) Greediness, (C) Expensive, (D) Crumbliness, (E) Tastiness, (F) Cookies liking, (G) Packaging liking]. Y-axis are the marginal means. X-axis are the type conditions. The black (darker) line corresponds to the transparent packaging values, whereas the gray (clearer) line the opaque ones. Error bars show confidence interval at 95%.

Interaction Between Labeling, Type, and Transparency

Tables 3, 4 show evidence of a significant interaction between labelingtypetransparency, concentrated on healthiness, expensive, lightness, tastiness, and cookies liking ratings. Since no significant effects were reported in Table 3 for labelingtype, we decided to independently assess the moderating effect of type of packaging on labelingtransparency, and the moderating effect of labeling on typetransparency of packaging (Gignac, 2019).

Moderating Effect of Type of Packaging on LabelingTransparency

A significant effect was found for labelingtransparency interaction for both types of packaging (jar - F[5,238] = 8.14, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.146; bag – F[5,238] = 3.65, p = 0.003, η2 = 0.071). This between participants test showed significant effects for the jar on expensive (F[1,241] = 8.91, p = 0.003, η2 = 0.036), lightness (F[1,241] = 5.01, p = 0.026, η2 = 0.020), tastiness (F[1,241] = 23.27, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.088), and cookies liking ratings (F[1,241] = 33.83, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.123) – healthiness (F[1,241] = 0.067, p = 0.796, η2 = 0.000). Concerning the bag, healthiness (F[1,242] = 5.73, p = 0.017, η2 = 0.023), lightness (F[1,242] = 5.98, p = 0.015, η2 = 0.024), and cookies liking ratings (F[1,242] = 5.17, p = 0.024, η2 = 0.021) showed significant effects - expensive (F[1, 242] = 1.56, p = 0.213, η2 = 0.006); tastiness (F[1,242] = 2.95, p = 0.087, η2 = 0.012). The labelingtransparency interaction moderated by type of packaging is visualized in Figure 5. In brief, the moderating effect of type of packaging on labelingtransparency seems to be more robust with jar than with bag types of packaging. Note also that the behavior of the estimated marginal means of lightness and cookies liking ratings (being these ratings the only ones showing significant effects in the two types of packaging) tends to be somewhat similar in both, jar and bag.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Interaction effect of labeling*transparency, while moderated by type of packaging (jar vs. bag), across the variables that prompted significant differences at 95% confidence in “Moderating Effect of Type of Packaging on Labeling*Transparency” [for jar, (A) expensive, (B) lightness, (C) tastiness, (D) cookies liking ratings; for bag, (E) healthiness, (F) lightness, (G) cookies liking ratings]. Y-axis are the marginal means. X-axis are the labeling conditions. The black (darker) line corresponds to the transparent packaging ratings, whereas the gray (clearer) line the opaque ones. Error bars show confidence interval at 95%.

Moderating Effect of Labeling on TypeTransparency of Packaging

A significant effect was found for typetransparency interaction only for unlabeled versions of packaging (unlabeled – F[5,238] = 4.98, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.095; labeled – F[5,238] = 0.70, p = 0.625, η2 = 0.014). Here, the between participants test showed significant effects on expensive (F[1,242] = 16.63, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.064), lightness (F[1,242] = 9.52, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.038), tastiness (F[1,242] = 9,74, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.039), and cookies liking (F[1,242] = 11.18, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.044), ratings – healthiness F[1,242] = 1.70, p = 0.193, η2 = 0.007). The typetransparency interactions moderated by labeling are also visualized in Figure 6. In summary, the moderating effect of labeling on typetransparency is mostly prompting significant effects in the unlabeled versions of packaging.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Interaction effect of type*transparency of packaging, while moderated by labeling (labeled vs. unlabeled), across the variables that prompted significant differences at 95% confidence in “Moderating Effect of Labeling on Type*Transparency of Packaging” [only for unlabeled, (A) expensive, (B) lightness, (C) tastiness, (D) cookies liking ratings]. Y-axis are the marginal means. X-axis are packaging types. The black (darker) line corresponds to the transparent ratings, whereas the gray (clearer) line the opaque ones. Error bars show confidence interval at 95%.

Summary of Results

The obtained results suggest that changes in the visual appearance of packaging, in terms of presence (vs. absence) of label, packaging type (jar vs. bag), and transparency (vs. opaqueness), modulate the expectations of dietary cookies in a digital environment (H1). For instance, concerning the main effects of each visual cue (labeling, type of packaging, transparency), the presence vs. absence of label in the packaging, as well as the ability, or not, of visually inspecting the cookies, had a significant effect on most of the ratings (e.g., H1a, H1c). Moreover, when it comes to the interaction effects, for example, there were moderating effects of type of packing on transparency, and on labelingtransparency (H1b).

Note that, when the participants were asked if they were consumers of dietary cookies, only 35.1% answered that they actually were (n = 174). Therefore, we decided to split the original dataset across those that reported consuming such types of cookies, and those who reported not doing so, and re-run two independent analyses. However, after running such analyses, no remarkable differences in the data were observed suggesting that such contrast in consumption habits triggered robust differences across the obtained results (cf. Lwin et al., 2014). Such null effects may also be related to the split of the sample and, hence, a consequent underpowered analysis.

Discussion

The general purpose of this study was to evaluate the role of packaging format, label, and transparency on dietary products expectations. In particular, we aimed at looking for ways to customize the digital experience of dietary cookies more accurately via its packaging. For this, we assessed whether different visual appearances of packaging (labeled vs. unlabeled, bag vs. jar, transparent vs. opaque), would modulate the expectations for dietary cookies when presented in a partial simulation of e-commerce environment. Our results suggest that such different presentations of packing can indeed alter specific aspects of the expectations of consumers when evaluating dietary cookies (H1).

The Role of Packaging Labeling When Looking for Dietary Cookies in Digital Environments (H1a)

As expected, the presence of labeling in the packaging primed the consumers toward a more precise understanding of the dietary characteristics of such cookies, when compared to unlabeled versions of the same packaging [De Luca et al., 2019; see section “Main Effect of Packaging Labeling (With vs. Without Label)”]. Hence, the short and precise information present in this experimental and customized label was able to effectively nudge the consumer toward the understanding of the principal dietary characteristics of the product at stake (i.e., Carrillo et al., 2012). What is more, even when the label did not specify any precisions regarding the sensory characteristics of the cookies, the presence of label also enhanced the ratings related to greediness, crumbliness, sweetness, tastiness, cookies, and packaging liking (cf. Pinto et al., 2017). Note that the cookies with labeling were also rated as the most expensive. Hence, we could further hypothesize that the presence of label gave support to a more meticulous framing of the cookies within a specific category (i.e., dietetic food), where prices are usually above the average when compared to the generic version of the same product.

The Role of Type of Packaging When Looking for Dietary Cookies in Digital Environments (H1b)

There was no evidence of a main effect of type of packaging on expectations. However, while assessing the interactions, there were moderating effects of type of packing on transparency, and on labelingtransparency. For instance, as shown in section “Interaction Effect Between Type and Transparency,” people tend to expect cookies to be more expensive when presented in a transparent jar, as compared to a transparent bag. Moreover, bag ratings seem to be generally less affected by changes in transparency, when compared to the jar. Hence, and even though type of packaging did not prompt as robust main effects, when compared to labeling and transparency, it can still be of importance to consider the type of packaging in order to get more precise effects out of these other two visual cues, and especially when it comes to combining type of packaging with transparency.

The Role of Packaging Transparency When Looking for Dietary Cookies in Digital Environments (H1c)

The results show that a transparent vs. opaque packaging can generally trigger higher expectations concerning the overall quality (which is consistent with Sabri et al., 2020), but not necessarily when it comes to expected healthiness, [see section “Main Effect of Packaging Transparency (Transparency vs. Opaqueness)”]. In particular, we saw that the transparent packaging generally prompted higher expectations with concerns to aspects that could be understood as qualitative (think of a product’s quality, liking for the product and packaging, greediness), as well as sensory ones (crumbliness, sweetness). On the other hand, however, the transparency of the packaging had an opposite effect when it comes to the perceived healthiness, where the cookies were expected to be less light, and higher in calories, when presented in a transparent (vs. opaque) packaging. In fact, when considering the interaction effects reported in “Interaction Between Labeling and Transparency,” the unlabeled and transparent packaging was expected to provide sweetest cookies when compared to the unlabeled, but opaque, presentation. Hence, we could conclude that allowing the visual inspection of the cookies can encourage the consumer toward more hedonic – thus, potentially less rational – choices during decision-making tasks related to the online consumption of dietary food (Simmonds et al., 2018), and that this might be independent of the information that the brand can deliver to the consumer via labeling (cf. Adam and Ali, 2014). The latter may also be in line with the fact that transparent packaging can lead to better imagery, thus eating simulation could be the mechanism explaining such an effect (Petit et al., 2021).

General Implications

A thoroughly customized packaging experience can have important consequences on the way a consumer interacts with a product. Based on the particular results of this study, it may be possible to more effectively nudge consumers toward healthier choices of cookies, while at the same time allowing brands of dietary cookies to better market such type of products. For instance, as shown in this study, if a brand of cookies is looking for to emphasize its dietary characteristics, they might as well prioritize communicating such attributes via effective labeling [as in section “Main Effect of Packaging Labeling (With vs. Without Label)”], and perhaps not rely on transparent packaging. However, another brand that may be more interested in evoking, say, greediness during decision-making tasks, could perhaps focus on the design of the cookies, and show them via transparent packaging [as in section “Main Effect of Packaging Transparency (Transparency vs. Opaqueness)”].

The obtained results are certainly relevant in a digital environment where vision (and to a lesser extent audition and touch), tends to be the principal sense (Petit et al., 2019). As a matter of fact, a profound customization of the visual aspects of packaging may be a good start when considering the future impact of new technologies on the stages of the consumer’s journey where, for instance, further advances in virtual/augmented reality will certainly transform the online shopping experience (Hoyer et al., 2020).

From a marketing perspective, this study also highlights how a food company – in an online environment – can work on its own identity by strengthen packaging communication. For instance, the obtained evidence offers packaging design solutions for safeguarding brand awareness, along with the possibility of enhancing quality and healthiness expectations during e-commerce decision-making tasks.

Limitations and Future Directions

Concerning the study’s limitations, only visual aspects of packaging were tested. Therefore, future similar studies could compare, not only packaging aspects, but also different appearance of cookies. When it comes to the particular visual aspects of packaging that were tested, future studies could further manipulate packaging material, where sensations related to smoothness vs. roughness could be further enhanced (e.g., while retesting H1b). In this study we only tested presence vs. absence of customized labeling. However, the customization of labeling in dietary cookies could also be further addressed in similar future studies by, e.g., testing different combinations of messages along with particular color and/or font customization. Here we only tested one type of transparency in packaging as well. It may be of use to compare in the future glossy vs. matte transparent cookie jars, as it has been suggested that such differences can affect the perception of a packaging in terms of, e.g., haptics (Decré and Cloonan, 2019). Important to note as well that this study has the particular limitation of not considering that the choice of a packaging’s type is not only about aesthetics, but it might also be related to the performance of such packaging across the corresponding supply chain.

In brief, and even though this assessment has the abovementioned limitations, this study invites toward continuing exploring the further customization of packaging for dietary product categories, and especially considering potential new consumers of these type of products in digital environments.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

FR-C, RC, and MD: conceptualization. FR-C, RC, MD, and CV: methodology. FR-C and CV: formal analysis. FR-C: original draft preparation and funding acquisition. FR-C, RC, and CV: writing—review and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

FR was also supported by Universidad de los Andes’ FAPA internal funding scheme (FAPA N.32; P18.100322.005/1). We would also like to thank the Research Funding of the Marketing Department, BI Norwegian Business School, for funding the data collection of this research.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Valentina Cardona for producing the experimental packaging. We would also like to thank the Research Funding the Department of Marketing, BI Norwegian Business School, for funding this research.

Footnotes

  1. ^ Data Osservatorio Doxa/Union Food; https://unionfood.iocominciobene.inc-press.com/; retrieved 13th of May 2021.
  2. ^ In order to determine the sample size, a power analysis was performed based on Friedman’s simplified determinations of statistical power (see Friedman, 1982, Table 1). Considering 95% confidence (α = 0.05), effect size (r) of 0.35, and a power effect of at least 0.80, the suggested sample size would more than 59 participants per between participants’ condition.
  3. ^ qualtrics.com
  4. ^ https://app.prolific.co/

References

Adam, M. A., and Ali, K. (2014). How Packaging Elements Impact Consumer’s Buying Decisions. Saarbrucken: Lap Lambert Academic Publishing.

Google Scholar

Allen, B. (2004). Good Housekeeping Great American Classics Cookbook. New York, NY: Hearst Books.

Google Scholar

Ares, G., and Deliza, R. (2010). Studying the influence of package shape and colour on consumer expectations of milk desserts using word association and conjoint analysis. Food Qual. Prefer. 21, 930–937. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2010.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, L., van Rompay, T. J. L., Schifferstein, H. N. J., and Galetzka, M. (2011). Tough package, strong taste: the influence of packaging design on taste impressions and product evaluations. Food Qual. Prefer. 22, 17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2010.06.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bloch, P. H., Brunel, F. F., and Arnold, T. J. (2003). Individual differences in the centrality of visual product aesthetics: concept and measurement. J. Consum. Res. 29, 551–565. doi: 10.1086/346250

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrillo, E., Varela, P., and Fiszman, S. (2012). Packaging information as a modulator of consumers’ perception of enriched and reduced-calorie biscuits in tasting and non-tasting tests. Food Qual. Prefer. 25, 105–115. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Citrin, A. V., Stem, D. E. Jr., Spangenberg, E. R., and Clark, M. J. (2003). Consumer need for tactile input: an internet retailing challenge. J. Bus. Res. 56, 915–922. doi: 10.1016/s0148-2963(01)00278-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Creusen, M. E. H., and Schoormans, J. P. L. (2005). The different roles of product appearance in consumer choice. J. Product. Innovat. Manage. 22, 63–81. doi: 10.1111/j.0737-6782.2005.00103.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crilly, N., Moultrie, J., and Clarkson, P. J. (2004). Seeing things: consumer response to the visual domain in product design. Design Stud. 25, 547–577. doi: 10.1016/j.destud.2004.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidson, I. (2018). Biscuit, Cookie and Cracker Production: Process, Production and Packaging Equipment. Cambridge, CA: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

De Luca, M., Campo, R., Trio, O., and Reinoso Carvalho, F. (2019). “Effects of Packaging in the Perception of Dietetic Cookies,” in Paper Presented at 22nd Excellence in Services International Conference, (Thessaloniki: Perrotis College).

Google Scholar

Decré, G. B., and Cloonan, C. (2019). A touch of gloss: Haptic perception of packaging and consumers’ reactions. J. Product Brand Manag. 28, 117–132. doi: 10.1108/jpbm-05-2017-1472

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, X., and Srinivasan, R. (2013). When do transparent packages increase (or decrease) food consumption? J. Marke. 77, 104–117. doi: 10.1509/jm.11.0610

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fenko, A. (2019). “Influencing healthy food choice through multisensory packaging design,” in Multisensory Packaging, eds C. Velasco and C. Spence (Cham: Palgrave Macmillan), 225–255. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-94977-2_9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fenko, A., Lotterman, H., and Galetzka, M. (2016). What’s in a name? The effects of sound symbolism and package shape on consumer responses to food products. Food Qual. Prefer. 51, 100–108. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.02.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fenko, A., Schifferstein, H. N. J., and Hekkert, P. (2010). Shifts in sensory dominance between various stages of user-product interactions. Appl. Ergon. 41, 34–40. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2009.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Festila, A., and Chrysochou, P. (2018). Implicit communication of food product healthfulness through package design: A content analysis. J. Consum. Behav. 17, 461–476. doi: 10.1002/cb.1732

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folkes, V., and Matta, S. (2004). The Effect of Package Shape on Consumers’. Judgments of Product Volume: Attention as a Mental Contaminant. J. Consum. Res.31, 390–401. doi: 10.1086/422117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franklin, M. J. (1979). British Biscuit Tins, 1868-1939: An Aspect of Decorative Packaging. London: New Cavendish Books.

Google Scholar

Friedman, H. (1982). Simplified determinations of statistical power, magnitude of effect and research sample sizes. Educat. Psychol. Measur. 42, 521–526. doi: 10.1177/001316448204200214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gignac, G. E. (2019). How2statsbook. Online Edition 1. Available online at: http://www.how2statsbook.com/. (accessed May 13, 2021)

Google Scholar

Guss, L. M. (1967). Packaging is Marketing. New York: American Management Association.

Google Scholar

Hagmann, D., and Siegrist, M. (2020). Nutri-Score, multiple traffic light and incomplete nutrition labelling on food packages: Effects on consumers’ accuracy in identifying healthier snack options. Food Qual. Prefer. 83:103894. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.103894

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris Interactive Survey (2019). Daily purchases, on-the-go habits and health credentials: 10 charts that explains UK attitudes to biscuits. The Grocer. Available online at: https://www.thegrocer.co.uk/download?ac=173943. (accessed May 13, 2021)

Google Scholar

Hawkes, C. (2010). Food packaging: The medium is the message. Publ. Health Nutr. 13, 297–299. doi: 10.1017/s1368980009993168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoyer, W. D., Kroschke, M., Schmitt, B., Kraume, K., and Shankar, V. (2020). Transforming the Customer Experience Through New Technologies. J. Interact. Marke. 51, 57–71. doi: 10.1016/j.intmar.2020.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ikonen, I., Sotgiu, F., Aydinli, A., and Verlegh, P. W. J. (2020). Consumer effects of front-of-package nutrition labeling: an interdisciplinary meta-analysis. J. Acad. Marke. Sci. 48, 360–383. doi: 10.1007/s11747-019-00663-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Imram, N. (1999). The role of visual cues in consumer perception and acceptance of a food product. Nutr. Food Sci. 99, 224–230. doi: 10.1108/00346659910277650

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koo, J., and Suk, K. (2016). The effect of package shape on calorie estimation. Int. J. Res. Marke. 33, 856–867. doi: 10.1016/j.ijresmar.2016.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krishna, A., Cian, L., and Aydinoglu, N. Z. (2017). Sensory Aspects of Package Design. J. Retail. 93, 43–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jretai.2016.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Küster, I., Vila, N., and Sarabia, F. (2019). Food packaging cues as vehicles of healthy information: Visions of millennials (early adults and adolescents). Food Res. Int. 119, 170–176. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2019.01.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Letona, P., Chacon, V., Roberto, C., and Barnoya, J. (2014). A qualitative study of children’s snack food packaging perceptions and preferences. BMC Publ. Health 14:1274. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-1274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lwin, M. O., Morrin, M., Tang, S. W. H., Low, J. Y., Nguyen, T., and Lee, W. X. (2014). See the seal? Understanding restrained eaters’ responses to nutritional messages on food packaging. Health Commun. 29, 745–761. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2013.789131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Market Analysis Report (2019). Healthy Biscuits Market Size, Share & Trends Analysis Report By Type (Functional & Digestive, Gluten-Free, Reduced Calorie), By Distribution Channel, By Region, And Segment Forecasts, 2019 – 2025. Pune: Grand View Research.

Google Scholar

Marques da Rosa, V., Spence, C., and Miletto Tonetto, L. (2018). Influences of visual attributes of food packaging on consumer preference and associations with taste and healthiness. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 43, 210–217. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12500

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mead, J. A., and Richerson, R. (2018). Package color saturation and food healthfulness perceptions. J. Bus. Res. 82, 10–18. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2017.08.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miklavec, K., Pravst, I., Raats, M. M., and Pohar, J. (2016). Front of package symbols as a tool to promote healthier food choices in Slovenia: Accompanying explanatory claim can considerably influence the consumer’s preferences. Food Res. Int. 90, 235–243. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2016.10.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miraballes, M., Fiszman, S., Gambaro, A., and Varela, P. (2014). Consumer perceptions of satiating and meal replacement bars, built up from cues in packaging information, health claims and nutritional claims. Food Res. Int. 64, 456–464. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2014.07.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Overmars, S., and Poels, K. (2015). A touching experience: Designing for touch sensations in online retail environments. Int. J. Des. 9, 17–31.

Google Scholar

Petit, O., Javornik, A., and Velasco, C. (2021). We eat first with our (digital) Eyes: Enhancing mental simulation of eating experiences via visual-enabling technologies. J. Retail. 416, 0022–4359. doi: 10.1016/j.jretai.2021.04.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petit, O., Velasco, C., and Spence, C. (2019). Digital sensory marketing: Integrating new technologies into multisensory online experience. J. Interact. Marke. 45, 42–61. doi: 10.1016/j.intmar.2018.07.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinson, C. (1986). An implicit product theory approach to consumers’ inferential judgments about products. Int. J. Res. Marke. 3, 19–38. doi: 10.1016/0167-8116(86)90040-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinto, V. R. A., Freitas, T. B. O., Dantas, M. I. S., Della Lucia, S. M., Melo, L. F., Minim, V. P. R., et al. (2017). Influence of package and health-related claims on perception and sensory acceptability of snack bars. Food Res. Int. 101, 103–113. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2017.08.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raudenbush, B., Meyer, B., Corley, W. E. N., and Patterson, S. (2002). Rating of pleasantness and intensity for beverage served in containers congruent and incongruent with expectancy. Percept. Motor Skill 94, 671–674. doi: 10.2466/pms.2002.94.2.671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinoso Carvalho, F., Moors, P., Wagemans, J., and Spence, C. (2017a). The influence of color on the consumer’s experience of beer. Front. Psychol. 8:2205. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinoso Carvalho, F., Wang, Q. J., van Ee, R., Persoone, D., and Spence, C. (2017b). “Smooth operator”: Music modulates the perceived creaminess, sweetness, and bitterness of chocolate. Appetite 108, 383–390. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.10.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, G. L. (2011). Packaging materials for biscuits and their influence on shelf life. In Manley’s Technology of Biscuits, Crackers and Cookies. Sawston: Woodhead Publishing, 247–267.

Google Scholar

Sabri, O., Van Doan, H., Malek, F., and Bachouche, H. (2020). When is transparent packaging beneficial? Int. J. Retail Distribut. Manag. 48, 781–801. doi: 10.1108/ijrdm-03-2019-0097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schifferstein, H. N. J. (2006). The relative importance of sensory modalities in product usage: a study of self-reports. Acta Psychol. 121, 41–64. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schifferstein, H. N. J., and Cleiren, M. (2005). Capturing product experiences: a split-modality approach. Acta Psychol. 118, 293–318. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2004.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schifferstein, H. N. J., and Desmet, P. M. A. (2007). The effect of sensory impairments on product experience and personal well-being. Ergonomics 50, 2026–2048. doi: 10.1080/00140130701524056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selame, T., and Koukos, P. (2002). Is your package shelf-evident? Design Manag. J. 13, 25–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1948-7169.2002.tb00327.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silayoi, P., and Speece, M. (2004). The importance of packaging attributes: a conjoint analysis approach. Eur. J. Marke. 41, 1495–1517. doi: 10.1108/03090560710821279

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silayoi, P., and Speece, M. (2007). Packaging and purchase decisions: An exploratory study on the impact of involvement level and time pressure. Br. Food J. 106, 607–628. doi: 10.1108/00070700410553602

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simmonds, G., and Spence, C. (2017). Thinking inside the box: How seeing products on, or through, the packaging influences consumer perceptions and purchase behaviour. Food Qual. Prefer. 62, 340–351. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.11.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simmonds, G., Woods, A. T., and Spence, C. (2018). ‘Show me the goods’: Assessing the effectiveness of transparent packaging vs. product imagery on product evaluation. Food Qual. Prefer. 63, 18–27. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.07.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simmonds, G., Woods, A. T., and Spence, C. (2019). “‘Shaping perceptions’: Exploring how the shape of transparent windows in packaging designs affects product evaluation. Food Qual. Prefer. 75, 15–22. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review. Attent. Percept. & Psychophys. 73, 971–995. doi: 10.3758/s13414-010-0073-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spence, C., and Carvalho, F. M. (2019). Assessing the influence of the coffee cup on the multisensory tasting experience. Food Qual. Prefer. 75, 239–248. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spence, C., and Velasco, C. (2018). On the multiple effects of packaging colour on consumer behaviour and product experience in the ‘food and beverage’ and ‘home and personal care’categories. Food Qual. Prefer. 68, 226–237. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.03.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spence, C., and Wan, X. (2015). Beverage perception and consumption: The influence of the container on the perception of the contents. Food Qual. Prefer. 39, 131–140. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.07.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinhauser, J., and Hamm, U. (2018). Consumer and product-specific characteristics influencing the effect of nutrition, health and risk reduction claims on preferences and purchase behavior – a systematic review. Appetite 127, 303–323. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinhauser, J., Janssen, M., and Hamm, U. (2019). Consumers’ purchase decisions for products with nutrition and health claims: what role do product category and gaze duration on claims play? Appetite 141:104337. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tijssen, I., Zandstra, E. H., de Graaf, C., and Jager, G. (2017). Why a ‘light’ product package should not be light blue: Effects of package colour on perceived healthiness and attractiveness of sugar- and fat-reduced products. Food Qual. Prefer. 59, 46–58. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.01.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tu, Y., Yang, Z., and Ma, C. (2015). Touching taste: the haptic perception transfer of liquid food packaging materials. Food Qual. Prefer. 39, 124–130. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuorilla, H., and Cardello, A. V. (2002). Consumer responses to an off-flavor in juice in the presence of specific health claims. Food Qual. Prefer. 13, 561–569. doi: 10.1016/s0950-3293(01)00076-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Ooijen, I., Fransen, M. L., Verlegh, P. W. J., and Smit, E. G. (2017). Signalling product healthiness through symbolic package cues: Effects of package shape and goal congruence on consumer behavior. Appetite 109, 73–82. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Rompay, T. J. L., Fransen, M. L., and Borgelink, B. G. D. (2014). Light as a feather: Effects of packaging imagery on sensory product impressions and brand evaluation. Marke. Lett. 25, 397–407. doi: 10.1007/s11002-013-9260-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Velasco, C., and Spence, C. (eds) (2019). Multisensory Packaging: Designing New Product Experiences. Cham: Palgrave MacMillan.

Google Scholar

Velasco, C., Salgado-Montejo, A., Marmolejo-Ramos, F., and Spence, C. (2014). Predictive packaging design: Tasting shapes, typographies, names, and sounds. Food Qual. Prefer. 34, 88–95. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2013.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Velasco, C., Woods, A. T., Petit, O., Cheok, A. D., and Spence, C. (2016). Crossmodal correspondences between taste and shape, and their implication for product packaging: a review. Food Qual. Prefer. 52, 17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verbeke, W., Scholderer, J., and Lähteenmäki, L. (2009). Consumer appeal of nutrition and health claims in three existing product concepts. Appetite 52, 684–692. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vieira, V. A. (2010). Visual aesthetics in store environment and its moderating role on consumer intention. J. Consum. Behav. 9, 364–380. doi: 10.1002/cb.324

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vith, E. L., Steenhuis, I. H., Vlot, J. A., Wulp, A., Hogenes, M. G., Looije, D. H., et al. (2010). Actual use of a front-of-pack nutrition logo in the supermarket: consumers’ motives in food choice. Publ. Health Nutr. 13, 1882–1889. doi: 10.1017/s1368980010000637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yarar, N., Machiels, C. J. A., and Orth, U. R. (2019). Shaping up: How package shape and consumer body conspire to affect food healthiness evaluation. Food Qual. Prefer. 75, 209–219. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zellner, D. A., Strickhouser, D., and Tornow, C. E. (2004). Disconfirmed hedonic expectations produce perceptual contrast, not assimilation. Am. J. Psychol. 117, 363–387. doi: 10.2307/4149006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix

APPENDIX TABLE A
www.frontiersin.org

Appendix Table A. Summary of experimental design, including the variables sampled, and the system used to measure each variable.

Keywords: crossmodal correspondences, dietary cookies, expectation, packaging, well-being

Citation: Reinoso-Carvalho F, Campo R, De Luca M and Velasco C (2021) Toward Healthier Cookie Habits: Assessing the Role of Packaging Visual Appearance in the Expectations for Dietary Cookies in Digital Environments. Front. Psychol. 12:679443. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.679443

Received: 11 March 2021; Accepted: 15 June 2021;
Published: 22 July 2021.

Edited by:

Lieve Doucé, Hasselt University, Belgium

Reviewed by:

Cristian Rusu, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Valparaíso, Chile
Jason Bernard, McMaster University, Canada

Copyright © 2021 Reinoso-Carvalho, Campo, De Luca and Velasco. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Felipe Reinoso-Carvalho, Zi5zb3VuZEBnbWFpbC5jb20=; Zi5yZWlub3NvY0B1bmlhbmRlcy5lZHUuY28=

These authors share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.