Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 21 May 2021
Sec. Organizational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic The Psychology of Food Safety and Consumption View all 18 articles

Fitnesser’s Intrinsic Motivations of Green Eating: An Integration of Theory of Planned Behavior and Hedonic-Motivation System Adoption Model

\r\nYuan Chen,Yuan Chen1,2Bey-Fen Lee*Bey-Fen Lee3*Yen-Cheng Lu*Yen-Cheng Lu4*
  • 1Hubei University of Technology Engineering and Technology College, Wuhan, China
  • 2School of Management, Wuhan University of Technology, Wuhan, China
  • 3Department of Hospitality Management, Chung Hwa University of Medical Technology, Tainan, Taiwan
  • 4Department of Sport, Health, and Leisure, Chung Hwa University of Medical Technology, Tainan, Taiwan

Global climate change arouses people’s attention to environmental protection and, therefore, changes consumption habits. Food overconsumption not only produces extra waste but also pollutes the environment. Therefore, it is important to understand the factors that motivate people to eat green, an eco-friendly way to consume food. To keep the body in good shape, the fitnessers concern more about diet than the general people. This study explored intrinsic motivations, such as social recognition, environmental ethics, curiosity, joy of purchase, perceived usefulness, subjective norm, and perceived behavior control as constructs that affect fitnesser’s green eating intention. All constructs except curiosity have significant impacts on behavior intention. The results demonstrate that social recognition and environmental ethics have significant effects on curiosity, joy of purchase, perceived usefulness, subjective norm, and perceived behavior control. The mediation effects between social recognition and behavior intention are not supported. The mediators between environmental ethics and behavior intention are joy of purchase, perceived usefulness, subjective norm, and perceived behavior control.

Introduction

With the rapid growth of the world’s population and the increasing consumption of energy, environmental pollution caused by energy consumption has attracted wide attention. China has a population of 1.3 billion, accounting for 18.57% of the world’s population. Also, China is the world’s second largest economy (Barboza, 2010), and its food consumption and role in environmental protection cannot be ignored. Food is a basic need for human life and cannot be substituted. Food consumption accounts for 20–30% of the environmental impact in the West (Tukker and Jansen, 2006).

Fast economic development not only brings people a better life but also causes the environment a heavy burden. To prevent the environment from being destroyed by increasing pollution, the government supported various green industries and encouraged people’s green behaviors (Steg and Vlek, 2009). One of the behaviors is green eating, which is consuming food in an eco-friendly way. Key elements about eating green include eating locally grown foods, choosing organic foods if possible, limiting intake of processed or fast foods, and consuming meatless meals weekly (Weller et al., 2014). Green eating behavior can reduce energy consumption by not buying food in a distance and lessen waste production by eating fresh or organic food instead of processed food. For sustainable development, it is important to implement environmental protection education to the society, so as to understand the factors that influence green eating intention (Muposhi and Dhurup, 2017; Kim and Kim, 2018; Dalvi-Esfahani et al., 2020; Lacroix and Gifford, 2020).

Fitness has a broader meaning in Eastern countries. Not limited to bodybuilding, a fitness center could provide aerobics, sports, group classes, pools, steam rooms, Jacuzzis, saunas, or even massage rooms or lounge areas for social interaction. To maintain their body figure, fitnessers believe exercise can prevent aging, and selected diet can increase physical strength or even enhance immunity. For example, they tend to cut down on high sugar or starchy foods and eat high-fiber or low-calorie foods. Therefore, their awareness of food selection is more cautious than the general public. This study adopted the theory of planned behavior (TPB) to find out the determinants including social recognition, environmental ethics, curiosity, joy of purchase, perceived usefulness, subjective norm, and perceived behavior control to understand the impacts on fitnesser’s intention of green eating.

Theory of planned behavior is an extensively applied research model in the intention for green eating (e.g., Mundorf et al., 2018; Ahmad et al., 2020; Canova et al., 2020; Carfora et al., 2020; Dalvi-Esfahani et al., 2020; Malan et al., 2020; Ruangkanjanases et al., 2020). However, prior studies have adopted or extended TPB in several aspects for predicting intention toward green eating, and there are still two major limitations that remain to be solved. First, few of the studies have elaborated on how antecedents affect the key components for green eating. These limitations drive the possibilities for further exploration in green behaviors. Therefore, this research applied TPB as a basic framework and further attempted to include a cognitive construct (i.e., social recognition, environmental ethics, curiosity, joy of purchase, and perceived usefulness) to measure its effect on intention toward green eating. Second, because intrinsic motivation has a greater impact on human behavior than extrinsic motivation (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; Thomas and Velthouse, 1990), unlike the other scholars who apply only TPB in their studies, this study explored TPB, the hedonic-motivation system adoption model (HMSAM), and intrinsic constructs including social recognition and environmental ethics in the proposed model to examine fitnesser’s acceptance of green eating.

Literature Review and Theoretical Background

Theory of Planned Behavior

According to the theory of reasoned action (TRA), one’s behavior is based on his behavior intention, and one’s behavior intention is affected by his attitude and subjective norm (Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975). Originating from the TRA, TPB extends the TRA with perceived behavioral control as an antecedent variable of behavioral intention (Ajzen, 1991). TPB has been extensively adopted in explaining human behavior intention, not to mention environmental protection issues.

A number of studies agreed that TPB predicted different behaviors via external variables and antecedents (e.g., Chen and Hung, 2016; Paul et al., 2016; Hsu et al., 2017; Al-Jubari, 2019; Sun et al., 2019; Hoque and Hossan, 2020; Parash et al., 2020; Si et al., 2020; Uzun and Kilis, 2020). A meta-analysis conducted by Hagger et al. (2002) confirmed that the three aforesaid factors can be used to predict behavioral intentions and behavior. Therefore, this study hypothesized H1 and H2 as follows:

Hypothesis 1 (H1). Fitnesser’s subjective norm is positively correlated with their intention to eat green.

Hypothesis 2 (H2). Fitnesser’s perceived behavior control is positively correlated with their intention to eat green.

Hedonic-Motivation System Adoption Model

van der Heijden (2004) proposed his research result regarding hedonic-motivation system (HMS) by adding joy in the technology acceptance model (TAM) as a mediator between perceived ease of use and behavioral intention to use. Prior research indicated the influence of perceived usefulness on behavioral intention (e.g., Chen et al., 2009, 2013; Tsai et al., 2020). Moreover, empirical evidence also supported the relationship between perceived usefulness and purchase intention (e.g., Jamal and Sharifuddin, 2015; Moslehpour et al., 2018).

Grounded in flow-based cognitive absorption (CA), Lowry et al. (2012) improved the HMS by integrating intrinsic motivations, such as curiosity and joy in the HMSAM. Oluwajana et al. (2019) found that curiosity, joy, and perceived usefulness have a direct impact on behavior intention based on HMSAM. Past studies also provided empirical evidence linkages between behavioral intention intrinsic motivations (i.e., curiosity, joy, and perceived usefulness) (e.g., Koo and Choi, 2010; Shu, 2014; Mohammadi, 2015). According to the above discussion, this study proposed H3, H4, and H5 as follows:

Hypothesis 3 (H3). Fitnesser’s curiosity is positively correlated with their intention to eat green.

Hypothesis 4 (H4). Fitnesser’s joy of purchase is positively correlated with their intention to eat green.

Hypothesis 5 (H5). Fitnesser’s perceived usefulness is positively correlated with their intention to eat green.

Social Recognition

Social recognition is about public acknowledgment of people’s status, merits, or personality (Susewind and Walkowitz, 2020). Maslow’s (1970) hierarchy of needs classifies human needs into five levels to motivate human behavior. Social recognition serves as a postconsumption feedback on the viability of this social function (Fischer et al., 2010; McPhail et al., 2011; Stead et al., 2011). Thus, fitnessers use environmentally friendly products, and the sense of society can satisfy their need for status, achievement, recognition, and self-esteem. Similarly, in prior empirical evidence, social recognition is one of the influential antecedents in the formation of individual perceptions (e.g., Chuang and Dellmann-Jenkins, 2010; Kim et al., 2016). Therefore, this study proposed the following hypotheses:

Hypothesis 6 (H6). Fitnesser’s social recognition is positively correlated with the subjective norm to eat green.

Hypothesis 7 (H7). Fitnesser’s social recognition is positively correlated with the perceived behavioral control to eat green.

Martella et al. (2015) identified various human needs including social recognition and curiosity based on the gamification framework. To develop a successful constructivist-based learning environment, several issues including curiosity and social recognition that motivate students were the top priority (Herring, 2004).

As mentioned in the social attraction theory, society members who reflect the collective group standard may appear more attractive by other group members (Hogg and Hardie, 1991) and, hence, be beneficial from higher levels of social recognition, in which their subjective well-being is positively affected. Likewise, in a cross-country study regarding religious people, social recognition has a positive impact on happiness (Stavrova et al., 2013). Besides, prior research also found the linkage between social recognition and perceived usefulness (Wu and Chen, 2017). Therefore, our study proposed the following hypotheses:

Hypothesis 8 (H8). Fitnesser’s social recognition is positively correlated with the curiosity to eat green.

Hypothesis 9 (H9). Fitnesser’s social recognition is positively correlated with the joy to eat green.

Hypothesis 10 (H10). Fitnesser’s social recognition is positively correlated with the perceived usefulness to eat green.

Environmental Ethics

Environmental ethics ensures a healthy man–nature relationship (Misra, 1995). In the beginning, the importance between humans and nature has been ignored, but now, humans have a new understanding of ecosystems and of keeping the balance of the cultural and biological diversity of humans and other forms of life (Leopold, 1949; Ellis and Ramankutty, 2008; Leopold et al., 2010). Including the extent of human decent obligations to the environment, environmental ethics focuses on the collective action of human beings on nature (Holden, 2005). Humans and other creatures form an interdependent system on Earth, and humans are not superior to other creatures (Taylor, 1986). Rolston (2020) refers to the basis of environmental ethics as natural love of an individual, and the original experience might be hedonic.

In a recent study in the United Kingdom regarding small and medium enterprise (SME) owners, North and Nurse (2014) discovered that they have strong moral and environmental ethics in running “normal” businesses while having curiosity about the problem of climate change and its solutions. Therefore, the concerns of environmental issues have been prevailed to the “normal” citizen. McShane (2007) stated that environmental ethics should not give up on intrinsic value, like joy. On the enjoyment of life, Chinese fitnessers have become more and more concerned about the impact of ecological degradation, and advertisement emphasizing eco-friendliness appears to having significant impact on their perception of perceived usefulness and credibility (Chan, 2004). Therefore, this study proposed the following hypotheses:

Hypothesis 11 (H11). Fitnesser’s environmental ethics is positively correlated with the curiosity to eat green.

Hypothesis 12 (H12). Fitnesser’s environmental ethics is positively correlated with the joy to eat green.

Hypothesis 13 (H13). Fitnesser’s environmental ethics is positively correlated with the perceived usefulness to eat green.

In the study of food consumption (Kim, 2014), ecological concern refers to the concern about doing the right thing for animal welfare, the environment, and the ecosystem, which can be regarded as moral norms. Human should respect the environment via environmental ethics and beliefs and ensure the ethical connection between humans and the environment. These should also dominate the mind, as well as attitudes and behaviors (Chen and Hung, 2016). In this context, similar to the viewpoints of Leopold (1949), Taylor (1986), and Arvola et al. (2008), environmental ethics is significantly related to the three aforementioned factors in the TPB. Studies on ethical consumerism like Shaw and Shui (2002) used the TPB to explain customers’ behavioral intentions to purchase ethically produced goods. Therefore, the study proposed the following hypotheses:

Hypothesis 14 (H14). Fitnesser’s environmental ethics is positively correlated with the subjective norm to eat green.

Hypothesis 15 (H15). Fitnesser’s environmental ethics is positively correlated with the perceived behavior control to eat green.

According to the literature review and the hypotheses development, this study proposed a research model as shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The research model.

Research Method

Measurement scales for all constructs in this study were designed based on previous studies and were scored on a seven-point Likert scale, with higher scores indicating higher agreement with the question. Behavioral intention, subjective norm, and perceived behavioral control were adapted from Ajzen (1991), Paul et al. (2016), and Yadav and Pathak (2016). Perceived usefulness with three items was adopted from Chen et al. (2013) and Wu et al. (2015). Curiosity and joy with three items, respectively, were modified from Agarwal and Karahanna (2000) and Lowry et al. (2012). Social recognition with three scales was developed from Fischer et al. (2010) and McPhail et al. (2011). Finally, environmental ethics had three items adopted from Henriques and Sadorsky (1999). Before the formal questionnaire was sent out, a pilot test was conducted in order to revise the content of the questionnaire to avoid any discrepancies. Therefore, the content validity of this research could be improved, eliminating the occurrence of ambiguous words and inappropriate questioning through the pilot test.

The questionnaire was made available at a professional and famous online survey website1 and announced to the public in four major cities—Beijing, Shanghai, Guangzhou, and Shenzhen in China. The beginning of the questionnaire briefly explains the definition of green eating, and the first question determines whether the respondent has the experience of green eating. Only fitnessers with green eating experience were invited to participate in the survey and were instructed to answer all questionnaire items.

Beginning in early July 2020, the data collection process lasted for 3 weeks. To improve external validity, we collected data only from active fitnessers, rather than from the general public. After removing seven invalid questionnaires, 786 valid responses were analyzed by AMOS 24. Among the samples of this study, 47.80% were male and 52.20% were female. Furthermore, 16.93% were aged under 25 years, 61.31% were 26–45 years, 18.35% were 46–65 years, and 3.41% were above the age of 66 years.

Analysis Results

Measurement Model

Construct Validity

This study followed the two-step approach of structural equation modeling (SEM) proposed by Anderson and Gerbing (1988) to estimate the measurement and structural model. We chose SEM as the statistical method and AMOS as the analysis tool for two reasons. First, SEM is a family of statistical procedures that could handle the confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and path analysis. Particularly, CFA is an important procedure to measure construct validity and the path analysis is adopted to evaluate the research hypotheses (Byrne, 2001; Kline, 2011). Second, AMOS is one of the recommended analysis tools to perform the results of SEM (Babin et al., 2008). The first step examined construct reliability and validity of the measurement model using CFA, and the second step checked the path effects and their significance of the structural model. The measurement model was assessed by using the maximum likelihood estimation (MLE) in terms of factor loadings, reliability of measurement, convergent validity, and discriminant validity.

As shown in Table 1, all standardized factor loadings of questions are from 0.699 to 0.894. All composite reliability of the constructs ranging from 0.839 to 0.913 and Cronbach’s alpha ranging from 0.828 to 0.913 exceed 0.7 as recommended by Nunnally and Bernstein (1994), indicating that all constructs have internal consistency. Lastly, the average variance extracted (AVE) of the constructs ranging from 0.633 to 0.777 exceed 0.5 as suggested by Fornell and Larcker (1981) and Hair et al. (2010), showing that all constructs have adequate convergent validity. These results demonstrated that all measurement items had convergent validity.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Results of the measurement model.

As shown in Table 2, the bold numbers in the diagonal direction represent the square roots of AVEs and the off-diagonal numbers are correlations among constructs. Most of the numbers in the diagonal direction are greater than the off-diagonal numbers, and all the correlations are lower than 0.85; therefore, discriminant validity appears to be adequate for this study.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Discriminant validity of the measurement model.

Detection of Common Method Bias

Preventive measures in this research were taken in order to reduce the common method bias (CMB) that may result from sample collection of a single respondent’s cognitive information by self-reported measurement items and decrease the impact of CMB. In addition to anonymous surveys, this research attempted to hide the meaning of each question and separate measurement items for different constructs as much as possible. The variable results in Tables 1, 2 had an appropriate degree of construct validity, which also indicated that the results were not largely affected by CMB. In addition, this study adopted Harman’s single-factor test to evaluate the severity of CMB (Podsakoff et al., 2003). The model fit of the CFA for the 24 measurement items in this study was better than the single-factor model of CFA significantly. This result could be seen that the impact of CMB was not serious in this research.

Structural Model Analysis

By using the maximum likelihood method, this study performed structural model testing to estimate the hypothesized relationships of the proposed model. Model fit indicators determine the degree of whether the sample data fit the structural equation model. Schumacker and Lomax (2010) and Kline (2011) recommended a variety of criteria to determine the model fit of a structural model. Jackson et al. (2009) suggested that the commonly used model fit reporting guidelines are χ2, df, χ2/df ratio, GFI, RMSEA, SRMR, CFI, and TLI.

Table 3 demonstrates several model fit indicators and the thresholds recommended by previous studies. Except for χ2, all model fit indicators exceed the recommended levels suggested by Schumacker and Lomax (2010). Because χ2 is very sensitive to a large sample, the ratio of χ2 to its degree of freedom was computed. For a good model fit, the ideal ratio should be below three. Instead of evaluating each index independently, Hu and Bentler (1999) proposed that more strict combination rules should be applied to model fit indices so the type I errors could be controlled. The model fit indicators satisfy most of the independent level of recommended fits and the combination rule. Thus, it has been proven that the proposed model of most of the constructs has a good fit.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Model fit.

The results support the research hypotheses regarding the validity of the research model (as shown in Table 4); 54.3% of CU can be explained by SR and EE constructs, 74.2% of JP can be explained by SR and EE constructs, 64.7% of PU can be explained by SR and EE constructs, 62.8% of SN can be explained by SR and EE constructs, 62% of PBC can be explained by SR and EE constructs, and 54% of BI can be explained by CU, JP, PU, SN, and PBC constructs.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Structural model analysis.

Mediation Effects

Bootstrapping mediation analysis can provide confidence intervals to examine the indirect effects. One of the preferred bootstrapping mediation analysis methods is bias-corrected bootstrapping (Williams and MacKinnon, 2008), which is used in this study. As shown in Table 5, the total effect SR→BI, p > 0.05, bias-corrected confidence interval (CI) does include zero [CI of SR→BI = (–0.051, 0.448)]. The existence of total effect was not supported. It was not necessary to test the mediation effect. The total effect ER→BI, p < 0.05, bias-corrected CI does not include zero [CI of ER→BI = (0.281, 0.919)]. The existence of total effect was supported. The indirect effect EE→CU→BI, p > 0.05, both bias-corrected CI includes zero [CI of EE→CU→BI = (–0.036, 0.163)]. Consequently, the hypothesis of the existence of indirect effect was not supported. The indirect effect EE→JP→BI bias-corrected CI does not include zero [CI of EE→JP→BI = (0.094, 0.652)]. Thus, the hypothesis of the existence of indirect effect was supported. The indirect effect ENV→PU→BI bias-corrected CI does not include zero [CI of EE→PU→BI = (0.007, 0.405)]. Therefore, the hypothesis of the existence of indirect effect was supported. The indirect effect ENV→SN→BI bias-corrected CI does not include zero [CI of ENV→SN→CU = (0.007, 0.25)]. Consequently, the hypothesis of the existence of indirect effect was supported. The indirect effect ENV→PBC→BI bias-corrected CI does not include zero [CI of ENV→PBC→BI = (0.037, 0.401)]. Accordingly, the hypothesis of the existence of indirect effect was supported.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Analysis of mediation effects.

Conclusion

Theoretical Contributions

Due to the complex nature of sustainability, it is common for researchers to focus only on the economic aspect of environmental solutions and implications. However, this study explored fitnesser’s intrinsic motivations of green eating. Most of the environmental studies suggested to address the extension of TPB in interpreting human behavior in ecological protection issues (e.g., Bagheri et al., 2019; Suki and Suki, 2019; Si et al., 2020). Therefore, integrating only one theory is not sufficient to explain complicated human behavior. This study proposed a research model incorporating TPB and HMSAM to discuss the compact of social recognition and environmental ethics on fitnesser’s green eating intention. In addition, the mediation effects of curiosity, joy, perceived usefulness, subjective norm, and perceived behavior control were investigated. The results supported hypotheses H1 to H10. Both fitnessers’ social recognition and their environmental ethics are positively correlated with the HMSAM constructs such as curiosity, joy of purchase, and perceived usefulness and the TPB constructs like subjective norm and perceived behavior control.

Social recognition refers to the positive response of society to individual social behavior. The praise and recognition of others is helpful to promote one’s social status. In this study, fitnessers with a higher level of social recognition tend to have a higher curiosity in understanding how green eating can protect environment, have greater pleasure when eating green, and have a better understanding of the usefulness of green eating in ecological protection.

Environmental ethics is the study of environmental issues from an ethical perspective. Since environmental protection has become a manifestation in today’s society, many fitnessers are committed to environmental protection initiatives and are proud of being environmentalists. They are eager to learn more about environmental protection and hope to protect the environment better. They “feel better right now” when they have hedonic goals (Lindenberg and Steg, 2007). They feel that they are doing their part for environmental protection when they eat green and, therefore, feel happy inside.

Fitnessers with a rich knowledge of environmental protection may gain social recognition from their friends. On the other hand, to obtain social recognition, they want to know more or be curious about the mechanism between green eating and environmental protection. When they are involved in these issues, they have full understanding about what green eating can do to the environment. Consequently, they feel that eating green is useful for ecological protection and they are happy to do it in their daily lives. This explained the correlation between social recognition and joy of purchase. Similarly, fitnessers with higher environmental ethics have no doubt in their mind that green eating is useful for environmental protection.

Subjective norm is the perception of the social pressures that individuals experience when they take a particular behavior. It is not surprising that a consumer with a higher social recognition has a higher subjective norm. Fitnessers are possible to cater to their friends to gain higher social recognition, so they will not perform a certain behavior. In other words, they will comply with their friends, to follow the subjective norm, because it is important to raise their social recognition.

As mentioned above, people with higher social recognition have the tendency to cope with their friends better. That is, they have better control of themselves whether or not to perform a particular behavior. Similarly, if fitnessers have higher environmental ethics, they expect to be able to master their behavior and will not do anything that is harmful to the environment. This explained why fitnessers with a higher level of environmental ethics tend to have a higher joy of purchase. Therefore, both social recognition and environmental ethics have a positive impact on perceived behavior control.

This study not only explores the influence of social recognition and environmental ethics on behavior intention, but also discusses the mediation effects of curiosity, joy of purchase, perceived usefulness, subjective norm, and perceived behavior control. Based on the mediation effect analysis, since the total effect of social recognition to behavior intention is not significant, constructs between social recognition and behavior intention are not discussed. However, the total effect of environmental ethics to behavior intention is significant, and the mediation effects of joy of purchase, perceived usefulness, subjective norm, and perceived behavior are supported. In other words, except for the H11 that fitnesser’s curiosity is positively correlated with their intention to eat green being not supported, H12–H15 dealing with environmental ethics having impacts on the behavior intention through the four abovementioned mediators are all sustained.

Intrinsic value is the degree to which an activity is considered to be personally enjoyable (Chiu and Wang, 2008; Jiang et al., 2020). Straume and Vittersø (2012) claimed that the individuals’ feelings of joy or pleasure could affect their behavior. A number of empirical studies have also confirmed the positive effect of hedonic values on consumer behavior intention. Hedonic perception influenced fitnesser’s intentions of adoption significantly (Song, 2014). Choi and Kim (2016) found that both enjoyment and perceived usefulness affected behavioral intention positively. Similarly, in this study, perceived usefulness mediated behavior intention. This result is supported by Chang et al. (2005) in their search of quality antecedents. In a web learning tools study, Lai (2017) found that perceived behavior control mediated behavior intention, and so did the subjective norm. In a study of genetically modified foods, Kim et al. (2014) pointed out that subjective norm and perceived behavior control are positive determinants of behavior intention, though this research proves that both subjective norm and perceived behavior control are mediators between environmental ethics and behavior intention.

Managerial Implications

There are four theoretical contributions of this article. First, this research combines TPB and HMSAM to extend the research on green eating and to build up the research model from social recognition, environmental ethics, and the two aforesaid models. Second, there is no prior research using the HMS adoption model in discussing fitnesser’s intention in green eating. This study proves that joy of purchase and perceived usefulness positively affect fitnesser’s behavior intention that has filled up the research gap. Third, this study indicates that the relationship between environmental ethics and behavior intention is mediated by joy of purchase, perceived usefulness, subjective norm, and perceived behavior control. Fourth, arousing fitnesser’s social recognition and environmental ethics is helpful in increasing their intention of green eating. Unlike many previous studies that only focused on interpreting consumer behavior with TPB, this research raises the research domain to a different level by integrating social recognition and environmental ethics into the research model.

There are three practical contributions of this study. First, this study validates that increasing fitnesser’s social recognition and environmental ethics can not only raise their joy of purchase, perceived usefulness, subjective norm, and perceived behavior control but also improve their behavioral intention toward green eating. If manufacturers want to increase their sales on green eating, other than increase fitnesser’s social recognition and environmental ethics, they have to enhance fitnesser’s abovementioned four elements to change fitnesser’s behavior intention to eat green.

Second, fitnesser’s joy of purchase, perceived usefulness, subjective norm, and perceived behavior control should be promoted so their intention to eat green will be influenced. Because the mediation effects of the said four constructs are significant in this study, companies can change the fitnesser’s behavior intention to eat green if their aforesaid variables can be improved.

Third, this study demonstrates that fitnesser’s social recognition and environmental ethics are positively associated with joy of purchase, perceived usefulness, subjective norm, and perceived behavior control and are also positively associated with fitnesser’s behavior intention. Both fitnesser’s social recognition and environmental ethics indirectly affect fitnesser’s behavior intention to eat green positively via joy of purchase, perceived usefulness, subjective norm, and perceived behavior control.

Research Limitations and Future Work

Although this study provided some useful insights and viewpoints, it had several limitations and should be addressed in further research. First, the aim of this study was to determine the constructs that influence fitnesser’s behavior intention of green eating. Future research could try to integrate different theories and constructs to better interpret fitnesser’s behavior intention in comparison with this study. Second, the sample collection and research design of this study were undertaken in China. Future research could broaden the sample collection by adding samples from other countries or areas. Multiculture comparison of consumer behavior intention could be explored if possible. Finally, this study deployed a questionnaire survey that only provided cross-sectional data. The concept of environmental protection and the innovation of green eating may change over time; therefore, future research should try to conduct a longitudinal study to reveal the different effects of social recognition and environmental ethics on fitnesser’s behavior intention in different time periods.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.wjx.cn/

References

Agarwal, R., and Karahanna, E. (2000). Time flies when you’re having fun: cognitive absorption and beliefs about information technology usage. MIS Q. 24, 665–694. doi: 10.2307/3250951

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahmad, N., Ghazali, N., Abdullah, M. F., Nordin, R., Mohd Nasir, I. N., and Mohd Farid, N. A. (2020). Green marketing and its effect on consumers’ purchase behaviour: an empirical analysis. J. Int. Bus. Econ. Entrep. 5, 46–55.

Google Scholar

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 50, 179–211.

Google Scholar

Al-Jubari, I. (2019). College students’ entrepreneurial intention: testing an integrated model of SDT and TPB. Sage Open 9:21582440198. doi: 10.1177/2158244019853467

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, J. C., and Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: a review and recommended two-step approach. Psychol. Bull. 103, 411–423. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.103.3.411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arvola, A., Vassallo, M., Dean, M., Lampila, P., Saba, A., Lahteenmaki, L., et al. (2008). Predicting intentions to purchase organic food: the role of affective and moral attitudes in the Theory of Planned Behavior. Appetite 50, 443–454. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babin, B. J., Hair, J. F., and Boles, J. S. (2008). Publishing research in marketing journals using structural equation modeling. J. Mark. Theory Pract. 16, 279–286. doi: 10.2753/mtp1069-6679160401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagheri, A., Bondori, A., Allahyari, M. S., and Damalas, C. A. (2019). Modeling farmers’ intention to use pesticides: an expanded version of the theory of planned behavior. J. Environ. Manag. 248:109291. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvman.2019.109291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barboza, D. (2010). China Passes Japan as Second-Largest Economy. New York, NY: The New York Times, 15.

Google Scholar

Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS, EQS, and LISREL: comparative approaches to testing for the factorial validity of a measuring instrument. Int. J. Test. 1, 55–86. doi: 10.1207/s15327574ijt0101_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canova, L., Bobbio, A., and Manganelli, A. M. (2020). Buying organic food products: the role of trust in the Theory of Planned Behavior. Front. Psychol. 11:575820. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.575820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carfora, V., di Massimo, F., Rastelli, R., Catellani, P., and Piastra, M. (2020). Dialogue management in conversational agents through psychology of persuasion and machine learning. Multimed. Tools Appl. 79, 35949–35971. doi: 10.1007/s11042-020-09178-w

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, R. Y. (2004). Consumer responses to environmental advertising in China. Mark. Intell. Plann. 22, 427–437. doi: 10.1108/02634500410542789

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, I. C., Li, Y. C., Hung, W. F., and Hwang, H. G. (2005). An empirical study on the impact of quality antecedents on tax payers’ acceptance of Internet tax-filing systems. Gov. Inf. Q. 22, 389–410. doi: 10.1016/j.giq.2005.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, S. C., Chen, H. H., and Chen, M. F. (2009). Determinants of satisfaction and continuance intention towards self-service technologies. Ind. Manag. Data Syst. 109, 1248–1263. doi: 10.1108/02635570911002306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, S. C., and Hung, C. W. (2016). Elucidating the factors influencing the acceptance of green products: an extension of theory of planned behavior. Technol. Forecast. Soc. Change 112, 155–163. doi: 10.1016/j.techfore.2016.08.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, S. C., Liu, M. L., and Lin, C. P. (2013). Integrating technology readiness into the expectation–confirmation model: an empirical study of mobile services. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 16, 604–612. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiu, C. M., and Wang, E. T. (2008). Understanding Web-based learning continuance intention: the role of subjective task value. Inf. Manag. 45, 194–201. doi: 10.1016/j.im.2008.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, J., and Kim, S. (2016). Is the smartwatch an IT product or a fashion product? A study on factors affecting the intention to use smartwatches. Comput. Hum. Behav. 63, 777–786. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.06.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chuang, N. K., and Dellmann-Jenkins, M. (2010). Career decision making and intention: a study of hospitality undergraduate students. J. Hosp. Tour. Res. 34, 512–530. doi: 10.1177/1096348010370867

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. New York, NY: Harper and Row.

Google Scholar

Dalvi-Esfahani, M., Alaedini, Z., Nilashi, M., Samad, S., Asadi, S., and Mohammadi, M. (2020). Students’ green information technology behavior: beliefs and personality traits. J. Clean. Prod. 257:120406. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2020.120406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellis, E. C., and Ramankutty, N. (2008). Putting people in the map: anthropogenic biomes of the world. Front. Ecol. Environ. 6, 439–447. doi: 10.1890/070062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, M., Völckner, F., and Sattler, H. (2010). How important are brands? A cross-category, cross-country study. J. Mark. Res. 47, 823–839. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.47.5.823

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fishbein, M., and Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, Attitude, Intention and Behavior. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Google Scholar

Fornell, C., and Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 18, 39–50. doi: 10.2307/3151312

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L., and Biddle, S. J. (2002). A meta-analytic review of the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior in physical activity: predictive validity and the contribution of additional variables. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 24, 3–32. doi: 10.1123/jsep.24.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., and Anderson, R. E. (2010). Multivariate Data Analysis: A Global Perspective, 7th Edn. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Henriques, I., and Sadorsky, P. (1999). The relationship between environmental commitment and managerial perceptions of stakeholder importance. Acad. Manag. J. 42, 87–99. doi: 10.2307/256876

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herring, M. C. (2004). Development of constructivist-based distance learning environments: a knowledge base for K-12 teachers. Q. Rev. Distance Educ. 5, 231–242.

Google Scholar

Hogg, M. A., and Hardie, E. A. (1991). Social attraction, personal attraction, and self- categorization-, a field study. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 17, 175–180. doi: 10.1177/014616729101700209

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holden, A. (2005). Achieving a sustainable relationship between common pool resources and tourism: the role of environmental ethics. J. Sustain. Tour. 13, 339–352. doi: 10.1080/09669580508668561

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoque, M. Z., and Hossan, M. A. (2020). Understanding the influence of belief and belief revision on consumers’ purchase intention of liquid milk. Sage Open 10:2158244020922972.

Google Scholar

Hsu, C. L., Chang, C. Y., and Yansritakul, C. (2017). Exploring purchase intention of green skincare products using the theory of planned behavior: testing the moderating effects of country of origin and price sensitivity. J. Retail. Cons. Serv. 34, 145–152.

Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Modeling 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, D. L., Gillaspy, J. A. Jr., and Purc-Stephenson, R. (2009). Reporting practices in confirmatory factor analysis: an overview and some recommendations. Psychol. Methods 14, 6–23. doi: 10.1037/a0014694

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jamal, A., and Sharifuddin, J. (2015). Perceived value and perceived usefulness of halal labeling: the role of religion and culture. J. Bus. Res. 68, 933–941. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.09.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, S., Liu, R. D., Ding, Y., Fu, X., Sun, Y., Jiang, R., et al. (2020). Implicit theories and engagement in math among Chinese adolescent students: a moderated mediation model of intrinsic value and academic self-efficacy. Front. Psychol. 11:1325. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S. S., Jung, J., and Wang, K. C. (2016). Hospitality and tourism management students’ study and career preferences: comparison of three Asian regional groups. J. Hosp. Leis. Sport Tour. Educ. 19, 66–84. doi: 10.1016/j.jhlste.2016.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, W. H., and Kim, K. S. (2018). Pro-environmental intentions among food festival attendees: an application of the value-belief-norm model. Sustainability 10:3894. doi: 10.3390/su10113894

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y. G. (2014). Ecological concerns about genetically modified (GM) food consumption using the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB). Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 159, 677–681. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.12.467

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y. G., Jang, S. Y., and Kim, A. K. (2014). Application of the theory of planned behavior to genetically modified foods: moderating effects of food technology neophobia. Food Res. Int. 62, 947–954. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2014.03.057

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling, 3 Edn. New York, NY: Guilford.

Google Scholar

Koo, D. M., and Choi, Y. Y. (2010). Knowledge search and people with high epistemic curiosity. Comput. Hum. Behav. 26, 12–22. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2009.08.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacroix, K., and Gifford, R. (2020). Targeting interventions to distinct meat-eating groups reduces meat consumption. Food Qual. Prefer. 86:103997. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.103997

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, H. J. (2017). Examining civil servants’ decisions to use Web 2.0 tools for learning, based on the decomposed theory of planned behavior. Interact. Learn. Environ. 25, 295–305. doi: 10.1080/10494820.2015.1121879

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leopold, A. (1949). A Sand County Almanac and Sketches Here and There. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Leopold, M., Voelkel, J., Dethier, D., Williams, M., and Caine, N. (2010). Mountain permafrost–a valid archive to study climate change? Examples from the Rocky Mountains Front Range of Colorado, USA. Nova Acta Leopoldina, 112, 281–289.

Google Scholar

Lindenberg, S., and Steg, L. (2007). Normative, gain and hedonic goal frames guiding environmental behavior. J. Soc. Issues 63, 117–137. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2007.00499.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lowry, P. B., Gaskin, J., Twyman, N., Hammer, B., and Roberts, T. (2012). Taking ‘fun and games’ seriously: proposing the hedonic-motivation system adoption model (HMSAM). J. Assoc. Inf. Syst. 14, 617–671. doi: 10.17705/1jais.00347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malan, H., Amsler Challamel, G., Silverstein, D., Hoffs, C., Spang, E., Pace, S. A., et al. (2020). Impact of a scalable, multi-campus “Foodprint” seminar on college students’ dietary intake and dietary carbon footprint. Nutrients 12:2890. doi: 10.3390/nu12092890

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martella, R., Kray, C., and Clementini, E. (2015). “A gamification framework for volunteered geographic information,” in AGILE 2015, eds F. Bacao, M. Santos, and M. Painho (Cham: Springer), 73–89. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-16787-9_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maslow, A. H. (1970). Motivation and Personality. New York, NY: Harper and Row.

Google Scholar

McPhail, D., Chapman, G. E., and Beagan, B. L. (2011). Too much of that stuff can’t be good”: Canadian teens, morality, and fast food consumption. Soc. Sci. Med. 73, 301–307. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McShane, K. (2007). Why environmental ethics shouldn’t give up on intrinsic value. Environ. Ethics 29, 43–61. doi: 10.5840/enviroethics200729128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Misra, R. P. (1995). Environmental Ethics: A Dialogue of Cultures. Delhi: Concept Publishing Company.

Google Scholar

Mohammadi, H. (2015). Social and individual antecedents of m-learning adoption in Iran. Comput. Hum. Behav. 49, 191–207. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moslehpour, M., Pham, V. K., Wong, W. K., and Bilgiçli, i (2018). e-purchase intention of Taiwanese consumers: sustainable mediation of perceived usefulness and perceived ease of use. Sustainability 10:234. doi: 10.3390/su10010234

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mundorf, N., Redding, C. A., and Paiva, A. L. (2018). Sustainable transportation attitudes and health behavior change: evaluation of a brief stage-targeted video intervention. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15:150. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15010150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muposhi, A., and Dhurup, M. (2017). The influence of green marketing tools on green eating efficacy and green eating behaviour. J. Econ. Behav. Stud. 9, 76–87. doi: 10.22610/jebs.v9i2.1651

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

North, P., and Nurse, A. (2014). ‘War Stories’: morality, curiosity, enthusiasm and commitment as facilitators of SME owners’ engagement in low carbon transitions. Geoforum 52, 32–41. doi: 10.1016/j.geoforum.2013.12.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nunnally, J. C., and Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric Theory, 3rd Edn. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Oluwajana, D., Idowu, A., Nat, M., Vanduhe, V., and Fadiya, S. (2019). The adoption of students’ hedonic motivation system model to gamified learning environment. J. Theor. Appl. Electron. Commer. Res. 14, 156–167. doi: 10.4067/S0718-18762019000300109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parash, M. H., Suki, N. M., Shimmi, S. C., Hossain, A. T., and Murthy, K. D. (2020). Examining students’ intention to perform voluntary blood donation using a theory of planned behaviour: a structural equation modelling approach. Transfus. Clin. Biol. 27, 70–77. doi: 10.1016/j.tracli.2020.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paul, J., Modi, A., and Patel, J. (2016). Predicting green product consumption using theory of planned behavior and reasoned action. J. Retail. Cons. Serv. 29, 123-134.

Google Scholar

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., and Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–904. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolston, H. III (2020). A New Environmental Ethics: The Next Millennium for Life on Earth. Abingdon: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Ruangkanjanases, A., You, J. J., Chien, S. W., Ma, Y., Chen, S. C., and Chao, L. C. (2020). Elucidating the effect of antecedents on consumers’ green purchase intention: an extension of the Theory of Planned Behavior. Front. Psychol. 11:1433. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schumacker, R. E., and Lomax, R. G. (2010). A Beginner’s Guide to Structural Equation Modeling, 3 Edn. Abingdon: Taylor and Francis Group, LLC.

Google Scholar

Shaw, D., and Shui, E. (2002). An assessment of ethical obligation and self-identity in ethical consumer decision-making: a structural equation modeling approach. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 26, 286–293. doi: 10.1046/j.1470-6431.2002.00255.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shu, W. (2014). Continual use of microblogs. Behav. Inf. Technol. 33, 666–677. doi: 10.1080/0144929x.2013.816774

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Si, H., Shi, J. G., Tang, D., Wu, G., and Lan, J. (2020). Understanding intention and behavior toward sustainable usage of bike sharing by extending the theory of planned behavior. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 152:104513. doi: 10.1016/j.resconrec.2019.104513

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, J. (2014). Understanding the adoption of mobile innovation in China. Comput. Hum. Behav. 38, 339–348. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.06.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stavrova, O., Fetchenhauer, D., and Schlösser, T. (2013). Why are religious people happy? The effect of the social norm of religiosity across countries. Soc. Sci. Res. 42, 90–105. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2012.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stead, M., McDermott, L., MacKintosh, A. M., and Adamson, A. (2011). Why healthy eating is bad for young people’s health: identity, belonging and food. Soc. Sci. Med. 72, 1131–1139. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.12.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steg, L., and Vlek, C. (2009). Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: an integrative review and research agenda. J. Environ. Psychol. 29, 309–317. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.10.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Straume, L. V., and Vittersø, J. (2012). Happiness, inspiration and the fully functioning person: separating hedonic and eudaimonic well-being in the workplace. J. Posit. Psychol. 7, 387–398. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2012.711348

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, L., Zhou, X., and Sun, Z. (2019). Improving cycling behaviors of dockless bike-sharing users based on an extended theory of planned behavior and credit-based supervision policies in China. Front. Psychol. 10:2189. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suki, N. M., and Suki, N. M. (2019). Examination of peer influence as a moderator and predictor in explaining green purchase behaviour in a developing country. J. Clean. Produc. 228, 833–844.

Google Scholar

Susewind, M., and Walkowitz, G. (2020). Symbolic moral self-completion–social recognition of prosocial behavior reduces subsequent moral striving. Front. Psychol. 11:560188. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.560188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, P. (1986). Respect for Nature: A Theory of Environmental Ethics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Thomas, K. W., and Velthouse, B. A. (1990). Cognitive elements of empowerment: an ‘interpretive’ model of intrinsic task motivation. Acad. Manag. Rev. 15, 666–681. doi: 10.2307/258687

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsai, H., Lee, Y. P., and Ruangkanjanases, A. (2020). Understanding the effects of antecedents on continuance intention to gather food safety information on websites. Front. Psychol. 11:579322. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.579322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tukker, A., and Jansen, B. (2006). Environmental impacts of products: a detailed review of studies. J. Ind. Ecol. 10, 159–182. doi: 10.1162/jiec.2006.10.3.159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uzun, A. M., and Kilis, S. (2020). Investigating antecedents of plagiarism using extended theory of planned behavior. Comput. Educ. 144:103700. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2019.103700

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Heijden, H. (2004). User acceptance of hedonic information systems. MIS Q. 28, 695–704. doi: 10.2307/25148660

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weller, K. E., Greene, G. W., Redding, C. A., Paiva, A. L., Lofgren, I., Nash, J. T., et al. (2014). Development and validation of green eating behaviors, stage of change, decisional balance, and self-efficacy scales in college students. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 46, 324–333. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2014.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J., and MacKinnon, D. P. (2008). Resampling and distribution of the product methods for testing indirect effects in complex models. Struct. Equ. Modeling 15, 23–51. doi: 10.1080/10705510701758166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, B., and Chen, X. (2017). Continuance intention to use MOOCs: integrating the technology acceptance model (TAM) and task technology fit (TTF) model. Comput. Hum. Behav. 67, 221–232. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.10.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, J. H., Wu, C. W., Lee, C. T., and Lee, H. J. (2015). Green purchase intentions: an exploratory study of the Taiwanese electric motorcycle market. J. Bus. Res. 68, 829–833. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.11.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yadav, R., and Pathak, G. S. (2016). Young consumers’ intention towards buying green products in a developing nation: extending the theory of planned behavior. J. Clean. Prod. 135, 732–739. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2016.06.120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: intrinsic motivation, green eating, theory of planned theory, hedonic-motivation system adoption model, social recognition, environmental ethics

Citation: Chen Y, Lee B-F and Lu Y-C (2021) Fitnesser’s Intrinsic Motivations of Green Eating: An Integration of Theory of Planned Behavior and Hedonic-Motivation System Adoption Model. Front. Psychol. 12:670243. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.670243

Received: 20 February 2021; Accepted: 06 April 2021;
Published: 21 May 2021.

Edited by:

Fu-Sheng Tsai, Cheng Shiu University, Taiwan

Reviewed by:

Jui-Mei Yien, University of Kang Ning, Taiwan
Maria Gianni, University of Macedonia, Greece

Copyright © 2021 Chen, Lee and Lu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Bey-Fen Lee, bWljaGVsbGVAbWFpbC5od2FpLmVkdS50dw==; Yen-Cheng Lu, anVkb0BtYWlsLmh3YWkuZWR1LnR3

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.