Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 07 July 2021
Sec. Psychology of Language

Preserved Perspective Taking in Free Indirect Discourse in Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine and University Hospital Cologne, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany
  • 2Institute of Language and Literature I, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany
  • 3Institute of Language and Literature – Linguistics, University of Wuppertal, Wuppertal, Germany
  • 4Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, Cognitive Neuroscience (INM-3), Research Centre Juelich, Juelich, Germany

Perspective taking has been proposed to be impaired in persons with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), especially when implicit processing is required. In narrative texts, language perception and interpretation is fundamentally guided by taking the perspective of a narrator. We studied perspective taking in the linguistic domain of so-called Free Indirect Discourse (FID), during which certain text segments have to be interpreted as the thoughts or utterances of a protagonist without explicitly being marked as thought or speech representations of that protagonist (as in direct or indirect discourse). Crucially, the correct interpretation of text segments as FID depends on the ability to detect which of the protagonists “stands out” against the others and is therefore identifiable as implicit thinker or speaker. This so-called “prominence” status of a protagonist is based on linguistic properties (e.g., grammatical function, referential expression), in other words, the perspective is “hidden” and has to be inferred from the text material. In order to test whether this implicit perspective taking ability that is required for the interpretation of FID is preserved in persons with ASD, we presented short texts with three sentences to adults with and without ASD. In the last sentence, the perspective was switched either to the more or the less prominent of two protagonists. Participants were asked to rate the texts regarding their naturalness. Both diagnostic groups rated sentences with FID anchored to the less prominent protagonist as less natural than sentences with FID anchored to the more prominent protagonist. Our results that the high-level perspective taking ability in written language that is required for the interpretation of FID is well preserved in persons with ASD supports the conclusion that language skills are highly elaborated in ASD so that even the challenging attribution of utterances to protagonists is possible if they are only implicitly given. We discuss the implications in the context of claims of impaired perspective taking in ASD as well as with regard to the underlying processing of FID.

Introduction

One of two key symptoms of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) refers to social communication and interaction disturbances (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). One explanation for these phenomena is an impaired ability to take the perspective of others (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985; Frith et al., 1991), also referred to as Theory of Mind (ToM; Premack and Woodruff, 1978) or mentalizing (Fonagy et al., 2004; Frith and Frith, 2006). This impairment has often been demonstrated in language-based tasks with children with ASD1 (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985; Baron-Cohen, 1989; Leslie and Thaiss, 1992; Swettenham, 1996; Hutchins et al., 2012; Begeer et al., 2014). Adolescents or adults with ASD and normal intelligence usually pass comparable false-belief tasks designed to probe second-order ToM tasks as successfully as control participants (Bowler, 1992; Happé, 1994; Ponnet et al., 2004; Scheeren et al., 2013). In these tasks, participants are prompted with explicit questions regarding the mental state of a protagonist. These tasks probe an explicit and, hence, better accessible type of perspective taking. On the other hand, tasks that require a more implicit type of perspective taking appear to be problematic for adolescents or adults with ASD, even under conditions of normal intelligence. This is especially revealed when participants are asked to not only infer a protagonist’s mental state, but also to provide reasons for their attributions (Ozonoff et al., 1991; Bowler, 1992; Happé, 1994; Dziobek et al., 2006; Callenmark et al., 2014), similarly, when eye movement is measured to assess overt attention in false-belief tasks (Senju et al., 2009; Schneider et al., 2013; Schuwerk et al., 2015). Impairments are also visible when inferring a protagonist’s mental state based on photo or video material (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001; Ponnet et al., 2004; Dziobek et al., 2006), which might explain why participants with ASD rely in their impressions formation of others significantly more on verbal than on non-verbal information (Kuzmanovic et al., 2011).

The interpretation of an utterance does not only depend on the linguistic content and its context, but also and essentially on the person of the speaker. An utterance of a sentence containing a so-called predicate of personal taste (e.g., “licorice is tasty”; Lasersohn (2005)) might be true for one, but not for another person. Furthermore, utterances including deictic expressions referring to persons (“I”, “you”), places (“here,” “there”) and/or time (“now”, “then”) can only be successfully interpreted in their context (i.e., speaker, reader/listener, location, time). In contrast to spoken language, written text does not always allow for an unambiguous identification of the speaker or perspectival center. It has been proposed (Zeman, 2017) that processing of so-called Free Indirect Discourse (FID; Banfield (1982)) shares an important aspect with perspective taking involved in ToM as operationalized in many false belief tasks, namely the ability to identify and differentiate between separate viewpoints at the same time. Importantly, we believe that FID processing differs from false-belief tasks insofar as perspective taking in FID is implicit. While in false-belief tasks commonly mastered by adults with ASD and normal intelligence the instruction to take a perspective is explicit, in FID it is implicit as readers are not instructed to take the perspective of a certain protagonist, but rather switch perspectives automatically in order to reach a sensible interpretation. Harris and Potts (2009) showed that certain context-sensitive markers have the potential to alter text interpretation so that perspective is shifted away from the first-person narrator to a competing protagonist. Kaiser (2015) demonstrated that FID cues increase perspectival-center-oriented text interpretation. However, these studies do not consider contexts in which multiple protagonists can serve as potential anchors for the utterance in FID mode.

In FID, utterances or thoughts are to be ascribed to a protagonist without explicitly mentioning her/him as the source of the utterance or thought. In the following example: “When Thomas entered the pub a guy in a black coat punched him right in the face with his bare hand. Ouch, how that hurt!” the reader will most likely understand that the last sentence expresses the experience of Thomas, whereas it is much less likely that the punching guy complains about his hand hurting. Without any explicit linguistic markers (e.g., quotation marks), FID is commonly indicated by the use of more subtle signals (Banfield, 1982; Steube, 1985), such as an exclamative (“Ouch!”) or a judgmental statement (“that hurt”). Often, FID can only be interpreted correctly when certain parts of the sentence such as deictic adverbials of space and time or expressions such as “Ouch” are anchored to the protagonist’s perspective (e.g., it is Thomas who feels pain, not the narrator) while others such as pronouns and tenses are anchored to the narrator’s perspective (e.g., for Thomas, being punched does not hurt in the past, but in the present) (Schlenker, 2004; Eckardt, 2014). In other words, the interpretation of FID requires the identification of the implicit anchor for a specific thought or utterance and, hence, taking the perspective of one protagonist as opposed to another (Example 1).

(A) On Monday morning Jaqueline was running to the classroom in a hurry. In the hallway she bumped into her classmatem. Now she would have to go to the nurse with that clumsy oaf.

(B) On Monday morning Arne was running to the classroom in a hurry. In the hallway he bumped into his classmatef. Now she would have to go to the nurse with that clumsy oaf.

(C) On Monday morning Arne was running to the classroom in a hurry. In the hallway he bumped into his classmatef. She went to the nurse with him.

(D) On Monday morning Arne was running to the classroom in a hurry. On the hallway he bumped into his classmatef. He went to the nurse with her.

Example 1: One variation of a scenario as it appeared in our study in the four different conditions A, B, C, and D. The last sentence of item A and B is an instance of FID that needs to be anchored to one of the two protagonists of the preceding sentences to be interpreted sensibly. Items C and D do not contain FID. All texts were presented in German, followed the same structure and were similar in style. German words may denote a specific gender (e.g., classmate, German: “Klassenkameradin,” or “Klassenkamerad”), indicated with “f” (female), and “m” (male).

In our study we follow a so-called prominence-based account for FID anchoring (Hinterwimmer, 2019), according to which the prominence status is the key for perspective ascription. Prominence refers to the property of a linguistic element (e.g., a syllable, a word, a sentence) as “standing out” in contrast to a group of similar elements (Streefkerk, 2002; Himmelmann and Primus, 2015). The protagonist who is more prominent in terms of grammatical function and type of referential expression (i.e., the expression we use to refer to an object or a person, e.g., “Thomas”, “he”) is more plausible as the anchor for FID than a competing protagonist (Hinterwimmer and Meuser, 2019). Based on the assumption that FID anchoring requires implicit perspective taking, these findings indicate that FID anchored to the more prominent protagonist is perceived as more natural and therefore receives higher ratings on a scale indicating acceptability by test persons, because it is easier or more common to take the prominent protagonist’s perspective. For the purpose of our study we systematically varied grammatical function and type of referential expression as influential factors for a protagonist’s prominence status. In the hierarchy of grammatical functions, a subject is more prominent than an indirect object, which is in turn more prominent than a direct object and so forth (Himmelmann and Primus, 2015). With respect to referential expression a protagonist that is familiar to the reader is more prominent than a protagonist that is unfamiliar (Jasinskaja et al., 2015). We make use of these prominence-lending features by claiming that a protagonist who is introduced with her/his first name and picked up by a pronoun in subject position is easier identified as the perspectival center for FID ascription than a competing protagonist who is introduced with an indefinite noun phrase in object position, which was already shown to be the case in an acceptability rating study by Hinterwimmer and Meuser (2019).

So far, it has not been clarified which particular linguistic types of perspective taking are consistently affected in exactly what way in ASD during speech and language production and perception, especially with regard to the shifting of perspectival centers. While FID perception has not been investigated in ASD so far, the production and perception of referential expressions has been studied already. While people with ASD and normal intelligence perform well in verbal perspective taking tasks, subtle differences indicate problems with respect to ToM in language production. The general population tends to adjust their choice of referential expressions to the listener or reader (i.e., depending on the context, we choose to substitute names with pronouns; Achim et al. (2017)). Adults with ASD use more full noun phrases during narratives when they could use pronouns instead, while, on the other hand, they use more pronouns when full noun phrases would be less ambiguous and hence would make it easier to understand the narration (Colle et al., 2008). This finding could indicate a reduced ToM in ASD with regard to the listener (Colle et al., 2008). This behavior has, however, not consistently been reported (Arnold et al., 2009). In a perception study investigating spatial perspective taking, participants with ASD showed unimpaired performance and neural activation comparable to a control group during the perception of written text referring to two people by their first names in third person, namely the participant and another person. On the other hand, when the task required perspective shifts induced by references to the participant as “you”, performance decreased and neural patterns differed compared to the control group (Mizuno et al., 2011).

In our web-based study, we investigate for the first time the perception of shifting perspectival centers by means of FID in written language in adults with ASD. This implicit form of perspective taking might not be as easily accomplished by adults with ASD as by adults without ASD. Therefore, we expected to identify difficulties in FID processing in persons with ASD. In our study, participants judged the naturalness of sentences including FID anchored to protagonists of different prominence status. Based on the idea that texts in which the required perspective taking is easier to accomplish are linked to higher naturalness ratings, and considering the reported perspective taking difficulties in people with ASD in implicit ToM tasks, we anticipated lower naturalness ratings in people with ASD for texts associated with implicit perspective taking, especially if the required perspective shift is an unusual one. More specifically, we pursued the following hypotheses:

H1: The difference between naturalness ratings for texts including FID (here: condition A) and ratings for texts not including FID (here: condition D) will be greater in the ASD group in comparison to the control group.

H2: The difference between naturalness ratings for texts including FID anchored to the less prominent protagonist (here: condition B) and ratings for texts including FID anchored to the more prominent protagonist (here: condition A) will be greater in the ASD group in comparison to the control group. If H1 is supported, differences between ratings for condition A and B might play a minor role.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Only participants who were monolingual native speakers of German were included in the study. For the ASD group, we recruited 45 adults with ASD via a mailing list of the Outpatient Clinic for Autism in adulthood at the University Hospital of Cologne. Of these, 41 participants had a diagnosis of Asperger syndrome (F.84.5 according to ICD-10), four participants indicated a diagnosis of high-functioning autism, one of these a diagnosis of childhood autism (F.84.0). For the control group, we recruited 45 adults without a diagnosis of ASD via the intranet of the University Hospital Cologne, publicly accessible notice boards and personal contacts (Table 1 for sample characteristics).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample characteristics.

In the group of participants with ASD, 25 of 45 participants with ASD reported that they had experienced depressive episodes. Participants with ASD indicated the following medication for the treatment of psychological, psychiatric and neurological conditions: antidepressants (15 participants), mood stabilizer (1), neuroleptic medication (1). Control participants indicated no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders. No psychotropic medication was reported by any participant in the control group. Scores for verbal intelligence as measured with the Wortschatztest (WST, Schmidt and Metzler (1992)) indicated average or above-average verbal intelligence in all participants (Table 1) and did not differ between groups (two-samples t-test, t(88) = −0.63, p = 0.530). Depressive symptoms measured with the Beck depression inventory II (BDI-II, Beck et al. (1996)) were significantly higher in participants with ASD than in control participants (Table 1, Welch two-samples t-test, t(77.1) = −3.34, p = 0.001), with symptoms ranging from none to clinically relevant symptoms in both groups. Scores indicating autistic traits measured with the autism quotient (AQ, Baron-Cohen et al. (2001)) were significantly higher in participants with ASD compared to the control group (Table 1, Welch two-samples t-test, t(75.2) = −23.08, p < 0.001). Scores indicating empathetic traits measured with the empathy quotient (EQ, Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright (2004)) were significantly lower in participants with ASD compared to the control group (Table 1, Welch two-samples t-test, t(63.1) = 16.15, p < 0.001).

Text Material

We presented short German narrative texts with three sentences each. We developed 24 different scenarios with a common theme. Each scenario was varied systematically in four different conditions, resulting in a total of 96 different texts. The conditions varied with respect to utterances with FID (conditions A and B; see example 1) or without FID in neutral story continuation (conditions C and D; see example 1. See Table 2 for an overview of experimental conditions and the Supplementary Material for the complete list of texts). The content of the utterance with FID was thematically ambiguous with respect to two protagonists that were both potential candidates for the perspectival center, i.e., the thought presented as FID in the last sentence of the text could plausibly be linked to either one of the two protagonists, if the pronoun in the third sentence did not allow for unambiguous resolution. The utterance with FID thus varied with respect to the pronoun that indicated which one of the two protagonists was the anchor of the thought.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Overview of experimental conditions; “P” stands for “protagonist”.

In the first sentence (S1) of each text, one of two protagonists was introduced by a proper name in subject position, and an explicit reference to the past (e.g., “Monday morning”) was included. In the second sentence (S2) the protagonist introduced in S1 was picked up with a personal pronoun in subject position interacting with a second protagonist who was referred to with a full noun phrase and who was anchored to the first protagonist with a possessive pronoun (e.g., “her/his classmate”). Contrary to the English equivalent, the German noun phrases used in our stimuli were each linked to a specific gender (female/male). Therefore, both protagonists (P1 and P2) differed with regard to gender so that the FID in S3 could only reasonably be anchored to either P1 or P2.

The target sentence (S3) in condition A and condition B was an utterance in FID mode. It featured three indicators of FID: (i) a temporal adverbial referring to the present (e.g., “now,” “today”) or an immediate or close future (e.g., “soon,” “tomorrow”) contrasting with the temporal adverbials in S1, (ii) a verb in subjunctive II mode (e.g., “would”), and (iii) a colloquial term or qualitative noun (e.g. “clumsy oaf”). Conditions C and D served as control conditions. Unlike the target sentence S3 in FID conditions, S3 in control conditions did not feature any markers of FID. The target sentence continued the story in neutral narrative story mode. Control condition D continued with P1 in subject position while in condition C, P2 was the subject. Thus, the two neutral conditions resembled the test conditions regarding content and syntactic structure.

In order to investigate the anchoring of FID we manipulated our texts with regard to the two protagonists in three different ways, with respect to (i) the grammatical function of the first expression referring to them (subject or object), (ii) the number of references (two or three), and iii) the type of referring expression (first name and pronouns or noun phrase and colloquial term). Based on previous findings (Hinterwimmer and Meuser, 2019) we predicted that in control participants FID anchored to the more prominent protagonist, i.e., the one in subject position, referred to with their first name and picked up by an adequate personal pronoun (condition A), would more likely be accepted as the perspectival center of a sentence in FID than the competing protagonist who was introduced with a noun phrase in object position in the second sentence (condition B). Texts in condition A should thus be rated more natural than texts in condition B.

As our manipulation of the utterance in FID mode involved a change or continuation of the subject with respect to one of the two protagonists, we included two control conditions C and D to account for the effect of subject change based on differences in referential chains: In condition C, the pronoun in subject position of the final sentence picked up the object of the preceding sentence, while in condition D, it picked up the subject. If texts of condition C would be rated comparable to texts of condition D, we might conclude that differences between the two FID conditions cannot be explained by (dis)continuity of referential chains alone. As both story continuations were equally coherent in terms of content, both control conditions C and D should be equally acceptable.

We included 40 filler texts similar to the 96 target texts in length and complexity (see Supplementary Material). In order to mask our manipulation, some filler texts were deliberately designed to yield low acceptability by an odd choice of pronouns, i.e., in the last sentence, a personal pronoun was used which referred back to an inanimate entity that occurred in object position in the previous sentence in which a personal pronoun was used to refer to the protagonist (“[…] He ate the cakem. He was made of marzipan.”). All four conditions were equally distributed across four lists so that every participant was presented with only one condition (A, B, C, or D) of each of the 24 scenarios and the total set of 40 filler texts, resulting in 64 texts in total that were presented to each participant in random order.

Procedure

The experiment was programmed and presented on Ibex farm, a platform for online experiments (Drummond, 2020). It was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 2013) and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Medical Faculty of the University of Cologne. Informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to participation. Demographic data and information on clinical diagnoses and medication was collected. In the following rating, participants were instructed to judge the naturalness of the third sentence in the context of the first two sentences of each presented text on a scale from 1 (labeled “very unnatural”) to 7 (labeled “very natural”). For each text, presentation duration including response time was limited to 25 seconds. After completing the naturalness ratings, participants were given the opportunity to report what they noticed about the task in an open format. Psychological questionnaires were obtained afterward: WST, AQ, EQ, BDI-II. The BDI-II was included due to the high incidence of depressive symptoms in persons with ASD (Ghaziuddin et al., 2002). Finally, participants had the opportunity to make assumptions with regard to the aims of the study. The whole procedure engaged participants for approximately one hour. They were debriefed and compensated for their participation with a gift voucher of ten Euro.

Analysis

Data was analyzed using R (R Core Team, 2019) in RStudio (RStudio Team, 2016). We fitted Bayesian ordinal models using the brms package (Bayesian Regression Models using Stan, v2.10.0; Bürkner (2017); Bürkner and Vuorre (2019)). Factors were sum-coded. Weakly informative priors were used for group-level effects as well as for random intercepts (normal distribution; mean = 0; standard deviation = 2) and fixed intercepts (normal distribution; mean = 4, i.e., the center of the rating scale; standard deviation = 2). Estimated parameters are reported in terms of posterior means and 95% credibility intervals. To investigate the evidence for or against the investigated effects, we compared models by calculating Bayes factors applying the bayesfactor_models function from the bayestestR package (Makowski et al., 2019) which uses bridge sampling (Gronau et al., 2020). All models ran with four sampling chains of 12,000 iterations each including a warm-up period of 2,000 iterations.

Models

To test hypothesis 1 and thus the influence of FID and diagnosis, i.e., to identify differences between the groups regarding naturalness ratings for texts with FID and ratings for comparable texts without FID, a Bayesian ordinal mixed model was fitted to the ratings from conditions A and D. Fixed effects used in the model were FID, group and their interaction. Additionally, we included random intercepts and slopes for the factor subject as well as random intercepts for text. To test hypothesis 2 and thus the influence of protagonist prominence and diagnosis, i.e., to identify differences between the groups regarding naturalness ratings for texts with FID anchored to the more prominent protagonist and ratings for texts with FID anchored to the less prominent protagonist, a Bayesian ordinal mixed model was fitted to the data of the acceptability ratings for conditions A and B. Fixed effects used in the model were prominence, group and their interaction. Additionally, we included random intercepts and slopes for the factor subject as well as random intercepts for text. To demonstrate that a subject shift toward the less prominent protagonist does not in general lead to lower ratings, but only in FID conditions, we ran a Bayesian ordinal mixed model for naturalness ratings of our control conditions that did not include FID, i.e., neutral condition C including a subject shift toward the less prominent protagonist and neutral condition D not including a subject shift. Fixed effects used in the model were subject shift, group and their interaction. Additionally, we included random intercepts and slopes for the factor subject as well as random intercepts for text. Because texts in conditions C and D are minimally different, which is not the case for texts in conditions A and B, differences between C and D are not fully equivalent to differences in A and B. Thus, the resulting conditions do not allow to test our hypotheses in a single model. Therefore, we addressed our hypotheses in separate models. To investigate evidence for or against the presence of effects, we additionally ran the following models for comparison with each of these models: the respective null model not including the group level factors; the model including only one of either factor; and the model including the linear combination of both factors. We report respective Bayes factors of model comparisons and follow the interpretation by Jeffreys (1939).

Explorative Analyses

We carried out correlational explorative analyses to identify possible relationships between the naturalness ratings and parameters we collected in addition to the ratings, i.e., psycho(patho)logical measures and age. To account for individual rating behavior, we standardized the ratings for each participant applying a rank-based non-linear transformation to the ratings of all four conditions, which for each participant results in normally distributed rating values centered around zero. Influences due to individually different scale use are therefore minimized. We investigated correlations across and within the two groups for the difference between ratings for condition A and D with our parameters (i.e., AQ, EQ, BDI-II, WST, AQ-scores for the subscales attention switching, communication and imagination, age). Differences between conditions were calculated by subtracting the standardized ratings for condition D from the standardized ratings for condition A. Likewise, correlations were investigated between our parameters and the difference between the standardized ratings for conditions A and B. We report Pearson correlation coefficients or Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients reaching significance at the 5% confidence-level.

Results

In general, texts in conditions A and B including FID were rated less natural (condition A: M = 4.34, SD = 2.23; condition B: M = 2.85, SD = 1.91) than conditions C and D not including FID (condition C: M = 4.96, SD = 2.00; condition D: M = 5.17, SD = 2.04). Across the whole sample naturalness ratings for texts in condition A were higher than for texts in condition B. Ratings did not show any statistically meaningful difference between both diagnostic groups. See Figure 1 for an overview of mean ratings per condition for both diagnostic groups.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mean naturalness ratings for the four conditions and filler texts in the ASD group (A) and the control group (B). Diamonds indicate means.

Comparison of FID Condition A and Neutral Condition D

FID affected the ratings by lowering the units on the latent rating scale by 0.48 (95% CI = [−0.64, −0.32]). An ASD diagnosis showed a general tendency to lower the ratings (b = −0.18, 95% CI = [−0.43, 0.07]), however, the influence of a diagnosis on the ratings was smaller than that of FID. The interaction of FID and group hardly affected the ratings (b = 0.03, 95% CI = [0.36, 0.29]). Model comparisons indicated extreme evidence only for an influence of FID. They further revealed moderate evidence for the absence of a group effect. Strong evidence was found against an interaction effect. Bayes factors for the models in comparison to the null model: full model: BF > 1000; model with linear combination: BF > 1000; model including only the factor group: BF = 0.16; model including only the factor FID: BF > 1000. We further investigated rating patterns in the two groups using the marginal_effects function from the brms package. The model results and the rating behavior within the two groups did not support the assumption of FID affecting rating behavior of the two groups differently. Thus, hypothesis 1 is not supported by our data.

Correlation analyses (see Table 3) showed that age was positively correlated with the difference between standardized ratings for conditions A and D in the control group (rP = 0.39, p = 0.008; ASD group: rP = 0.17, p = 0.277). This indicates that the difference between ratings for sentences including FID and ratings for sentences without FID decreases with age in the control group.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlations between psycho(pathological) measures and rating differences for compared conditions.

Comparison of FID Conditions A and B

Reducing protagonist prominence generally affected the ratings by lowering the units on the latent rating scale by 0.85 (95% CI = [1.06, 0.65]). ASD diagnosis lowered the ratings. However, this tendency was smaller than the effect of protagonist prominence (b = −0.12, 95% CI = [−0.37, 0.14]). The interaction showed that reduced prominence tended to result in higher ratings in the ASD group in comparison to the control group (b = 0.17, 95% CI = [0.23, 0.57]). Model comparisons indicated extreme evidence for an influence of reduced protagonist prominence. They further revealed moderate evidence for an absence of a group effect as well as for an absence of an interaction effect (Bayes factors for the models in comparison to the null model: full model: BF > 1000; model with linear combination: BF > 1000; model including only the factor group: BF = 0.1; model including only the factor prominence: BF > 1000). The model results and the rating behavior within the two groups did not support the assumption of prominence affecting rating behavior of the two groups differently. Thus, hypothesis 2 is not supported by our data.

Comparable to the correlation analysis for the comparison of conditions A and D, correlation analyses showed that age was positively correlated with the difference between standardized ratings for conditions A and B in the control group only (rP = 0.30, p = 0.045; ASD group: rP = −0.06, p = 0.709). This indicates that the difference between ratings for FID anchored to the less prominent protagonist and ratings for FID anchored to the more prominent protagonist decreases with age in the control group. Moreover, correlations of our psycho(patho)logical measures with the difference between standardized ratings for conditions A and B showed a statistically significant correlation across the sample (rS = 0.23, p = 0.031), which appears to mainly be driven by the sample with ASD: In this group, the scores of the AQ subscale attention switching were moderately negatively correlated with the difference between standardized ratings for conditions A and B (rS = 0.35, p = 0.018). This indicates that participants with ASD reporting more problems regarding attention switching tend to give less divergent ratings for conditions A and B.

Comparison of Neutral Conditions C and D

The analysis suggests that a subject shift alongside the respective referential expression lowered the ratings (b = 0.16, 95% CI = [0.30, 0.02]). The factor group showed a tendency to also lower the ratings (b = 0.15, 95% CI = [0.39, 0.09]). The interaction hardly influenced the ratings (b = 0.03, 95% CI = [0.25, 0.32]). Model comparisons, however, showed no reliable evidence for the presence of any of these effects and tendencies in our data, as indicated by Bayes factors favoring the null model over all other models while at the same time lacking robustness (Bayes factors for the models in comparison to the null model: full model: BF < 0.001; model with linear combination: BF = 0.01; model including only the factor group: BF = 0.13; model including only the factor subject shift: BF = 0.08).

Further Explorative Analyses

Visual inspection of naturalness ratings distributions suggested bimodality. To test if bimodality was present in our data, we tested for each ratings distribution in each condition in each group the deviance from unimodality. We used the R package diptest (v0.75-7; Mächler, 2015) which applies Hartigan’s dip test (Hartigan and Hartigan, 1985). The results indicated that unimodality was not given at a 95%-confidence level in condition C in the ASD group as well as in condition B and C in the control group. In the remaining conditions, unimodality was not given at a 90%-confidence level. Therefore, the visual impression was corroborated by the test. We performed a median split of the data to identify if there was a difference between people that tend to give higher ratings and people that tend to give lower ratings. To this end, we split the groups into two subgroups (high-rating subjects and low-rating subjects) based on their ratings in condition D, which we set as the reference condition for this analysis, because it does not contain FID nor a subject shift. We then ran the models already introduced above again with the additional factor subgroup (high-rating vs low-rating) along its interaction terms with the other factors.

The results of this analysis of subgroups showed that the negative effect of FID on the ratings in condition A as opposed to condition D seemed to be mediated mostly by participants who rated high in condition D. Most importantly, this pattern did not differ statistically in the two subgroups of both the ASD and the control group. Further, the results indicated that FID anchoring to the less prominent protagonist lead to lower ratings in all subgroups. Most importantly, this pattern did not differ statistically for the ASD subgroups and the control subgroups, indicating that the tendency for high or low ratings is more fundamental than the differential response behavior due to diagnostic groups.

Participants’ Feedback

Most participants found the texts – at least to some degree – confusing, stylistically clumsy, illogical, and/or grammatically wrong. Several participants perceived a lack of coherence due to sudden subject or perspective shifts (supposedly in the case of neutral and FID texts) or due to the third sentence containing ambiguous reference (supposedly in the case of filler texts). Six participants (five with ASD) noticed and/or found the shifts of perspective in the third sentence confusing (supposedly with regard to FID texts), referring to this factor as “perspective shift”, “shifting perspective” to the protagonist that the story was not about, “shift of (emotional) narrative perspective”, “brutal shift of the narrative perspective,” “illogical perspective,” and “ambiguous perspective.” The markers we used to indicate FID were partly perceived as unnatural, both by people with ASD and control participants. Not only markers of FID were mentioned in the feedback, but also our markers of prominence. Three participants with ASD reported problems with the interpretation of task instructions for the judgment of naturalness or a difficulty to integrate naturalness regarding the narrative style and naturalness regarding the content into a comprehensive rating of naturalness. Some participants felt torn between what to base their rating on, e.g., whether they should base their rating on what would be considered natural with regard to the behavior of the protagonists and the content of the story, or rather on whether this was a narrative form that could naturally be encountered. Three participants in the ASD group reported that they found it hard to make a decision within the time limit.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate the perception of FID and prominence in the context of FID in participants with ASD. In contrast to our hypotheses, we did not observe any difference in the performance between persons with ASD in comparison to unaffected control persons. The first focus related to hypothesis H1 was the acceptability of FID in the ASD group compared to the control group based on naturalness ratings of sentences including FID (condition A) as opposed to neutral sentences not containing FID (condition D). The second focus related to hypothesis H2 was the study of the difference between naturalness of FID anchored to a more prominent protagonist as opposed to a less prominent one (conditions A and B). Contrary to both hypotheses, the ratings were comparable and did not differ between the diagnostic groups, neither with respect to the presence of FID (conditions A vs. D, hypothesis H1) nor with respect to anchoring to more or less prominent protagonists (conditions A vs. B, hypothesis H2). Technically speaking, the factor group did not improve the adequacy of the statistical model. Taken together, both hypotheses had to be rejected.

FID Processing

Across the whole sample, naturalness ratings were lower for sentences with FID (conditions A and B) compared to sentences without FID (conditions C and D). This result is in accordance with findings of a previous study in the general population in which test items with FID received lower ratings in general. Additionally, in that study test items with FID anchored to the perspective of a more prominent protagonist yielded higher acceptability ratings than test items with FID anchored to the perspective of a less prominent protagonist (Hinterwimmer and Meuser, 2019). We could replicate this effect in our study, further supporting the notion of prominence as a relevant factor for anchoring FID. In our control analysis in neutral conditions, i.e., non-FID sentences, we showed that a subject shift as manipulated via grammatical function and referential expression shows a tendency, but not a reliable decrease, to lower acceptability ratings when pronouns need to be resolved. This indicates that the effect of prominence reported above cannot be explained by subject shift alone.

Interestingly, we found that the control sample as well as the ASD sample could both be divided into two subgroups with different rating tendencies. Persons who generated high ratings in condition D were more strongly affected by FID, whereas the effect of prominence for FID anchoring was comparable across subgroups. The explanation for these subgroups’ behavior might be trivial: High-raters might tend to rate the acceptability of texts with FID worse compared to low-raters, because they have more rating variance available to indicate their perception. However, individual factors might also play a role such as different perspective taking abilities (Kaiser and Cohen, 2012) or language dexterity. Interestingly, this pattern was visible across the control and the ASD sample, which further underlines that rating patterns for FID in general and prominence-dependent FID anchoring in particular are not affected in ASD.

With respect to the processes involved, we propose that the anchoring of FID depends both on perspective taking as well as on linguistic markers, more specifically, on perspective taking and the ascription of the perspectival center of a text which in turn depends on the linguistic notion of prominence (Hinterwimmer, 2019). That leaves two strategies to anchor an utterance in FID mode which may be both involved: (i) the reader may ascribe an utterance in FID mode to the perspectival center of the text and/or (ii) they may ascribe an utterance in FID mode to a protagonist based on linguistic markers i.e., prominence-lending cues.

Influence of Age

Another interesting observation was the correlation of the ratings with age. In the control group, we report a relationship of age with the naturalness-ratings for sentences with FID as opposed to sentences without FID, in other words, both types of sentences are rated more similar with increasing age. The same relationship was found for age and the naturalness-ratings for FID anchored to the less prominent protagonist as opposed to FID anchored to the more prominent protagonist. This might be related to a cognitive decline that also involves language comprehension (Burke and Shafto, 2007) as well as referential processing such as in anaphor resolution based on problems recalling contextual information (Light and Capps, 1986). FID processing might be affected in older participants in a similar fashion, since it requires anchoring to a protagonist previously introduced in the context. Furthermore, tracking of protagonist prominence relations has been suggested to be affected in older adulthood (Hendriks et al., 2014). More generally, studies in older participants show that ToM abilities decrease with age across different experimental tasks (Henry et al., 2013).

In contrast to these aforementioned aspects that putatively explain the reduced FID sensitivity in older participants, greater linguistic experience could on the other hand allow for easier processing (Crocker and Keller, 2006) which could in turn lead to an increased acceptance of sentences in FID mode in older people, but also to easier processing of FID anchoring to less prominent protagonists as opposed to more prominent ones. Additionally, psycho-affective changes associated with higher age might play a role, such as a more positive mindset in general (Carstensen et al., 2010). Finally, age-associated cognitive decline affecting text processing may be compensated for by other abilities that improve with age such as crystallized abilities like vocabulary, or change with age such as allocation of attention during reading (Stine-Morrow et al., 2008).

Notably, we did not observe any such relationship with age in persons with ASD. Research on aging in people with ASD is sparse in general and often inconsistent (Happé and Charlton, 2012; Howlin and Magiati, 2017). While some cognitive abilities seem to decline in ASD similarly to the general population (Howlin and Magiati, 2017), others are less affected than in the general population, such as working memory (Lever et al., 2015) or align with control participants with age resulting in comparable abilities in both groups, such as ToM abilities (Lever and Geurts, 2016). Thus, different lifetime trajectories of cognitive abilities responsible for FID processing might explain the different rating behavior in ASD with increasing age.

Conceptual Issues

Theory of Mind (ToM)

One key capacity associated with perspective taking is ToM, the ability to ascribe mental states to oneself and others, also closely related to language (Gernsbacher and Yergeau, 2019). In adolescents or adults with ASD, language abilities can partly explain performance in ToM tasks (Peterson and Miller, 2012; Lombardo et al., 2015) and strange stories tasks (Abell and Hare, 2005). Based on clinical diagnoses and WST performances, we can make sure that participants with ASD did not display any substantial language problems.

Our findings are in concordance with research showing that text-based second-order ToM abilities in high-functioning adults and adolescents with ASD are largely unimpaired (Bowler, 1992; Happé, 1994; Ponnet et al., 2004; Scheeren et al., 2013; Schuwerk et al., 2015; Murray et al., 2017). However, in contrast to our data, second-order implicit ToM abilities have indeed been reported to be affected in ASD in some studies (Ponnet et al., 2004; Dziobek et al., 2006; Murray et al., 2017). Our data show that persons with ASD are not compromised in this specific FID task. Language-related ToM impairments have been argued to be subtle (Colle et al., 2008). The most obvious interpretation seems to be that adult persons with ASD with good verbal intelligence are obviously able to learn the complex processes of perspective taking that can be expressed via written language, even if implicit perspective taking is required.

However, it is also possible that our purely behavioral measures in this web-based study were not sensitive enough to identify group differences. Previous studies have shown difficulties associated with second-order ToM tasks despite correct task responses, e.g., regarding the causal reasoning about others’ mental states (Ozonoff et al., 1991; Bowler, 1992; Happé, 1994; Dziobek et al., 2006), eye movements (Senju et al., 2009; Scheeren et al., 2013; Schneider et al., 2013; Schuwerk et al., 2015; Murray et al., 2017) as well as regarding the attribution of belief which has been shown to not happen automatically (Senju et al., 2009) and to be more difficult for adults with ASD than for control participants (Bradford et al., 2018). Future studies on FID in ASD should therefore also include either a non-text-based ToM task to assess if persons with ASD show second-order ToM impairments in other domains or a FID component that requires faster responses, possibly as a task in an ongoing interaction with another person.

Embodiment

There is strong evidence that readers tend to create complex mental models of the presented situation including the protagonists’ experiences (e.g., Zwaan and Radvansky (1998)) for which also spatial grounding is a necessary prerequisite (Beveridge and Pickering, 2013). Listeners or readers might even embody the protagonists to re-experience their actions (Kiefer and Pulvermüller, 2012) which possibly facilitates empathizing with them (van Berkum, 2019). Furthermore, participants also adopt a story’s timeline as they need more time to remember events if more time has passed in the story’s timeline (Zwaan, 1996; Carreiras et al., 1997). If taking the perspective of a protagonist is accompanied by embodiment, FID anchoring could possibly be embodied, too. Spatial perspective taking related to embodiment seems to play a role in FID interpretation as indicated by its correlation with FID sensitivity (Kaiser and Cohen, 2012). Furthermore, embodiment has been shown to be relevant for referential expressions: In written texts, processing of singular second person pronouns (Brunyé et al., 2011; Gianelli et al., 2011) as well as third person pronouns, but the latter only with spatial anchoring (Gianelli et al., 2011), are usually accompanied by embodiment in control participants. This effect seems to happen automatically (Ditman et al., 2010).

If embodiment is indeed involved in FID anchoring, the use of third-person pronouns such as in our texts might pose an obstacle for identifying its influence on FID anchoring, because embodiment seems to be limited in this case (Gianelli et al., 2011). In a study investigating different text styles on spatial grounding, Salem et al. (2017) found that FID alongside spatial anchors presented within the text did not increase self-reported identification with the protagonist nor did it affect spatial perspective taking of participants.

A disturbance of embodiment was proposed to offer an explanation for problems adults with ASD have with certain mentalizing tasks especially in the spatial domain (Pearson et al., 2013). But embodiment does not appear to be necessary, depending on the task, mental rotation processes could be employed (Pearson et al., 2013; Conson et al., 2015). In such a spatial task, participants with ASD showed mostly unimpaired performance when written texts referred to the participant or the other person with first names (Mizuno et al., 2011). In our study, we assume that participants did not make use of any such strategies related to visual perspective taking, as we have not systematically varied spatial information in our texts.

Executive Control

Basic abilities required for perspective taking are inhibitory control (Brown-Schmidt, 2009; Wardlow, 2013) and working memory capacity (Lin et al., 2010; Wardlow, 2013). Both of these executive abilities have been reported to be impaired in persons with ASD (Demetriou et al., 2018; Habib et al., 2019).

The ability to shift between or integrate different perspectives requires the balanced inhibition of one or more of potentially competing perspectives (MacWhinney, 2000; Frith and de Vignemont, 2005). Competing tasks demanding executive control hinder the correct selection of perspectives (Qureshi et al., 2010). Schwarzkopf et al. (2014) hypothesized for the visual domain that persons with ASD do in fact implicitly take the perspective of others. However, to decode behaviorally relevant interpretations of the perspective of another person, an attentional shift away from their own perspective toward another person’s perspective is necessary, which might be less easily accomplished in ASD (Schwarzkopf et al., 2014). Our explorative correlation analysis suggests that people with ASD reporting more problems with attention switching tend to give less divergent ratings for conditions A and B. One related explanation could be that impaired attention switching might lead to less perspective taking and to reduced sensitivity for cognitively effortful FID anchoring as opposed to effortless FID anchoring. However, because we did not investigate executive functions, these claims are speculative. Potentially, implicit methods could in principle reveal processing differences in ASD while behavior is otherwise unimpaired (e.g., Bradford et al. (2018)).

Executive control is not only relevant for the shifting of perspective, but also for keeping track of a story or a conversation, and thus for establishing and maintaining prominence relations, accordingly, working memory abilities have been shown to have a positive effect on the cognitive maintenance of shared conversational information or “common ground” in ASD (Schuh et al., 2016). Other abilities impaired in ASD such as planning and fluency (Demetriou et al., 2018) might play a role in predicting, updating and maintaining common ground, and thus the tracking of prominence relations. Our results suggest largely preserved abilities regarding inhibiting less prominent anchors for the interpretation of FID, of storing information in working memory to predict upcoming information and of shifting attention toward the different perspectival centers to interpret FID. Thus, in our task, participants with ASD appear to track prominence relations comparable to control participants.

Limitations

One limitation of the study was that we did not test any of the capacities discussed under the umbrella terms of ToM, embodiment or executive control. Our results therefore offer a first insight into how FID is processed at the behavioral level, but cannot yet inform us about potential differences regarding their underlying cognitive processes.

Our web-based study did not allow us to measure reaction times. Considering the issue of response time, further studies investigating persons with ASD should potentially allow for longer time frames for the participants’ response or use different methods like self-paced reading to accommodate different needs regarding the duration of stimulus presentation. To stimulate embodied text processing and thus increase perspective taking, longer and more vivid texts might be helpful (MacWhinney, 2000).

Conclusion

In this paper, we have shown that implicit perspective taking based on verbal abilities in the context of FID is fully preserved in ASD. We replicated the results of previous studies in healthy control persons (Hinterwimmer and Meuser, 2019) that the prominence status of protagonists in written short stories affects acceptability judgments of FID anchored to these protagonists. Our results suggest intact processing of FID in adults with ASD. We speculate that a possible impairment with respect to second-order ToM in ASD can possibly be compensated or can be successfully dealt with in the verbal domain when conventionalized linguistic operations are applied. Further investigations of FID interpretation in ASD will benefit from additional measures beyond naturalness ratings, such as implicit measures like reaction time, eye movement, neurophysiological measures or neuroimaging that might shed light on specific processes involved in perspective taking such as ToM, embodiment or executive control, possibly with a focus on discerning attention switching abilities and conventionalized linguistic operations. With respect to treatment, this result implies that interventions can potentially make use of these language-based resources when focusing on impairments, such as inferring mental states from photos or video animations (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001; Ponnet et al., 2004; Dziobek et al., 2006) or beliefs (Bradford et al., 2018) and intentions (Ozonoff et al., 1991; Bowler, 1992; Happé, 1994; Dziobek et al., 2006).

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because data handling is restricted to the collaborating institutes by our ethics proposal to secure sensitive data such as psycho(patho-)logical data. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to JZ, juliane.zimmermann@uk-koeln.de.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Commission of Cologne University’s Faculty of Medicine. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

SH and KV contritbuted to theoretical discussions. JZ and SM designed and conducted the study. JZ analyzed the data and wrote the first manuscript version with contributions from SM. All authors read and modified the manuscript several times. All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

The study was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) – Project-ID 281511265 – SFB 1252.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.675633/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ The use of “person-first” terminology in the context of ASD is controversial (Kenny et al., 2016; Vivanti, 2020). We apply a clinical perspective that focuses on common symptoms (or the absence thereof), which has been argued to be adequate depending on the context (Tepest, 2021).

References

Abell, F., and Hare, D. J. (2005). An experimental investigation of the phenomenology of delusional beliefs in people with Asperger syndrome. Autism 9, 515–531. doi: 10.1177/1362361305057857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Achim, A. M., Achim, A., and Fossard, M. (2017). Knowledge likely held by others affects speakers’ choices of referential expressions at different stages of discourse. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 21–36. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1234059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Available Online at: http://dsm.psychiatryonline.org/book.aspx?bookid=556 (accessed February 21, 2018)

Google Scholar

Arnold, J. E., Bennetto, L., and Diehl, J. J. (2009). Reference production in young speakers with and without autism: effects of discourse status and processing constraints. Cognition 110, 131–146. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banfield, A. (1982). Unspeakable Sentences: Narration and Representation in the Language of Fiction. Boston: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: a case of specific developmental delay. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 30, 285–297. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00241.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., and Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 163–175. doi: 10.1023/b:jadd.0000022607.19833.00

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., and Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 21, 37–46. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., and Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 31, 5–17.

Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., and Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Begeer, S., De Rosnay, M., Lunenburg, P., Stegge, H., and Terwogt, M. M. (2014). Understanding of emotions based on counterfactual reasoning in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism 18, 301–310. doi: 10.1177/1362361312468798

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beveridge, M. E. L., and Pickering, M. J. (2013). Perspective taking in language: integrating the spatial and action domains. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:577.

Google Scholar

Bowler, D. M. (1992). “Theory of mind” in Asperger’s syndrome. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 33, 877–893.

Google Scholar

Bradford, E. E. F., Hukker, V., Smith, L., and Ferguson, H. J. (2018). Belief-attribution in adults with and without autistic spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 11, 1542–1553. doi: 10.1002/aur.2032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown-Schmidt, S. (2009). The role of executive function in perspective taking during online language comprehension. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 16, 893–900. doi: 10.3758/pbr.16.5.893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunyé, T. T., Ditman, T., Mahoney, C. R., and Taylor, H. A. (2011). Better you than I: perspectives and emotion simulation during narrative comprehension. J. Cogn. Psychol. 23, 659–666. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2011.559160

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, D. M., and Shafto, M. A. (2007). “Language and aging,” in The Handbook of Aging and Cognition, eds F. I. M. Craik and T. A. Salthouse (East Sussex: Psychology Press).

Google Scholar

Bürkner, P.-C. (2017). brms: an r package for bayesian multilevel models using stan. J. Stat. Softw. 80, 1–28.

Google Scholar

Bürkner, P.-C., and Vuorre, M. (2019). Ordinal regression models in psychology: a tutorial: advances in methods and practices in psychological science. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 2:251524591882319.

Google Scholar

Callenmark, B., Kjellin, L., Rönnqvist, L., and Bölte, S. (2014). Explicit versus implicit social cognition testing in autism spectrum disorder. Autism 18, 684–693. doi: 10.1177/1362361313492393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carreiras, M., Carriedo, N., Alonso, M. A., and Fernández, A. (1997). The role of verb tense and verb aspect in the foregrounding of information during reading. Mem. Cogn. 25, 438–446. doi: 10.3758/bf03201120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carstensen, L., Turan, B., Scheibe, S., Ram, N., Hershfield, H., Samanez-Larkin, G., et al. (2010). Emotional experience improves with age: evidence based on over 10 years of experience sampling. Psychol. Aging 26, 21–33. doi: 10.1037/a0021285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colle, L., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., and van der Lely, H. K. J. (2008). Narrative discourse in adults with high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 28–40.

Google Scholar

Conson, M., Mazzarella, E., Esposito, D., Grossi, D., Marino, N., Massagli, A., et al. (2015). “Put myself into your place”: embodied simulation and perspective taking in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 8, 454–466. doi: 10.1002/aur.1460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crocker, M. W., and Keller, F. (2006). “Probabilistic grammars as models of gradience in language processing,” in Gradience in Grammar: Generative Perspectives, eds R. Vogel and M. Schlesewsky (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Demetriou, E. A., Lampit, A., Quintana, D. S., Naismith, S. L., Song, Y. J. C., Pye, J. E., et al. (2018). Autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis of executive function. Mol. Psychiatry 23, 1198–1204.

Google Scholar

Ditman, T., Brunyé, T. T., Mahoney, C. R., and Taylor, H. A. (2010). Simulating an enactment effect: pronouns guide action simulation during narrative comprehension. Cognition 115, 172–178. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drummond, A. (2020). Ibex Farm. Ibex Farm. Available Online at: https://spellout.net/ibexfarm (accessed May 15, 2020).

Google Scholar

Dziobek, I., Fleck, S., Kalbe, E., Rogers, K., Hassenstab, J., Brand, M., et al. (2006). Introducing MASC: a movie for the assessment of social cognition. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 623–636. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0107-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eckardt, R. (2014). The Semantics of Free Indirect Discourse: How Texts Allow Us to Mind-read and Eavesdrop. Leiden: Brill.

Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E., and Target, M. (2004). Affektregulierung, Mentalisierung und die Entwicklung des Selbst. Milton Park: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Frith, C. D., and Frith, U. (2006). The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron 50, 531–534.

Google Scholar

Frith, U., and de Vignemont, F. (2005). Egocentrism, allocentrism, and Asperger syndrome. Conscious Cogn. 14, 719–738. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2005.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, U., Morton, J., and Leslie, A. M. (1991). The cognitive basis of a biological disorder: autism. Trends Neurosci. 14, 433–438. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(91)90041-r

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gernsbacher, M. A., and Yergeau, M. (2019). Empirical failures of the claim that autistic people lack a theory of mind. Arch. Sci. Psychol. 7, 102–118. doi: 10.1037/arc0000067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghaziuddin, M., Ghaziuddin, N., and Greden, J. (2002). Depression in persons with autism: implications for research and clinical care. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 32, 299–306.

Google Scholar

Gianelli, C., Farnè, A., Salemme, R., Jeannerod, M., and Roy, A. C. (2011). The agent is right: when motor embodied cognition is space-dependent. PLoS One 6:e25036. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0025036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gronau, Q. F., Singmann, H., and Wagenmakers, E. (2020). bridgesampling: an r package for estimating normalizing constants. J. Stat. Softw. 10, 1–29.

Google Scholar

Habib, A., Harris, L., Pollick, F., and Melville, C. (2019). A meta-analysis of working memory in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. PLoS One 14:e0216198. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0216198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 129–154. doi: 10.1007/bf02172093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Happé, F., and Charlton, R. A. (2012). Aging in autism spectrum disorders: a mini-review. Gerontology 58, 70–78.

Google Scholar

Harris, J., and Potts, C. (2009). “Predicting perspectival orientation for appositives,” in Proceedings from the Annual Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, Vol. 2009, (Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society).

Google Scholar

Hartigan, J. A., and Hartigan, P. M. (1985). The dip test of unimodality. Ann. Stat. 13, 70–84.

Google Scholar

Hendriks, P., Koster, C., and Hoeks, J. C. (2014). Referential choice across the lifespan: why children and elderly adults produce ambiguous pronouns. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 29, 391–407. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2013.766356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., Ruffman, T., and Bailey, P. E. (2013). A meta-analytic review of age differences in theory of mind. Psychol. Aging 28, 826–839. doi: 10.1037/a0030677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Himmelmann, N. P., and Primus, B. (2015). “Prominence beyond prosody - a first approximation,” in Proceedings of the International Conference, ed. A. De Dominicis (Viterbo: DISUCOM Press), 38–58.

Google Scholar

Hinterwimmer, S. (2019). Prominent protagonists. J. Pragmatics 154, 79–91. doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2017.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinterwimmer, S., and Meuser, S. (2019). “Erlebte rede und protagonistenprominenz,” in Rede- und Gedankenwiedergabe in narrativen Strukturen – Ambiguitäten und Varianz Linguistische Berichte Sonderheft 27, eds S. Engelberg, C. Fortmann, and I. Rapp (Tübingen: Helmut Buske Verlag), 177–200.

Google Scholar

Howlin, P., and Magiati, I. (2017). Autism spectrum disorder: outcomes in adulthood. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 30, 69–76. doi: 10.1097/yco.0000000000000308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutchins, T. L., Prelock, P. A., and Bonazinga, L. (2012). Psychometric evaluation of the theory of mind inventory (ToMI): a study of typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 327–341. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1244-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jasinskaja, K., Chiriacescu, S. I., Donazzan, M., von Heusinger, K., and Hinterwimmer, S. (2015). “Prominence in discourse,” in Prominences in Linguistics. Proceedings of the pS-prominenceS International Conference, ed. A. De Dominicis (Viterbo: DISUCOM Press), 134–153.

Google Scholar

Jeffreys, H. (1939). Theory of Probability. Oxford: The Clarendon Press.

Google Scholar

Kaiser, E. (2015). Perspective-shifting and free indirect discourse: experimental investigations. Semant. Linguist. Theory 25, 346–372. doi: 10.3765/salt.v25i0.3436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaiser, E., and Cohen, A. (2012). In someone else’s shoes: a psycholinguistic investigation of FID and perspective taking. in Proceedings of the Talk Presented at the Conference “Quotation: Perspectives from Philosophy and Linguistics. Bochum: Ruhr-University-Bochum.

Google Scholar

Kenny, L., Hattersley, C., Molins, B., Buckley, C., Povey, C., and Pellicano, E. (2016). Which terms should be used to describe autism? perspectives from the UK autism community. Autism 20, 442–462. doi: 10.1177/1362361315588200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiefer, M., and Pulvermüller, F. (2012). Conceptual representations in mind and brain: theoretical developments, current evidence and future directions. Cortex 48, 805–825. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuzmanovic, B., Schilbach, L., Lehnhardt, F. G., Bente, G., and Vogeley, K. (2011). A matter of words: impact of verbal and nonverbal information on impression formation in high-functioning autism. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 5, 604–613. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lasersohn, P. (2005). Context dependence, disagreement, and predicates of personal taste. Linguist. Philos. 28, 643–686. doi: 10.1007/s10988-005-0596-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leslie, A. M., and Thaiss, L. (1992). Domain specificity in conceptual development: neuropsychological evidence from autism. Cognition 43, 225–251. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90013-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lever, A. G., and Geurts, H. M. (2016). Age-related differences in cognition across the adult lifespan in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 9, 666–676. doi: 10.1002/aur.1545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lever, A. G., Werkle-Bergner, M., Brandmaier, A. M., Ridderinkhof, K. R., and Geurts, H. M. (2015). Atypical working memory decline across the adult lifespan in autism spectrum disorder? J. Abnorm. Psychol. 124, 1014–1026. doi: 10.1037/abn0000108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Light, L. L., and Capps, J. L. (1986). Comprehension of pronouns in young and older adults. Dev. Psychol. 22, 580–585. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.22.4.580

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, S., Keysar, B., and Epley, N. (2010). Reflexively mindblind: using theory of mind to interpret behavior requires effortful attention. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 551–556. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.12.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lombardo, M. V., Lai, M. C., Auyeung, B., Holt, R. J., Allison, C., Smith, P., et al. (2015). Enhancing the precision of our understanding about mentalizing in adults with autism. BioRxiv [Preprint] doi: 10.1101/034454

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mächler, M. (2015). diptest: Hartigan’s Dip Test Statistic for Unimodality - Corrected. Available Online at: https://github.com/mmaechler/diptest (accessed March 15, 2021).

Google Scholar

MacWhinney, B. (2000). Perspective taking and grammar. Jpn. Soc. Lang. Sci. 1, 1–25.

Google Scholar

Makowski, D., Ben-Shachar, M. S., Chen, S. H. A., and Lüdecke, D. (2019). Indices of effect existence and significance in the bayesian framework. Front. Psychol. 10:2767.

Google Scholar

Mizuno, A., Liu, Y., Williams, D. L., Keller, T. A., Minshew, N. J., and Just, M. A. (2011). The neural basis of deictic shifting in linguistic perspective-taking in high-functioning autism. Brain 134, 2422–2435. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, K., Johnston, K., Cunnane, H., Kerr, C., Spain, D., Gillan, N., et al. (2017). A new test of advanced theory of mind: the “strange stories film task” captures social processing differences in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 10, 1120–1132. doi: 10.1002/aur.1744

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., and Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 32, 1081–1105. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1991.tb00351.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearson, A., Ropar, D., and Hamilton, A. F. C. (2013). A review of visual perspective taking in autism spectrum disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:652.

Google Scholar

Peterson, E., and Miller, S. (2012). The eyes test as a measure of individual differences: how much of the variance reflects verbal IQ? Front. Psychol. 3:220.

Google Scholar

Ponnet, K. S., Roeyers, H., Buysse, A., De Clercq, A., and Van der Heyden, E. (2004). Advanced mind-reading in adults with Asperger syndrome. Autism 8, 249–266. doi: 10.1177/1362361304045214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Premack, D., and Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav. Brain Sci. 1, 515–526. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x00076512

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qureshi, A. W., Apperly, I. A., and Samson, D. (2010). Executive function is necessary for perspective selection, not Level-1 visual perspective calculation: evidence from a dual-task study of adults. Cognition 117, 230–236. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2019). R: a Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

RStudio Team (2016). RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Boston, MA: RStudio, Inc.

Google Scholar

Salem, S., Weskott, T., and Holler, A. (2017). Does narrative perspective influence readers’ perspective-taking? an empirical study on free indirect discourse, psycho-narration and first-person narration. Glossa J. Gen. Linguist. 2:61. doi: 10.5334/gjgl.225

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheeren, A. M., de Rosnay, M., Koot, H. M., and Begeer, S. (2013). Rethinking theory of mind in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 54, 628–635. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlenker, P. (2004). Context of thought and context of utterance: a note on free indirect discourse and the historical present. Mind Lang. 19, 279–304. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0017.2004.00259.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, K.-H., and Metzler, P. (1992). WST-Wortschatztest. Weinheim: Beltz Test GmbH.

Google Scholar

Schneider, D., Slaughter, V. P., Bayliss, A. P., and Dux, P. E. (2013). A temporally sustained implicit theory of mind deficit in autism spectrum disorders. Cognition 129, 410–417. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuh, J., Eigsti, I.-M., and Mirman, D. (2016). Discourse comprehension in autism spectrum disorder: effects of working memory load and common ground. Autism Res. 9, 1340–1352. doi: 10.1002/aur.1632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuwerk, T., Vuori, M., and Sodian, B. (2015). Implicit and explicit theory of mind reasoning in autism spectrum disorders: the impact of experience. Autism 19, 459–468. doi: 10.1177/1362361314526004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarzkopf, S., Schilbach, L., Vogeley, K., and Timmermans, B. (2014). “Making it explicit” makes a difference: evidence for a dissociation of spontaneous and intentional level 1 perspective taking in high-functioning autism. Cognition 131, 345–35403. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., and Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: an absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science 325, 883–885. doi: 10.1126/science.1176170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steube, A. (1985). Erlebte rede aus linguistischer sicht. Zeitschrift für Germanistik 6, 389–406.

Google Scholar

Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Soederberg Miller, L. M., Gagne, D. D., and Hertzog, C. (2008). Self-regulated reading in adulthood. Psychol. Aging 23, 131–153. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Streefkerk, B. M. (2002). Prominence. Acoustic and Lexical/Syntactic Correlates. Available Online at: https://www.bibliotheek.nl/catalogus/titel.240864522.html/prominence–acoustic-and-lexical-syntactic-correlates/ (accessed January 15, 2019)

Google Scholar

Swettenham, J. G. (1996). What’s inside someone’s head? conceiving of the mind as a camera helps children with autism acquire an alternative to a theory of mind. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 1, 73–88. doi: 10.1080/135468096396712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tepest, R. (2021). The meaning of diagnosis for different designations in talking about Autism. J. Autism. Dev. Disord. 51, 760–761. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04584-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Berkum, J. J. A. (2019). “Language comprehension and emotion,” in The Oxford Handbook of Neurolinguistics, eds de Zubicaray and N. O. Schiller (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Vivanti, G. (2020). Ask the editor: what is the most appropriate way to talk about individuals with a diagnosis of autism? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 50, 691–693. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04280-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wardlow, L. (2013). Individual differences in speaker’s perspective taking: the roles of executive control and working memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 766–772. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0396-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Medical, and Association. (2013). World medical association declaration of helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA 310, 2191–2194. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeman, S. (2017). Confronting perspectives: modeling perspectival complexity in language and cognition. Glossa A J. General Linguist. 2:6.

Google Scholar

Zwaan, R. A. (1996). Processing narrative time shifts. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Memory Cogn. 22, 1196–1207. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.22.5.1196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zwaan, R. A., and Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychol. Bull. 123, 162–185. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.123.2.162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism spectrum disorder (ASD), perspective taking, free indirect discourse (FID), perspectival centers, mentalizing, theory of mind (ToM)

Citation: Zimmermann JT, Meuser S, Hinterwimmer S and Vogeley K (2021) Preserved Perspective Taking in Free Indirect Discourse in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front. Psychol. 12:675633. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.675633

Received: 04 March 2021; Accepted: 11 June 2021;
Published: 07 July 2021.

Edited by:

Antonio Benítez-Burraco, University of Seville, Spain

Reviewed by:

Mikhail Kissine, Université libre de Bruxelles, Belgium
Aparna Nadig, McGill University, Canada

Copyright © 2021 Zimmermann, Meuser, Hinterwimmer and Vogeley. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Juliane T. Zimmermann, juliane.zimmermann@uk-koeln.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.