Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 01 July 2021
Sec. Educational Psychology

Exploring Emotion Regulation and Perceived Control as Antecedents of Anxiety and Its Consequences During Covid-19 Full Remote Learning

\nTing Zhao
Ting Zhao1*Zongmei Fu
Zongmei Fu1*Xi LianXi Lian2Linning YeLinning Ye3Wei HuangWei Huang1
  • 1School of Foreign Languages for Business, Southwestern University of Finance and Economics, Chengdu, China
  • 2School of English Education, Guangdong University of Foreign Studies, Guangzhou, China
  • 3Department of Computer Science, Southwestern University of Finance and Economics, Chengdu, China

Maintaining the emotional well-being of learners during a pandemic is important. This study explored the effects of two emotion regulation strategies (cognitive reappraisal, expressive suppression) and perceived control on full remote learners' anxiety during Covid-19, and their relationship to perceived learning. Structural equation modeling was used to analyze 239 questionnaires completed by Chinese graduate students taking a course remotely from home for 13 weeks. This study showed that reappraisal was positively related to perceived control, whereas suppression was negatively related to perceived control. Reappraisers perceived more learning, whereas suppressors experienced more anxiety. Anxiety was significantly and negatively related to perceived learning. Mediation analyses showed the existence of different patterns of mediation in the pathways from the two types of emotion regulation to perceived learning. These findings are discussed in relation to relevant studies conducted during non-pandemic periods and Covid-19, and based on the results we highlight the need for interventions aimed at developing adaptive emotion regulation strategies and reducing anxiety in emergency remote learning.

Introduction

On March 11, 2020, the World Health Organization (WHO) announced Covid-19 (coronavirus disease) as a pandemic (WHO Regional Office for Europe, 2020). Learning in brick-and-mortar classrooms was urgently suspended and replaced by full remote learning. This sudden change placed a severe psychological burden on learners (Pakpour et al., 2020; Sycamore, 2020), in addition to the negative emotions directly or indirectly caused by the pandemic, such as anxiety about being infected, loneliness due to lockdowns, and worries over financial strain and academic delays (AlAteeq et al., 2020; Cao et al., 2020; Gubler et al., 2020; Husky et al., 2020; Savitsky et al., 2020).

Emotions are critical to learning (Zull, 2006). Evidence across multiple disciplines, including psychology, education, and neuroscience, consistently shows that the frequency and intensity of academic emotions can contribute to or interfere with learning processes and academic achievement (Pekrun et al., 2011; Seli et al., 2016; Tyng et al., 2017). Anxiety exacerbated during Covid-19 remote learning is thought to be a barrier to learning success (Gillis and Krull, 2020). It is important to understand how learners' anxiety in such emergency remote learning (ERL) situations arises and the mechanisms that trigger this negative emotion.

Perceived control has been shown to be an important antecedent of anxiety in learning (e.g., Goetz et al., 2006; Marchand and Gutierrez, 2012; Shao et al., 2020). Students' perceived control over Covid-19 remote learning was found to vary from individual to individual (Dhawan, 2020). The use of specific emotion regulation strategies has also been associated with different levels of anxiety (Gross and John, 2003; Hofmann et al., 2009; Schutte et al., 2009; but also see Eastabrook et al., 2013). These strategies can be broadly categorized into antecedent-focused (e.g., cognitive reappraisal) and response-focused strategies (e.g., suppressing emotions) (Gross and John, 2003; Gross, 2013). Over the years, different theoretical models have emphasized the classification of these strategies as adaptive or maladaptive (Aldao et al., 2010). Two strategies of regulation that have long been considered adaptive and maladaptive are reappraisal and suppression, respectively (Aldao et al., 2010).

The present study primarily aimed to test a model of proposed relationships between emotion regulation (cognitive reappraisal, expressive suppression), perceived control, anxiety as an achievement emotion, and perceived learning during Covid-19 remote learning. Perceived learning is indicative of student achievement (Rockinson-Szapkiw et al., 2016). The present study also aimed to explore the mediation effects of perceived control and anxiety on the association between the two types of emotion regulation and perceived learning. The results of this study will hopefully advance our understanding of anxiety associated with ERL during global public health crises and the mechanisms by which it acts on learning outcomes, and in practice, provide evidence-based recommendations on how learning-related anxiety can be effectively reduced in ERL.

Literature Review and Hypothesis Development

Anxiety and Covid-19 Remote Learning

Emotions in academic contexts were once neglected because they were considered contrary to rational thinking and hindered effective teaching and learning (Cleveland-Innes and Campbell, 2012). However, emotions cannot be ignored, as learning has been found to be fraught with emotional experiences (Dirkx, 2008). A series of studies by Pekrun et al. (2002, 2004, 2011) have shown that learners often experience a variety of, both positive and negative, emotions during traditional learning. Research on online learning has also shown that the emotions felt by learners vary (e.g., Artino and Jones, 2012; Tempelaar et al., 2012).

According to models that categorize emotions (Schlosberg, 1954; Russell, 1980), emotions can be perceived as varying along valence (positive-negative) and arousal (low-high intensity). In terms of object focus (Pekrun, 2006), achievement emotions can be distinguished into activity emotions (e.g., in studying situations) and outcome emotions (e.g., facing success and failure). According to valence, arousal, and object focus, anxiety is commonly described as a negatively valenced emotion with high intensity, and it can be elicited during an activity or by an outcome. Anxiety is produced by a combination of multiple components involving uneasiness, nervousness, worries, avoidance motivation, and related peripheral physiological activity (Pekrun, 2006). Anxiety, among all emotions, is one of the most frequently studied in technology-based learning environments (Loderer et al., 2020). In the present study, we chose to focus on anxiety, in part because it has a strong research base and great value for attention, especially during a global pandemic outbreak.

Emotions that arise in online learning are related to self, others, task, and technology (Wosnitza and Volet, 2005). Evidence suggests that online students' anxiety may be due to the following aspects: unpreparedness to learn an online course (Abdous, 2019), working with “unknown others” (Hilliard et al., 2020), course-specific demands and requirements (Zembylas, 2008), and unfamiliarity with information technology (Fuller et al., 2016). During Covid-19, learners experienced some anxieties specific to emergency periods. Changes in ways of learning and learning environments are a contributing factor to anxiety. These changes are sudden and compulsive in nature (Bozkurt and Sharma, 2020). The transition to ERL results in the absence of a scheduled university environment, and a shift to a home environment, which poses a significant challenge for many students (Sycamore, 2020). With learning and classroom routines disrupted, students struggled with the transition to remote learning (Biber et al., 2020). In addition, remote learners' anxiety during Covid-19 has been found to be associated with the following aspects: falling academically behind other peers (Pakpour et al., 2020), increased academic workload (Wang et al., 2020c), and uncertainty about academic performance (Cao et al., 2020).

Antecedents and Consequences of Anxiety

An important antecedent of anxiety is related to reduced perceived sense of control (Roseman and Evdokas, 2004; Pekrun, 2006). Perceived control is conceptualized as one's perceived ability to shape or influence an event, taking into account situational demands, coping potential, and regulatory ability (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984). In the field of education, perceived academic control specifically refers to the degree of control learners perceive they have over the impact of academic outcomes. Perceived control is closely related to another concept, self-efficacy, because both fall within the expectancy component of self-concept (Pintrich and De Groot, 1990). Perceived academic control is a relatively stable psychological disposition (encompassing both state-like and trait-like components) that affects learners' motivation and achievement striving (Perry et al., 2001, 2005b).

According to Pekrun's (2006) control-value theory (CVT), perceived control plays a pivotal role in influencing achievement emotions. Studies applying this theory in traditional classrooms reveal that low perceived control is associated with increased anxiety and low academic achievement (e.g., Perry et al., 2001; Pekrun et al., 2004; Goetz et al., 2006). Although few studies have applied CVT to the study of online learners, Marchand and Gutierrez (2012) found that self-efficacy was negatively related to learning anxiety. This pattern of relationship was replicated in science learning during Covid-19 (Yang X. et al., 2020).

The association between anxiety and learning outcomes has been inconsistent in the literature. Some studies observed negative associations (Pekrun et al., 2009; Artino et al., 2010; You and Kang, 2014), and even, as CVT suggests, anxiety partially mediated the relationship between control and achievement (Butz et al., 2015). Other studies, however, found non-significant associations (Tempelaar et al., 2012; Heckel and Ringeisen, 2019). These inconsistent results may be related to the different levels of anxiety in the investigated samples. Drawing on attentional control theory (Eysenck et al., 2007), high levels of anxiety may impair performance because of attentional control deficits in the ability to maintain task goals. However, low or moderate levels of anxiety may lead individuals to devote more resources and effort to maintaining performance. In a study conducted during Covid-19, Biber et al. (2020) analyzed the questionnaires about online learning completed by 1,640 students enrolled in a wellness course in the spring semester of 2020. Anxiety was shown to be negatively related to perceived instructional effectiveness.

Emotion Regulation: Reappraisal and Suppression

Emotions are regulatory in nature. Emotion regulation strategies regulate (increase, maintain, or decrease) emotions by intervening in specific stages of the emotion production system using conscious and non-conscious strategies (Gross, 2001). Depending on the time point of the intervention, antecedent-focused emotion regulation occurs before full activation of emotional responses, whereas response-focused emotion regulation strategies are used following emotional activation (Gross, 1998). According to the process model of emotion regulation, antecedent-focused strategies include situation selection, situation modification, attentional deployment, and cognitive change, whereas response-focused strategy is represented by response modulation (Gross, 1998, 2002; Gross and Thompson, 2007).

The strategies representing antecedent-focused and response-focused emotion regulation families, respectively, are cognitive reappraisal (a cognitive-change strategy) and expressive suppression (a response-modulation strategy). These two strategies have been studied most frequently compared to other emotion regulation strategies. Reappraisal regulates an emotional impact by changing the way events are evaluated, and operates primarily through meaning-evaluation mechanisms (Gross, 1998, 2001). It overlaps conceptually to some extent with positive reinterpretation (Carver et al., 1989) in the coping literature. Specific methods of reappraisal are recontrual (i.e., changing the situational construal) and repurposing (i.e., changing the goal set) (Uusberg et al., 2019). An example of reappraisal is when remote learners try to see the technical difficulties encountered on a distance learning platform as an opportunity to learn new technologies. Emotion suppression involves inhibiting or hiding the emotional reactions that have arisen (Gross and John, 2003). An example of suppression is that the negative emotions triggered by the difficulties and challenges encountered by learners in Covid-19 remote learning are not expressed, but hidden.

According to the appraisal model that aims to unify appraisal, emotion regulation, and emotion generation (Yih et al., 2019), changes in appraisal are one mechanism by which emotion regulation strategies regulate emotions. One of the underlying components of appraisal is likelihood, which includes certainty about the current situation as well as future expectations and outcome probabilities. Research in psychology has shown that regulation strategies have an effect on perceived control (Fontaine et al., 1993; Dijkstra and Homan, 2016). In Dijkstra and Homan (2016), perceived sense of control was identified as an important explaining variable in the relationship between regulation and psychological health.

A large body of research has shown that reappraisal is associated with less negative affect and more well-being, whereas suppression is associated with more negative affect and less well-being (Gross and John, 2003; Hofmann et al., 2009; Schutte et al., 2009; Ehring et al., 2010; Dijkstra and Homan, 2016; Low et al., 2017; but see Yeung and Fung, 2012; Eastabrook et al., 2013, for diverging findings). Unlike suppression as a response-focused strategy, reappraisal is antecedent-focused and is typically used before emotions unfold and, therefore, consumes fewer cognitive resources (Cutuli, 2014).

Reappraisal and suppression have been studied in traditional learning and test-taking situations (e.g., Nett et al., 2011; Sorić et al., 2013; Ben-Eliyahu and Linnenbrink-Garcia, 2015). These two emotion regulation strategies in online learning have received some (albeit limited but emerging) academic attention. For example, in Webster and Hadwin (2015), online learners were found to reappraise the value of tasks for the purpose of reducing negative emotions, especially anxiety. Xu et al. (2018) found that emotion management and cognitive reappraisal were positively associated with online homework effort and completion amount, and with online learning satisfaction. In a study examining web-based learning (Vuorela and Nummenmaa, 2004), suppression was generally used less frequently than reappraisal. Suppression was found to be associated with an increase in dropout rates among learners of massive open online courses (Dmoshinskaia, 2016).

The use of emotion regulation during Covid-19 has been explored in studies in the fields of health psychology, media communication, and child development (Gubler et al., 2020; Rubaltelli et al., 2020; Shorer and Leibovich, 2020; Yang Y. et al., 2020). For example, Gubler et al. (2020) investigated public perception of having their lives restricted during the pandemic in Switzerland. The use of reappraisal was associated with less loneliness and higher well-being, whereas suppression was associated with higher loneliness. Reappraisal might have helped individuals to view public life restrictions in a more positive light. Conversely, suppressors might be less likely to communicate or share experiences with others and therefore felt more isolated.

To the best of our knowledge, the issue of emotion regulation (cognitive reappraisal, expressive suppression), which deserves academic attention in the context of Covid-19 full remote learning, has not been explored so far. The present study sought to fill this gap by examining how reappraisal and suppression are related to remote learners' perceived control, anxiety and perceived learning. Based on the literature reviewed, we propose the following model and hypotheses (Figure 1). In addition, the present study explored the possible mediating roles of perceived control and anxiety in the association between the two types of emotion regulation and perceived learning.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Research model and hypotheses proposed.

H1: Cognitive reappraisal is positively related perceived control (H1a) and perceived learning (H1c), but negatively related to anxiety (H1b).

H2: Expressive suppression is negatively related to perceived control (H2a) and perceived learning (H2c), but positively related to anxiety (H2b).

H3: Perceived control is negatively related to anxiety (H3a), but positively related to perceived learning (H3b).

H4: Anxiety is negatively related to perceived learning.

H5: Cognitive reappraisal indirectly affects perceived learning through perceived control and anxiety.

H6: Expressive suppression indirectly affects perceived learning through perceived control and anxiety.

Methods

Context, Participants, and Procedures

On 29 January, 2020, due to the outbreak of Covid-19, the Ministry of Education of the People's Republic of China issued a policy of “suspending classes without stopping learning” (http://www.moe.gov.cn/jyb_xwfb/gzdt_gzdt/s5987/202001/t20200129_416993.html), which required students at all educational levels to take courses at home. Using a convenience sample, this study investigated a group of graduate students' perceptions of a full remote course on academic writing and research methods. They were enrolled in a medium-sized public university in southwest China. Prior to the Covid-19 outbreak, this course was routinely taught face-to-face in brick-and-mortar classrooms. During the pandemic outbreak, students took the first 13 weeks of the 18-week course (late February to late May) remotely at home, and most of them returned to university to take the course face-to-face in the 14th week.

A total of 323 graduate students enrolled in this 2-h per week course. The course was designed to improve students' academic literacy through lectures on six modules (e.g., components of an academic paper, introduction to quantitative research methods, and logic and argumentation). The course was mainly taught online (synchronously webcasted by lecturers) using Tencent Classroom (a professional webcasting platform in China's education sector), and also included synchronous and asynchronous classroom and group discussions, as well as online question and answer sessions. The course was assessed based on assignments, online class participation, and a course paper submitted at the end of the semester.

In week 13, an invitation with a link to the questionnaire was sent out via the course lecturers to invite their students to participate in this study. The introduction page of the questionnaire described the nature of this study, gave consent forms, and informed each student that his participation was completely voluntary and his response would be kept confidential. The survey was hosted on Wenjuanxing (a Chinese online platform providing functions equivalent to Qualtrics). It should be pointed out that ethics approval was not required at the time this study was conducted as per the local legislation and institutional requirements. However, informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Initially, we received a total of 272 questionnaires (response rate: 84%). Prior to the main survey, a pilot study was conducted with 15 graduate students. We removed questionnaires that were completed within 100 s based on the researchers' evaluation of the pilot study and the average time to complete the questionnaire in the main survey. In addition, we removed questionnaires completed by selecting the same option for all or the majority of the scaled measurement items, including the reversed ones. According to the above criteria, 239 valid questionnaires were entered into the analysis. Participants were between 21 and 30 years of age (Mean = 23.74; SD = 1.22), and 32% of them were male and 68% were female. They originated from different provinces and each studied at home during the time the present study was conducted. They majored in economics, business management, finance, and accounting.

Measures

The orientation instructions of the questionnaire asked participants to indicate what they thought about and felt in relation to learning the course on academic writing and research methods during the Covid-19 remote learning period. The questionnaire consists of 32 scaled measurement items written in Chinese.

In order to minimize possible common method variance (CMV), ex-ante approaches were adopted in the design of questionnaires (Chang et al., 2010). First, as mentioned above, an introductive message was used to ensure the anonymity and confidentiality of survey takers' responses. Second, to confirm the clarity of wording, the questionnaire was pre-tested with the graduate students in the pilot study. Third, we randomized the order of items cross different constructs.

Emotion Regulation

We used the questionnaire developed by Gross and John (2003) to measure emotion regulation (cognitive reappraisal, expressive suppression) on a scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Reappraisal scale consists of six items (e.g., “When I wanted to feel less negative emotion, I changed the way I was thinking about the situation”), and suppression scale consists of four items (e.g., “I controlled my emotions by not expressing them”). The Chinese translations of these items were adopted from Chen et al. (2020).

Perceived Control

Students' perceived control was measured using eight items from the Perceived Academic Control Scale (PACS) designed by Perry et al. (2001). We slightly adapted the items to fit the present study context by changing “in my psychology course” “in my course” to “in this course” (e.g., “I had a great deal of control over my academic performance in this course”). Participants responded on a five-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). The PACS has been used in examining online learning (e.g., Tempelaar et al., 2012; You and Kang, 2014; Buhr et al., 2019). We adopted the Chinese wording of PACS used in Ju (2012).

Anxiety

Anxiety was measured using eight items taken from the Achievement Emotions Questionnaire (AEQ) developed by Perry et al. (2005a). One sample item is I got tense and nervous while studying this course. Participants responded on a five-point Likert scale (1 = not at all to 5 = very much). AEQ was used to examine the emotions of online learners in previous studies (Artino, 2010; Tempelaar et al., 2012; Heckel and Ringeisen, 2019). The present study adopted the Chinese wording used in Dong and Yu (2007).

Perceived Learning

Perceived learning was measured using the Cognitive, Affective and Psychomotor (CAP)Perceived Learning Scale designed by Rovai et al. (2009). The scale was designed for both face-to-face and online learning. Given that the course was not designed to develop students' psychomotor skills, only six items measuring cognitive and affective learning were used (e.g., “I can organize course material into a logical structure”). Participants responded on a seven-point Likert scale (1 = not at all to 7 = very much).

To ensure the validity of the translation of CAP Scale, a back-translation procedure was used (Brislin, 1970). First, a professional translator who was a native Chinese speaker and fluent in English from a translation center at the university translated all original items from English to Chinese. Then, another expert translator who was also fluent in both languages translated the items back to English independently. Two of the researchers confirmed the wording of the Chinese version by comparing the two English versions.

Demographic Characteristics

The participants were asked to report gender and age. Gender and age were used as covariates in the present study, as evidence indicates that emotion varies with gender and age in educational contexts (Frenzel et al., 2007; Pekrun et al., 2011; Reed et al., 2014), and during Covid-19 pandemic periods (Klaiber et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2020a).

Data Analysis Strategy

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS v.25 and SPSS Amos v.24. We followed a two-stage approach suggested by Anderson and Gerbing (1988) to analyze the data: validating the measurement model and then the structural model. More specifically, we first assessed the reliability of each measurement scale. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was then used to test the construct validity of each scale. The goodness-of-fit of measurement models were reported. After this, structural equation modeling (SEM) was conducted with gender and age as covariates to test hypotheses using the method of maximum likelihood estimation. SEM is a multivariate statistical method used to test a prior hypotheses (Kline, 2011). Model fit was estimated using the following statistical indices, as recommend by Hu and Bentler (1999): the chi-square goodness-of-fit (x2), the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residuals (SRMR). For CFI and TLI, a value >0.95/0.90 indicates a good/adequate model fit. For RMSEA, a value ≤ 0.06/0.08 indicates a good/adequate model fit, and for SRMR, a value ≤ 0.08/0.10 indicates a good/ adequate model fit.

Finally, a serial mediation analysis was performed using Preacher and Hayes (2004) method. Perceived control and anxiety served as mediators of the association between emotion regulation (reappraisal, suppression) and perceived learning. Bootstrapping with 5,000 resamples was performed to test the significance of the mediation effects. Bootstrap method was used because it does not make distributional assumptions on residuals; thus, inference can be made even if the errors do not follow a normal distribution or constant error variance (Efron and Tibshirani, 1994; Fox, 2015). All effects and bias corrected 95% confidence intervals were estimated. The effects were considered significant if the confidence interval did not contain zero. The magnitude of mediations were assessed using the ratio of the indirect effect to the total effect (Preacher and Kelley, 2011).

Results

Preliminary Analysis

The reliability of the five constructs were measured by Cronbach's Alpha and composite reliability. As shown in Table 1, the Cronbach's Alpha and composite reliability of each construct met the acceptable level of 0.70 (Nunnally, 1978) and 0.60 (Fornell and Larcker, 1981; Bagozzi and Yi, 1988), respectively.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Factor loadings, and measurement scales reliability and validity.

The construct validity was measured using CFA. Standardized factor loadings and goodness of fit indexes of each construct are reported in Table 1. All indicators of each factor had loadings > 0.50 (Bagozzi and Yi, 1988). All the one-factor CFA models showed an adequate or good model fit according to the traditional cutoff criteria given above. The five-factor CFA model also showed a good model fit: χ2 (454) = 536.076, p < 0.05, CFI = 0.969, TLI = 0.966, RMSEA = 0.028, SRMR = 0.050.

Discriminant validity was checked using heterotrait-monotrait ratio of correlations (HTMT) (Henseler et al., 2015). As shown in Table 2, all HTMT values are < 0.90; thus, discriminant validity was also supported.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. HTMT analysis.

A single-factor test was performed to investigate possible CMV (Harman, 1976). Un-rotated confirmatory factor analysis shows that one factor explained 27% of the variance (threshold value < 50%). With caution, while CMV cannot be ruled out as a contributing factor in the present research, it did not appear to be a significant factor.

Descriptive statistics and a correlation matrix of the study variables are given in Tables 3, 4, respectively. Mean scores ranged from 3.02 to 4.86. Standard deviation scores ranged from 0.62 to 0.97. All skewness and kurtosis scores fell in the acceptable ranges of normality recommended by Kline (2011) (skewness between −3 and +3, kurtosis between −10 and +10).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Descriptive statistics for main constructs.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Bivariate correlations for main constructs, gender, and age.

The results of the bivariate correlations are generally in line with our expectations. Reappraisal was positively associated with perceived control (r = 0.41, p < 0.001) and perceived learning (r = 0.45, p < 0.001), but negatively associated with anxiety (r = −0.27, p < 0.001). Suppression was negatively correlated with perceived control (r = −0.25, p < 0.001) and perceived learning (r = −0.25, p < 0.001), but positively associated with anxiety (r = 0.37 p < 0.001). In addition, perceived control was negatively correlated with anxiety (r = −0.39, p < 0.001), and anxiety was negatively associated with perceived learning (r = −0.45, p < 0.001).

Testing the Hypothesize Model

The indices showed that the data fitted the structural model: χ2 (512) = 595.261, p < 0.05, CFI = 0.968, TLI = 0.965, RMSEA = 0.026, SRMR = 0.049. The model explained a large proportion of variance in perceived control (R2 = 0.29), anxiety (R2 = 0.32), and perceived learning (R2 = 0.56). The structural model with standardized coefficients is presented in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Structural model on the interplay of the study constructs. Dashed pathways are not significant; standardized coefficients are presented; **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Covariates were included in the model but are not presented for simplicity.

Regarding H1a and H1c, as expected, cognitive reappraisal was positively related to perceived control (H1a: β = 0.44, p < 0.001) and perceived learning (H1c: β = 0.25, p < 0.001). H1b posits that reappraisal is significantly and negatively related to anxiety. This hypothesis was not supported (p > 0.05). As to H2a and H2b, as anticipated, suppression was negatively related to perceived control (H2a: β = −0.24, p < 0.01), but positively related to anxiety (H2b: β = 0.33, p < 0.001). Suppression was not significantly related to perceived learning; thus, H2c was not supported (p > 0.05). Regarding H3a-b, perceived control was negatively related to anxiety (H3a: β = −0.31, p < 0.001), but positively related to perceived learning (H3b: β = 0.44, p < 0.001); thus, both hypotheses were supported. H4 posits that anxiety is negatively related to perceived learning, and this hypothesis was supported (H4: β = −0.23, p < 0.01).

Testing Mediation Effects

Table 5 shows the mediation analysis results. We found the indirect effect of perceived control in the association between cognitive reappraisal and learning to be significant [standardized estimate (Std. estimate) = 0.197, 95% CI: 0.157, 0.382], whereas the indirect effect passing through anxiety was not (Std. estimate = 0.024, 95% CI: −0.005, 0.094). We also found a significant serial mediation effect via both perceived control and anxiety (Std. estimate = 0.032, 95% CI: 0.016, 0.094). Taken together, H5 is partially supported.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Mediation analysis results.

The path through perceived control alone explained 39.4% of the reappraisal-learning association, the path through anxiety alone explained 4.8% of the association, and the path through both control and anxiety explained 6.4% of the association. The results indicated that reappraisal influenced perceived learning mainly through the mediator of perceived control. Moreover, the indirect effect of perceived control in the reappraisal-anxiety association was found to be significant (Std. estimate = −0.137, 95% CI: −0.187, −0.056), and accounted for 57.1% of the association.

In the suppression-learning association, the indirect effect of perceived control alone (Std. estimate = −0.108, 95% CI: −0.264, −0.085) and that of anxiety alone (Std. estimate = −0.075, 95% CI: −0.218, −0.049) were both found to be significant. A significant serial mediation effect through both control and anxiety was also found (Std. estimate = −0.017, 95% CI: −0.068, −0.008). Thus, we found support for H6.

The percent of suppression-learning association mediated was 47.8% through perceived control alone, 33.2% through anxiety alone, and 7.5% through the path including both control and anxiety. The results suggested that suppression affected perceived learning mainly through perceived control alone and through anxiety alone. Moreover, the indirect effect of perceived control in the suppression-anxiety association was found to be significant (Std. estimate = 0.075, 95% CI: 0.028, 0.135), and explained 18.7% of the association.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to investigate the impact of emotion regulation strategies on control appraisal and anxiety among remote learners in the context of a global health crisis, and thus to identify the mechanisms by which anxiety arises. Students who attended the course remotely from home completed a questionnaire related to the measures of the studied variables. As expected, reappraisers perceived higher control with higher learning, whereas suppressors had lower perceived control and higher learning anxiety. Students with high perceived control experienced less anxiety and higher perceived learning. Higher anxiety was associated with lower learning. The paths of reappraisal to anxiety and suppression to perceived learning in the model were not as significant as expected. We explored the possible mediation effects of perceived control and anxiety, and all indirect effects in the association between emotion regulation and perceived learning were found to be significant, except for the indirect effect of anxiety alone in the reappraisal-learning association. According to the magnitude of mediations, perceived control was a particularly important mediator in the reappraisal-learning association, and both perceived control and anxiety were important mediators in the suppression-learning association.

A key proposition of the appraisal model (Yih et al., 2019) is that emotion regulation strategies can have an impact on appraisal of a situation (e.g., perceived control) by changing the pieces of information on which the appraisal process operates or the antecedents of the appraisal. Perceived control is one mechanism through which emotion regulation influences emotions. This proposition was supported by the existence of mediating roles of perceived control found in reappraisal-anxiety and suppression-anxiety associations in the present study. This finding is broadly consistent with that in Dijkstra and Homan (2016) study. Reappraisal and suppression are not likely to affect psychological state directly alone, but are also related to the extent of personal control over a situation, which is subsequently associated with positive or negative consequences.

As predicted by Pekrun's (2006) control-value theory, control processes are antecedents of emotions. The negative relationship between control and anxiety found in this study is consistent with previous literature (Endler et al., 2001; Pekrun et al., 2004; Butz et al., 2015). Control processes monitor emotional states, in “normal” learning situations, but also in emergency learning under a global health crisis. The importance of perceived control in the present study also lies in its direct effect on perceived learning and in its mediating role in the association between emotion regulation and learning. Individuals with high perceived control hold an attitude of responsibility for results (Rotter, 1966). They believe that learning outcomes largely depend on the effort they put in and actively respond to the challenges posed by changes. During the pandemic, high control learners might have actively taken various methods to solve the problems encountered in full remote learning, such as strict time management and removal of distractions from learning (Gelles et al., 2020). These measures are likely to have facilitated perceived learning.

Consistent with previous literature (Jamieson et al., 2010, 2018), reappraisers perceived more learning in the present study. Reappraisers are more likely to hold a stress-is-enhancing mindset, flexibly viewing stress as an opportunity for growth (Hagger et al., 2020). According to broadening and building theory (Fredrickson, 1998), the positive mindset held by reappraisers helps to broaden thought–action repertoire. Adapting learning to a specific, as-yet-unencountered environment requires cognitive flexibility, and a positive mindset is beneficial to the development of this flexibility. In addition, our participants who used more reappraisals had higher perceived control and then had lower anxiety, which may explain the observed serial mediational pathway associating reappraisal with learning through both perceived control and anxiety.

Chronic attempts to avoid an unwanted thought can lead to an increase in the frequency, severity, and accessibility of that thought (Wegner et al., 1987; Lavy and Van den Hout, 1990). This paradoxical effect of suppression elucidates a positive relationship between suppression and anxiety. Suppression is also detrimental to interpersonal relationships. Suppression decreases the likelihood of emotional sharing, social support, and relationship closeness (Gross, 2002; Gross and John, 2003). Studies of loneliness in individuals during Covid-19 showed that suppressors were more likely to feel lonely (Gubler et al., 2020). Loneliness might be one of the factors that contributed to increased anxiety during the pandemic (Banerjee and Rai, 2020). In addition, some Covid-19 remote students felt anxious due to the fear of falling behind their peers (Pakpour et al., 2020). Suppression of emotions prevented adequate communication between remote learners to understand each other's learning progress, in which case they perceived themselves as learning less effectively than others.

Similar to Low et al.'s (2017) study 2 in general psychology, the authors found that spontaneous use of emotional suppression did not directly predict success, but indirectly influenced success via a negative emotion (depression). Our participants' use of expressive suppression did not directly and significantly lead to a decrease in perceived learning. More use of suppression was associated with higher anxiety, which was then associated with less perceived learning. Our data provide preliminary evidence showing an important mediating role of anxiety in the relationship between suppression and perceived learning in educational contexts. Additionally, we found support for the existence of a serial mediation linking suppression to learning through both perceived control and anxiety.

According to the attentional control theory (Eysenck et al., 2007), high anxiety affects the inhibitory function responsible for suppressing irrelevant information and reduces the attention given to the task being performed. For high-anxiety learners, attention is focused on anxious reactions rather than on processing learning tasks. Anxiety experienced during Covid-19 full remote learning was shown to be negatively related to perceived learning. This result is consistent with some studies (Artino et al., 2010; You and Kang, 2014), but not with others (Tempelaar et al., 2012; Heckel and Ringeisen, 2019). Our participants reported a higher anxiety mean (3.13) than the theoretical median (3, middle value of a five-point Likert scale). The participants, overall, had more than moderate levels of anxiety. Additionally, some studies have clearly shown exacerbated anxiety during Covid-19 (Husky et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2020a,b; Arribathi et al., 2021). One explanation for the negative relationship between anxiety and perceived learning found in the current study may be related to our participants' anxiety levels.

This study has several limitations that we would like to highlight. First, the results of a study based on a convenience sample may limit its generalizability or external validity. Our study was conducted on a full remote course taken by graduate students at a university in China during Covid-19. Because of the differences in the specific implementation of emergency remote teaching across countries and grade levels (Hall et al., 2020), it remains to be examined whether the results of this study can be replicated in other countries or at other educational levels. The second limitation is that the present study used learners' self-reported data. Concerns about the reliability and validity of studies using this type of data have been discussed in the literature (e.g., Demetriou et al., 2015), including over- and under-reporting and social desirability bias (Gonyea, 2005). Future research could consider also using more objective data, such as logs and test scores, to provide a more comprehensive examination of learners' behaviors, emotional experiences, and academic performance. Third, although the current study used existing instruments to measure reappraisal and suppression, these instruments were not developed specifically for emergency learning situations or even academic contexts, which may reduce the validity of the measurement instruments used. There is a need to confirm the current results in future studies using questionnaire instruments developed specifically for the investigation of emergency learning contexts.

Despite these limitations, this study provides some insight into teaching and learning during emergencies. These findings highlight the need for interventions to reduce remote learners' anxiety. Training in emotion regulation has been shown to be effective in improving the ability to cope with various emotions and reducing anxiety (De Witte et al., 2017). It is thus recommended that researchers, classroom teachers, mental health providers, and school authorities design and develop (preferably Internet-mediated) training programs aimed at developing the adoption of healthy emotion regulation strategies in students with low levels of adaptive regulation. Moreover, perceived control was found to be an important antecedent of anxiety and perceived learning. Educators can adopt instructional strategies aimed at enhancing students' perceived control over full remote learning. For example, they can provide technical support to students when needed, give emotional support to ensure that students are actively engaged in learning, and teach students how to properly use social media for communication through training sessions (Sobaih et al., 2020).

Finally, the implications of this study for future research are as follows. According to the process model of emotion regulation (Gross, 1998, 2002; Gross and Thompson, 2007), regulatory strategies consist of five families. We focused only on reappraisal and suppression. Situation selection, attentional deployment, and situation modification were not explored in this study, so it would be worthwhile to investigate how these strategies affect perceived control and achievement emotions in ERL. Moreover, the relationship between anxiety and perceived learning is not consistent across studies (e.g., Artino et al., 2010; Tempelaar et al., 2012; You and Kang, 2014; Heckel and Ringeisen, 2019). We speculate that their relationship is moderated by learning contexts. It would be useful to examine which constituents of learning contexts (e.g., type of course, course difficulty, assessment method) moderate the relations between anxiety and perceived learning.

Conclusion

This study examined the effects of two emotion regulation strategies on perceived control over learning and anxiety in a sample of graduate students taking a remote course during the pandemic. The results showed that cognitive reappraisal was positively related to perceived control and perceived learning. Expressive suppression was negatively related to perceived control, but positively related to anxiety. Anxiety was significantly, negatively related to perceived learning. We found the existence of different patterns of mediation in the pathways from the two types of emotion regulation to perceived learning. These results were interpreted in the light of the model that aims to unify emotion regulation and perceived control, the control-value theory and the attentional control theory. For learners in emergency remote learning situations, rather than holding emotions inside, understanding and accepting emotions and allowing them to be regulated using cognitive reappraisal can promote control over a situation, healthy emotional development, and learning outcomes.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

TZ: conceptualization, methodology, data analysis, and writing. ZF and XL: conceptualization, writing, and editing. LY: data analysis, writing, and editing. WH: methodology. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Foundation of Humanities and Social Sciences of the Ministry of Education of China (Grant No. 19XJC740006) and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities in China (Grant No. JBK200568).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Abdous, M. H. (2019). Influence of satisfaction and preparedness on online students' feelings of anxiety. Intern. High. Educ. 41, 34–44. doi: 10.1016/j.iheduc.2019.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

AlAteeq, D. A., Aljhani, S., and AlEesa, D. (2020). Perceived stress among students in virtual classrooms during the COVID-19 outbreak in KSA. J. Taibah Univ. Med. Sci. 15, 398–403. doi: 10.1016/j.jtumed.2020.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., and Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 217–237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, J. C., and Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: a review and recommended two-step approach. Psychol. Bull. 103, 411–423. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.103.3.411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arribathi, A. H., Suwarto, Miftakhu Rosyad, A., Budiarto, M., Supriyanti, D., and Mulyati. (2021). An analysis of student learning anxiety during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Study In Higher Education. J. Contin. High. Educ. 1, 1–14. doi: 10.1080/07377363.2020.1847971

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Artino, A. R. (2010). Online or face-to-face learning? Exploring the personal factors that predict students' choice of instructional format. Intern. High. Educ. 13, 272–276. doi: 10.1016/j.iheduc.2010.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Artino, A. R., and Jones, K. D. (2012). Exploring the complex relations between achievement emotions and self-regulated learning behaviors in online learning. Intern. High. Educ. 15, 170–175. doi: 10.1016/j.iheduc.2012.01.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Artino, A. R., La Rochelle, J. S., and Durning, S. J. (2010). Second-year medical students' motivational beliefs, emotions, and achievement. Med. Educ. 44, 1203–1212. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2923.2010.03712.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagozzi, R. P., and Yi, Y. (1988). On the evaluation of structural equation models. J. Acad. Market. Sci. 16, 74–94. doi: 10.1007/BF02723327

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banerjee, D., and Rai, M. (2020). Social isolation in Covid-19: the impact of loneliness. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 66, 1–3. doi: 10.1177/0020764020922269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Eliyahu, A., and Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (2015). Integrating the regulation of affect, behavior, and cognition into self-regulated learning paradigms among secondary and post-secondary students. Metacogn. Learn. 10, 15–42. doi: 10.1007/s11409-014-9129-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biber, D. D., Melton, B., and Czech, D. R. (2020). The impact of COVID-19 on college anxiety, optimism, gratitude, and course satisfaction. J. Am. Coll. Health 2, 1–6. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2020.1842424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bozkurt, A., and Sharma, R. C. (2020). Emergency remote teaching in a time of global crisis due to coronavirus pandemic. Asian J. Dist. Educ. 15, i–vi. doi: 10.5281/zenodo.3778083

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 1, 185–216. doi: 10.1177/135910457000100301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buhr, E. E., Daniels, L. M., and Goegan, L. D. (2019). Cognitive appraisals mediate relationships between two basic psychological needs and emotions in a massive open online course. Comput. Human Behav. 96, 85–94. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.02.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butz, N. T., Stupnisky, R. H., and Pekrun, R. (2015). Students' emotions for achievement and technology use in synchronous hybrid graduate programmes: a control-value approach. Res. Learn. Technol. 23, 1–16. doi: 10.3402/rlt.v23.26097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, W., Fang, Z., Hou, G., Han, M., Xu, X., Dong, J., et al. (2020). The psychological impact of the COVID-19 epidemic on college students in China. Psychiatry Res. 287, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., and Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: a theoretically based approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 56, 267–283. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, S.-J., van Witteloostuijn, A., and Eden, L. (2010). From the editors: common method variance in international business research. J. Int. Bus. Stud. 41, 178–184. doi: 10.1057/jibs.2009.88

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, W., Zhang, G., Tian, X., Luo, J., Gao, R., and Yang, T. (2020). Test of the emotion regulation questionnaire in the middle school students. Chin. Ment. Health J. 34, 56–61. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1000-6729.2020.3.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cleveland-Innes, M., and Campbell, P. (2012). Emotional presence, learning, and the online learning environment. Int. Rev. Res. Open Distrib. Learn. 13, 269–292. doi: 10.19173/irrodl.v13i4.1234

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cutuli, D. (2014). Cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression strategies role in the emotion regulation: an overview on their modulatory effects and neural correlates. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8, 1–6. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Witte, N. A., Sutterlin, S., Braet, C., and Mueller, S. C. (2017). Psychophysiological correlates of emotion regulation training in adolescent anxiety: evidence from the novel PIER task. J. Affect. Disord. 214, 89–96. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demetriou, C., Ozer, B. U., and Essau, C. A. (2015). Self-report questionnaires. Encyc. Clin. Psychol. 1–6. doi: 10.1002/9781118625392.wbecp507

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhawan, S. (2020). Online learning: a panacea in the time of COVID-19 crisis. J. Educ. Technol. Syst. 49, 5–22. doi: 10.1177/0047239520934018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dijkstra, M. T., and Homan, A. C. (2016). Engaging in rather than disengaging from stress: effective coping and perceived control. Front. Psychol. 7, 1–12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dirkx, J. M. (2008). The meaning and role of emotions in adult learning. New Dir. Adult Cont. Educ. 2008, 7–18. doi: 10.1002/ace.311

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dmoshinskaia, N. (2016). Dropout Prediction in MOOCs: Using Sentiment Analysis of Users‘ Comments to Predict Engagement (Unpublished master's thesis). University of Twente, Enschede, Netherlands.

Google Scholar

Dong, Y., and Yu, G. (2007). The development and application of academic emotions questionnaire. Acta Psychol. Sin. 39, 852–860. doi: 10.1631/jzus.2007.B0507

CrossRef Full Text

Eastabrook, J. M., Flynn, J. J., and Hollenstein, T. (2013). Internalizing symptoms in female adolescents: associations with emotional awareness and emotion regulation. J. Child Fam. Stud. 23, 487–496. doi: 10.1007/s10826-012-9705-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Efron, B., and Tibshirani, R. J. (1994). An introduction to the Bootstrap. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. doi: 10.1201/9780429246593

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehring, T., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Schnülle, J., Fischer, S., and Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotion regulation and vulnerability to depression: spontaneous versus instructed use of emotion suppression and reappraisal. Emotion 10, 563–572. doi: 10.1037/a0019010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Endler, N. S., Speer, R. L., Johnson, J. M., and Flett, G. L. (2001). General self-efficacy and control in relation to anxiety and cognitive performance. Curr. Psychol. 20, 36–52. doi: 10.1007/s12144-001-1002-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., and Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion 7, 336–353. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fontaine, K. R., Manstead, A. S., and Wagner, H. (1993). Optimism, perceived control over stress, and coping. Eur. J. Pers. 74, 267–281. doi: 10.1002/per.2410070407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fornell, C., and Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Market. Res. 18, 39–50. doi: 10.1177/002224378101800104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, J. (2015). Applied Regression Analysis and Generalized Linear Models. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 300–319. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.300

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., and Goetz, T. (2007). Girls and mathematics—A “hopeless” issue? A control-value approach to gender differences in emotions towards mathematics. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 22, 497–514. doi: 10.1007/BF03173468

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuller, R. M., Vician, C., and Brown, S. A. (2016). E-Learning and individual characteristics: the role of computer anxiety and communication apprehension. J. Comp. Inform. Syst. 46, 103–115. doi: 10.1080/08874417.2006.11645917

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelles, L. A., Lord, S. M., Hoople, G. D., Chen, D. A., and Mejia, J. A. (2020). Compassionate flexibility and self-discipline: student adaptation to emergency remote eeaching in an integrated engineering energy course during COVID-19. Educ. Sci. 10:304. doi: 10.3390/educsci10110304

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillis, A., and Krull, L. M. (2020). COVID-19 remote learning transition in Spring 2020: class structures, student perceptions, and inequality in college courses. Teach. Sociol. 48, 283–299. doi: 10.1177/0092055X20954263

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goetz, T., Pekrun, R., Hall, N., and Haag, L. (2006). Academic emotions from a social-cognitive perspective: antecedents and domain specificity of students' affect in the context of Latin instruction. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 76, 289–308. doi: 10.1348/000709905X42860

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonyea, R. M. (2005). Self-reported data in institutional research: review and recommendations. New Dir. Inst. Res. 2005, 73–89. doi: 10.1002/ir.156

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 271–299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2001). Emotion regulation in adulthood: Timing is everything. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 10, 214–219. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00152

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology 39, 281–291. doi: 10.1017/S0048577201393198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J. (2013). Emotion regulation: taking stock and moving forward. Emotion 13, 359–365. doi: 10.1037/a0032135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J., and John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J., and Thompson, R. A. (2007). “Emotion regulation: conceptual foundations,” in Handbook of Emotion Regulation, ed J. J. Gross (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 3–24.

Gubler, D. A., Makowski, L. M., Troche, S. J., and Schlegel, K. (2020). Loneliness and well-being during the Covid-19 pandemic: associations with personality and emotion regulation. J. Happiness Stud. 1–20. doi: 10.1007/s10902-020-00326-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagger, M. S., Keech, J. J., and Hamilton, K. (2020). Managing stress during the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic and beyond: reappraisal and mindset approaches. Stress Health 36, 396–401. doi: 10.1002/smi.2969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, T., Connolly, C., Ó Grádaigh, S., Burden, K., Kearney, M., Schuck, S., et al. (2020). Education in precarious times: a comparative study across six countries to identify design priorities for mobile learning in a pandemic. Inform. Learn. Sci. 121, 433–442. doi: 10.1108/ILS-04-2020-0089

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harman, H. H. (1976). Modern Factor Analysis. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago press.

Google Scholar

Heckel, C., and Ringeisen, T. (2019). Pride and anxiety in online learning environments: achievement emotions as mediators between learners' characteristics and learning outcomes. J. Comp. Assist. Learn. 35, 667–677. doi: 10.1111/jcal.12367

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henseler, J., Ringle, C. M., and Sarstedt, M. (2015). A new criterion for assessing discriminant validity in variance-based structural equation modeling. J. Acad. Market. Sci. 43, 115–135. doi: 10.1007/s11747-014-0403-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hilliard, J., Kear, K., Donelan, H., and Heaney, C. (2020). Students' experiences of anxiety in an assessed, online, collaborative project. Comput. Educ. 143, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2019.103675

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, S. G., Heering, S., Sawyer, A. T., and Asnaani, A. (2009). How to handle anxiety: the effects of reappraisal, acceptance, and suppression strategies on anxious arousal. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 389–394. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L.-,t., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equat. Model. A Multidiscip. J. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Husky, M. M., Kovess-Masfety, V., and Swendsen, J. D. (2020). Stress and anxiety among university students in France during Covid-19 mandatory confinement. Compr. Psychiatry 102, 1–3. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2020.152191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jamieson, J. P., Crum, A. J., Goyer, J. P., Marotta, M. E., and Akinola, M. (2018). Optimizing stress responses with reappraisal and mindset interventions: an integrated model. Anxiety Stress Coping 31, 245–261. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2018.1442615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jamieson, J. P., Mendes, W. B., Blackstock, E., and Schmader, T. (2010). Turning the knots in your stomach into bows: reappraising arousal improves performance on the GRE. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 208–212. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.08.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ju, Y. (2012). A Study of Relationship Between Academic Emotions, Perceived Academic Control and Academic Performance in the Secondary School Students. (Unpublished master's thesis. Sichuan Normal University, Chengdu, China.

Klaiber, P., Wen, J. H., DeLongis, A., and Sin, N. L. (2020). The ups and downs of daily life during COVID-19: age differences in affect, stress, and positive events. J. Gerontol. B 76, e30–e37. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbaa096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling, 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Lavy, E. H., and Van den Hout, M. A. (1990). Thought suppression induces intrusions. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 18, 251–258. doi: 10.1017/S0141347300010351

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Loderer, K., Pekrun, R., and Lester, J. C. (2020). Beyond cold technology: a systematic review and meta-analysis on emotions in technology-based learning environments. Learn. Instruct. 70, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Low, R. S., Overall, N. C., Hammond, M. D., and Girme, Y. U. (2017). Emotional suppression during personal goal pursuit impedes goal strivings and achievement. Emotion 17, 208–223. doi: 10.1037/emo0000218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marchand, G. C., and Gutierrez, A. P. (2012). The role of emotion in the learning process: comparisons between online and face-to-face learning settings. Intern. High. Edc. 15, 150–160. doi: 10.1016/j.iheduc.2011.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nett, U. E., Goetz, T., and Hall, N. C. (2011). Coping with boredom in school: an experience sampling perspective. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 36, 49–59. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric Theory, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Pakpour, A. H., Islam, M. A., Barna, S. D., Raihan, H., Khan, M. N. A., and Hossain, M. T. (2020). Depression and anxiety among university students during the COVID-19 pandemic in Bangladesh: a web-based cross-sectional survey. PLoS ONE 15:e0238162. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R. (2006). The control-value theory of achievement emotions: assumptions, corollaries, and implications for educational research and practice. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 18, 315–341. doi: 10.1007/s10648-006-9029-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J., and Maier, M. A. (2009). Achievement goals and achievement emotions: testing a model of their joint relations with academic performance. J. Educ. Psychol. 101, 115–135. doi: 10.1037/a0013383

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Barchfeld, P., and Perry, R. P. (2011). Measuring emotions in students' learning and performance: the achievement emotions questionnaire (AEQ). Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 36, 36–48. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.10.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Perry, R. P., Kramer, K., Hochstadt, M., and Molfenter, S. (2004). Beyond test anxiety: development and validation of the test emotions questionnaire (TEQ). Anxiety Stress Coping 17, 287–316. doi: 10.1080/10615800412331303847

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W., and Perry, R. P. (2002). “Positive emotions in education,” in Beyond Coping: Meeting Goals, Visions, and Challenges, ed E. Freydenberg (Oxford: Elsevier), 149–174. doi: 10.1093/med:psych/9780198508144.003.0008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, R. P., Goetz, T., and Perry, R. P. (2005a). Academic Emotions Questionnaire (AEQ): User's Manual. Munich: Department of Psychology; University of Munich.

Perry, R. P., Hall, N. C., and Ruthig, J. C. (2005b). “Perceived (academic) control and scholastic attainment in higher education,” in Higher Education: Handbook of Theory and Research, ed J. Smart (Dordrecht; Heidelberg; New York, NY; London: Springer), 363–436. doi: 10.1007/1-4020-3279-X_7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, R. P., Hladkyj, S., Pekrun, R. H., and Pelletier, S. T. (2001). Academic control and action control in the achievement of college students: a longitudinal field study. J. Educ. Psychol. 93, 776–789. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.4.776

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pintrich, P. R., and De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. J. Educ. Psychol. 82, 33–40. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.33

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., and Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 36, 717–731. doi: 10.3758/BF03206553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., and Kelley, K. (2011). Effect size measures for mediation models: quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psychol. Methods 16, 93–115. doi: 10.1037/a0022658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, A. E., Chan, L., and Mikels, J. A. (2014). Meta-analysis of the age-related positivity effect: age differences in preferences for positive over negative information. Psychol. Aging 29, 1–15. doi: 10.1037/a0035194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rockinson-Szapkiw, A. J., Wendt, J., Whighting, M., and Nisbet, D. (2016). The predictive relationship among the community of inquiry framework, perceived learning and online, and graduate students' course grades in online synchronous and asynchronous courses. Int. Rev. Res. Open Distrib. Learn. 17, 18–35. doi: 10.19173/irrodl.v17i3.2203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roseman, I., and Evdokas, A. (2004). Appraisals cause experienced emotions: experimental evidence. Cognition Emot. 18, 1–28. doi: 10.1080/02699930244000390

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychol. Monogr. Gen. Appl. 80, 1–28. doi: 10.1037/h0092976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rovai, A. P., Wighting, M. J., Baker, J. D., and Grooms, L. D. (2009). Development of an instrument to measure perceived cognitive, affective, and psychomotor learning in traditional and virtual classroom higher education settings. Intern. High. Educ. 12, 7–13. doi: 10.1016/j.iheduc.2008.10.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubaltelli, E., Tedaldi, E., Orabona, N., and Scrimin, S. (2020). Environmental and psychological variables influencing reactions to the COVID-19 outbreak. Br. J. Health Psychol. 25, 1020–1038. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39, 1161–1178. doi: 10.1037/h0077714

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savitsky, B., Findling, Y., Ereli, A., and Hendel, T. (2020). Anxiety and coping strategies among nursing students during the covid-19 pandemic. Nurse Educ. Pract. 46, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.nepr.2020.102809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlosberg, H. (1954). Three dimensions of emotion. Psychol. Rev. 61, 81–88. doi: 10.1037/h0054570

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schutte, N. S., Manes, R. R., and Malouff, J. M. (2009). Antecedent-focused emotion regulation, response modulation and well-being. Curr. Psychol. 28, 21–31. doi: 10.1007/s12144-009-9044-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seli, P., Wammes, J. D., Risko, E. F., and Smilek, D. (2016). On the relation between motivation and retention in educational contexts: the role of intentional and unintentional mind wandering. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23, 1280–1287. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0979-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shao, K., Pekrun, R., Marsh, H. W., and Loderer, K. (2020). Control-value appraisals, achievement emotions, and foreign language performance: a latent interaction analysis. Learn. Instruct. 69, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2020.101356

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shorer, M., and Leibovich, L. (2020). Young children's emotional stress reactions during the COVID-19 outbreak and their associations with parental emotion regulation and parental playfulness. Early Child Dev. Care 1–11. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2020.1806830

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sobaih, A. E. E., Hasanein, A. M., and Abu Elnasr, A. E. (2020). Responses to COVID-19 in higher education: social media usage for sustaining formal academic communication in developing countries. Sustainability 12, 1–18. doi: 10.3390/su12166520

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorić, I., Penezi,ć, Z., and Burić, I. (2013). Big five personality traits, cognitive appraisals and emotion regulation strategies as predictors of achievement emotions. Psihol. Teme 22, 325–349.

Google Scholar

Sycamore, R. (2020). Anxiety during COVID-19. J. Paramed. Pract. 12:247. doi: 10.12968/jpar.2020.12.6.247

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tempelaar, D. T., Niculescu, A., Rienties, B., Gijselaers, W. H., and Giesbers, B. (2012). How achievement emotions impact students' decisions for online learning, and what precedes those emotions. Intern. High. Educ. 15, 161–169. doi: 10.1016/j.iheduc.2011.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyng, C. M., Amin, H. U., Saad, M. N. M., and Malik, A. S. (2017). The influences of emotion on learning and memory. Front. Psychol. 8, 1–22. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01454

CrossRef Full Text

Uusberg, A., Taxer, J. L., Yih, J., Uusberg, H., and Gross, J. J. (2019). Reappraising reappraisal. Emot. Rev. 11, 267–282. doi: 10.1177/1754073919862617

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuorela, M., and Nummenmaa, L. (2004). Experienced emotions, emotion regulation and student activity in a web-based learning environment. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 19, 423–436. doi: 10.1007/BF03173219

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, C., Pan, R., Wan, X., Tan, Y., Xu, L., Ho, C. S., et al. (2020a). Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 1–25. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17051729

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, C., Zhao, H., and Zhang, H. (2020b). Chinese college students have higher anxiety in new semester of online learning during COVID-19: a machine learning approach. Front. Psychol. 11:3465. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.587413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., Hegde, S., Son, C., Keller, B., Smith, A., and Sasangohar, F. (2020c). Investigating mental health of US college students during the COVID-19 pandemic: Cross-sectional survey study. J. Med. Internet Res. 22:e22817. doi: 10.2196/22817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webster, E. A., and Hadwin, A. F. (2015). Emotions and emotion regulation in undergraduate studying: examining students' reports from a self-regulated learning perspective. Educ. Psychol. 35, 794–818. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2014.895292

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., and White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 53, 5–13. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

WHO Regional Office for Europe. (2020). World Health Organization. Retrieved from: http://www.euro.who.int/en/health-topics/health-emergencies/coronavirus-covid-19/news/news/2020/3/whoannounces-covid-19-outbreak-a-pandemic (accessed October 15, 2020).

Google Scholar

Wosnitza, M., and Volet, S. (2005). Origin, direction and impact of emotions in social online learning. Learn. Instruct. 15, 449–464. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2005.07.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, J., Fan, X., and Du, J. (2018). A study of the validity and reliability of the online homework emotion regulation scale. Measurement 115, 243–248. doi: 10.1016/j.measurement.2017.10.050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, X., Zhang, M., Kong, L., Wang, Q., and Hong, J. C. (2020). The effects of scientific self-efficacy and cognitive anxiety on science engagement with the “questionobservation-doing-explanation” model during school disruption in COVID-19 pandemic. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 30, 380–393. doi: 10.1007/s10956-020-09877-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Y., Liu, K., Li, S., and Shu, M. (2020). Social media activities, emotion regulation strategies, and their interactions on people's mental health in COVID-19 pandemic. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 1–16. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17238931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeung, D. Y., and Fung, H. H. (2012). Impacts of suppression on emotional responses and performance outcomes: an experience-sampling study in younger and older workers. Journals of Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 67, 666–676. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yih, J., Uusberg, A., Taxer, J. L., and Gross, J. J. (2019). Better together: a unified perspective on appraisal and emotion regulation. Cogn. Emot. 33, 41–47. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2018.1504749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

You, J. W., and Kang, M. (2014). The role of academic emotions in the relationship between perceived academic control and self-regulated learning in online learning. Comput. Educ. 77, 125–133. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2014.04.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zembylas, M. (2008). Adult learners' emotions in online learning. Dist. Educ. 29, 71–87. doi: 10.1080/01587910802004852

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zull, J. E. (2006). “Key aspects of how the brain learns,” in The Neuroscience of Adult Learning, eds S. Johnson and K. Taylor (San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass), 3–9. doi: 10.1002/ace.213

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive reappraisal, expressive suppression, perceived control, anxiety, Covid-19 remote learning

Citation: Zhao T, Fu Z, Lian X, Ye L and Huang W (2021) Exploring Emotion Regulation and Perceived Control as Antecedents of Anxiety and Its Consequences During Covid-19 Full Remote Learning. Front. Psychol. 12:675910. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.675910

Received: 04 March 2021; Accepted: 07 June 2021;
Published: 01 July 2021.

Edited by:

Andrew M. Lane, University of Wolverhampton, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Zhonghua Zhang, The University of Melbourne, Australia
Kai-Lin Yang, National Taiwan Normal University, Taiwan

Copyright © 2021 Zhao, Fu, Lian, Ye and Huang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ting Zhao, emhhb3RpbmcmI3gwMDA0MDtzd3VmZS5lZHUuY24=; Zongmei Fu, ZnV6bSYjeDAwMDQwO3N3dWZlLmVkdS5jbg==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.