Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 07 October 2021
Sec. Cognition

Guilt and Shame of What Might Have Been in Optimistic Offender Drivers

  • Faculty of Psychology, University of Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain

Habitual offender drivers are required to recover points lost on their driving license by attending reeducation courses, an experience that may, upon reflection of the incident in question, induce feelings of guilt or shame for the infractions they committed. A simulated driving task studied optimistic offender drivers to analyze the extent to which the controllability of the situational context influenced their use of internal and external factors in counterfactual thoughts and emotions such as guilt and shame. The study involved 160 drivers, of whom 54 were categorized as repeat offender drivers while 106 drivers attended courses for advanced professional driving licenses. The participants drove along a route in a driving simulator, which had been previously adjusted for the difficulty to generate a perception of high or low control. Based on the outcome obtained by the participants in this stage, each driver had to report which resources they required to improve their outcomes. Different factor ANOVAs were used to analyze our findings. The results indicated that optimistic offenders, unlike other groups (i.e., optimistic non-offender and pessimistic non-offender), thought that their results could have been better if external factors had been present (i.e., upward counterfactuals), both under conditions of high and low control. They believed their results would have been worse had it not been for their internal resources (i.e., downward counterfactuals), especially under conditions of low control. Concerning emotions of guilt and shame, offender optimists had the lowest values in both conditions compared with the other groups. We may contend that optimistic offender drivers thought they could have obtained better outcomes if external factors had been involved. In the low control condition, they justified that if it were not for such internal skills, their results could have been worse. When they generated such thoughts, the emotions of guilt and shame were minimal.

Introduction

Most traffic accidents are the result of risky behaviors performed by drivers. Cognitive-motivational theoretical models of traffic psychology analyze how drivers perceive risk and make decisions. Experimental evidence suggests that a relatively stable cognitive bias tends to exist among drivers, leading them to determine how likely they will be to have an accident (Harré et al., 2005). In some cases, the perception of risk of the driver may be connected to the situation of assumed risk, causing the driver to behave cautiously, judiciously, etc., as predicted by the so-called Zero-Risk Model (Näätänen and Summala, 1974). In other cases, when faced with a challenging situation, drivers may adopt driving skills focused on elements such as technique, ability, and mastery. Such skills explain the subjective perception the driver maintains regarding the perceived risk of being involved in a road accident and the perceived level of risk he or she is willing to assume, as outlined in Risk Homeostasis Theory (Wilde, 1982). As contended by Task Difficulty Allostasis (Fuller, 2011), this sense of risk may vary, impacting the adjustment to the perceived difficulty of the task and the prior driving skills and experience of each individual as the difficulty perceived increases.

The scientific literature encompasses numerous studies that focus on risky driving, which has been associated with different factors: tailgating, driving under the influence of drugs or alcohol, distractions, road rage, speeding, drowsy driving, and the non-use of safety belts (Dula et al., 2011; Mãirean and Havârneanu, 2018). There are fewer studies that analyze the cognitions generated by the subject regarding the “why and how” things happen. Malle and Tate (2006), for example, studied both the reasoned explanations provided by drivers regarding their intentional behavior and how the drivers believed they could achieve the desired results. Our focus here is on a line of research analyzing alternative thoughts generated after the event has taken place or after the result has been previously obtained. This type of thinking is known as “counterfactual thinking” (Roese and Morrison, 2009; Byrne, 2016; Epstude, 2018).

These types of thoughts are important because they imagine changing certain aspects of the mental representation of reality. The cognitive process developed is the creation of imagined alternatives and a process of comparison between these alternatives and the actual results obtained (Byrne, 2016). Therefore, the focus of counterfactual thinking is on thoughts related to what might have been, or how the past might have been different had certain aspects been different (Smallman and Summerville, 2018). Counterfactual thinking is represented as conditional propositions, which contain an “if” antecedent followed by a “then” consequent. This type of conditional structure commonly reflects a causal inference: “If I had been more cautious, (then) the crash could have been avoided” (that is, driving more cautiously is enough to avoid a collision). In this case, neither the antecedent action nor the consequent result happened, so counterfactual thinking focuses on what could have been, not on what happened. We can contemplate real relationships in this causal inference that the subject regards as very probable (i.e., if I drive prudently, then I avoid a collision with other vehicles). These types of thoughts, therefore, are meant to explain the past while simultaneously preparing for the future. They involve various associations, including causal associations, and affect intentions as well as decisions. Counterfactual thoughts are, therefore, precursors in the formation of intentions to carry out future actions.

Different counterfactuals can be contemplated, depending on the following dimensions. If we consider a “direction,” two types of counterfactuals can be distinguished: upward, which reflects mental simulations where the possible results are better than those obtained in reality; and downward, which is related to mental simulations where the possible results are worse than those obtained in reality. Conversely, if we consider the “structure” dimension, a counterfactual thought could be additive, as when an event could or should have transpired [e.g., if I had respected the zebra crossing, (then) I would not have hit the pedestrian], as opposed to when the event should not have occurred (e.g., if I had not gone so fast, I wouldn’t have crashed); it would reflect a subtractive counterfactual. Finally, there is the “focus” dimension. A counterfactual thought could focus on oneself or other people or circumstances. This dimension could also focus on controllable vs. uncontrollable factors, although the factors in this dimension are typically covaried. One possibility would be that self-focused counterfactuals were controllable (e.g., If only I had paid more attention …) and other focused counterfactuals were uncontrollable (e.g., If it had not been raining…). Another possibility would be that self-focused counterfactuals were uncontrollable (e.g., If I were younger …) and other-focused counterfactuals were controllable (e.g., If there had been more surveillance on the highway …) (Epstude, 2018; Smallman and Summerville, 2018).

The connection between counterfactual thoughts and emotions is established under the framework of “what could have been,” compared to the current moment (Suls and Wheeler, 2000; Mandel, 2003; Markman et al., 2008; Epstude and Roese, 2011; Tasso et al., 2017; Epstude, 2018). Seta et al. (2015) reported that when alternative worlds are better than the results experienced, emotion and cognition may be more closely linked to alternative realities than to the results obtained. Nevertheless, as a general rule, when the reality is compared with its counterfactual alternative, the emotions can be amplified (Giguère et al., 2014; Byrne, 2016). If the discrepancies between the result obtained and the desired result are minor, positive emotions may appear (e.g., relief, satisfaction, and sympathy). Conversely, greater discrepancies may result in the appearance of negative emotions (e.g., regret, guilt, and shame). In particular, under the latter condition, individuals may demonstrate repetitive, intrusive, and negative cognitions, referred to as ruminative thoughts, which may amplify negative affective states, making it difficult to solve problems (Spellman and Mandel, 1999; Wade et al., 2008; Cavalera et al., 2018).

Specifically in the case of upward counterfactual thoughts when the antecedent focuses on a personal choice (e.g., “If I had not drunk, I would have avoided road collisions”), moral emotions of shame or guilt appear if the established criteria and norms have not been met in the performance of the appropriate behaviors (Zeelenberg et al., 1998; Etxebarria, 2003; Tangney et al., 2007; Giguère et al., 2014). However, these moral emotions have their differences. Guilt appears when someone perceives that he or she is responsible for the failure in a situation considered controllable (Tangney and Dearing, 2003). The emotion of guilt facilitates the appearance of reparative behaviors to modify the result obtained (Tangney et al., 2014). Instead, when someone experiences shame, he or she attributes this to his or her failures, and not to his or her behavior in situations where there is no perceived control (Tracy and Robins, 2007). When a person experiences shame, he or she forms negative judgments about his or her abilities, generating a desire to flee and disappear (Tangney et al., 2014). Thus, a negative evaluation of own behavior and feelings of remorse or regret may be associated with guilt, while a negative evaluation of the self and feelings of helplessness or insignificance may be associated with shame (Tangney and Dearing, 2003; Schmader and Lickel, 2006; Tracy and Robins, 2007).

Different authors (Niedenthal et al., 1994; Roese and Olson, 1997) have studied the role of counterfactual thinking in reference to guilt and shame. They argue that guilt is usually associated with upward counterfactual thoughts in the form of “If I hadn’t overtaken the car, I would not have crashed,” where the alternative picks up a specific behavior, as a cause of the unwanted result. Shame, on the other hand, emphasizes the cause in the proposed alternatives, questioning the abilities of the person and focusing on the need for a change in personal behavior to undo the result. One upward counterfactual thought associated with shame, where the person has no ability, could be “If I weren’t such a distracted person, I would not have had this crash.” Finally, it should be noted that downward counterfactual thinking is linked to feelings such as satisfaction or relief. This type of thinking can fulfill a function of mood repair, as it tends to make the person feel better (Roese and Olson, 1995; Sanna, 1998; Sanna et al., 2001).

The process of self-regulation is consistent with this approach. It is triggered when an individual considers his or her present status and compares it to a more desirable one in which behavior and emotion reflect feedback control (Carver and Scheier, 2016). In their theory of the self-regulation of behavior, Scheier and Carver (1985) contend that optimists strive to achieve a desirable outcome, which they believe themselves capable of attaining. In contrast, pessimists consider the outcomes to be unattainable and will, consequently, either desist or fail to commit to the actions required to attain the target outcome. Sundry studies have focused on analyzing how different future scenarios are constructed in the minds of optimists and pessimists (Sharot, 2011; Garrett and Sharot, 2017). It is important to consider the difference between optimism and pessimism as cognitive expectations associated with future events, and counterfactual thinking as a cognitive process involving the imagination of alternative outcomes to past events.

This leads us to the focus in this study on the conception of optimistic vs. pessimistic expectations, as proposed by Carver and Scheier (2016), which indicates that dispositional optimistic expectations generally focus on the results to be obtained. This study will consider a fundamental aspect of the human behavior factor in road safety, namely, the personality traits of the drivers and their disposition, in particular, for optimism and pessimism (del Valle and Mateos, 2008; del Valle, 2019; del Valle and Matus, 2019). Similar to the study of Giguère et al. (2014), we believe that counterfactual thoughts are decisive in the study of the self-regulatory functions of guilt and shame. The consideration that counterfactual thoughts create different scenarios or alternatives in the achievement of the proposed objectives can provide insight into the causal attribution that subjects make of the result of the current situation. Different authors (Markman et al., 1995; Roese and Olson, 1997; Nasco and Marsh, 1999) point out, after obtaining a negative result, upward counterfactual thoughts are more frequent than downward counterfactuals because the upward counterfactuals reflect what the participants could have done to obtain better results. The functionality of downward counterfactual thoughts is related to the justification of the results obtained since they imagine it could have been worse.

A determining variable that mediates between optimistic and pessimistic expectations and counterfactual thoughts is the perceived control that drivers believe they have because it primarily functions as a risk moderator (Girotto et al., 1991; Helweg-Larsen and Shepperd, 2001; Klein and Helweg-Larsen, 2002). Several studies have found that drivers who overestimate their driving skills and their perceived degree of control are more inclined to drive dangerously while maintaining a more optimistic view on the risk of having an accident (Sümer et al., 2006; Fernandes et al., 2007; Harre and Sibley, 2007). The degree of control or perceived controllability is a perception of an individual of his or her capacity, available resources, or opportunities to obtain positive results or to avoid negative ones through his or her behavior (Thompson, 1981). Similar to the study of Epstude and Roese (2008), we contend that people exaggerate the personal control they believe they have over a given situation. People who generate counterfactual thoughts tend to report factors that they believe could be manipulated, controlled, or altered in some way (Byrne, 2016). There are events in daily life that are controllable (e.g., use of the seat belt) and others that are uncontrollable (e.g., traffic jams). When people generate counterfactual thoughts, they tend to imagine how the results would have been different if they had behaved differently, albeit, to a greater extent, in situations, they believe they control (Girotto et al., 1991). These controllable situations have a greater likelihood of eliciting upward (i.e., “it could have been better”) than downward counterfactuals (Roese and Olson, 1995), while upward counterfactuals enhance retrospective control perceptions (McMullen et al., 1995; Nasco and Marsh, 1999). For example, Davis et al. (1995) have noted that the “controllability effect” appears in traffic accidents involving the loss of a loved one. Drivers that have not caused the accident focus on alternatives to their behavior rather than on the alternative actions of the driver who caused the accident (“If we had not gone by car that day, we would not have had the accident”).

In the context of driving, perceived high control can inflate estimation of the drivers of their ability, whereby both the optimism and the perceived controllability of the event maintain a close relationship (Harris, 1996; White et al., 2004). Optimistic biases of an individual are expressed in their perception of personal risk, so that, in the event of an accident, the individual tends to attribute it more to external factors (e.g., rain, a blowout), as opposed to internal factors related to driving (Harré et al., 2005). Optimistic offender drivers in their driving habits make the decision to carry out risky behaviors, even knowing that these involve high probabilities of risk (e.g., increasing speed, crossing an amber traffic light, not respecting the distance of security, or checking the mobile). McKenna (1993) indicated that people believe themselves to be less likely to suffer a road accident than others when they are driving (i.e., personal control). When they are passengers, the chances of having an accident are no different than those of other people. The illusion of control is what prompts them to attribute accident-free driving to their ability rather than the effect of external factors (Hammond and Horswill, 2002).

The last variable is related to drivers who have committed traffic violations, such as driving over the alcohol limit, etc. When these offending drivers lose points on their licenses, they must attend reeducation courses to recover some or all of the points lost. In Spain, these courses are known as “intervention, awareness, and road reeducation courses in the license points system, the Directorate General for Traffic (DGT—Dirección General de Tráfico).” Various studies have been conducted in Spain with drivers who have lost their license (Cuervo and Villanueva, 2015; Valero et al., 2017; Faílde et al., 2018; Padilla et al., 2018; Martí-Belda et al., 2019). We have conducted previous studies focusing on this group of offender drivers, and we have analyzed a type of anticipatory thinking, called pre-factual thoughts. del Valle (2019) has obtained a cognitive profile of optimistic offender drivers, indicating that they believe themselves to be more likely to achieve their desired outcomes regardless of the driving conditions, considering themselves to be more skillful drivers than their peers. In another study, del Valle and Matus (2019) reported similar findings, indicating that optimistic offender drivers consider themselves more likely to achieve their desired outcomes and do not question their skills or resources. They believe their results would be even better were they assisted by external factors. However, this group does not record better results than the other groups, so this thinking is not realistic.

There are no conclusive studies in the scientific literature that analyze alternative thoughts related to “what could have been” (i.e., counterfactual thoughts) and the emotions of guilt and shame in optimistic offender drivers. Therefore, this study aimed to analyze the extent to which the controllability of the situational context in a simulated driving task performed by optimistic offender drivers who attend reeducation courses influenced their use of internal and external factors in counterfactual thoughts and negative emotions (i.e., guilt and shame). As regards this objective, we consider the extent to which optimistic offender drivers, under conditions of induced control, are different from non-offenders drivers due to the effects of the internal and external factors on counterfactual thinking and in the experiences of guilt and shame.

Materials and Methods

Participants

One hundred and eighty-two voluntary drivers anonymously took part in the study. Twenty-two of the volunteers were disqualified: 20 because they did not meet the inclusion criteria in the study groups according to optimistic vs. pessimistic expectations (i.e., four were dispositional pessimistic offenders, six were defensive pessimistic offenders, and 10 were defensive pessimistic non-offenders). Two optimistic offenders were discarded for not completing the study correctly and/or for not following the instructions throughout the different stages. Consequently, the final sample consisted of 160 participants.

The study involved three groups. The first criterion to establish the group study was based on driving offenses, offender (N = 54), and non-offender (N = 106). The offender group was formed by drivers who, with the purpose of regaining their licenses or the points deducted for their repeated offenses, were attending courses on road intervention, awareness, and rehabilitation as stipulated within the framework of the points-based license system applied by the Directorate General for Traffic (DGT) in Spain. The non-offender group consisted of people who were either attending courses for professional drivers, known as Certificate of Professional Proficiency (CAP- Certificado de Aptitud Profesional), or attending courses for obtaining another type of license. None of the drivers in the non-offender group had previously attended a course to recover points, nor had they lost any points over the past 2 years. None of the drivers in the offender group had previously attended an advanced driving course for professional drivers. The data were gathered from various driving schools in the city of Salamanca (Spain).

Sample Descriptions

The participants in the study were primarily male (76.3%), whose average age was 37.5, who were single (51.9%), and who had completed primary (30%) or higher (32.5%) education. Table 1 shows the statistics of the demographic characteristics and measures among the proposed groups.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Statistics of demographic characteristics and measures between Study Group.

To confirm whether the groups were identical, chi-square tests were conducted in the case of categorical variables, while one ANOVA was conducted for independent samples in the case of quantitative samples. To analyze possible interactions between groups, a Bonferroni correction was applied. No significant differences were found in sex χ22 = 2.23, p = 0.329, or in marital status χ24 = 1.32, p = 0.857. Regarding the level of education, optimistic offenders were found to have received lower scores in primary school, more scores in secondary school, and fewer scores in a 3-year-degree program than the pessimistic non-offender (χ26 = 13.77, p = 0.032). No significant differences were found in age [F(2,157) = 1.17, p = 0.314]; no significant differences between groups were discovered.

Variables and Measurement

Mood States

The Spanish version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988; Sandin et al., 1999) is a questionnaire used to measure the general emotional state of an individual according to responses provided. The questionnaire includes 20 items with 10 negative and 10 positive affects, which are scored on a 5-point Likert scale of 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5 (extremely). In our study, the Cronbach’s α for the negative and positive subscales are 0.83 and 0.85, respectively.

Personal feelings questionnaire (PFQ-2; Harder and Zalma, 1990; translated into Spanish by Popa, 2013) was used to assess guilt and shame. This is a questionnaire consisting of 22 items, with a Likert-type response format, where 0 means “never” and 4 “many times,” depending on how often the person experiences the feelings reflected in each item. Of the 22 feelings, only those that measure feelings of guilt (e.g., remorse, regret, worry about hurting someone…) and feelings of shame (e.g., stupidity, embarrassment, humiliation…) are scoring, specifically 16 items. The remaining six items have the function of concealing the purpose of the questionnaire and are not used to obtain the results. In our study, the internal consistency of the Shame subscale and the Guilt subscale was found to be adequate (Cronbach’s α = 0.78 and 0.77, respectively).

Optimistic and Pessimistic Expectations

The Spanish version of the Life Orientation Test-Revised (LOT-R; Scheier et al., 1994; Perczek et al., 2000) is a tool used to measure optimism and pessimism as they relate to the personality disposition of an individual. The questionnaire comprises six items and generates a continuous distribution of scores. Respondents answer each question on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = I completely agree, 5 = I completely disagree). Cronbach’s α for pessimism and optimism was found to be 0.80 and 0.86, respectively.

The Spanish version of the Optimism-Pessimism Questionnaire (OPQ; Norem and Cantor, 1986; Fernández and Bermúdez, 1999) aims to estimate the personality disposition of the respondent on a dimension of optimism and pessimism. The questionnaire is a 9-item measure and uses a 10-point Likert scale of 1 (I completely disagree) to 11 (I completely agree) for three subscales: Optimistic, pessimistic, and referring to past performance, comprising four, four, and one item, respectively. The internal consistency for the optimism scale was 0.84, while the result for the defensive pessimism scale was 0.83.

Counterfactual Thinking

We followed the procedure used in previous research to assess counterfactual thoughts (Petrocelli et al., 2011; Ferrante et al., 2013; Scholl and Sassenberg, 2014; Roese et al., 2017). In the introduction, we noted that counterfactual thinking involves mental simulations of different alternatives that could have been undertaken to achieve a different result than the one obtained. In the instructions in assessing the counterfactual thoughts, we told the participants that we were interested in knowing which alternative antecedents (i.e., behaviors, circumstances, etc.) they believed were necessary for achieving the desired result or avoiding an unwanted one.

Three judges agreed on the process of analyzing and coding the different counterfactual thoughts. Each sentence included two interactive components: the “If…” antecedent to indicate an alternative action or condition, which could have but did not occur; and the “Then…” consequent to identify a desired alternative result, which could have been, but was not, obtained. Each counterfactual thought was coded according to its direction (upward or downward) and whether the alternative antecedents included external or internal resources.

The judges used upward counterfactuals to code whether the possible results were better than those obtained in reality. The thought was coded as downward counterfactual if the possible results were worse than those obtained in reality. Similarly, the type of resource (internal or external) was used to code counterfactual thinking. For example, judges should code the statement “If I had driven more cautiously, then the accident wouldn’t have occurred” as upward counterfactual and internal factors. In the case of “If the tire had burst, then I could have had an accident,” the judges should code it as downward counterfactual and external factors. By following this procedure, “upward counterfactual” and “downward counterfactual” can each result in a single score, reflecting the resources used by the participant.

Experimental Conditions

In order to measure the driving skills and the different cognitive capabilities of each of the participants, they were asked to drive the “Advanced Road Safety Education” module in the DriveSim simulator. Two types of tasks were created for the experimental design of the study. The situational control was adjusted to allow for either low or high control. In addition to using the instructions provided with each task, we also selected different routes on the traffic simulator. For each of the tasks, we modified the instructions. In the low control task, the instructions referred to difficulty, whereas, in the high control task, they referred to manageable aspects.

The level of difficulty on the traffic simulator was adjustable, allowing us to induce low or high situational controllability. Low controllability was induced with a more complex and aggressive model, which involved difficult weather conditions, such as rain or fog, the presence of other drivers, and more aggressive traffic. These conditions resulted in more aggressive driving such as abrupt acceleration and braking. High situational controllability was induced with a less complex model, which involved pleasant dry and sunny weather conditions, and no additional drivers on the road. Upon completing the routes, the simulator showed the participants which mistakes they had made.

Procedure

The study met the approval of the Bioethics Committee of the University of Salamanca. We developed a software program to provide all the data and enable us to control the sample selection processes and the application of the different phases of the study. During the first phase of the study, the participants were told that their cognitive capabilities and driving skills would be measured and analyzed. They were required to sign a letter of informed consent (model CBEA1) adapted to the characteristics of the project, prior to participating in the study, and their sociodemographic data were collected. The emotion (i.e., PANAS) and expectations questionnaire (i.e., LOT-R and OPQ) were administered in the second stage when the sample selection criteria were applied.

The participants were assigned to the groups based on the scores they obtained in the LOT and the OPQ. We followed the procedure carried out by Fernández and Bermúdez (2000) to establish the optimistic vs. pessimistic groups. Different authors (Cantor and Norem, 1989; Elliot and Church, 2003) found that, within the established continuum between pessimism and optimism, there was a group called defensive pessimism. Considering that prior studies (Sanna, 1998; Norem and Chang, 2002; del Valle and Mateos, 2008) have shown that defensive pessimists have a differential profile in terms of emotional state and factual thoughts, we have discarded such cases from our analysis to define the study groups more reliably. The defensive pessimist group and the dispositional pessimism group, would declare pessimistic expectations about the future and focus on possible difficulties and negative results; moreover, they would display a lower perceived control and high levels of anxiety (Showers, 1992). As opposed to dispositional pessimists, defensive pessimists can recall successes achieved in the past about the same task-related problems and difficulties. Additionally, as with optimists, defensive pessimists are capable of taking on any tasks necessary to avoid the anticipated negative result.

In an effort to more precisely define the study groups, optimistic and dispositional pessimistic participants had coincided in their respective groups in both questionnaires, a procedure followed in previous experiments (Cantor and Norem, 1989; Elliot and Church, 2003). We considered both the item of past performance and the subscale of pessimism in the OPQ. This allowed us to distinguish between defensive pessimists, who reported a higher past performance, as indicated by a score of seven or more, and dispositional pessimists, who acknowledge previous negative behavior, as indicated by a score of six or less on the response scale.

Excluded individuals (i.e., defensive pessimists and dispositional pessimist offenders) performed a shorter task, which was not included in the study. The remaining members of the study sample were pseudo-randomly assigned one of the two conditions proposed (i.e., high control or low control). The participants were unaware of the existence of both conditions. The results of the questionnaire were used to determine the degree of optimism and pessimism of each participant. Using this information, the computer program assigned the participants to one of the conditions. We hoped that a similar number of participants would comprise each group. During the third phase, the participants were required to complete the assigned task. Upon completing the route, the participants were able to view the mistakes they had made and discuss them with the researcher.

Counterfactual thoughts were assessed during the next stage. The program software displayed a grammatical structure of a conditional subordinate clause to the participants, who then had to write the circumstances under which the action of the main sentence took place “If., then.” In the first part of the structure (If), the participants were asked to fill in the gaps using internal or external factors that would have been necessary to achieve the desired results of the task or avoid the undesired ones. When each of the sentences had been completed, the participant had to choose those items (i.e., guilt and shame) in the PFQ-2 questionnaire that best reflected their emotional state according to the chosen phrase.

Experimental Design

The experimental design of the study considered the following variables: group study (optimistic offenders, optimistic non-offenders, and pessimistic non-offenders) and task condition (high control vs. low control), with two dependent variable resources in upward and downward counterfactual thinking.

We have already specifically noted that the dependent variables reflect the resources (internal and external) used by the study participants when they generate counterfactual thoughts. The degree of agreement between raters on the assignment of a categorical variable was measured (Kappa = 0.71, p < 0.001). A single score was recorded for each type (upward and downward) of counterfactual thinking. The score recorded for external factors was subtracted from the score for internal factors, which could result in a negative score if the participant mentioned more internal than external factors. Another dependent variable involved emotions (i.e., guilt vs. shame). In all cases, the tests were subjected a posteriori to a Bonferroni correction to analyze the interactions.

Results

First, different ANOVAs were used to analyze whether the established groups presented significant differences in the LOT and the PANAS questionnaires. Second, additional ANOVAs were carried out to analyze whether the study groups presented differences in the total number of counterfactual thoughts and to analyze the mistakes made during the simulator task. Third, a correlation analysis was applied to analyze the existence of significant relationships between errors committed and negative emotions (i.e., guilt and shame). Fourth, to analyze whether the study groups in the proposed conditions presented differences in two variables—counterfactual factor (upward and downward) and negative emotions (i.e., guilt and shame)—two multivariate repeated measures analysis of variance (MRM-ANOVA) were carried out.

The SPSS v.25 Statistics software was used to perform the analysis. A level of significantly lower than α = 0.05 was the reference established to consider the differences significant. When significant differences were present, all the analyses calculated the size effect.

Preliminary Results

As regards the questionnaires administered, the scores of the questionnaires in each group were normally distributed. To establish whether the groups presented differences in the LOT-R, we carried out a one-way ANOVA and post hoc test for the LOT-R score for the groups. Statistically significant differences were noted in the LOT [F(2,157) = 199.89, p < 0.001] between all groups. To establish whether the groups presented differences in the positive PANAS and negative PANAS, we carried out two one-way ANOVAs and a post hoc test for positive PANAS and negative PANAS scores for the groups. We also found differences in the positive PANAS [F(2,63) = 147.01, p < 0.001] and the negative PANAS [F(2,157) = 441.19, p < 0.001] scores, in addition to significant differences between the pessimistic non-offender and the optimistic offender (p < 0.001) and optimistic non-offender (p < 0.001) groups (as shown in Table 1). However, no significant differences were found between optimistic non-offenders and optimistic offenders.

According to the total number of counterfactual thoughts, to establish whether there were differences in the total number of counterfactual thoughts between groups and conditions, we carried out an ANOVA on the total number of counterfactual thoughts. We did not find differences according to the groups [F(2,154) = 2.14, p = 0.121], nor under the conditions [F(2,154) = 0.78, p = 0.379], and we found differences under the interaction Groups × Conditions [F(2,154) = 42.36, p < 0.001] (as shown in Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics in counterfactual thinking as a function of Task Condition and Study Group.

Regarding mistakes made during the task with the simulator, we carried out an ANOVA on total number of mistakes. We did not find differences between groups and conditions [F(2,154) = 1.39, p = 0.252], nor between groups [F(2,154) = 2.03, p = 0.134], but we have found that, under the condition of high control, there were significantly fewer mistakes than under the condition of low control [F(2,154) = 129.80, p < 0.001, eta2 = 0.457] (see Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Difference in means and standard error between the optimistic offender and the optimistic non-offender and pessimistic non-offender groups under task condition for the following variables: mistake, upward counterfactuals, downward counterfactuals, guilt, and shame.

When we analyzed whether the number of mistakes occurring during the driving task was related to the levels of shame and guilt, between groups and conditions, we found that only the optimistic offender group under the condition of low control showed relationships between mistakes and shame (rx = 0.515 p = 0.007).

Counterfactual Thinking

We performed a MRM-ANOVA, which involved two factors. The first was a between-subjects factor based on “Group Study” (optimistic offender, optimistic non-offender, and pessimistic non-offender), and the second was a within-subjects factor based on “Task Conditions” (low control vs. high control) as dependent variable resources in the counterfactual factor (upward and downward).

Analyses revealed a significant interaction between Group Study × Task conditions × Counterfactual thoughts was significant [F(2,154) = 33.91, p < 0.001, eta2 = 0.31]. Group Study × Counterfactual thoughts [F(2,154) = 53.59, p < 0.001, eta2 = 0.41]. Task condition × Counterfactual thoughts [F(1,154) = 1.508, p = ns]. Bonferroni adjusted post hoc analyses tests revealed that significant differences were found in “high control” condition in upward counterfactuals [F(2,154) = 11.737, p < 0.001, eta2 = 0.13], but no statistically significant differences were found among all groups. While significant differences were found between groups comprising optimistic offenders and optimistic non-offenders (p < 0.001) and pessimistic non-offenders (p = 0.017), no statistically significant differences were found between optimistic non-offenders and pessimistic non-offenders (p = 0.145). Moreover, Bonferroni-adjusted post hoc analyses revealed differences in “high control” condition in downward counterfactuals [F(2,154) = 29.404, p < 0.001, eta2 = 0.28]. Significant differences were found between groups comprising optimistic offenders and optimistic non-offenders (p < 0.001) and optimistic non-offenders and pessimistic non-offenders (p < 0.001), but not significant differences were shown between optimistic offenders and pessimistic non-offenders (p = 0.100); (as shown in Table 3).

In “low control” condition in upward counterfactuals, Bonferroni-adjusted post hoc test showed differences [F(2,154) = 50.679, p < 0.001, eta2 = 0.40]. While statistically significant differences were found between the groups comprising optimistic offenders and optimistic non-offenders (p < 0.001) and pessimistic non-offenders (p = 0.017), none were found between the optimistic non-offenders and pessimistic non-offenders (p = 0.100). Bonferroni-adjusted post hoc analyses revealed differences in “low control” condition in downward counterfactuals [F(2,154) = 44.258, p < 0.001, eta2 = 0.36]. Statistically significant differences were found between all the groups: optimistic offenders and optimistic non-offenders (p < 0.001) and pessimistic non-offenders (p < 0.001), significant differences were found between the optimistic non-offenders and pessimistic non-offenders (p < 0.001) (see Table 3).

Emotion

A two-way MRM-ANOVA was conducted to determine the effects of Group Study and Control Task in Negative Emotions factor (guilty and shame).

Analyses revealed a significant interaction between Group Study × Task conditions × Negative Emotions was significant [F(2,154) = 19.51, p < 0.001, eta2 = 0.20]. Group Study × Negative Emotions [F(2,154) = 2.10, p = ns]. Task condition × Negative Emotions [F(1,154) = 37.73, p < 001, eta2 = 0.20]. After performing the Bonferroni-adjusted post hoc analyses tests, we found differences in the “high control” condition in guilt [F(2,154) = 81.26, p < 0.001, eta2 = 0.51], analyses showed significant differences between all groups (p < 0.001). Additionally, Bonferroni-adjusted post hoc analyses revealed differences in “high control” condition in shame [F(2,154) = 16.99, p < 0.001, eta2 = 0.18]. Significant differences were found among all the groups: optimistic offenders and optimistic non-offenders (p = 0.004) and pessimistic non-offenders (p > 0.001), and significant differences were shown between the optimistic non-offenders and pessimistic non-offenders (p = 0.035). In “low control” condition in guilt, Bonferroni-adjusted post hoc test showed there were significant differences [F(2,154) = 114.55, p < 0.001, eta2 = 0.60], and analyses showed statistically significant differences between all groups (p < 0.001). Bonferroni-adjusted post hoc analyses revealed that there were significant differences in “low control” condition in shame [F(2,154) = 156.33, p < 0.001, eta2 = 0.67]. Significant differences were found among all the groups (p < 0.001) (as shown in Table 3).

Discussion

We have analyzed to what extent the optimistic offender drivers, under conditions of induced control, differed from non-offender drivers in the effect of internal and external factors on counterfactual thinking and emotions (i.e., guilt and shame). It was found that optimistic offenders in both high- and low-control conditions used upward counterfactuals linked to external resources more than the other groups. When these participants generated these thoughts, they indicated that they could have obtained better results if there had been external factors involved. In relation to the downward counterfactuals, these participants had a different profile, depending on the control conditions. In a low-control condition, they were linked to a greater extent with internal factors. The belief that things could have been worse if they had not done something highlights protective functionality on an emotional level. With respect to emotions, we observed that this group recorded the lowest scores for negative emotions (i.e., guilt and shame) in both conditions and compared to the rest.

Consistent with previous studies (del Valle, 2019; del Valle and Matus, 2019), optimistic offenders overestimated their overall level of success. We believe that counterfactual thinking can have a dysfunctional implication. When drivers generate upward counterfactual thinking in explaining their failures or unwanted results, they overestimate their abilities (Petrocelli et al., 2012). Different studies have indicated how optimistic offender drivers have a greater tendency to qualify as more skilled compared with other drivers (Dogan et al., 2012; Horswill et al., 2013; Stephens and Ohtsuka, 2014; Mãirean and Havârneanu, 2018). These participants focus on external aspects of the situation (“If it hadn’t snowed, then I could have avoided the accident”) to justify their unwanted results, as noted by Girotto et al. (2007). Most of the causal attributions involved in upward counterfactuals tend to diminish the extent of the problem (i.e., if you had only had more time.) instead of considering other possibilities (i.e., inadequate knowledge or a misunderstanding of the problem) (Sherman and McConnell, 1995).

We contend that it is precisely this self-assessment that these drivers make of the abilities that lead them to ignore, or at least underestimate, the negative feedback provided by the environment (Allison et al., 1996; Petrocelli et al., 2012). The fact that these drivers ignore what happened, thinking about what “could have been,” may explain why these drivers have the lowest values in negative emotions (i.e., guilt and shame), both in low- and high-control conditions, as we will be commenting on in due course.

In relation to downward counterfactuals, the optimistic offenders in the low-control condition note how the results obtained could have been worse if they had not done something. These drivers also used this thinking to justify the mistakes made. In addition, these thoughts could have an affective function, helping people to feel better, as they reduce the potentially negative impact of considering an unattained result (Epstude and Roese, 2008; Sweeny and Vohs, 2012; Byrne, 2016). Similar to the study of Smallman and Summerville (2018), we think that one of the functions of counterfactuals is to provide reasons for poor performance, facilitating different excuses. This type of counterfactual implicitly denies the existence of any possibility of improvement, and it can reduce the motivation to change and improve. In these cases, counterfactuals can justify self-esteem of an individual while concurrently reducing the motivation for successive improvement (McCrea, 2008).

As mentioned earlier, optimistic offenders recorded the lowest values of negative emotions (i.e., guilt and shame). Tangney and Dearing (2003) have commented that emotions such as guilt depend on the negative judgment of a person of his or her action. This emotion tends to appear in situations in which a failure is perceived; there is a perception of controllability in his or her actions, and, therefore, the driver is attributed internal responsibility for it (e.g., “If I had not had that drink, the collision would have been avoided”). Some authors (Fedewa et al., 2005) have commented that guilt can encourage actions to amend the result generated; on the one hand, these drivers do not feel guilt, and, on the other, they attribute responsibility for the result to external aspects (e.g., If the pedestrian had not crossed the road, the accident would have been avoided). The fact that these drivers, in conditions of low control, demonstrate significant relationships between a higher number of errors committed and a higher level of shame leads us to suggest that it is shame, with the non-acceptance of internal limitations or the ability of the driver, which might explain that these drivers evade their responsibilities and indicate that they could have obtained better results if there had been external factors involved (i.e., upward counterfactuals). Fee and Tangney (2000) indicated that shame proneness was related to procrastination tendencies, whereas guilt proneness was not. These authors indicated that procrastination was a means of self-protection.

Optimistic non-offender drivers recorded a different profile than the group previously commented. We have found that downward counterfactual thoughts require special attention because they are the ones that recorded differences depending on the control conditions (as shown in Figure 1). McMullen and Markman (2000) have commented that if a downward counterfactual conjures a negative affect by considering the real possibility that the outcome could have been far worse, it could serve as an admonition to change the behavior of one. We have observed that in situations of higher control when these drivers thought things could have been worse had they not taken different measures (i.e., downward counterfactual), they also experienced more guilt than shame. Following the theories of counterfactual reasoning posited by Mandel (2005), subjects may focus not only on how the events could have been avoided but also on how they were caused. The fact that these subjects report greater guilt than shame may explain the regret they may experience when, despite controlling the situation, they perceive their failures and responsibility (Tangney and Dearing, 2003; Mandel and Dhami, 2005).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Effect of interaction between (upward and downward) counterfactual factor on study group and task condition.

Under conditions of low control, we can see how drivers recorded higher levels of shame and guilt, experienced simultaneously (as shown in Figure 1). Similar to the study of Tangney et al. (2007), we contend that guilt becomes maladaptive when merged with shame. Alicke et al. (2008) have reported the assertion among counterfactual reasoning theorists that the emotional response of an observer to an event is amplified when accompanied by the knowledge that an outcome could have been averted. Under situations of low control, in which bad results are also obtained, it is not functional to think that the results would have been worse if the driver had not carried out actions to avoid negative results (i.e., downward counterfactuals), and these could have been avoided. Furthermore, under this condition, downward counterfactual thoughts did not show the functionality indicated by Sanna (1998), Sanna et al. (2001). They gave this thought a restorative mood function, which could also be used to feel better, or as a feeling of relief.

In the group of pessimistic non-offenders, upward counterfactual thoughts varied, depending on the conditions (as shown in Figure 1). When these drivers generated this type of thoughts, they attributed the result to their abilities under conditions of low controllability (i.e., an internal, stable, and uncontrollable factor). Under this condition, they tended to feel shame. Similar to the study of Tracy and Robins (2007), we understand that shame appears when the responsibility for the result is focused on the ability of a person, not on a specific behavior “If I were a more skilled driver, I would not have had this crash.” This alternative clearly emphasizes an internal factor, which underpins a negative evaluation of the self-image of the driver. This emotion may evoke more reticent behavior, which serves to distance the driver from the situation (Tangney and Dearing, 2003). Various authors (Sanna, 1998; Nasco and Marsh, 1999; Branscombe et al., 2003) have reported that the preparative function of the upward counterfactuals is observed among individuals whose self-efficacy is high, but not low. Markman and Miller (2006) have found that people with moderate depression did not obtain benefits when considering how they could have prevented the negative results. This leads us to suggest that upward counterfactuals can become dysfunctional since they would foster the emotion of shame, as shown in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Effect of interaction between (guilt and shame) negative emotion factor on study group and task condition.

As we have been commenting, the functionality of counterfactual thoughts and negative emotions appears under situations with unfavorable outcomes, which elicit a more causal than the favorable type of reasoning (Boninger et al., 1994). The negative emotions in optimistic offenders do not vary under the different control conditions. This is not the case in the other two groups. Similar to the study of Murrar et al. (2019), we believe that shame can negatively influence behavior, while guilt can do so positively. The drivers show the influence of shame under the condition of low control on both optimistic and pessimistic non-offenders. In turn, the role of guilt appears in the high-control condition of these groups. The level of guilt is heightened when people imagine how a change in their actions could have produced a different result; for example, “I would not have been fined if I had not speeded.” In contrast, the level of shame is heightened when they imagine how a change in their personality could have produced a different result; for example, “I would not have had the accident if I were not so clumsy” (Niedenthal et al., 1994). As it is understandably difficult to change personality traits, shame is, therefore, described as a maladaptive emotion since it promotes withdrawal and hinders the reparation process, while guilt is seen as an adaptive emotion, as it motivates repair (Tangney et al., 2014; Carpenter et al., 2016).

Limitations and Future Directions

Few studies have focused on analyzing counterfactual thoughts and their link to negative emotions among optimistic offenders compared with non-offender drivers. Nevertheless, certain limitations must be taken into account when interpreting these results; the first of which is related to the groups of drivers. The scientific literature follows a theoretical continuum between optimism and pessimism, featuring additional categories, which may also be differentiated regarding the degree of perceived personal control over a positive outcome. There are, on the one hand, unrealistic optimistic drivers who consider themselves to possess extreme internal controllability and to be exempt from external difficulties that might influence the achievement of their objectives. There is also, however, a “defensive pessimistic” category that can be found halfway between the two extremes of optimism and pessimism. These individuals attempt to cope with their anxiety by anticipating the negative aspects of a situation, while simultaneously planning measures that enable them to achieve the desired outcome (Fernández and Bermúdez, 2001; del Valle and Mateos, 2008; del Valle, 2019). The second limitation involved ignoring pessimistic dispositional offenders because very few drivers view that category according to expectations. We acknowledge that data for this group would provide important information in our effort to interpret the results obtained in this study on counterfactual thinking and the negative emotions of pessimistic drivers. The third and final limitation is related to the use of objective measures to evaluate emotions linked to counterfactual thoughts. This article has focused on the evaluation of negative emotions. The experimental model in future studies should incorporate measures of positive emotions. We believe that positive emotions (e.g., satisfaction) could be characteristic of the optimistic group of offenders.

The importance of this study is in establishing the cognitive profile of drivers whose behavior behind the wheel is considered by the Directorate General for Traffic (DGT) in Spain to be unacceptable and in need of change. Our study focused on a group of optimistic offender drivers and on analyzing the role of causal attributions in counterfactual thinking as well as the emotions caused by conditions of induced control. The study of thoughts of this nature focuses on the subjective perception of control that drivers think they have. In turn, it could explain why individuals drive dangerously in a more or less voluntary manner. Awareness of how this cognitive process works and its impact on driving could foster a change in dangerous driving habits.

Implications

Our findings should help to enhance the effectiveness of driver reeducation courses, following a loss of license points, and reduce the likelihood of a relapse, which could advance efforts to prevent road accidents. We maintain that these courses are highly non-specific and do not address the characteristics of drivers that have lost all their license points. Therefore, identifying the cognitive and emotional profiles of drivers and seeing how they are connected to driving behavior is an important first step in an endeavor to develop strategies and reduce dangerous driving.

In conclusion, it is a fact that highly skilled drivers, or those who believe they are, may, be at greater risk as a result of their propensity to take risks on the road. Overestimating their abilities and not understanding their limitations are critical safety factors (Gregersen, 1996), especially when these drivers tend not to experience negative emotions when they fail. This is the case of optimistic offender drivers, unable to determine the causes of their failures. Regardless of the conditions, these drivers attribute their errors to external and uncontrollable factors, which means they do not experience, as far as possible, negative emotions in the face of failure. These failures will, therefore, tend to be repeated, and the planning of future actions will not be effective (Petrocelli and Harris, 2011), which entails a loss of opportunities to make future improvements, as indicated by different authors (Allison et al., 1996; Markman et al., 2007).

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Salamanca. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.668138/full#supplementary-material

References

Alicke, M. D., Buckingham, J., Zell, E., and Davis, T. (2008). Culpable control and counterfactual reasoning in the psychology of blame. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 1371–1381. doi: 10.1177/0146167208321594

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allison, S. T., Mackie, D. M., and Messick, D. M. (1996). Outcome biases in social perception: implications for dispositional inference, attitude change, stereotyping, and social behavior. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 28, 53–93. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60236-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boninger, D. S., Gleicher, F., and Strathman, A. (1994). Counterfactual thinking: from what might have been to what may be. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 297–307. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Branscombe, N. R., Wohl, M. J. A., Owen, S., Allison, J. A., and N’gbala, A. (2003). Counterfactual thinking, blame assignment, and well-being in rape victims. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 25, 265–273. doi: 10.1207/S15324834BASP2504_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, R. M. J. (2016). Countefactual thought. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 135–157. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cantor, N., and Norem, J. K. (1989). Defensive pessimism and stress and coping. Soc. Cogn. 7, 92–112. doi: 10.1521/soco.1989.7.2.92

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carpenter, T. P., Tignor, S. M., Tsang, J. A., and Willett, A. (2016). Dispositional self-forgiveness, guilt- and shame-proneness, and the roles of motivational tendencies. Pers. Individ. Dif. 98, 53–61. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carver, C. S., and Scheier, M. F. (2016). “Self-regulation of action and affect,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications, 3rd Edn, eds K. D. Vohs and R. F. Baumeister (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 3–23.

Google Scholar

Cavalera, C., Pepe, A., Zurloni, V., Diana, B., Realdon, O., Todisco, P., et al. (2018). Negative social emotions and cognition: shame, guilt and working memory impairments. Acta Psychol. 188, 9–15. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuervo, K., and Villanueva, L. (2015). Analysis of risk and protective factors for recidivism in Spanish youth offenders. Int. J. Offender Ther. Comp. Criminol. 59, 1149–1165. doi: 10.1177/0306624X14557917

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, C. G., Lehman, D. R., Wortman, C. B., Silver, R. C., and Thompson, S. C. (1995). The undoing of traumatic life events. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 21, 109–124. doi: 10.1177/0146167295212002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

del Valle, C. H. C. (2019). Cognitive profile of optimistic and pessimistic drivers attending re-education courses. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 65, 598–609. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2018.04.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

del Valle, C. H. C., and Mateos, P. M. (2008). Dispositional pessimism, defensive pessimism and optimism: the effect of induced mood on prefactual and counterfactual thinking and performance. Cogn. Emot. 22, 1600–1612. doi: 10.1080/02699930801940289

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

del Valle, C. H. C., and Matus, M. (2019). Effects of alcohol and perceived controllability in optimistic offender drivers. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 64, 58–69. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2019.04.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dogan, E., Steg, L., Delhomme, P., and Rothengatter, T. (2012). The effects of non-evaluative feedback on drivers’ self-evaluation and performance. Accid. Anal. Prev. 45, 522–528. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2011.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dula, C. S., Geller, E. S., and Chumney, F. L. (2011). A social-cognitive model of driver aggression: taking situations and individual differences into account. Curr. Psychol. 30, 324–334. doi: 10.1007/s12144-011-9120-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliot, A., and Church, M. (2003). A motivational analysis of defensive pessimism and self-handicapping. J. Pers. 71, 369–396. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.7103005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epstude, K. (2018). “Counterfactual thinking,” in The Psychology of Thinking about the Future, eds G. Oettingen, A. Timur Servincer, and P. M. Gollwitzer (New York, NY: The Guilford press).

Google Scholar

Epstude, K., and Roese, N. J. (2008). The functional theory of counterfactual thinking. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 12, 168–192. doi: 10.1177/1088868308316091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epstude, K., and Roese, N. J. (2011). When goal pursuit fails. The functions of counterfactual thought in intention formation. Soc. Psychol. 42, 19–27. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Etxebarria, I. (2003). “Las emocionesautoconscientes: culpa, vergüenza y orgullo [Self-conscious emotions: guilt, shame and pride],” in Motivación y Emoción. La Adaptación Humana[Motivation and Emotion. Human adaptation], eds E. G. Fernández-Abascal, M. P. Jiménez, and M. D. Martín (Madrid: Centro de Estudios Ramón Areces), 369–393.

Google Scholar

Faílde, J. M., García, M. A., Carrera, M. V., Rodríguez, Y., Lameiras, M., and Ruiz, L. (2018). A qualitative approach to the study of the characteristics of individuals convicted of road traffic offences. Int. J. Law Psychiatry 59, 10–19. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2018.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fedewa, B. A., Burns, L. R., and Gomez, A. A. (2005). Positive and negative perfectionism and the shame/guilt distinction: adaptive and maladaptive characteristics. Pers. Individ. Dif. 38, 1609–1619. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.09.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fee, R. L., and Tangney, J. P. (2000). Procrastination: a means of avoiding shame or guilt? J. Soc. Behav. Pers. 15:167.

Google Scholar

Fernandes, R., Job, R. S., and Hatfield, J. (2007). A challenge to the assumed generalizability of prediction and countermeasure for risky driving: different factors predict different risky driving behaviors. J. Safety Res. 38, 59–70. doi: 10.1016/j.jsr.2006.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández, E., and Bermúdez, J. (1999). Estructura factorial del optimismo y pesimismo [Factorial structure of optimism and pessimism]. Bol. Psicol. 63, 7–26.

Google Scholar

Fernández, E., and Bermúdez, J. (2000). Control situacional y focalización en la tarea en pesimistas defensivos y optimistas [Situational control and focusonthetask in defensivepessimists and optimists]. Estud. Psicol. 67, 23–40. doi: 10.1174/02109390060206444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández, E., and Bermúdez, J. (2001). Estrategias cognitivas, controlabilidad situacional y estado afectivo: el caso del pesimismo defensivo y el optimismo [Cognitivestrategies, situationalcontrollability, and affectivestate: the case of defensivepessimism and optimism]. Rev. Psicopatol. Psicol. Clín. 6, 71–89. doi: 10.5944/rppc.vol.6.num.2.2001.3906

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrante, D., Girotto, V., Stragà, M., and Walsh, C. (2013). Improving the past and the future: atemporal asymmetry in hypothetical thinking. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 142, 23–27. doi: 10.1037/a0027947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuller, R. (2011). “Driver control theory: from task difficulty homeostasis to risk allostasis,” in Handbook of Traffic Psychology, ed. B. E. Porter (San Diego, CA: Academic Press). doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-381984-0.10002-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett, N., and Sharot, T. (2017). Optimistic update bias holds firm: three tests of robustness following Shah et al. Conscious. Cogn. 50, 12–22. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giguère, B., Lalonde, R. N., and Taylor, D. M. (2014). Drinking too much and feeling bad about it? How group identification moderates experiences of guilt and shame following norm transgression. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 40, 617–632. doi: 10.1177/0146167214521836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Girotto, V., Ferrante, D., Pighin, S., and Gonzalez, M. (2007). Postdecisional counterfactual thinking by actors and readers. Psychol. Sci. 18, 510–515. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01931.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Girotto, V., Legrenzi, P., and Rizzo, A. (1991). Event controllability in counterfactual thinking. Acta Psychol. 78, 111–133. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(91)90007-M

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gregersen, N. P. (1996). Young drivers’ overestimation of their own skill—an experiment on the relation between training strategy and skill. Accid. Anal. Prev. 28, 243–250. doi: 10.1016/0001-4575(95)00066-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammond, T. B., and Horswill, M. S. (2002). The influence of desire for control on drivers’ risk-taking behaviour. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 4, 271–277. doi: 10.1016/S1369-8478(01)00028-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harder, D. H., and Zalma, A. (1990). Two promising shame and guilt scales: a construct validity comparison. J. Pers. Assess. 55, 729–745. doi: 10.1080/00223891.1990.9674108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harre, N., and Sibley, C. G. (2007). Explicit and implicit self-enhancement biases in drivers and their relationship to driving violations and crash-risk optimism. Accid. Anal. Prev. 39, 1155–1161. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2007.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harré, N., Foster, S., and O’Neill, M. (2005). Self-enhancement, crash-risk optimism and the impact of safety advertisements on young drivers. Br. J. Psychol. 96, 215–230. doi: 10.1348/000712605X36019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, P. (1996). Sufficient grounds for optimism? The relationship between perceived controllability and optimistic bias. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 15, 9–52. doi: 10.1521/jscp.1996.15.1.9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helweg-Larsen, M., and Shepperd, J. A. (2001). Do moderators of the optimistic bias affect personal or target risk estimates? A review of the literature. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 5, 74–95. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0501_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horswill, M. S., Taylor, K., Newnam, S., Wetton, M., and Hill, A. (2013). Even highly experienced drivers benefit from a brief hazard perception training intervention. Accid. Anal. Prev. 52, 100–110. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2012.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, C. T. F., and Helweg-Larsen, M. (2002). Perceived control and the optimistic bias: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Health 17, 437–446. doi: 10.1080/0887044022000004920

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mãirean, C., and Havârneanu, C. E. (2018). The relationship between drivers’ illusion of superiority, aggressive driving, and self-reported risky driving behaviors. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 55, 167–174. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2018.02.037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malle, B. F., and Tate, C. (2006). “Explaining the past, predicting the future,” in Judgments Over Time: The Interplay of Thoughts, Feelings, and Behaviors, eds L. J. Sanna and E. Chang (New York: Oxford University Press). doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195177664.003.0011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mandel, D. R. (2003). Counterfactuals, emotions, and context. Cogn. Emot. 17, 139–159. doi: 10.1080/02699930302275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mandel, D. R. (2005). “Counterfactual and causal explanation: from early theoretical views to new frontiers,” in Routledge Research International Series in Social Psychology. The Psychology of Counterfactual Thinking, eds D. R. Mandel, D. J. Hilton, and P. Catellani (Abingdon-on-Thames: Routledge), 11–27.

Google Scholar

Mandel, D. R., and Dhami, M. K. (2005). “What i did” versus “what i might have done”: effect of factual and counterfactual thinking on blame, guilt, and shame in prisoners. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 41, 637–645. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2004.08.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markman, K. D., and Miller, A. K. (2006). Depression, control, and counterfactual thinking: functional for whom? J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 25, 210–227. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2006.25.2.210

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markman, K. D., Elizaga, R. A., Ratcliff, J. J., and McMullen, M. N. (2007). The interplay between counterfactual reasoning and feedback dynamics in producing inferences about the self. Think. Reason. 13, 188–206. doi: 10.1080/13546780600927157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markman, K. D., Gavanski, I., Sherman, S. J., and McMullen, M. N. (1995). The impact of perceived control on the imagination of better and worse possible worlds. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 21, 588–595. doi: 10.1177/0146167295216005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markman, K. D., McMullen, M. N., and Elizaga, R. A. (2008). Counterfactual thinking, persistence, and performance: a test of the Reflection and evaluation model. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 44, 421–428. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2007.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martí-Belda, A., Pastor, J. C., Montoro, L., Bosó, P., and Roca, J. (2019). Persistent traffic offenders. Alcohol consumption and personality as predictors of driving disqualification. Eur. J. Psychol. Appl. Legal Context 11, 81–92. doi: 10.5093/ejpalc2019a3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrea, S. M. (2008). Self-handicapping, excuse making, and counterfactual thinking: consequences for self-esteem and future motivation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95, 274–292. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.95.2.274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKenna, F. P. (1993). It won’t happen to me: unrealistic optimism or illusion of control? Br. J. Psychol. 84, 39–50. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1993.tb02461.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McMullen, M. N., and Markman, K. D. (2000). Downward counterfactuals and motivation: the wake-up call and the pangloss effect. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 26, 575–584. doi: 10.1177/0146167200267005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McMullen, M. N., Markman, K. D., and Gavanski, I. (1995). “Living in neither the best nor worst of all possible worlds: antecedents and consequences of upward and downward counterfactual thinking,” in What Might Have Been: The Social Psychology of Counterfactual Thinking, eds N. J. Roese and J. M. Olson (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 133–167.

Google Scholar

Murrar, S., Isenberg, N., Niedenthal, P., and Brauser, M. (2019). Shame and guilt among ice hockey players in the penalty box. Motiv. Emot. 43, 940–947. doi: 10.1007/s11031-019-09786-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Näätänen, R., and Summala, H. (1974). A model for the role of motivational factors in drivers’ decision-making. Accid. Anal. Prev. 6, 243–261. doi: 10.1016/0001-4575(74)90003-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasco, S. A., and Marsh, K. L. (1999). Gaining control through counterfactual thinking. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 25, 556–568. doi: 10.1177/0146167299025005002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niedenthal, P. M., Tangney, J. P., and Gavanski, I. (1994). ‘If only i weren’t’ versus ‘if only i hadn’t’: distinguishing shame and guilt in counterfactual thinking. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 585–595. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.4.585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norem, J. K., and Cantor, N. (1986). Defensive pessimism: “harnessing” anxiety as motivation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52, 1208–1217. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norem, J. K., and Chang, E. C. (2002). The positive psychology of negative thinking. J. Clin. Psychol. 58, 993–1001. doi: 10.1002/jclp.10094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Padilla, J. L., Doncel, P., Gugliotta, A., and Castro, C. (2018). Which drivers are at risk? Factors that determine the profile of the reoffender driver. Accid. Anal. Prev. 119, 237–247. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2018.07.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perczek, R., Carver, C. S., Price, A. A., and Pozo-Kaderman, C. (2000). Coping, mood, and aspects of personality in Spanish translation and evidence of convergence with the English version. J. Pers. Assess. 74, 63–87. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA740105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrocelli, J. V., and Harris, A. K. (2011). Learning inhibition in the monty hall problem: the role of dysfunctional counterfactual prescriptions. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 37, 1297–1311. doi: 10.1177/0146167211410245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrocelli, J. V., Percy, E. J., Sherman, S. J., and Tormala, Z. L. (2011). Counterfactual potency. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100, 30–46. doi: 10.1037/a0021523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrocelli, J. V., Seta, C. E., Seta, J. J., and Prince, L. B. (2012). If only i could stop generating counterfactual thoughts: when counterfactual thinking interferes with academic performance. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, 1117–1123. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.03.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Popa, A. E. (2013). Validation of the Personal Feelings Questionnaire 2 in a Spanish Sample. TFM(No Publicado). Madrid: Universidad Complutense de Madrid.

Google Scholar

Roese, N. J., and Olson, J. M. (1995). Outcome controllability and counterfactual thinking. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 21, 620–628. doi: 10.1177/0146167295216008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roese, N. J., and Olson, J. M. (1997). Counterfactual thinking: the intersection of affect and function. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 29, 1–59. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60015-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roese, N. J., Smallman, R., and Epstude, K. (2017). Do episodic counterfactual thoughts focus oncontrollable action: The role of self-initiation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 73, 14–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roese, N., and Morrison, M. (2009). The psychology of counterfactual thinking. Hist. Soc. Res. 34, 16–26.

Google Scholar

Sandin, B., Chorot, P., Lostao, L., Joiner, T. E., Santed, M. A., and Valiente, R. M. (1999). Escalas PANAS de afecto positivo y negativo: validación factorial y convergencia transcultural. [The PANAS scales of positive and negative affect: factor analytic validation and cross-cultural convergence]. Psicothema 11, 37–51.

Google Scholar

Sanna, L. J. (1998). Defensive pessimism and optimism: the bitter-sweet influence of mood on performance and prefactual and counterfactual thinking. Cogn. Emot. 12, 635–665. doi: 10.1080/026999398379484

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanna, L. J., Meier, S., and Wegner, E. A. (2001). Counterfactuals and motivation: mood as input to affective enjoyment and preparation. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 235–256. doi: 10.1348/014466601164830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheier, M. F., and Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping and health: assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychol. 4, 219–247. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.4.3.219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., and Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery and self-esteem). A re-evaluation of the life orientation test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 1063–1078. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmader, T., and Lickel, B. (2006). The approach and avoidance function of guilt and shame emotions: comparing reactions to self-caused and other-caused wrongdoing. Motiv. Emot. 30, 42–55. doi: 10.1007/s11031-006-9006-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scholl, A., and Sassenberg, K. (2014). Where could we stand if I had…? How social power impacts counterfactual thinking after failure. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 53, 51–61. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seta, C. E., Seta, J. J., Petrocelli, J. V., and McCormick, M. (2015). Even better than the real thing: alternative outcome bias affects decision judgements and decision regret. Think. Reason. 21, 446–472. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2015.1034779

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharot, T. (2011). The optimism bias. Curr. Biol. 21, R941–R945. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, S. J., and McConnell, A. R. (1995). “Dysfunctional implications of counterfactual thinking: when alternatives to reality fail us,” in What Might Have Been: The Social Psychology of Counterfactual Thinking, eds N. J. Roese and J. M. Olson (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 199–232.

Google Scholar

Showers, C. (1992). The motivational and emotional consequences of considering positive or negative possibilities for an upcoming event. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 63, 474–484. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smallman, R., and Summerville, A. (2018). Counterfactual thought in reasoning and performance. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass. 12:e12376. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12376

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spellman, B. A., and Mandel, D. R. (1999). When possibility informs reality: counterfactual thinking as a cue to causality. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 8, 120–123. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephens, A. N., and Ohtsuka, K. (2014). Cognitive biases in aggressive drivers: does illusion of control drive us off the road? Pers. Individ. Dif. 68, 124–129. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.04.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suls, J., and Wheeler, L. (2000). Handbook of Social Comparison. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-4237-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sümer, N., Özkan, T., and Lajunen, T. (2006). Asymmetric relationship between driving and safety skills. Accid. Anal. Prev. 38, 703–711. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2005.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sweeny, K., and Vohs, K. D. (2012). On near misses and completed tasks: the nature of relief. Psychol. Sci. 23, 464–468. doi: 10.1177/0956797611434590

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tangney, J. P., and Dearing, R. L. (2003). Shame and Guilt. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., and Martinez, A. G. (2014). Two faces of shame: the role of shame and guilt in predicting recidivism. Psychol. Sci. 25, 799–805. doi: 10.1177/0956797613508790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., and Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 345–372. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tasso, A., Sarlo, M., and Lotto, L. (2017). Emotions associated with counterfactual comparisons drive decision-making in footbridge-type moral dilemmas. Motiv. Emot. 41, 410–418. doi: 10.1007/s11031-017-9607-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, S. C. (1981). Will it hurt less if I can control it? A complex answer to a simple question. Psychol. Bull. 90, 89–101. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.90.1.89

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tracy, J. L., and Robins, R. W. (2007). “The self in self-conscious emotions,” in The Self-Conscious Emotions, eds J. L. Tracy, R. W. Robins, and J. P. Tangney (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 3–16.

Google Scholar

Valero, S., Bosch, R., Corominas, M., Giannoni, A., Barrau, V., Ramos-Quiroga, J. A., et al. (2017). Psychopathology and traffic violations in subjects who have lost their driving license. Compr. Psychiatry 76, 45–55. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wade, N. G., Vogel, D. L., Liao, K. Y.-H., and Goldman, D. B. (2008). Measuring state-specific rumination: development of the rumination about an interpersonal offence scale. J. Couns. Psychol. 55, 419–426. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.55.3.419

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, M. P., Eiser, J. R., and Harris, P. R. (2004). Risk perception of mobile phone use while driving. Risk Anal. 24, 323–344. doi: 10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00434.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilde, G. J. S. (1982). The theory of risk homeostasis: implications for safety and health. Risk Anal. 2, 209–226. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.1982.tb01384.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeelenberg, M., van Dijk, W. W., van der Pligt, J., Manstead, A. S. R., van Empelen, P., and Reinderman, D. (1998). Emotional reactions to outcomes of decisions: the role of counterfactual thought in the experience of regret and disappointment. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 75, 117–141. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1998.2784

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: offender drivers, optimism, counterfactual thinking, negative emotions, road safety

Citation: del Valle CHC (2021) Guilt and Shame of What Might Have Been in Optimistic Offender Drivers. Front. Psychol. 12:668138. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.668138

Received: 15 February 2021; Accepted: 14 September 2021;
Published: 07 October 2021.

Edited by:

Xuemin Zhang, Beijing Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Cesare Massimo Cavalera, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Italy
Francesca Baralla, University of Molise, Italy
Pierluigi Cordellieri, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Copyright © 2021 del Valle. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Carlos Hugo Criado del Valle, ZGVsdmFsbGVAdXNhbC5lcw==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.