- 1Programa de Doctorado en Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de La Frontera, Temuco, Chile
- 2Departamento de Psicología, Universidad Católica de Temuco, Temuco, Chile
- 3Departamento de Matemática y Estadística, Universidad de La Frontera, Temuco, Chile
- 4Núcleo Científico y Tecnológico en Ciencias Sociales (LICSA), Universidad de La Frontera, Temuco, Chile
Cyberbullying has progressively increased due to the massive use of the internet and social networks. Bystanders constitute the largest group, occupying a key role in the evolution of the cyberbullying situation and its consequences for the victim. Research shows different ways in which bystanders behave, suggesting different types of sub-roles associated with different study variables. The objective of this literature review is to identify and characterize the roles of bystanders in cyberbullying situations that involve adolescent students. To achieve this objective, a systematic search was carried out in the Web of Science, PubMed, and Scopus databases for articles published between 2015 and 2020, resulting in 233 articles. Articles were then selected by relevant title and summary. Subsequently, the inclusion and exclusion criteria were applied, resulting in a total of nine articles. The findings of this review allowed us to identify two to five types of bystanders, the largest type representing outsiders and the smallest type representing assistants of the aggressor. The identified types of bystanders are characterized for variables such as sex, age, previous experience, and empathy. The results are discussed considering the available theoretical and empirical evidence.
Introduction
Technology and the internet have transformed society through a process of mutual interaction, allowing communication at any time and transcending territorial borders (Castells, 2014). Internet use has become widespread in adolescents, with 92.9% reporting having at least one social media account (Barry et al., 2017) and using an average of three different platforms daily (Vannucci and Ohannessian, 2019). Constant exposure to social networks and the internet raises concern about the possible negative effects on the well-being of adolescents, specifically due to phenomena such as cyberbullying, which is positively associated with online time (Lee and Shin, 2017; Shapka et al., 2018; Craig et al., 2020), and favored by the use of smartphones, which allow connection at any time and place (Martin et al., 2018).
Cyberbullying can be defined as a form of intentional harassment that is directed at a particular person, carried out through electronic and digital means and produced by an imbalance of power associated with greater development of technical skills when using the internet (Smith et al., 2013; Olweus and Limber, 2018). Additionally, anonymity is considered a significant risk factor in this phenomenon (Barlett et al., 2016, 2020). On the other hand, cyberbullying is considered a social phenomenon that can negatively affect victims, aggressors, and bully/victims, significantly increasing the risk of suicide (DeSmet et al., 2014, 2019; Hellfeldt et al., 2020; Kim et al., 2020).
Regarding the prevalence of cyberbullying, one in three adolescent participants reported having been a victim of cyberbullying (United Nations International Children's Emergency Fund (UNICEF), 2019). Research by Antoniadou et al. (2019) classified cyberbullying participants as bystanders (75%), bully/victims (11.2%), victims (8.2%), and aggressors (5.6%). Thus, the percentage of bystanders in cyberbullying situations was significantly higher than the other roles, making it relevant to deepen the understanding of bystander characteristics and behavior due to the impact their actions may have on the development of the situation and the experience of the victim and the aggressor.
The Role of Bystanders in Cyberbullying and Its Relationship With Personal and Contextual Variables
At present, the role of bystanders in cyberbullying has received little attention, despite its relevance and conceptual differences with traditional bullying (Garaigordobil, 2017; Sarmiento et al., 2019). Specifically, Kozubal et al. (2019) reported that, when bystanders are exposed to a human face with an expression of sadness, they can modify their behavior and not reinforce cyberbullying. However, this is far from what happens on internet platforms since the facial expression of the victim is rarely seen.
Regarding the role of bystanders and sex, some studies support the hypothesis that adolescent women tend to show more supportive behaviors toward the victim compared to men (Machackova et al., 2016; Allison and Bussey, 2017; Campbell et al., 2017; Patterson et al., 2017), but there is also evidence that there are no significant differences between the sexes (Kozubal et al., 2019). Regarding bystanders and age, studies showed that there are no behavioral differences (Machackova et al., 2016; Campbell et al., 2017), although Pabian et al. (2016) reported that students had a less empathetic response 6 months after the first measurement, which could reflect a desensitization effect over time.
Regarding the role of the bystanders and their association with the socio-affective variables in cyberbullying, the presence of greater moral disengagement, less empathy, and a lower perception of responsibility and self-efficacy is reported, since bystanders do not see the emotional response of the victim, which interferes with their evaluation of the situation (Barlińska et al., 2018; Domínguez-Hernández et al., 2018; Knauf et al., 2018). On the other hand, there are contextual variables that affect the emergence and type of behavior displayed by bystanders in the face of cyberbullying, including degree of friendship, severity of the incident, actions of other bystanders (Domínguez-Hernández et al., 2018), and whether the situation is non-anonymous (You and Lee, 2019) or those situations that occur in private (DeSmet et al., 2014; Patterson et al., 2017). The last variable is linked to personal characteristics since adolescents with greater empathy will decide to intervene regardless of whether it is in private or in public (Wang, 2020).
Regarding the role of bystanders in traditional bullying, Salmivalli et al. (1996) and Salmivalli (2010) referred to different bystander types: assistants of the aggressor (join the aggressor), reinforcers of the aggressor (laugh or encourage the aggressor), outsiders (do not get involved in the situation), and defenders of the victim (support victims). This classification is used by the renowned anti-bullying program, Kiusaamista Vastaan (KiVa) (Salmivalli and Poskiparta, 2012), developed in Finland, which works with victims, aggressors, and bystanders. Similarly, within the role of the bystanders, specific types can be differentiated, making it important not to homogenize them since this would hinder the understanding of the implications that the different forms of behavior have on the other participants (Moxey and Bussey, 2019).
As a result of the above, it can be pointed out that there is not much knowledge about how bystanders behave in cyberbullying situations, making it necessary to continue generating data on the subject (Garaigordobil, 2017; Sarmiento et al., 2019; Íñiguez-Berrozpe et al., 2020). Along with this, the studies that address types of bystander behaviors denominate, group, and characterize them in different ways, making it difficult to understand the sub-roles and their characteristics. Therefore, the objective of this study was to identify and characterize the roles of bystanders in cyberbullying in adolescent students.
Methods
This article reports the results of a systematic review of the different roles of bystanders in cyberbullying. A systematic literature search was carried out following the guidelines of Bramer et al. (2018) in the Web of Science, PubMed, and Scopus databases in November 2020. The question that guided the search strategy was: What are the characteristics of bystander roles in cyberbullying situations in adolescent students? To carry out the review, a combination of search terms were applied, including Medical Subject Headings (MeSH), for example, bystander, cyberbystanders, adolescent (MeSH), student (MeSH), cyberbullying (MeSH), participant role, and bystander intervention. From several articles (n = 233), those with a title related to the topic of interest (n = 98) were first screened followed by those having a relevant abstract (n = 47). Subsequently, the full articles were reviewed following the standard quality assessment criteria used to evaluate primary research articles (Kmet et al., 2004), and the inclusion and exclusion criteria were applied, resulting in nine articles being selected (Figure 1).
Inclusion Criteria
Articles that reported an identification and/or characterization of the types of bystanders in cyberbullying were included. Only scientific articles published in English or Spanish between the years 2015 and 2020 that included samples of adolescents were considered for the review.
Exclusion Criteria
Articles that only addressed one type of bystander or that referred to this role in a general way were excluded. Articles that addressed traditional bullying, violence, or aggression between school children or cyberbullying in preschool children, elementary school children, or adults were excluded. Theses, conference proceedings, systematic reviews, or articles in a language other than English or Spanish or whose publication date preceded 2015 were not considered for the review.
Results
Nine high-quality investigations were analyzed (Table 1), mostly representing cross-sectional studies. Due to their design, it was evident that the objectives, study design, sample selection method, measurement instruments, data analysis methods, and results were rigorously described and were appropriate in each of the selected studies.
The studies came from different countries (Belgium, Spain, Czech Republic, Thailand, Germany, Israel, and Australia). The mean sample size of the nine studies was 1,572.7, but there was high variability (SD = 1,444.33). The sample sizes were between 321 and 5,036 adolescent students. Participant ages ranged from 9 to 23 years with most between 12 and 17 years, since the objective of the review was to address adolescent bystanders. Regarding the sex of the participants, the study by Quirk and Campbell (2015) stands out because it had a much higher percentage of women (75.4%), compared to the other investigations which had distributions close to 50% between the sexes.
Bystander Roles in Cyberbullying: How Many and Who
Studies That Identified Two Bystander Groups
The study by DeSmet et al. (2016) referred to two groups of bystanders: those who show positive behavior (44.9%), that is, who defend or support the victim or report the incident to adults or peers, and those who show negative behavior (55.1%), which is related to passive behavior or enjoying and/or reinforcing the behavior of the aggressor. Olenik-Shemesh et al. (2015) reported a passive bystander role that included those who did not get involved in the cyberbullying situation (55.4%) and an active bystander role (44.6%) that incorporated adolescents who supported the victim. On the other hand, Machackova and Pfetsch (2016) made a distinction between adolescents who supported the victims and those who reinforced the actions of the aggressor, but the study did not report the percentage of each role.
Studies That Identified Three Bystander Groups
The studies by Panumaporn et al. (2020) and Erreygers et al. (2016) noted the following distinction: adolescents willing to intervene or help the victims (42.3–34.6%, respectively); adolescents who ignore the cyberbullying situation (53.6–28%); and finally, bystanders who join the cyberbully (4.6–26.3%) (Erreygers et al., 2016; Panumaporn et al., 2020).
Studies That Identified Four Bystander Groups
These studies showed a similar distribution and percentage of adolescents in each of the roles. Specifically, the following trends were observed: outsiders, 63.2–60.7%; defenders, 25–30.5%; reinforcers, 7.4–5.4%; and assistants 4.4–3.3% (Quirk and Campbell, 2015; Song and Oh, 2018). Also, Schultze-Krumbholz et al. (2018) distinguished four sub-roles but made a differentiation in terms of the types of the defender, with aggressive defenders characterized by behaviors such as confronting the aggressor, activating others, and comforting victims (9.5%) and prosocial (52.2%), who are more likely to support the victim and inform their parents. The other bystander subtypes indicated are outsiders (28.4%) and assistants (2.8%).
Studies That Identify Five Bystander Groups
The study by González-Cabrera et al. (2019) had the most extensive classification, grouping the participants into five sub-roles: defender of the victim (54.6%), supporter of the victim (18.3%), outsiders (22.7%), cyberbullying reinforcer (1.6%), and assistants of the aggressor (2.2%). There was a distinction made between positive behavior in the case of those who defended the victim, those who interrupted the situation, and those who provided help, in contrast with adolescents who only supported the victim but did not stop the aggressor.
Bystander Roles in Cyberbullying: Who and Why
Regarding the different roles of cyberbystanders and gender, three investigations observed a higher percentage of men in the groups that presented negative behaviors (Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2015; DeSmet et al., 2016), specifically, in the roles of assistants of the aggressor (Quirk and Campbell, 2015; Schultze-Krumbholz et al., 2018) and outsiders (González-Cabrera et al., 2019). Meanwhile, women tended to show an active role, providing help to the victim of cyberbullying (Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2015; Machackova et al., 2016; González-Cabrera et al., 2019; Panumaporn et al., 2020). On the contrary, other studies analyzed did not find gender differences (Erreygers et al., 2016; Machackova and Pfetsch, 2016; Schultze-Krumbholz et al., 2018; Song and Oh, 2018).
With regard to age, four investigations indicated that younger adolescents were more likely to carry out positive interventions in cyberbullying situations than older adolescents, who tended to show negative or passive behavior (Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2015; Quirk and Campbell, 2015; DeSmet et al., 2016; Erreygers et al., 2016). Consequently, two studies showed a higher percentage of older adolescents in the group of bystanders who joined the aggressor (Erreygers et al., 2016; Panumaporn et al., 2020). On the other hand, three of the nine investigations did not find differences between ages (Machackova et al., 2016; Schultze-Krumbholz et al., 2018; Song and Oh, 2018).
In relation to personal variables, these were reported to have a significant association with the different bystander roles and were present with greater frequency in the results obtained. It was shown that experiences of previous victimization, those experienced directly or indirectly through close reports, increased the interventions to provide help to the victim in a cyberbullying situation (DeSmet et al., 2016; Erreygers et al., 2016; Panumaporn et al., 2020). However, previous experiences in the role of aggressor were linked to bystanders who were willing to join the cyberbully (Panumaporn et al., 2020). Therefore, Song and Oh (2018) reported that defenders do not share experiences of this type. Another relevant variable in the research is the self-efficacy perceived by adolescents regarding their intervention in the cyberbullying situation (Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2015; DeSmet et al., 2016).
With regard to social skills, the reviewed studies showed that empathy levels were low in adolescents who showed negative behaviors (DeSmet et al., 2016), such as those in the roles of assistants to the aggressor and outsiders (Erreygers et al., 2016; Schultze-Krumbholz et al., 2018), specifically, an association was shown with cognitive empathy (Barlińska et al., 2018; Schultze-Krumbholz et al., 2018). In contrast, adolescents who were willing to intervene had higher empathy levels (Erreygers et al., 2016; Schultze-Krumbholz et al., 2018; Song and Oh, 2018). Machackova et al. (2016) reported that affective empathy predicts support for the victim, while cognitive empathy did not show significant results. Along the same line, another variable associated with bystander roles was moral disengagement, which was related to interventions that supported the victim, while the greater the moral disengagement, the more the passive behavior increased (DeSmet et al., 2016; Song and Oh, 2018).
Discussion
The objective of this research was to identify and characterize the roles of cyberbullying bystanders in adolescent students. The results allowed the identification of nine articles (Table 1), whose participants were adolescents from different countries (Belgium, Spain, Czech Republic, Thailand, Germany, Israel, and Australia), that met the standard quality assessment criteria (Kmet et al., 2004). The findings of this study showed that bystanders are not a homogeneous group in terms of characteristics and behavior. Specifically, the selected studies identified between two to five types of bystanders. Although the investigations that identified two groups of bystanders (Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2015; DeSmet et al., 2016; Machackova and Pfetsch, 2016), represent a progress in the studies that consider them a completely homogeneous group, they are still considered too general and could limit the understanding of the particularities of bystander roles in cyberbullying (Patterson et al., 2017; Knauf et al., 2018; Wright et al., 2018).
Differences were established with adolescents who show negative behavior, since their actions may be oriented toward either ignoring the situation or joining the aggressor (Erreygers et al., 2016; Panumaporn et al., 2020). The classification of four types of cyberbystanders (assistants, reinforcers, outsiders, and defenders) stood out because the sizes of these groups were similar in the Republic of Korea (Song and Oh, 2018) and in Australia (Quirk and Campbell, 2015), and this classification of bystanders coincided with that of Salmivalli et al. (1996) in regard to traditional bullying. Another finding revealed that, in most of the studies reviewed, there were high percentages of adolescents who did not intervene in cyberbullying situations (Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2015; Quirk and Campbell, 2015; DeSmet et al., 2016; Erreygers et al., 2016; Song and Oh, 2018), contrary to research that shows that the highest percentage of adolescents assume the role of the spectator who intervenes, defends, and helps the victim (Schultze-Krumbholz et al., 2018; González-Cabrera et al., 2019). In this sense, the distinction between defenders who show prosocial behavior and defenders who manifest aggressive behavior stands out (Schultze-Krumbholz et al., 2018), showing that these students also assume other roles in cyberbullying such as those of victim or aggressor. This reflects that the internet favors less stable roles.
With regard to the different types of bystander roles and their characterization, it can be noted that several studies associated a higher percentage of males with roles that manifest negative or passive behavior, such as reinforcing or joining the aggressor (Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2015; Quirk and Campbell, 2015; DeSmet et al., 2016; Schultze-Krumbholz et al., 2018; González-Cabrera et al., 2019), while women tended to show behavior aimed at providing help to the cyberbullying victim (Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2015; Machackova et al., 2016; González-Cabrera et al., 2019; Panumaporn et al., 2020). This coincides with previous research on the role of bystanders, which associated women with greater prosociality (Allison and Bussey, 2017; Campbell et al., 2017; Patterson et al., 2017). Findings related to age were contradictory, with research showing that younger adolescents tended to carry out positive interventions in cyberbullying situations compared to older adolescents who showed negative or passive behavior (Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2015; Quirk and Campbell, 2015; DeSmet et al., 2016; Erreygers et al., 2016; Panumaporn et al., 2020), which could be explained as a desensitization effect (Pabian et al., 2016).
Along the same lines, the previous experiences that adolescents have had both in the role of victim and in the role of the aggressor are relevant (DeSmet et al., 2016; Erreygers et al., 2016; Song and Oh, 2018; Panumaporn et al., 2020), concurring with other investigations (Charaschanya and Blauw, 2017; Domínguez-Hernández et al., 2018; Kozubal et al., 2019; Huang et al., 2020). In the case of students in the spectator role with previous experiences as an aggressor, it is evident that they could continue to affect the lives of other students by supporting cyberbullying situations. This result shows the need to continue making efforts to curb the increase in cyberbullying in a highly digitized society. On the other hand, the self-efficacy perception of the adolescent and the belief that their actions can interrupt the cyberbullying situation arose as a factor addressed in the literature (Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2015; DeSmet et al., 2016), since adolescents who did not intervene had lower levels of social and emotional self-efficacy and therefore reported a higher perception of fear (Olenik-Shemesh et al., 2015).
Regarding social skills, many studies addressed empathy, noting that it was related to adolescents who intervened positively and with those who showed negative behaviors (DeSmet et al., 2016; Erreygers et al., 2016; Schultze-Krumbholz et al., 2018; Song and Oh, 2018). It is relevant to note that greater empathy is shown when the positive intervention involves prosocial and non-aggressive behaviors. It should also be taken into account that different investigations show a significant association specifically with cognitive empathy: in contrast, this relationship is not observed for affective empathy (Barlińska et al., 2018; Schultze-Krumbholz et al., 2018), although there is no agreement on this (Kozubal et al., 2019). It is also linked to sex, since women view cyberbullying situations as more serious and intervene more frequently, which could be explained by greater empathy (Huang et al., 2019; 2020). However, according to DeSmet et al. (2016), empathy is a less significant predictor in cyberbullying than in traditional bullying due to the lower emotional participation of students linked to the characteristics of digital platforms. In turn, greater moral disengagement was associated with the different bystander roles that exhibit passive behavior (DeSmet et al., 2016; Song and Oh, 2018). Coincidentally, studies have linked difficulties in the development of social skills with cyberbullying, observing that moral disengagement is related to problems in evaluating the situation they are witnessing (Domínguez-Hernández et al., 2018; Knauf et al., 2018; Antoniadou et al., 2019).
It should be noted that the efforts to identify and characterize the types of bystanders were aimed at generating a greater understanding of who, how many, and why adolescents belong in each of the bystander groups. Achieving greater knowledge in the area will allow better planning and development of interventions, considering that bystanders do not represent a group with homogeneous characteristics. Thus, it is relevant to include distinctions between spectators related to personal and social variables that transcend the binary categorization of active/passive behavior or positive/negative behavior. It is also necessary to integrate contextual variables, transitioning from individual to social (Gálvez-Nieto et al., 2020), since cyberbullying is a phenomenon that requires a multidisciplinary approach.
Author Contributions
All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.
Funding
KP-L received financial support by the National Agency for Research and Development (ANID)/Scholarship Program/DOCTORADO BECAS CHILE/2020 - 21200712.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
Allison, K. R., and Bussey, K. (2017). Individual and collective moral influences on intervention in cyberbullying. Comput. Hum. Behav. 74, 7–15. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.04.019
Antoniadou, N., Kokkinos, C. M., and Fanti, K. A. (2019). Traditional and cyber bullying/victimization among adolescents: Examining their psychosocial profile through latent profile analysis. Int. J. Bullying Prevent. 1, 85–98. doi: 10.1007/s42380-019-00010-0
Barlett, C. P., Gentile, D. A., and Chew, C. (2016). Predicting cyberbullying from anonymity. Psychol. Popular Media Culture 5:171. doi: 10.1037/ppm0000055
Barlett, C. P., Heath, J. B., Madison, C. S., DeWitt, C. C., and Kirkpatrick, S. M. (2020). You're not anonymous online: The development and validation of a new cyberbullying intervention curriculum. Psychol. Popular Media 9, 135–144. doi: 10.1037/ppm0000226
Barlińska, J., Szuster, A., and Winiewski, M. (2018). Cyberbullying among adolescent bystanders: role of affective versus cognitive empathy in increasing prosocial cyberbystander behavior. Front. Psychol. 9:799. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00799
Barry, C. T., Sidoti, C. L., Briggs, S. M., Reiter, S. R., and Lindsey, R. A. (2017). Adolescent social media use and mental health from adolescent and parent perspectives. J. Adolesc. 61, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2017.08.005
Bramer, W. M., de Jonge, G. B., Rethlefsen, M. L., Mast, F., and Kleijnen, J. (2018). A systematic approach to searching: an efficient and complete method to develop literature searches. J. Medical Library Assoc. 106, 531–541. doi: 10.5195/JMLA.2018.283
Campbell, M. A., Whiteford, C., Duncanson, K., Spears, B., Butler, D., and Slee, P. T. (2017). Cyberbullying Bystanders: Gender, Grade, and Actions among Primary and Secondary School Students in Australia. Int. J. Technoethics. 8, 44–55. doi: 10.4018/IJT.2017010104
Castells, M. (2014). The impact of the internet on society: a global perspective. Change. 19, 127–148. Retrieved from: http://aasa.ut.ee/augsburg/literature/CASTELLS_BBVA-OpenMind-book-Change-19-key-essays-on-how-internet-is-changing-our-lives-Technology-Internet-Innovation.pdf
Charaschanya, A., and Blauw, J. (2017). A study of the direct and indirect relationships between online disinhibition and depression and stress being mediated by the frequency of cyberbullying from victim and perpetrator perspectives. Scholar Hum. Sci. 9, 275–301. Retrieved from: http://www.assumptionjournal.au.edu/index.php/Scholar/article/view/3003
Craig, W., Boniel-Nissim, M., King, N., Walsh, S. D., Boer, M., Donnelly, P. D., et al. (2020). Social media use and cyber-bullying: a cross-national analysis of young people in 42 countries. J. Adolescent Health, 66, S100–S108. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.03.006
DeSmet, A., Bastiaensens, S., Van Cleemput, K., Poels, K., Vandebosch, H., Cardon, G., et al. (2016). Deciding whether to look after them, to like it, or leave it: a multidimensional analysis of predictors of positive and negative bystander behavior in cyberbullying among adolescents. Comput. Hum. Behav. 57, 398–415. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.12.051
DeSmet, A., De Bourdeaudhuij, I., Walrave, M., and Vandebosch, H. (2019). Associations between bystander reactions to cyberbullying and victims' emotional experiences and mental health. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Network. 22, 648–656. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2019.0031
DeSmet, A., Veldeman, C., Poels, K., Bastiaensens, S., Van Cleemput, K., Vandebosch, H., et al. (2014). Determinants of self-reported bystander behavior in cyberbullying incidents amongst adolescents. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Network. 17, 207–215. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2013.0027
Domínguez-Hernández, F., Bonell, L., and Martínez-González, A. (2018). A systematic literature review of factors that moderate bystanders' actions in cyberbullying. Cyberpsychology 12. doi: 10.5817/CP2018-4-1
Erreygers, S., Pabian, S., Vandebosch, H., and Baillien, E. (2016). Helping behavior among adolescent bystanders of cyberbullying: the role of impulsivity. Learn. Individual Differ. 48, 61–67. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.03.003
Gálvez-Nieto, J. L., García, J. A., Vera-Bachmann, D., Trizano-Hermosilla, I., and Polanco, K. (2020). Multilevel latent class cluster analysis of school climate: individual, family and community factors. Revista de Psicodidáctica 25, 85–92. doi: 10.1016/j.psicoe.2020.01.002
Garaigordobil, M. (2017). Conducta antisocial: conexión con bullying/cyberbullying y estrategias de resolución de conflictos. Psychosoc. Intervent. 26, 47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.psi.2015.12.002
González-Cabrera, J. M., León-Mejía, A., Machimbarrena, J. M., Balea, A., and Calvete, E. (2019). Psychometric properties of the cyberbullying triangulation questionnaire: a prevalence analysis through seven roles. Scand. J. Psychol. 60, 160–168. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12518
Hellfeldt, K., López-Romero, L., and Andershed, H. (2020). Cyberbullying and psychological well-being in young adolescence: the potential protective mediation effects of social support from family, friends, and teachers. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:45. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17010045
Huang, C. L., Yang, S. C., and Hsieh, L. S. (2019). The cyberbullying behavior of adolescents in an online gaming environment. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 106, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.104461
Huang, C. L., Zhang, S., and Yang, S. C. (2020). How students react to different cyberbullying events: past experience, judgment, perceived seriousness, helping behavior and the effect of online disinhibition. Comput. Hum. Behav. 110:106338. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2020.106338
Íñiguez-Berrozpe, T., Cano-Escoriaza, J., Cortés-Pascual, A., and Elboj-Saso, C. (2020). Structural model of concurrence among relational bullying and cyberbullying: victims, aggressors and bystanders. Revista Española de Investigaciones Sociol. 171, 63–84. doi: 10.5477/cis/reis.171.63
Kim, J., Walsh, E., Pike, K., and Thompson, E. A. (2020). Cyberbullying and victimization and youth suicide risk: the buffering effects of school connectedness. J. School Nurs. 36, 251–257. doi: 10.1177/1059840518824395
Kmet, L. M., Cook, L. S., and Lee, R. C. (2004). Standard Quality Assessment Criteria for Evaluating Primary Research Papers From a Variety of Fields. Edmonton, AB: Alberta Heritage Foundation for Medical Research (AHFMR).
Knauf, R. K., Eschenbeck, H., and Hock, M. (2018). Bystanders of bullying: social-cognitive and affective reactions to school bullying and cyberbullying. Cyberpsychology 12. doi: 10.5817/CP2018-4-3
Kozubal, M., Szuster, A., and Barlińska, J. (2019). Cyberbystanders, affective empathy and social norms. Stud. Psychol. 61, 120–131. doi: 10.21909/sp.2019.02.777
Lee, C., and Shin, N. (2017). Prevalence of cyberbullying and predictors of cyberbullying perpetration among Korean adolescents. Comput. Hum. Behav. 68, 352–358. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.11.047
Machackova, H., Dedkova, L., Sevcikova, A., and Cerna, A. (2016). Bystanders' supportive and passive responses to cyberaggression. J. School Violence 17, 99–110. doi: 10.1080/15388220.2016.1222499
Machackova, H., and Pfetsch, J. (2016). Bystanders' responses to offline bullying and cyberbullying: the role of empathy and normative beliefs about aggression. Scand. J. Psychol. 57, 169–176. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12277
Martin, F., Wang, C., Petty, T., Wang, W., and Wilkins, P. (2018). Middle school students' social media use. J. Educ. Technol. Soc. 21, 213–224. Retrieved from: https://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/26273881.pdf?refreqid=excelsior%3A6e8a0fc0568faaffe1867db9f44d853c
Moxey, N., and Bussey, K. (2019). Styles of bystander intervention in cyberbullying incidents. Int. J. Bullying Prevent. 2, 6–15. doi: 10.1007/s42380-019-00039-1
Olenik-Shemesh, D., Heiman, T., and Eden, S. (2015). Bystanders' behavior in cyberbullying episodes. J. Interpers. Violence 32, 23–48. doi: 10.1177/0886260515585531
Olweus, D., and Limber, S. P. (2018). Some problems with cyberbullying research. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 19, 139–143. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.04.012
Pabian, S., Vandebosch, H., Poels, K., Van Cleemput, K., and Bastiaensens, S. (2016). Exposure to cyberbullying as a bystander: an investigation of desensitization effects among early adolescents. Comput. Hum. Behav. 62, 480–487. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.04.022
Panumaporn, J., Hongsanguansri, S., Atsariyasing, W., and Kiatrungrit, K. (2020). Bystanders' behaviours and associated factors in cyberbullying. General Psychiatry 33, 1–8. doi: 10.1136/gpsych-2019-100187
Patterson, L. J., Allan, A., and Cross, D. (2017). Adolescent bystander behavior in the school and online environments and the implications for interventions targeting cyberbullying. J. School Violence 16, 361–375. doi: 10.1080/15388220.2016.1143835
Quirk, R., and Campbell, M. (2015). On standby? A comparison of online and offline witnesses to bullying and their bystander behaviour. Educ. Psychol. 35, 430–448. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2014.893556
Salmivalli, C. (2010). Bullying and the peer group: a review. Aggression Violent Behav. 15, 112–120. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2009.08.007
Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Björkqvist, K., Österman, K., and Kaukiainen, A. (1996). Bullying as a group process: participant roles and their relations to social status within the group. Aggres. Behav. 22, 1–15. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1996)22:1<1::AID-AB1>3.0.CO;2-T
Salmivalli, C., and Poskiparta, E. (2012). KiVa antibullying program: overview of evaluation studies based on a randomized controlled trial and national rollout in Finland. Int. J. Conflict Violence, 6, 293–301. doi: 10.4119/ijcv-2920
Sarmiento, A., Herrera-López, M., and Zych, I. (2019). Is cyberbullying a group process? Online and offline bystanders of cyberbullying act as defenders, reinforcers and outsiders. Comput. Hum. Behav. 99, 328–334. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.05.037
Schultze-Krumbholz, A., Hess, M., Pfetsch, J., and Scheithauer, H. (2018). Who is involved in cyberbullying? Latent class analysis of cyberbullying roles and their associations with aggression, self-esteem, and empathy. Cyberpsychology 12. doi: 10.5817/CP2018-4-2
Shapka, J. D., Onditi, H. Z., Collie, R. J., and Lapidot-Lefler, N. (2018). Cyberbullying and cybervictimization within a cross-cultural context: a study of Canadian and Tanzanian adolescents. Child Dev. 89, 89–99. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12829
Smith, P. K., del Barrio, C., and Tokunaga, R. S. (2013). “Definitions of bullying and cyberbullying: how useful are the terms?,” in Routledge Monographs in Mental Health. Principles of Cyberbullying Research: Definitions, Measures, and Methodology, eds S. Bauman, D. Cross, and J. Walker (New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor and Francis Group), 26–40.
Song, J., and Oh, I. (2018). Factors influencing bystanders' behavioral reactions in cyberbullying situations. Comput. Hum. Behav. 78, 273–282. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.10.008
United Nations International Children's Emergency Fund (UNICEF) (2019). More Than a Third of Young People in 30 Countries Report Being a Victim of Online Bullying: U-Report Highlights Prevalence of Cyberbullying and Its Impact on Young People. Available online at: http://www.unicef.org/press-releases/unicef-poll-more-third-young-people-30-countries-report-being-victim-online-bullying (accessed August 18, 2020).
Vannucci, A., and Ohannessian, C. M. (2019). Social media use subgroups differentially predict psychosocial well-being during early adolescence. J. Youth Adolesc. 48, 1469–1493. doi: 10.1007/s10964-019-01060-9
Wang, S. (2020). Standing up or standing by: Bystander intervention in cyberbullying on social media. New Media Soc/ 119:1461444820902541. doi: 10.1177/1461444820902541
Wright, M. F., Wachs, S., and Harper, B. D. (2018). The moderation of empathy in the longitudinal association between witnessing cyberbullying, depression, and anxiety. Cyberpsychology 12. doi: 10.5817/CP2018-4-6
Keywords: cyberbullying, bystanders, cyberbystanders, roles, adolescents, literature review
Citation: Polanco-Levicán K and Salvo-Garrido S (2021) Bystander Roles in Cyberbullying: A Mini-Review of Who, How Many, and Why. Front. Psychol. 12:676787. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.676787
Received: 06 March 2021; Accepted: 19 April 2021;
Published: 28 May 2021.
Edited by:
Claudio Longobardi, University of Turin, ItalyReviewed by:
Dorit Olenik-Shemesh, Open University of Israel, IsraelLidia Scifo, Libera Università Maria SS. Assunta, Italy
Copyright © 2021 Polanco-Levicán and Salvo-Garrido. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Sonia Salvo-Garrido, c29uaWEuc2Fsdm8mI3gwMDA0MDt1ZnJvbnRlcmEuY2w=