- 1Center of Linguistics, School of Arts and Humanities, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal
- 2Center for Psychology at University of Porto, Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
- 3TRIE-Transdisciplinary Research Center for Innovation & Entrepreneur Ecosystems, Manuel Teixeira Gomes Higher Education Institute (ISMAT), Portimão, Portugal
There is evidence for the positive impact of mindfulness in children. However, little is known about the techniques through which mindfulness practice results in differential outcomes. Therefore, this study intended to systematically review the available evidence about the efficacy of meditation techniques used by mindfulness-based programs on cognitive, socio-emotional, and academic skills of children from 6 to 12 years of age. The review was registered on the PROSPERO database, and the literature search was conducted according to PICO criteria and PRISMA guidelines. The EBSCO databases were searched, and 29 studies were eligible: nine randomized controlled trials and 20 quasi-experimental studies. All the included randomized controlled trials were rated as having a high risk of bias. Overall, the evidence for mindfulness techniques improving cognitive and socio-emotional skills was reasonably strong. Specifically, for cognitive skills, results showed that all the interventions used “body-centered meditations” and “mindful observations.” Regarding socio-emotional skills, although all the studies applied “body-centered meditations” and “mindful observations,” “affect-centered meditations” were also frequent. For academic skills, just one quasi-experimental trial found improvements, thus making it difficult to draw conclusions. Further research is crucial to evaluate the unique effects of different meditation techniques on the cognitive, social-emotional, and academic skills of children.
Systematic Review Registration: Identifier: RD42019126767.
Introduction
“Mindfulness” is a term frequently used to describe a mental faculty related to attention, awareness, retention/memory, and/or discernment (Davidson and Kaszniak, 2015). A popular definition of mindfulness entails a deliberate conscious awareness of the present moment without judgment (Kabat-Zinn, 2003). Another common meaning of mindfulness is related to a specific form of meditative practice (Goleman, 1988; Goleman and Davidson, 2018). This type of meditation fosters the ability to bring a non-judging awareness to a specific thing and strengthens our ability to notice our mind wandering (Goleman and Davidson, 2018). Importantly, mindfulness is a psychological process that can be developed through practice (Kabat-Zinn, 2003; Bishop et al., 2004), and several studies have shown that this specific training leads to improvements in psychological wellbeing and mental health (e.g., Bowen et al., 2006; Chiesa, 2009; Chiesa and Serretti, 2011; for a review see Keng et al., 2011). These positive effects have also been highlighted among children (for a review and meta-analyses see Klingbeil et al., 2017 and Maynard et al., 2017). Still, little is known about the techniques through which mindfulness practice results in those benefits, particularly for children. Thus, in this study, we performed a systematic review of the current literature about the efficacy of meditation techniques used by mindfulness-based programs on the cognitive, social-emotional, and academic skills of children.
Mindfulness-based interventions employ several training techniques (Kabat-Zinn, 2003), which involve a huge number of diverse practices. To do justice to this diversity, several attempts were made to classify these practices, and different taxonomies included a diversity of techniques from various backgrounds and contexts (Nash and Newberg, 2013; Schmidt, 2014; Lutz et al., 2015). For instance, Singer et al. (2016) developed a classification of practices based on Buddhist traditions, contemplative sciences, and neuroscientific research. The authors distinguished three broad classes of mental skills: (1) present-moment attention and body awareness, including breathing meditation and body scan as core exercises; (2) socio-affective abilities such as gratitude, compassion, prosocial motivation, and accepting difficult emotions through loving-kindness meditation and dyadic exercises as core practices; and (3) socio-cognitive capacities such as metacognition and perspective taking that incorporates core practices like observing thoughts, meditation, and dyadic perspective-taking exercises. Furthermore, Hildebrandt et al. (2017) explored the differential effects of these classes of mental abilities and found that present-moment attention practices increased attentional facets, but only socio-affective and socio-cognitive training led to broad changes in ethical-motivational skills such as nonjudgmental attitude, compassion, and self-compassion.
In 2019, Matko and Sedlmeier developed a new classification system for meditation techniques to make it accessible and understandable to practitioners/researchers with different backgrounds. Through a survey with 100 experienced meditators, the authors found seven main clusters of techniques: (1) body-centered meditation (i.e., concentrating on energy center or channeling, body scan, breath abdomen, observing the body, and breath nose); (2) mindful observation (i.e., observation of thoughts or emotions, long meditation, and sitting in silence); (3) contemplation (i.e., contemplating on a question, contradiction, or paradox); (4) mantra meditation (meditation with sound, singing sutras or mantras, and repeating syllables); (5) visual concentration (i.e., visualizations and concentrating on an object); (6) affect-centered meditation (i.e., cultivating compassion and opening up to blessings); and (7) meditation with movement (i.e., meditation with movement, manipulating the breath, walking, and observing senses).
Several reviews have examined the efficacy of mindfulness practice on adults, and there is evidence for the positive impacts of mindfulness training (Baer, 2003; Grossman et al., 2004; Mackenzie et al., 2005; Smith et al., 2005; Ott et al., 2006; Matchim and Armer, 2007; Toneatto and Nguyen, 2007; Winbush et al., 2007; Praissman, 2008; Teixeira, 2008; Carmody and Baer, 2009; Ledesma and Kumano, 2009). Some of these reviews applied meta-analytic methods to quantify the efficacy of this intervention (Baer, 2003; Grossman et al., 2004; Ledesma and Kumano, 2009), and robust evidence for the positive impact of mindfulness practice was found. Research with children is not yet as extensive as with adults, but it is growing rapidly. For example, two recent meta-analyses indicated the increased interest in the utility of mindfulness training in young people. Specifically, Klingbeil et al. (2017) reported data from participants between 4 and 18 years of age and analyzed two broad categories of outcome measures: (i) skills of mindfulness, attention, and meta-cognition/cognitive flexibility and (ii) academic performance and emotional/behavioral regulation. As a result, the authors found significant improvements across outcomes in all categories. Maynard et al. (2017) analyzed data from studies implementing mindfulness training in schools to participants aged between 4 and 20 years and also found that this intervention had a small-to-medium effect on cognitive and socio-emotional skills.
Research has repeatedly shown that mindfulness training improves the performance of children on tasks that assess cognitive functioning such as attention or executive functions (e.g., Semple et al., 2010; Leonard et al., 2013; Britton et al., 2014; Schonert-Reichl et al., 2015; Felver et al., 2017; Lawler et al., 2019). For example, 12 sessions of mindfulness training improved the performance of preschoolers on an attention task, while no changes were observed in the passive control group (Quan et al., 2019). For executive functions, after 8 weeks of mindfulness training, parents reported improvements in children in inhibition, shift, emotional control, initiative, working memory, planning, organization of materials, and monitoring skills (7–9 years old) (Flook et al., 2010).
The impact of mindfulness training has also been studied on emotional mental health (Bohlmeijer et al., 2010; Fjorback et al., 2011; Gotink et al., 2015; Guendelman et al., 2017). For example, after 12 weeks of mindfulness, yoga movements, and breathing training, fourth- and fifth-grade students (i.e., 9- and 10-year-old children) reduced involuntary responses to stress (such as rumination and intrusive thoughts) when compared with a waitlist control group (Mendelson et al., 2010). Napoli et al. (2005) examined the effects of a 24-week mindfulness intervention on attention and anxiety levels in first- and third-grade students, and results showed that mindfulness training reduced attentional problems and anxiety in children. Furthermore, mindfulness interventions have been found to improve social-emotional skills. For instance, teachers reported significant increases in optimism and improvements on classroom social behaviors of students (9–13 years of age) who participated in a mindfulness education program (Schonert-Reichl and Lawlor, 2010).
As mindfulness has shown positive effects on many aspects of wellbeing, studying the impact of mindfulness in schools has been worth it (Huppert and Johnson, 2010). For instance, studies have found a positive relationship between mindfulness and academic performance (McCloskey, 2015; Lin and Mai, 2018), probably because it reduces stress and anxiety (McCloskey, 2015), increases attention and memory (Lin and Mai, 2018), and/or enhances specific skills such as openness, attention, or inquiry (Docksai, 2013). As an example, performance on a reading comprehension test was significantly improved after participation in an intensive 2-week mindfulness training (Mrazek et al., 2013).
Even though research has accumulated evidence suggesting that mindfulness training improves cognitive, socio-emotional, and academic skills, studies are needed to clarify which intervention techniques produce change (Shapiro and Carlson, 2009). Yet, a systematic review focusing on the meditation techniques used by mindfulness-based programs that result in those outcomes has not been conducted. Thus, our primary aim was to identify different techniques to improve cognitive, social-emotional, and academic outcomes. Specifically, we explored a multiplicity of meditation-based techniques. As research with children is growing rapidly, we focused our attention on school-age children. We also selected other study inclusion criteria to maintain the focus on interventions delivered with high quality, i.e., the studies should include an active or an inactive/passive control condition to provide a comparable condition and quantitative measures should be reported as outcomes. We expected to analyze the frequency and impact of different techniques in order to understand which practices are frequently used for promoting different types of skills. Our conclusions are likely to reveal how future research on the effectiveness of mindfulness interventions may be improved.
Method
Search Strategy
A systematic literature search was carried out using MEDLINE and EBSCO (PsycINFO, CINAHL, ERIC) databases from the year 2009 to March 2019. An update was conducted from the year 2019 to March 2021. The review was conducted according to PRISMA guidelines (Moher et al., 2009) and was registered on the PROSPERO database for systematic reviews (registration 2019: CRD42019126767). The following keywords were used to conduct the search: Child* OR Children* OR “Primary School” OR “Elementary School” OR “Primary Education” OR “Elementary Education” AND (Mindfulness* OR Mindful*) AND (Intervention* OR Training* OR Program* OR Exercise* OR Techniques*) NOT (“Clinical Population” OR “Clinical Patients” OR Patients* OR Clinical* OR Inpatient* OR Outpatient* OR Disorder* OR Disabilities*). Filters for source types (academic Journals), age (6–12 years), and language (English) were applied. An additional search through other sources was conducted.
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
Using the Population, Interventions, Comparison, Outcomes (PICO) framework, inclusion and exclusion criteria were based on the following research question: In typical school-age children, what meditation techniques used by mindfulness-based programs, compared with other types of intervention and/or placebo conditions, are more effective in developing cognitive, social-emotional, and academic outcomes? (Table 1).
Table 1. Population/participant, intervention/indicator, comparator/control, outcome (PICO) framework.
In order to be included, studies had to: (i) include typically developing children aged 6–12 years; (ii) clearly describe the mindfulness techniques employed; (iii) include an active or an inactive/passive control condition to provide a comparable condition to test the effects of techniques; (iv) include measures of cognitive, socio-emotional, or academic skills as outcomes; (v) provide quantitative measures; and (vi) be published in English. Reviews, meta-analyses, editorials, opinion papers, and dissertations were excluded.
Risk of Bias (Quality) Assessment
To assess the risk of bias in randomized trials, we used the Cochrane Collaboration's tool, namely, the RoB 2.0 (Sterne et al., 2019). This tool assesses five domains of bias: (1) bias due to randomization, (2) bias due to deviations from intended intervention, (3) bias due to missing data, (4) bias due to outcome measurement, and (5) bias due to selection of the reported result. The risk of bias was assessed by two authors (AV and SM) independently from each other (selecting “low risk,” “high risk,” or “no information” of bias). The unclear risk was selected when details were not reported or unknown. Discrepancies were resolved through discussion. Given that we anticipated that most of the studies would be at high risk of bias, we did not restrict analyses based on this parameter.
Data Extraction
We developed the data extraction from all eligible articles based on the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic review and Meta-Analysis Protocols (PRISMA-P; Moher et al., 2009) flow diagram, following four stages: (1) identification, (2) screening, (3) eligibility, and (4) inclusion. Once the references had been obtained, we used the Rayyan software (Ouzzani et al., 2016) to compile the articles. Studies were initially identified by title and abstract, according to the inclusion criteria established. This full search was evaluated for inclusion by two authors (SM and AF) independently from each other, and discrepancies were resolved by discussion. A study was included when both reviewers independently assess it as satisfying the inclusion criteria. A third author (MF) mediated in the event of disagreement following discussion. Extracted information included: studies characteristics (viz. general study characteristics, effects of interventions, and mindfulness techniques). The types of techniques used within each intervention were coded according to the seven main clusters suggested by Matko and Sedlmeier (2019).
Results
Trial Flow
A total of 250 articles were identified from the databases using the search strategy previously described. Five articles were added through other sources. Thirty-two duplicates were removed, and 223 articles were screened by title and abstract. Of these, 191 reports were excluded since they did not meet the inclusion criteria. There was almost perfect agreement between the two judges (Cohen's κ = 0.97). Thus, 32 papers were included, and their full-text analyzed, of which 29 met inclusion criteria. This trial flow is presented in a PRISMA flow diagram in Figure 1.
Figure 1. Preferred Reporting Items in Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) flow diagram of selection of studies. *Other sources for identification of further articles included Google Scholar and “snowballing” techniques such as searching non-indexed journals and references of all full-text articles.
General Study Characteristics
Settings
Twelve of the 29 studies included in this review were conducted in the United States. The other studies were conducted in Canada (n = 2), Israel (n = 2), United Kingdom (n = 2), Spain (n = 2), Australia (n = 1), Brazil (n = 1), Germany (n = 1), Korea (n = 1), Italy (n = 1), Netherlands (n = 1), New Zealand (n = 1), Philippines (n = 1), and Portugal (n = 1) (Table 1). Four studies were published between 2010 and 2012, seven studies were published between 2014 and 2015, while 15 studies were published between 2016 and 2018, and three studies were published between 2019 and 2021 (Table 2).
Participants
Age varied across studies. Four studies included 6-year-olds, nine studies recruited 7-year-olds, 13 studies recruited 8-year-olds, 19 studies recruited children with 9 years old, 16 studies included children with 10 years old, and 15 studies included children with 11 years old (Table 2). Sample sizes for intervention groups ranged from 15 to 223 and for control groups from 8 to 231.
Research Design
Nine randomized controlled trials (RCT) and 20 quasi-experimental design (QED; studies using a comparison group design, but participants were not randomly assigned to conditions) were included in the review (Table 2). Regarding control groups, only one study compared the performance of the mindful group with an active and a passive control group (i.e., concentration training and business as usual, respectively). Nineteen studies included only passive control groups (i.e., waitlist and business as usual) and nine included only active control groups (i.e., handcrafts condition, coding training, history course, emotional literacy program, silent reading period, social responsibility program, physical education, and exposure to mindful awareness; Table 2).
Risk of Bias
Several authors fail to report design characteristics (i.e., allocation concealment, blinding, and incomplete outcome data) to conduct an accurate assessment of the risk of bias. There was almost perfect agreement between the two judges that conduct the assessment (Cohen's κ = 0.89). Overall, there was a high-risk of bias across the 9 RCTs included in the review, with variation in high-risk areas across studies see Figure 2 for a table reporting each domain of risk for each study and Figure 3 for a summary of risk across studies.
Intervention Characteristics
Duration, intensity, and dosage of mindful interventions varied across the 29 included studies. For studies reporting adequate information, a wide range of daily doses of formal mindfulness practice were reported, from 4 to 90 min per session. Interventions ranged from 2 weeks to 9 months. Studies also varied in terms of how frequently children met to receive the intervention, from one time per week to daily interventions. The most frequent option was sessions of 45 min once a week for 8 weeks (Table 3).
Table 3. Intervention characteristics, meditation techniques, and main findings of the included studies.
As can be seen in Table 3, the included studies examined several mindfulness interventions that, in most cases, were linked to previously existing mindfulness programs, such as mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) for depression or mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR). Importantly, only nine programs were assessed with RCTs, namely, the MBCT adapted to the Kamalayan curriculum, the MBSR—adapted program, the Calmer Choice, the Meditation condition formulated according to Roth's Integrative Contemplative Pedagogy, the Kripalu Yoga in the Schools, the Innerkids Program, the Master Mind, the MindUP program, and the Paws.b.
Effects of Interventions
Overall, as shown in Table 4, among the included studies, 16 assessed cognitive skills, 21 evaluated socio-emotional abilities, and 3 explored academic-related skills. Within these studies, 100% found significant effects for cognitive skills, 90% showed a significant impact on socio-emotional abilities, and 33% suggested a significant improvement in academic skills.
Table 4. Mindfulness meditation techniques and significant cognitive, social-emotional, and academic outcomes of the included studies.
The RCTs showed effects for measures assessing cognitive (viz. executive functions, attention, and self-control) and social-emotional (viz. stress physiology, empathy, perspective taking, emotional control, optimism, school self-concept, symptoms of depression, anxiety, peer aggression, prosocial behavior, and peer acceptance) skills (Tables 3, 4).
The QEDs also found positive effects for cognitive skills (i.e., overall executive functions, attention, concentration, inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, and immediate auditory-verbal memory), social-emotional abilities (viz. stress, wellbeing, mindfulness, self-esteem, resilience, psychological happiness, empathy, perspective-taking, emotional control, optimism, symptoms of depression, internalizing problems, peer aggression, prosocial behavior, increased peer acceptance, reduced anxiety, self-control, self-regulation, improvements in mental health problems, quality of life, self-compassion, acceptance, relaxation, happiness, aggressive behaviors, and social competence), and academic skills (viz. school self-concept, science and reading grades, and classroom behavior; Table 3).
Exploring the Effects of Meditation Techniques on Cognitive, Socio-Emotional, and Academic Skills
Overall, all interventions incorporated “body-centered meditations” and “mindful observations.” Almost half of the interventions included “affect-centered meditations” (55%), “mantra meditations” (55%), “meditations with movement” (52%), and “visual concentration” (48%) (Table 3). Patterns of relationships between meditation techniques and cognitive, socio-emotional, and academic outcomes were identified and are presented below (detailed description presented in Table 4).
Cognitive Performance
As detailed in Table 4, results showed that interventions improving cognitive outcomes frequently used “body-centered meditations” and “mindful observations,” while less frequent techniques to improve cognitive abilities were “affect-centered meditations,” “meditations with movement,” “visual concentrations,” and “mantra meditation.” The six RCTs that found improvements in cognitive skills used “body-centered meditations” (n = 6), “mindful observations” (n = 6), “affect-centered meditations” (n = 4), “meditations with movement” (n = 3), “mantra meditations” (n = 3), and “visual concentration” (n = 2). The 10 QEDS that showed benefits in cognitive outcomes employed “body-centered meditations” (n = 10), “mindful observations” (n = 10), “meditations with movement” (n = 7), “mantra meditations” (n = 5), “visual concentration” (n = 5), and “affect-centered meditations” (n = 4) (Table 4).
Socio-emotional Abilities
Findings suggested that, although all the interventions improving socio-emotional outcomes included “body-centered meditations” and “mindful observations,” “affect-centered meditations” were also frequently applied. Less frequent techniques used to improve socio-emotional outcomes were “meditations with movement,” “visual concentrations,” and “mantra meditation.” Three RCTs found improvements in socio-emotional abilities and applied “body-centered meditations” (n = 3), “mindful observations” (n = 3), “affected-centered meditations” (n = 3), “meditations with movement” (n = 2), “mantra meditation” (n = 1), and “visual concentration” (n = 1). Sixteen QEDs also showed benefits in socio-emotional skills and used “body-centered meditations” (n = 16), “mindful observations” (n = 16), “mantra meditations” (n = 10), “visual concentration” (n = 10), “affect-centered meditations” (n = 10), and “meditations with movements” (n = 10) (Table 4).
Academic Skills
Just one QED found improvements in academic skills. This study used “body-centered meditations,” “mindful observations,” “mantra meditations,” “visual concentration,” and “affect-centered meditations” (Table 4).
Discussion
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first review of the efficacy of specific meditation techniques used by mindfulness-based programs on the cognitive, social-emotional, and academic skills of children. A total of 29 studies (nine RCTs and 20 QED studies) met the selection criteria. Overall, results provided support for the use of mindfulness interventions to improve cognitive and social-emotional outcomes but found no support for the use of these interventions to enhance academic skills. Regarding meditation mindfulness techniques, all the interventions used “body-centered meditations” and “mindful observations.” In addition, “affect-centered meditations” were also frequently applied to improve socio-emotional outcomes. Less frequent techniques were “meditations with movements,” “mantra meditation,” and “visual concentration.” Thus, the effective techniques used in mindfulness-based programs differ in terms of activation and amount of body orientation, dimensions that highlight the role of embodied cognition in meditation. The most frequent techniques for improving cognitive and socio-emotional outcomes were active practices with a lower amount of body orientation. For socio-emotional outcomes, effective practices also included a higher abstract and conceptual focus and a neutral amount of body orientation. However, given the high risk of bias across the included studies in several domains, caution is needed in interpreting the results.
The findings of the present review support the favorable impacts of mindfulness interventions on cognitive outcomes, as expected (Flook et al., 2010). The next largest area of impact was related to socio-emotional skills. Indeed, our findings show that many of the outcomes were linked to emotional regulation processes involved in mindfulness training that also correspond to what was expected and highlighted in previous research (Bohlmeijer et al., 2010; Fjorback et al., 2011; Gotink et al., 2015; Guendelman et al., 2017).
Among the included studies, the specific techniques used to improve cognitive and socio-emotional outcomes were very similar, that is, all the interventions used “body-centered meditations” and “mindful observations.” There are several ways in which this pattern of results can be explained. In fact, “body-centered meditations” and “mindful observations” may be more effective than other techniques in improving cognitive and socio-emotional outcomes given that they provide more explicit instructions, possibly making it easier for children to use upon times of strong emotions. For instance, research showed that the redirection of attention to the body can improve attention, regulate stress, and enable a deeper understanding of our emotional-motivational state (Bornemann et al., 2015; Fissler et al., 2016). The body scan (i.e., focusing attention sequentially on various parts of the body) is another meditation frequently included in these clusters of techniques. This practice was associated with the components of observing and non-reacting, promoting wellbeing, and decreasing anxiety (Carmody and Baer, 2009).
In addition, “affect-centered meditations” appear to be a good strategy for improving socio-emotional skills that includes positive feelings and kindness, cultivates self-care, or provides meaningful experiences of connection with others. Previous research has shown that after this kind of practice, adult participants revealed a significant change in brain regions previously linked with empathy, compassion, and emotion regulation (Klimecki et al., 2012). Also, it seems that practices focused on affect and perspective taking produced significant decreases in the release of cortisol (i.e., a stress hormone), suggesting that this component may be associated with a significant reduction in physiological stress (Engert et al., 2017).
Regarding academic skills, although previous research suggested positive effects (McCloskey, 2015; Lin and Mai, 2018), just three QED studies included in this review aimed at investigating this domain, which makes it difficult to draw conclusions related to mindfulness techniques. However, the effects found for cognitive and socio-emotional outcomes might be related to the measures that are typically used to assess these particular skills (i.e., self-reports versus the administrative measures used to evaluate academic achievements; Maynard et al., 2017).
Our findings highlight the need for examining the unique contribution of intervention components in mindfulness-based interventions, as suggested by previous research. For instance, Carmody and Baer (2008) reported that practicing mindfulness movement (yoga), but not sitting meditation and body scan, was associated with higher levels of nonjudgment of inner experience. Thus, indeed, different mindfulness practices may target different aspects of psychological health.
Previous research suggests that one main factor associated with the variable results across studies is the amount of mindfulness practice introduced (Zenner et al., 2014). Our findings also highlighted the idea that the optimal meditation duration, intensity, and dosage for children are still unknown. In the included studies, children meditated, approximately, from 4 to 90 min per session, from 2 weeks to 9 months. Thus, the dosage and frequency of mindfulness meditation varied significantly between studies, and the total time meditating may be related to cognitive or emotional changes. Some studies appear to have reduced the amount of meditation time when compared with mindfulness interventions for adults, which commonly involve as much as 45 min of practice per day (Teasdale et al., 2000; Segal et al., 2002). Also, some mindfulness interventions with adolescents found effects with 20 or more minutes of practice per day (Saltzman and Goldin, 2008; Biegel et al., 2009). Still, other studies have reported significant effects of mindfulness intervention in children and adolescents with just 5 min of daily meditation (Saltzman and Goldin, 2008; Zylowska et al., 2008; Britton et al., 2010). Thus, further research should examine the impact of increased mindfulness meditation time, and this issue deserves more systematic investigation (Greenberg and Harris, 2012).
Despite these results showing that specific mindfulness training techniques can have different benefits for children, limitations of our findings should be reported: (i) although we reported study characteristics that are indicators of study quality, the risk of bias assessment was conducted only for randomized studies; (ii) the heterogeneity of the studies is considerable, and due to the vast array of practices, this review only examined clusters of techniques; and (iii) the frequent lack of blinded raters, randomization, active comparison groups, and small samples of the included studies mitigate the impact of our findings. Future studies must address these issues to support empirical evidence about the effect of mindfulness techniques on the development of children. A well-designed intervention should have the following key features: (a) randomization of participants into the experimental and control groups; (b) control for participants and expectations of an informant through blinding and assessment of expectations before the beginning of the intervention to control for possible placebo effects; and (c) comparison of the performance of the experimental group to both active and passive control conditions (e.g., Kendall, 2003).
Furthermore, a content analysis across the included studies would be useful to conduct an in-depth review of specific mindfulness techniques. Through this particular approach, methodological rigor is increased as qualitative data are categorized deductively or inductively (Forman and Damschroder, 2007).
Another area to further develop is not only to examine which mindfulness techniques are effective but also to understand which components are necessary (e.g., does movement enhance mindfulness practice?) and what works for whom. Indeed, studies have found that baseline characteristics predict intervention outcomes (Cordon et al., 2009) and that mindfulness-based interventions may be ineffective (e.g., Jazaieri et al., 2012) or contraindicated for specific conditions (Ma and Teasdale, 2004; Arch and Ayers, 2013). Regarding children and adolescents, the effects of mindfulness-based training programs have been associated with preexisting characteristics, such as levels of executive function, age, and family environment (e.g., Barnes et al., 2010; Flook et al., 2010; Schonert-Reichl and Lawlor, 2010). Therefore, researchers need to be aware of the possibility of both positive and adverse effects that certain practices could have on children with different characteristics (Greenberg and Harris, 2012).
Finally, since mindfulness-based programs consist of a variety of techniques, there may be elements other than the mindfulness component that are effective. So, an important distinction to be further explored is the unique effect of specific mindfulness exercises apart from other meditative practices.
Overall, this study represents a preliminary attempt to isolate the effects of different meditation techniques on the positive outcomes associated with cognitive, socio-emotional, and academic skills. Although in the past many studies on mindfulness training can be criticized for their lack of scientific rigor (Toneatto and Nguyen, 2007; Chiesa and Serretti, 2011), more recent studies provide strong evidence for the utility of such interventions, and it is hoped that these data encourage further studies on the unique effects of different mindfulness techniques.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.
Author Contributions
MF, SM, PA, SC, and TL contributed to the conception and design of the manuscript. MF, SM, AV, AC, and LR were responsible for the acquisition of data. MF, SM, and PA contributed to the interpretation of data. MF wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This research was supported by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (FCT; Grants 2020.01866.CEECIND, UID/PSI/00050/2013) and was conducted within the M2S Project funded through the Operational Programme for Competitiveness and Internationalization, supported by FEDER and national funds allocated to FCT (NORTE-01-0145-FEDER-028404).
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Alampay, L. P., Galvez Tan, L. J. T., Tuliao, A. P., Baranek, P., Ofreneo, M. A., Lopez, G. D., et al. (2019). A pilot randomized controlled trial of a mindfulness program for Filipino children. Mindfulness 11, 303–316. doi: 10.1007/s12671-019-01124-8
Arch, J. J., and Ayers, C. R. (2013). Which treatment worked better for whom? moderators of group cognitive behavioral therapy versus adapted mindfulness based stress reduction for anxiety disorders. Behav. Res. Ther. 51, 434–442. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2013.04.004
Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: a conceptual and empirical review. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 10, 125–143. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpg015
Bakosh, L. S., Snow, R. M., Tobias, J. M., Houlihan, J. L., and Barbosa-Leiker, C. (2016). Maximizing mindful learning: mindful awareness intervention improves elementary school students' quarterly grades. Mindfulness 7, 59–67. doi: 10.1007/s12671-015-0387-6
Bakosh, L. S., Tobias Mortlock, J. M., Querstret, D., and Morison, L. (2018). Audio-guided mindfulness training in schools and its effect on academic attainment: contributing to theory and practice. Learn. Instr. 58, 34–41. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.04.012
Barnes, V. A., Gregoski, M. J., Tingen, M. S., and Treiber, F. A. (2010). Influences of family environment and meditation efficacy on hemodynamic function among African American adolescents. J. Complement. Integr. Med. 7:1326. doi: 10.2202/1553-3840.1326
Bauer, C. C. C., Rozenkrantz, L., Caballero, C., Nieto-Castanon, A., Scherer, E., West, M. R., et al. (2020). Mindfulness training preserves sustained attention and resting state anticorrelation between default-mode network and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a randomized controlled trial. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 5356–5369. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25197
Bergen-Cico, D., Razza, R., and Timmins, A. (2015). Fostering self-regulation through curriculum infusion of mindful yoga: a pilot study of efficacy and feasibility. J. Child Fam. Stud. 24, 3448–3461. doi: 10.1007/s10826-015-0146-2
Biegel, G. M., Brown, K. W., Shapiro, S. L., and Schubert, C. M. (2009). Mindfulness-based stress reduction for the treatment of adolescent psychiatric outpatients: A randomized clinical trial. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 77, 855–866. doi: 10.1037/a0016241
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., et al. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 11, 230–241. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bph077
Bohlmeijer, E., Prenger, R., Taal, E., and Cuijpers, P. (2010). The effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction therapy on mental health of adults with a chronic medical disease: a meta-analysis. J. Psychosom. Res. 68, 539–544. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.10.005
Bornemann, B., Herbert, B. M., Mehling, W. E., and Singer, T. (2015). Differential changes in self-reported aspects of interoceptive awareness through 3 months of contemplative training. Front. Psychol. 5:1504. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01504
Bowen, S., Witkiewitz, K., Dillworth, T. M., Chawla, N., Simpson, T. L., Ostafin, B. D., et al. (2006). Mindfulness meditation and substance use in an incarcerated population. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 20, 343–347. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.20.3.343
Britton, W. B., Bootzin, R. R., Cousins, J. C., Hasler, B. P., Peck, T., and Shapiro, S. L. (2010). The contribution of mindfulness practice to a multicomponent behavioral sleep intervention following substance abuse treatment in adolescents: a treatment-development study. Subst. Abus. 31, 86–97. doi: 10.1080/08897071003641297
Britton, W. B., Lepp, N. E., Niles, H. F., Rocha, T., Fisher, N. E., and Gold, J. S. (2014). A randomized controlled pilot trial of classroom-based mindfulness meditation compared to an active control condition in sixth-grade children. J. Sch. Psychol. 52, 263–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2014.03.002
Butzer, B., LoRusso, A., Shin, S. H., and Khalsa, S. B. S. (2017). Evaluation of yoga for preventing adolescent substance use risk factors in a middle school setting: a preliminary group-randomized controlled trial. J. Youth Adolesc. 46, 603–632. doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0513-3
Carmody, J., and Baer, R. A. (2008). Relationships between mindfulness practice and levels of mindfulness, medical and psychological symptoms and well-being in a mindfulness-based stress reduction program. J. Behav. Med. 31, 23–33. doi: 10.1007/s10865-007-9130-7
Carmody, J., and Baer, R. A. (2009). How long does a mindfulness-based stress reduction program need to be? a review of class contact hours and effect sizes for psychological distress. J. Clin. Psychol. 65, 627–638. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20555
Chiesa, A. (2009). Zen meditation: an integration of current evidence. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 15, 585–592. doi: 10.1089/acm.2008.0416
Chiesa, A., and Serretti, A. (2011). Mindfulness based cognitive therapy for psychiatric disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 187, 441–453. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.08.011
Cordon, S. L., Brown, K. W., and Gibson, P. R. (2009). The role of mindfulness-based stress reduction on perceived stress: preliminary evidence for the moderating role of attachment style. J. Cogn. Psychother. 23, 258–269. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.23.3.258
Crescentini, C., Capurso, V., Furlan, S., and Fabbro, F. (2016). Mindfulness-oriented meditation for primary school children: Effects on attention and psychological well-being. Front. Psychol. 7:805. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00805
Davidson, R. J., and Kaszniak, A. W. (2015). Conceptual and methodological issues in research on mindfulness and meditation. Am. Psychol. 70, 581–592. doi: 10.1037/a0039512
de Carvalho, J. S., Pinto, A. M., and Marôco, J. (2017). Results of a mindfulness-based social-emotional learning program on portuguese elementary students and teachers: a quasi-experimental study. Mindfulness 8, 337–350. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0603-z
Devcich, D. A., Rix, G., Bernay, R., and Graham, E. (2017). Effectiveness of a mindfulness-based program on school children's self-reported well-being: a pilot study comparing effects with an emotional literacy program. J. Appl. Sch. Psychol. 33, 309–330. doi: 10.1080/15377903.2017.1316333
Engert, V., Kok, B. E., Papassotiriou, I., Chrousos, G. P., and Singer, T. (2017). Specific reduction in cortisol stress reactivity after social but not attention-based mental training. Sci. Adv. 3:e1700495. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1700495
Enoch, M. R., and Dixon, M. R. (2017). The use of a child-based acceptance and commitment therapy curriculum to increase attention. Child Fam. Behav. Ther. 39, 200–224. doi: 10.1080/07317107.2017.1338454
Felver, J. C., Tipsord, J. M., Morris, M. J., Racer, K. H., and Dishion, T. J. (2017). The effects of mindfulness-based intervention on children's attention regulation. J. Atten. Disord. 21, 872–881. doi: 10.1177/1087054714548032
Fissler, M., Winnebeck, E., Schroeter, T., Gummersbach, M., Huntenburg, J. M., Gaertner, M., et al. (2016). An investigation of the effects of brief mindfulness training on self-reported interoceptive awareness, the ability to decenter, and their role in the reduction of depressive symptoms. Mindfulness 7, 1170–1181. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0559-z
Fjorback, L. O., Arendt, M., Ørnbøl, E., Fink, P., and Walach, H. (2011). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy—a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 124, 102–119. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01704.x
Flook, L., Smalley, S. L., Kitil, M. J., Galla, B. M., Kaiser-Greenland, S., Locke, J., et al. (2010). Effects of mindful awareness practices on executive functions in elementary school children. J. Appl. Sch. Psychol. 26, 70–95. doi: 10.1080/15377900903379125
Forman, J., and Damschroder, L. (2007). “Qualitative content analysis,” in Empirical methods for bioethics: A prime, eds. L. Jacoby and L. A. Siminoff (Emerald Group Publishing Limited), 39–62.
Gotink, R. A., Chu, P., Busschbach, J. J. V., Benson, H., Fricchione, G. L., and Hunink, M. G. M. (2015). Standardised mindfulness-based interventions in healthcare: an overview of systematic reviews and meta-analyses of RCTs. PLoS ONE 10:e0124344. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124344
Gould, L. F., Dariotis, J. K., Mendelson, T., and Greenberg, M. T. (2012). A school-based mindfulness intervention for urban youth: exploring moderators of intervention effects. J. Community Psychol. 40, 968–982. doi: 10.1002/jcop.21505
Greenberg, M. T., and Harris, A. R. (2012). Nurturing mindfulness in children and youth: current state of research. Child Dev. Perspect. 6, 161–166. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00215.x
Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., and Walach, H. (2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits: a meta-analysis. J. Psychosom. Res. 57, 35–43. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00573-7
Guendelman, S., Medeiros, S., and Rampes, H. (2017). Mindfulness and emotion regulation: Insights from neurobiological, psychological, and clinical studies. Front. Psychol. 8:220. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00220
Hildebrandt, L. K., McCall, C., and Singer, T. (2017). Differential effects of attention-, compassion-, and socio-cognitively based mental practices on self-reports of mindfulness and compassion. Mindfulness. 8, 1488–1512. doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0716-z
Huppert, F. A., and Johnson, D. M. (2010). A controlled trial of mindfulness training in schools: The importance of practice for an impact on well-being. J. Posit. Psychol. 5, 264–274. doi: 10.1080/17439761003794148
Janz, P., Dawe, S., and Wyllie, M. (2019). Mindfulness-based program embedded within the existing curriculum improves executive functioning and behavior in young children: a waitlist controlled trial. Front. Psychol. 10:2052. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02052
Jazaieri, H., Goldin, P. R., Werner, K., Ziv, M., and Gross, J. J. (2012). A randomized trial of MBSR versus aerobic exercise for social anxiety disorder. J. Clin. Psychol. 68, 715–731. doi: 10.1002/jclp.21863
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 10, 144–156. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpg016
Kendall, J. M. (2003). Designing a research project: randomised controlled trials and their principles. Emerg. Med. J. 20, 164–168. doi: 10.1136/emj.20.2.164
Keng, S.-L., Smoski, M. J., and Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects of mindfulness on psychological health: a review of empirical studies. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31, 1041–1056. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.04.006
Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Lamm, C., and Singer, T. (2012). Functional neural plasticity and associated changes in positive affect after compassion training. Cereb. Cortex 23, 1552–1561. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs142
Klingbeil, D. A., Renshaw, T. L., Willenbrink, J. B., Copek, R. A., Chan, K. T., Haddock, A., et al. (2017). Mindfulness-based interventions with youth: a comprehensive meta-analysis of group-design studies. J. Sch. Psychol. 63, 77–103. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2017.03.006
Lawler, J. M., Esposito, E. A., Doyle, C. M., and Gunnar, M. R. (2019). A preliminary, randomized-controlled trial of mindfulness and game-based executive function trainings to promote self-regulation in internationally-adopted children. Dev. Psychopathol. 31, 1513–1525. doi: 10.1017/S0954579418001190
Ledesma, D., and Kumano, H. (2009). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and cancer: a meta-analysis. Psychooncology. 18, 571–579. doi: 10.1002/pon.1400
Leonard, N. R., Jha, A. P., Casarjian, B., Goolsarran, M., Garcia, C., Cleland, C. M., et al. (2013). Mindfulness training improves attentional task performance in incarcerated youth: A group randomized controlled intervention trial. Front. Psychol. 4:792. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00792
Lin, J. W., and Mai, L. J. (2018). Impact of mindfulness meditation intervention on academic performance. Innov. Educ. Teach. Int. 55, 366–375. doi: 10.1080/14703297.2016.1231617
Lutz, A., Jha, A. P., Dunne, J. D., and Saron, C. D. (2015). Investigating the phenomenological matrix of mindfulness-related practices from a neurocognitive perspective. Am. Psychol. 70, 632–658. doi: 10.1037/a0039585
Ma, S. H., and Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: replication and exploration of differential relapse prevention effects. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 72, 31–40. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.1.31
Mackenzie, M. J., Carlson, L. E., and Speca, M. (2005). Mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) in oncology: rationale and review. Evidence-Based Integr. Med. 2, 139–145. doi: 10.2165/01197065-200502030-00005
Matchim, Y., and Armer, J. M. (2007). Measuring the psychological impact of mindfulness meditation on health among patients with cancer: a literature review. Oncol. Nurs. Forum 34, 1059–1066. doi: 10.1188/07.ONF.1059-1066
Matko, K., and Sedlmeier, P. (2019). What is meditation? proposing an empirically derived classification system. Front. Psychol. 10:2276. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02276
Maynard, B. R., Solis, M. R., Miller, V. L., and Brendel, K. E. (2017). Mindfulness-based interventions for improving cognition, academic achievement, behavior, and socioemotional functioning of primary and secondary school students. Campbell Syst. Rev. 13, 1–144. doi: 10.1002/CL2.177
McCloskey, L. E. (2015). Mindfulness as an intervention for improving academic success among students with executive functioning disorders. Procedia—Soc. Behav. Sci. 174, 221–226. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.01.650
Mendelson, T., Greenberg, M. T., Dariotis, J. K., Gould, L. F., Rhoades, B. L., and Leaf, P. J. (2010). Feasibility and preliminary outcomes of a school-based mindfulness intervention for urban youth. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 38, 985–994. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9418-x
Moher D. Liberati A. Tetzlaff J. Altman D. G. The Prisma Group (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097
Mrazek, M. D., Franklin, M. S., Phillips, D. T., Baird, B., and Schooler, J. W. (2013). Mindfulness training improves working memory capacity and GRE performance while reducing mind wandering. Psychol. Sci. 24, 776–781. doi: 10.1177/0956797612459659
Napoli, M., Krech, P. R., and Holley, L. C. (2005). Mindfulness training for elementary school students: The attention academy. J. Appl. Sch. Psychol. 21, 99–125. doi: 10.1300/J370v21n01_05
Nash, J. D., and Newberg, A. (2013). Toward a unifying taxonomy and definition for meditation. Front. Psychol. 4:806. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00806
Ott, M. J., Norris, R. L., and Bauer-Wu, S. M. (2006). Mindfulness meditation for oncology patients: A discussion and critical review. Integr. Cancer Ther. 5, 98–108. doi: 10.1177/1534735406288083
Ouzzani, M., Hammady, H., Fedorowicz, Z., and Elmagarmid, A. (2016). Rayyan—a web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Syst. Rev. 5:210. doi: 10.1186/s13643-016-0384-4
Parker, A. E., Kupersmidt, J. B., Mathis, E. T., Scull, T. M., and Sims, C. (2014). The impact of mindfulness education on elementary school students: evaluation of the master mind program. Adv. Sch. Ment. Health Promot. 7, 184–204. doi: 10.1080/1754730X.2014.916497
Praissman, S. (2008). Mindfulness-based stress reduction: A literature review and clinician's guide. J. Am. Acad. Nurse Pract. 20, 212–216. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-7599.2008.00306.x
Quan, L., Yanan, S., Bin, L., and Tingyong, F. (2019). Mindfulness training can improve 3-and 4-year-old children's attention and executive function. Acta Psychol. Sin. 51:324. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00324
Ricarte, J. J., Ros, L., Latorre, J. M., and Beltrán, M. T. (2015). Mindfulness-based intervention in a rural primary school: effects on attention, voncentration and mood. Int. J. Cogn. Ther. 8, 258–270. doi: 10.1521/ijct_2015_8_03
Rodríguez-Ledo, C., Orejudo, S., Cardoso, M. J., Balaguer, Á., and Zarza-Alzugaray, J. (2018). Emotional intelligence and mindfulness: relation and enhancement in the classroom with adolescents. Front. Psychol. 9:2162. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02162
Saltzman, A., and Goldin, P. (2008). “Mindfulness based stress reduction for school-age children,” in Acceptance and mindfulness interventions for children and adolescents, eds. L. A. Greco and S. C. Hayes (Oakland, CA: New Harbinger and Context Press), 139–161.
Schmidt, S. (2014). “Opening up meditation for science: The ddevelopment of a meditation classification system,” in Meditation – Neuroscientific Approaches and Philosophical Implications., eds. S. Schmidt and H. Walach (Springer), 137–152.
Schonert-Reichl, K. A., and Lawlor, M. S. (2010). The effects of a mindfulness-based education program on pre- and early adolescents' well-being and social and emotional competence. Mindfulness 1, 137–151. doi: 10.1007/s12671-010-0011-8
Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Oberle, E., Lawlor, M. S., Abbott, D., Thomson, K., Oberlander, T. F., et al. (2015). Enhancing cognitive and social–emotional development through a simple-to-administer mindfulness-based school program for elementary school children: A randomized controlled trial. Dev. Psychol. 51, 52–66. doi: 10.1037/a0038454
Segal, Z., Williams, J., and Teasdale, J. (2002). Mindfulness-based Cognitive Therapy for Depression: A New Approach to Preventing Relapse. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Semple, R. J., Lee, J., Rosa, D., and Miller, L. F. (2010). A randomized trial of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for children: promoting mindful attention to enhance social-emotional resiliency in children. J. Child Fam. Stud. 19, 218–229. doi: 10.1007/s10826-009-9301-y
Shapiro, S. L., and Carlson, L. E. (2009). The art and science of mindfulness: Integrating mindfulness into psychology and the helping professions. Am. Psychol. Assoc. 11:885. doi: 10.1037/11885-000
Singer, T., Kok, B. E., Bornemann, B., Zurborg, S., Bolz, M., and Bochow, C. A. (2016). “The ReSource Project: Background, design, samples, and measurements,” in Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, 11–21.
Smith, J. E., Richardson, J., Hoffman, C., and Pilkington, K. (2005). Mindfulness-based stress reduction as supportive therapy in cancer care: systematic review. J. Adv. Nurs. 52, 315–327. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2005.03592.x
Sterne, J. A. C., Savovi,ć, J., Page, M. J., Elbers, R. G., Blencowe, N. S., Boutron, I., et al. (2019). RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ 366:14898. doi: 10.1136/bmj.l4898
Tarrasch, R. (2018). The effects of mindfulness practice on attentional functions among primary school children. J. Child Fam. Stud. 27, 2632–2642. doi: 10.1007/s10826-018-1073-9
Tarrasch, R., Margalit-Shalom, L., and Berger, R. (2017). Enhancing visual perception and motor accuracy among school children through a mindfulness and compassion program. Front. Psychol. 8:281. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00281
Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A., Soulsby, J. M., and Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 68, 615–623. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.4.615
Teixeira, M. E. (2008). Meditation as an intervention for chronic pain. Holist. Nurs. Pract. 22, 225–234. doi: 10.1097/01.HNP.0000326006.65310.a7
Thomas, G., and Atkinson, C. (2016). Measuring the effectiveness of a mindfulness-based intervention for children's attentional functioning. Educ. Child Psychol. 33, 51–64. Available online at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/293826420_Measuring_the_effectiveness_of_a_mindfulness-based_intervention_for_children's_attentional_functioning
Toneatto, T., and Nguyen, L. (2007). Does mindfulness meditation improve anxiety and mood symptoms? a review of the controlled research. Can. J. Psychiatry 52, 260–266. doi: 10.1177/070674370705200409
van de Weijer-Bergsma, E., Langenberg, G., Brandsma, R., Oort, F. J., and Bögels, S. M. (2014). The effectiveness of a school-based mindfulness training as a program to prevent stress in elementary school children. Mindfulness 5, 238–248. doi: 10.1007/s12671-012-0171-9
Viafora, D. P., Mathiesen, S. G., and Unsworth, S. J. (2015). Teaching mindfulness to middle school students and homeless youth in school classrooms. J. Child Fam. Stud. 24, 1179–1191. doi: 10.1007/s10826-014-9926-3
Vickery, C. E., and Dorjee, D. (2016). Mindfulness training in primary schools decreases negative affect and increases meta-cognition in children. Front. Psychol. 6:2025. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02025
Waldemar, J. O. C., Rigatti, R., Menezes, C. B., Guimarães, G., Falceto, O., and Heldt, E. (2016). Impact of a combined mindfulness and social–emotional learning program on fifth graders in a Brazilian public school setting. Psychol. Neurosci. 9, 79–90. doi: 10.1037/pne0000044
White, L. S. (2012). Reducing stress in school-age girls through mindful yoga. J. Pediatr. Heal. Care 26, 45–56. doi: 10.1016/j.pedhc.2011.01.002
Wimmer, L., Bellingrath, S., and von Stockhausen, L. (2016). Cognitive effects of mindfulness training: results of a pilot study based on a theory driven approach. Front. Psychol. 7:1037. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01037
Winbush, N. Y., Gross, C. R., and Kreitzer, M. J. (2007). The effects of mindfulness-cased stress reduction on sleep disturbance: a systematic review. EXPLORE 3, 585–591. doi: 10.1016/j.explore.2007.08.003
Yook, Y.-S., Kang, S.-J., and Park, I. (2017). Effects of physical activity intervention combining a new sport and mindfulness yoga on psychological characteristics in adolescents. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 15, 109–117. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2015.1069878
Zenner, C., Herrnleben-Kurz, S., and Walach, H. (2014). Mindfulness-based interventions in schools - A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 5:603. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00603
Keywords: meditation, mindfulness, mindfulness techniques, children, systematic review
Citation: Filipe MG, Magalhães S, Veloso AS, Costa AF, Ribeiro L, Araújo P, Castro SL and Limpo T (2021) Exploring the Effects of Meditation Techniques Used by Mindfulness-Based Programs on the Cognitive, Social-Emotional, and Academic Skills of Children: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 12:660650. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.660650
Received: 29 January 2021; Accepted: 30 September 2021;
Published: 12 November 2021.
Edited by:
Sisir Roy, National Institute of Advanced Studies, IndiaReviewed by:
Davinia M. Resurrección, Universidad Loyola Andalucía, SpainPadraic James Dunne, Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland, Ireland
Copyright © 2021 Filipe, Magalhães, Veloso, Costa, Ribeiro, Araújo, Castro and Limpo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Marisa G. Filipe, bWFyaXNhLmZpbGlwZSYjeDAwMDQwO2NhbXB1cy51bC5wdA==
†These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship