Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 28 May 2021
Sec. Developmental Psychology

Attachment and Autism Spectrum Disorder (Without Intellectual Disability) During Middle Childhood: In Search of the Missing Piece

  • Department of Psychology and Cognitive Sciences, University of Trento, Rovereto, Italy

Introduction

The study of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) represents one of the most challenging areas for attachment research in view of the complex interrelation between socio-communication impairments and attachment processes (McKenzie and Dallos, 2017; Vivanti and Nuske, 2017). The fascinating debate, which focuses on whether ASD hampers the development of secure attachment, has not received sufficient attention so far. Prior studies often focused on attachment behaviors in preschoolers with ASD, whereas the study of attachment representations in older children remains mainly unexplored. The most recent systematic review (Teague et al., 2017) highlighted crucial unresolved questions in the study of attachment and ASD with reference to the paucity of data about predictors, correlates and outcomes of attachment in children with ASD. For instance, there is mixed evidence on the association between parental sensitivity and child attachment security, which has been widely documented in typically developing children (Van IJzendoorn et al., 2007). Importantly, the need to adopt a longitudinal perspective to examine the association between attachment and emotional or behavioral difficulties has been also highlighted. To this aim, the study of attachment requires a developmental lens based on age-appropriate methods. In this regard, research has not yet addressed the importance of investigating attachment representations in middle childhood and adolescence. In the last decade, only a limited number of studies on attachment and ASD has been published on children without intellectual disabilities during middle childhood (Bauminger et al., 2010; Chandler and Dissanayake, 2014; Keenan et al., 2016; Sivaratnam et al., 2018). The assessment of attachment in each study is based on questionnaires such as the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment (IPPA, Armsden and Greenberg, 1987) and/or the Security Scale (Kerns et al., 1996). Age range and number of the participants vary across studies, including only children from 6 to 14 years with High-functioning ASD. ASD sample size across studies was fairly small, ranging from 21 children with ASD in the Chandler and Dissanayake’s study to 44 children of Bauminger et al. (2010), which, however, includes participants from two countries (24 from Israel and 20 from the United States).

These studies revealed no group differences on security of attachment to their caregivers between children with ASD and children typical development (TD), confirming previous findings on early childhood (Teague et al., 2017). Nonetheless, some relevant issues concerning the quality of attachment in school-aged children with ASD are still underinvestigated. Firstly, in contrast to the subsequent studies, Bauminger et al. (2010) found significant differences between groups (ASD vs. TD) using the IPPA, which is considered the most valid and reliable attachment self-reported measure during middle childhood (Jewell et al., 2019). This contradictory result may be due to the fact that a larger sample of size has been considered (n = 44) compared to the subsequent studies.

Thus, despite evidence on the lack of group differences are accumulating, this result needs to be further confirmed using larger samples. In addition, self-reported measures have been criticized for not sufficiently capturing the complexity of this phenomenon, with particular reference to the level of attachment implicit representations. Moreover, previous findings (Chandler and Dissanayake, 2014) only revealed a “negligible” parent-child agreement on the IPPA scores, suggesting that the use of defensive strategies as well as diminished self-awareness in these children could raise further doubts about the validity of attachment questionnaires in this specific population.

More importantly, prior research, except for case studies (Brewerton et al., 2017), exclusively used questionnaires, which are only based on child perception of the quality of parent-child relationship and thereby on conscious knowledge (evaluative-declarative). By contrast, implicit internal representations are mainly automated and unconscious, reflecting the transformation of a dyadic quality into an individual characteristic. The internalization of dyadic experiences into a representational system occurs during childhood, becoming more elaborated as language ability increases across development (Bowlby, 1973; Bretherton and Munholland, 2008). This transition led to the construction of a cognitive form of attachment (Kraemer, 1992) originally defined as internal working models (IWM; Bowlby, 1973). In this regard, interpersonal expectations, self-beliefs, perspective-taking and interpersonal schemas are strictly connected to information surrounding the relation between the self and the context (Thompson, 2008; Crittenden, 2016).

Following a dual process framework, observer-rated narratives assess non-conscious implicit processes, whereas self-reported questionnaires are based on child conscious explicit representations. The explicit conscious processes have been identified as externally-oriented high-order cognitions which follow the development of the implicit internally-oriented core processes related to one’s self-concept and their relationship with others (Howe and Courage, 1997). Prior research on adults highlighted that these distinct processes also correlated with different brain activation (Yaseen et al., 2016). Respectively, cortical activity underpins the explicit conscious representations of the attachment figure, whereas narratives methods based on implicit models of attachment are associated with subcortical structures (Northoff and Panksepp, 2008). Focusing on middle childhood, attachment narratives and self-reported measures were found to be complementary in predicting psychological symptoms in school-age children (Borelli et al., 2016). In fact, the variety of methods used for the assessment of attachment during middle childhood may reflect different conceptualization and can be used concurrently with the aim to evaluate different components of the common construct and neural network (Bosmans and Kerns, 2015; Yaseen et al., 2016). Although different methods (e.g., semi-projective, autobiographical interview, story stems, family drawing) are available for the assessment of implicit attachment representations in children and adolescents, to the best of our knowledge, there are no quantitative studies on middle childhood that filled this critical gap in ASD literature. Therefore, in this opinion article, we discuss the considerable prospects associated with the study of implicit attachment representations in children with ASD, highlighting the importance to cover this unaddressed topic. Given that attachment narrative-based assessment in this developmental stage often requires verbal abilities, we focus on children with ASD without intellectual disabilities.

Attachment Representations in ASD: What We Still Need to Know

Despite the lack of data, it has been suggested that individuals with a diagnosis of ASD may experience the construction of internal models of attachment as challenging (Rogers et al., 1993; Chandler and Dissanayake, 2014). Difficulties in interpersonal relatedness and intersubjectivity (Stern, 2004), which characterize ASD social-communication impairment could make it more difficult for both children and parents to give meaning and predict each other’s behaviors, therefore complicating the development of balanced implicit representations. These mechanisms may also undermine sensitive and responsive caregiving increasing the frequency of relational impasses, including non-consequential, non-attuned and unrewarding interactions (Guo et al., 2017). In fact, since the link between parental sensitivity and attachment security has not been confirmed in ASD literature, in this case the transmission of attachment representations may be disrupted by difficulties in mind-minded, insightful and reflective caregiving (Teague et al., 2017).

Following this hypothesis, although children with ASD (without intellectual disability) do not differ significantly from controls in attachment security assessed through observational procedure, it is plausible to expect a discontinuity from attachment behaviors to the level of representations. Moreover, additional factors may hinder the organization of adaptive and reflective mental processing with respect to attachment representations and interpersonal schemas. Specifically, child unusual brain structure and functional connectivity (Crittenden, 2017; Cole et al., 2019), atypical neuroendocrine processes and dysregulation (Naber et al., 2007; Sivaratnam et al., 2015) hypo or hyper-sensitivity to sensory stimulation (Marco et al., 2011) difficulties in multisensory integration (Stevenson et al., 2014) can pose multiple challenges for children with ASD. Moreover, an additional source of risk is the frequent exposure to potential traumatic events. Berg et al. (2016), documented a significant association between ASD and the occurrence of Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs), showing higher rates of income insufficiency, parental divorce/separation, exposure to neighborhood violence and household mental illness in individuals with ASD compared to controls. Research has also highlighted that the experience of bullying is 3–4 times more frequent for children with ASD compared to their typically developing peers (Hoover and Kaufman, 2018). Taken together, these factors may interfere with the development of a resolved attachment status of children with ASD, given that past dangerous events may activate unresolved traumatic memories affecting individuals’ strategic current functioning (Crittenden, 2016).

Interestingly, to date, only one study (Taylor et al., 2008) investigated attachment implicit representations in ASD, focusing on adults without intellectual disability. The Adult Attachment Interview (AAI, George Kaplan and Main) was used to assess pattern of attachment of 20 adults with High-functioning Autism and 20 matched controls. Results showed that only a 15% of the ASD sample has been classified as secure, which is a lower rate compared to those found in early childhood (Rutgers et al., 2004; Teague et al., 2017). AAI transcripts of ASD participants were less coherent highlighting lower levels of reflective functioning compared to matched controls. The rate of attachment security in this study is similar to those of other clinical samples as highlighted by previous metanalytic findings (van IJzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg, 1996). Additionally, these findings confirmed that individuals with ASD can develop secure attachment representations, albeit with a lesser extent compared to the control group. Beyond this pioneering study, these hypotheses have not been tested in ASD literature, thus further investigations need to be conducted to replicate this result in childhood and adolescence.

Furthermore, it is of particular interest to assess attachment outcomes through different methods that are not directly influenced by individual strategic control or introspective difficulties associated with ASD. The use of implicit measures may provide useful and reliable data on children’s representations of attachment figures as well as on current state of mind with respect to attachment in ASD. Notably, internal models of self and others shape information processing as well as emotion and behavior regulation, influencing the way in which children process and respond to social stimuli (Zimmermann and Iwanski, 2015). Biased expectations of others’ intentions related to at-risk information processing may increase emotional, social difficulties and the risk of psychiatric comorbidity, that constitute critical aspects for the development of children with ASD. Thus, reaching a more comprehensive knowledge about implicit attachment representations during middle childhood may help to better understand ASD attachment difficulties. This could clarify children’s needs of protection and comfort before the sensitive period of transition to adolescence, offering valuable information to personalized psychological interventions, educational support and to foster attachment relationships with parents and peers.

Moreover, the association between implicit representations, behavioral and emotional problems could shed light on the protective mechanisms related to attachment organization in children with ASD. In this regard, there is growing interest in identifying potential predictors of positive long-term developmental trajectories, adaptive functioning and health-related quality of life in individuals with ASD (Pugliese et al., 2016; Adams et al., 2019). Information on quality of attachment representations may provide a significant contribution to foster mental health, reducing the risk of co-occurring psychiatric disorders and unresolved traumatic experiences in individuals with ASD. Studies on school-age children with typical development, showed the significant association between insecure attachment representations and child externalizing-internalizing symptoms (Moss et al., 2009). Although there are no studies that support this association in children with ASD, a recent longitudinal study (Rozga et al., 2018) suggests that the sequelae of attachment security in ASD may be similar to those documented for TD children. Thus, enhancing an internal sense of security, resilience and adaptation in children with ASD should represent a priority of autism agenda, for which the understanding of attachment representations constitutes a necessary precondition.

In conclusion, our recommendation is to complement the limited body of research on attachment and ASD during middle childhood by including the assessment of implicit representations, their predictors, correlates and outcomes. Future research on children with ASD in middle childhood should consider a more global assessment of attachment combining and comparing different sources of information (e.g., parent-reported, self-reported, observer-rated). This challenge represents an intriguing opportunity to renew the scientific debate on attachment and ASD, which cannot be regarded as comprehensive, bearing in mind the lack of data and the multiple unanswered questions on implicit representations.

Author Contributions

MG contributed to the conceptualization of the article and drafted the manuscript. SdF substantially contributed to the conception of the manuscript and revised it critically for important intellectual content. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Adams, D., Clark, M., and Simpson, K. (2019). The relationship between child anxiety and the quality of life of children, and parents of children, on the autism spectrum. J. Autis. Devel. Disor. 50, 1756–1769. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-03932-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armsden, G. C., and Greenberg, M. T. (1987). The inventory of parent and peer attachment: Individual differences and their relationship to psychological well-being in adolescence. J. Youth Adolesc. 16, 427–454. doi: 10.1007/bf02202939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauminger, N., Solomon, M., and Rogers, S. J. (2010). Predicting friendship quality in autism spectrum disorders and typical development. J. Autis. Devel. Disor. 40, 751–761. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0928-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berg, K. L., Shiu, C. S., Acharya, K., Stolbach, B. C., and Msall, M. E. (2016). Disparities in adversity among children with autism spectrum disorder: a population−based study. Devel. Med. Child Neurol. 58, 1124–1131. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.13161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borelli, J. L., Somers, J., West, J. L., Coffey, J. K., De Los Reyes, A., and Shmueli-Goetz, Y. (2016). Associations between attachment narratives and self-report measures of attachment in middle childhood: Extending evidence for the validity of the Child Attachment Interview. J. Child Fam. Stud. 25, 1235–1246. doi: 10.1007/s10826-015-0310-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosmans, G., and Kerns, K. A. (2015). Attachment in middle childhood: Progress and prospects. N. Direct. Child Adolesc. Devel. 2015, 1–14. doi: 10.1002/cad.20100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Separation, anxiety and anger (Vol. 2). New York: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Bretherton, I., and Munholland, K. A. (2008). “Internal working models in attachment relationships: Elaborating a central construct in attachment theory,” in Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 102–127.

Google Scholar

Brewerton, N., Robson, K., and Crittenden, P. M. (2017). Formulating autism systemically: Part 2–A 12-year prospective case study. Clin. Child Psychol. Psych. 22, 390–401. doi: 10.1177/1359104517714381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chandler, F., and Dissanayake, C. (2014). An investigation of the security of caregiver attachment during middle childhood in children with high-functioning autistic disorder. Autism 18, 485–492. doi: 10.1177/1362361313486205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, E. J., Barraclough, N. E., and Andrews, T. J. (2019). Reduced connectivity between mentalizing and mirror systems in autism spectrum condition. Neuropsychologia 122, 88–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crittenden, P. M. (2016). Raising parents: Attachment, representation, and treatment, 2nd Edn. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Crittenden, P. M. (2017). Formulating autism systemically: Part 1–A review of the published literature and case assessments. Clin. Child Psychol. Psych. 22, 378–389. doi: 10.1177/1359104517713241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, Y., Garfin, D. R., Ly, A., and Goldberg, W. A. (2017). Emotion coregulation in mother-child dyads: A dynamic systems analysis of children with and without autism spectrum disorder. J. Abnor. Child Psychol. 45, 1369–1383. doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0234-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoover, D. W., and Kaufman, J. (2018). Adverse childhood experiences in children with autism spectrum disorder. Curr. Opin. Psych. 31:128. doi: 10.1097/yco.0000000000000390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howe, M. L., and Courage, M. L. (1997). The emergence and early development of autobiographical memory. Psychol. Rev. 104:499. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.104.3.499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jewell, T., Gardner, T., Susi, K., Watchorn, K., Coopey, E., Simic, M., et al. (2019). Attachment measures in middle childhood and adolescence: A systematic review of measurement properties. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 68, 71–82. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2018.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keenan, B. M., Newman, L. K., Gray, K. M., and Rinehart, N. J. (2016). Parents of children with ASD experience more psychological distress, parenting stress, and attachment-related anxiety. J. Autism Devel. Disor. 46, 2979–2991. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2836-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerns, K. A., Klepac, L., and Cole, A. (1996). Peer relationships and preadolescents’ perceptions of security in the child-mother relationship. Dev. Psychol. 32, 457–466. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.32.3.457

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraemer, G. W. (1992). A psychobiological theory of attachment. Behav. Brain Sci. 15, 493–511. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00069752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marco, E. J., Hinkley, L. B., Hill, S. S., and Nagarajan, S. S. (2011). Sensory processing in autism: a review of neurophysiologic findings. Pediatr. Res. 69, 48–54.

Google Scholar

McKenzie, R., and Dallos, R. (2017). Autism and attachment difficulties: Overlap of symptoms, implications and innovative solutions. Clin. Child Psychol. Psych. 22, 632–648. doi: 10.1177/1359104517707323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moss, E., Bureau, J. F., Béliveau, M. J., Zdebik, M., and Lépine, S. (2009). Links between children’s attachment behavior at early school-age, their attachment-related representations, and behavior problems in middle childhood. Int. J. Behav. Devel. 33, 155–166. doi: 10.1177/0165025408098012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naber, F. B., Swinkels, S. H., Buitelaar, J. K., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Dietz, C., et al. (2007). Attachment in toddlers with autism and other developmental disorders. J. Autism Devel. Disor. 37, 1123–1138. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0255-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Northoff, G., and Panksepp, J. (2008). The trans-species concept of self and the subcortical–cortical midline system. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 259–264. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pugliese, C. E., Anthony, L. G., Strang, J. F., Dudley, K., Wallace, G. L., Naiman, D. Q., et al. (2016). Longitudinal examination of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorders: Influence of executive function. J. Autism Devel. Disor. 46, 467–477. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2584-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, S. J., Ozonoff, S., and Maslin-Cole, C. (1993). Developmental aspects of attachment behavior in young children with pervasive developmental disorders. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 32, 1274–1282. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199311000-00023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rozga, A., Hesse, E., Main, M., Duschinsky, R., Beckwith, L., and Sigman, M. (2018). A short-term longitudinal study of correlates and sequelae of attachment security in autism. Attach. Hum. Devel. 20, 160–180. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2017.1383489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutgers, A. H., Bakermans−Kranenburg, M. J., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., and van Berckelaer−Onnes, I. A. (2004). Autism and attachment: a meta−analytic review. J. Child Psychol. Psych. 45, 1123–1134.

Google Scholar

Sivaratnam, C., Newman, L., and Rinehart, N. (2018). Emotion-recognition and theory of mind in high-functioning children with ASD: Relationships with attachment security and executive functioning. Res. Autism Spect. Disor. 53, 31–40. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2018.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sivaratnam, C. S., Newman, L. K., Tonge, B. J., and Rinehart, N. J. (2015). Attachment and emotion processing in children with autism spectrum disorders: Neurobiological, neuroendocrine, and neurocognitive considerations. Rev. J. Autism Devel. Disor. 2, 222–242. doi: 10.1007/s40489-015-0048-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, D. N. (2004). The Present Moment in Psychotherapy and Everyday Life (Norton Series on Interpersonal Neurobiology). WW Norton & Company.

Google Scholar

Stevenson, R. A., Siemann, J. K., Schneider, B. C., Eberly, H. E., Woynaroski, T. G., Camarata, S. M., et al. (2014). Multisensory temporal integration in autism spectrum disorders. J. Neurosci. 34, 691–697.

Google Scholar

Taylor, E. L., Target, M., and Charman, T. (2008). Attachment in adults with high-functioning autism. Attach. Hum. Devel. 10, 143–163. doi: 10.1080/14616730802113687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teague, S. J., Gray, K. M., Tonge, B. J., and Newman, L. K. (2017). Attachment in children with autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review. Res. Autism Spectr. Disor. 35, 35–50. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2016.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, R. A. (2008). Attachment-related mental representations: Introduction to the special issue. Attach. Hum. Devel. 10, 347–358. doi: 10.1080/14616730802461334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van IJzendoorn, M., and Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. (1996). Attachment representations in mothers, fathers, adolescents and clinical groups: a meta-analytic search for 32 normative data. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 64, 8–21. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.64.1.8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Rutgers, A. H., Bakermans−Kranenburg, M. J., Swinkels, S. H., Van Daalen, E., Dietz, C., et al. (2007). Parental sensitivity and attachment in children with autism spectrum disorder: Comparison with children with mental retardation, with language delays, and with typical development. Child Devel. 78, 597–608. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01016.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vivanti, G., and Nuske, H. J. (2017). Autism, attachment, and social learning: Three challenges and a way forward. Behav. Brain Res. 325, 251–259. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.10.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yaseen, Z. S., Zhang, X., Muran, J. C., Winston, A., and Galynker, I. I. (2016). Comparison of brain activity correlating with self-report versus narrative attachment measures during conscious appraisal of an attachment figure. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:90.

Google Scholar

Zimmermann, P., and Iwanski, A. (2015). Attachment in middle childhood: Associations with information processing. N. Direct. Child Adolesc. Devel. 2015, 47–61. doi: 10.1002/cad.20099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Autism Spectrum Disorder, attachment representations, internal working models, school-age, middle childhood

Citation: Giannotti M and de Falco S (2021) Attachment and Autism Spectrum Disorder (Without Intellectual Disability) During Middle Childhood: In Search of the Missing Piece. Front. Psychol. 12:662024. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.662024

Received: 31 January 2021; Accepted: 03 May 2021;
Published: 28 May 2021.

Edited by:

Ann Dowker, University of Oxford, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Ramona Cardillo, University of Padua, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Giannotti and de Falco. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michele Giannotti, bWljaGVsZS5naWFubm90dGlAdW5pdG4uaXQ=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.