Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 01 April 2021
Sec. Developmental Psychology

MAGNITIVE: Effectiveness and Feasibility of a Cognitive Training Program Through Magic Tricks for Children With Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder. A Second Clinical Trial in Community Settings

  • Department of Psychology, Universidad Francisco de Vitoria, Madrid, Spain

Previous studies have explored the impact of magic tricks on different basic cognitive processes yet there is a need of examining effectiveness of a cognitive training program through magic tricks for children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The present study examines the effectiveness and feasibility of the MAGNITIVE program, a manualized intervention for cognitive training through the learning of magic tricks. A total of 11 children with ADHD (from 8 to 12 years) participated in separated groups of two different community settings (hospital center and school), and were assessed at pre-treatment, post-treatment, and a 3-month later follow-up in different tasks involving processing speed, sustained attention, selective attention, and mental flexibility. Using non parametric statistical analyses and Reliable Change Index, the results showed that these children receiving MAGNITIVE particularly improved their performance in sustained attention, shifting attention, and mental flexibility, changes were also observed in processing speed performance yet further research is needed in terms of selective attention and inhibition, given the great individual differences within this sample. Changes were maintained when the program was finished. In terms of viability, the study proved a good treatment integrity in different contexts (hospital and school setting), adherence to the curriculum (attendance and some practice at home), and high levels of engagement satisfaction. In this second clinical trial, MAGNITIVE program appears to be a feasible training program for children with ADHD, as an alternative for medication when possible.

Introduction

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder whose principal symptoms are hyperactivity, inattention, and impulsivity [American Psychiatric Association (APA), 2013]. Considered an impairment of the executive functions (Barkley, 1997), ADHD affects a series of cognitive processes related to self-regulation, task organization, planning, working memory, cognitive flexibility, time and space organization, emotional regulation (Pineda et al., 1998; Willcutt et al., 2005a,b; Colomer et al., 2017; Fabio et al., 2018), automatic and controlled processes (Capri et al., 2020), and other alterations of basic functions such as processing speed (Woods et al., 2002; Willcutt et al., 2005a). This is a persistent condition that can cause significant personal, familial, social, and educational difficulties (DuPaul et al., 2001; Rodríguez-Salinas et al., 2006; Deault, 2010; Pinho et al., 2017; Velõ et al., 2019).

Several studies have been conducted in recent years into the effectiveness of non-pharmacological treatments for ADHD (Pelham and Fabiano, 2008; Sonuga-Barke et al., 2013; Richardson et al., 2015; Watson et al., 2015; Scionti et al., 2020; Shrestha et al., 2020; Veloso et al., 2020). These non-pharmacological strategies include behavior modification techniques and cognitive behavioral therapy (Fabiano et al., 2009); cognitive training (Tamm et al., 2010; Shuai et al., 2017); training in self-instruction and techniques to enhance the capacity to inhibit responses (Meichenbaum and Goodman, 1971) including computer-based interventions (Martinovic et al., 2016; Rossignoli-Palomeque et al., 2018), training in problem solving (Bransford and Stein, 1993), neurofeedback (Zuberer et al., 2018; Cueli et al., 2019), training in social skills (Sheridan et al., 1996; Storebo et al., 2012), peer intervention (Cordier et al., 2018), and training in organizational skills (Langberg et al., 2008); and psycho-educational strategies and instruction of parents and teachers in cognitive behavioral techniques (Miranda et al., 2002; Pelham and Fabiano, 2008; Montoya et al., 2011; Rimestad et al., 2019); within the multi-modal approach, of particular importance are interventions focused on producing changes in neuro-psychological functions (Pistoia et al., 2004).

Studies have found positive results from training in basic cognitive processes, leading to general improvement in executive functions (Johnstone et al., 2012; Dias and Seabra, 2016; Lambez et al., 2020; Veloso et al., 2020). Furthermore, studies show the benefit of combining training in executive functions with other therapeutic strategies such as training in self-instruction (Meichenbaum and Goodman, 1971), modeling, and self-reinforcement (and other behavior modification techniques), for the improvement of sustained attention, selective attention, planning, social skills, academic performance, and the principal symptomology of ADHD (Miranda et al., 2002; Arco Tirado et al., 2004; Pérez, 2007; Ramalho et al., 2011; Sun, 2017). Studies have analyzed the effects of computerized cognitive training programs by itself or as supplementary interventions, finding positive outcomes in Intelligence Quotient (IQ) test (Fabio et al., 2019), working memory (Dovis et al., 2015; Johnstone et al., 2017; Passarotti et al. 2020), and symptoms of ADHD (Prins et al., 2013).

Evidence shows that learning by games and practice using play activities among children can facilitate the internalization of learned strategies and increase motivation (Pérez, 2007; Blasco-Fontecilla et al., 2015; Martinovic et al., 2016). Practicing physical exercise appears as one of the most effective non-pharmacological interventions in order to reduce some of ADHD cognitive symptoms (Lambez et al., 2020). One play activity that has sparked particular interest recently is magic (Kuhn et al., 2008; Bagienski and Kuhn, 2019). It has been demonstrated that learning magic tricks requires self-control, concentration, selective attention, the capacity for sequencing, planning, problem solving, an adequate level of working memory, and constant practice; skills which all involve executive functions (Rensink and Kuhn, 2014). A number of studies have examined the impact of magic tricks on perception (Rensink, 2010), control of visual attention (Kuhn et al., 2014; Rensink, 2015), critical thinking and creativity (Wiseman and Watt, 2020), reasoning and cognitive abilities (Wiseman and Watt, 2018), and the underlying mechanisms of memory and mnesic distortions of the neural bases of causality (Parris et al., 2009; Rensink and Kuhn, 2014). Equally, practicing magic has proved beneficial in the development of motor skills, imagination, problem-solving, and self-esteem (Spencer, 2012; Green et al., 2013; Harte and Spencer, 2014), interpersonal communication, and resilience, although studies finding these benefits did not use deep statistical analysis but were based on the experience of therapists or patients (Wiseman and Watt, 2018; Bagienski and Kuhn, 2019).

MAGNITIVE, described in detail elsewhere (Bonete et al., un-published), is a manualized program for cognitive training through the learning of magic tricks. It was developed specifically as a non-pharmacological alternative for children with ADHD.

There is currently no manualized program for cognitive training through magic whose effectiveness has been studied on children with ADHD. A study by Spencer (2012) on the benefits of incorporating an organized and systematic set of simple magic tricks into academic curricula found improvements in behavior, fine motor skills, self-esteem, and socialization as well as improved planning and sequencing skills. However, the study did not analyze changes before and after the intervention with psychological tests of cognitive functions.

The first MAGNITIVE pilot study was conducted with seven children between the ages of 8 and 12 diagnosed with ADHD and who were not receiving pharmacological treatment. It consisted of a 10-week session conducted by a therapist who was an expert magician. The results showed significant improvement in processing speed and selective attention as a group. Individually, three of the seven children improved their sustained attention and two of them improved their information processing speed (Bonete et al., 2016). After this implementation, improvements were made in the content and application of the program, based on the input and feedback of the therapist-magician and the participants.

The principal aim of the present study is to examine the effectiveness and viability (Kraemer and Kupfer, 2005) of the MAGNITIVE program, designed to develop the cognitive abilities of children with ADHD. This second clinical trial was conceived based on the model proposed by Smith et al. (2007) for addressing methodological challenges in research on psychosocial interventions. At a second phase, manualization of the intervention is fundamental to standardize it and make the manual available to other professionals (Smith et al., 2007). The study set out to examine the preliminary effectiveness of the program and confirm its viability and applicability in a second sample to explore the viability to be delivered as planned across settings, following the recommended steps for the evaluation of psychosocial intervention programs (Smith et al., 2007; Leon et al., 2011). The effectiveness of MAGNITIVE was determined based on changes Pre-Post participation in cognitive tasks that are maintained at the follow-up. The hypothesis proposed here is that cognitive training through magic can improve the performance of tasks involving the evaluation of cognitive processes and executive functions (processing speed, sustained attention, selective attention, mental flexibility, and planning ability). The viability was determined through an analysis of the fidelity of the therapist to the program (treatment integrity), adherence to the treatment, and the satisfaction of the participants (Pavuluri et al., 2004; White et al., 2012).

Materials and Methods

Participants

The final sample consisted of 11 children diagnosed with ADHD from the pediatric ward of the El Escorial Hospital (eight boys and three girls) organized in two community settings (hospital center and school). Participation was voluntary. The participants were selected based on the following inclusion criteria: (1) between 8 and 12 years of age; (2) diagnosed with ADHD by their pediatrician and who had high scores at the EDAH Questionnaire (assessment of ADHD test; Farré i Riba and Narbona García, 2013); (3) have a global IQ score on the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT; Kaufman and Kaufman, 1997); (4) not be taking medication at the moment of the study; and (5) not suffer from any other serious psychiatric pathologies. Table 1 shows the descriptive characteristics of the sample including scores. Two of the children had dyslexia (5 and 6), but it was decided to include them in both the program and the data analysis to achieve a more representative sample and explore the effect of the program on children who are often diagnosed with this comorbidity (Germanò et al., 2010). See Figure 1 of the flow chart.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean symptom severity characteristics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) group.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. CONSORT flow chart of participants.

Procedure

After receiving the approval of the ethics committee, researchers got in touch with parents of patients who met the inclusion criteria and were interested in participating in a non-pharmacological treatment program. The parents were invited to a group information session about the MAGNITIVE program and the objectives of the study. Those interested in the voluntary participation of their children, provided their informed consent and contact details. Appointments were scheduled for an individual pre-treatment psychological assessment of the child and at least one of the parents was also interviewed (Pre). The parents were provided with a questionnaire for their child’s teachers. Parents and teachers reported EDAH scores as a supplementary measure for diagnostic confirmation. The final sample consisted of 11 children organized into two training groups of five and six participants. The treatment was held at the hospital for the first group (n = 5) and at a local school (n = 6) attended by the rest of the sample. A post-treatment assessment was conducted in the same week the program concluded (Post), as well as a follow-up evaluation 3 months later (Post-2) using the same tests. All assessments were performed by two research psychologists of the team. A psychotherapist, who is also an experienced magician, delivered the program with a facilitator who assisted with materials and coding integrity of the program.

Intervention: MAGNITIVE

The manualized MAGNITIVE program (Bonete et al., un-published) was designed for school children (in primary education) diagnosed with ADHD and implemented in small groups (5–6 participants). The program consisted of 10 group-training sessions of 60–75 min, including a break. Each session focused on learning a magic trick or developing the prerequisite skills necessary to perform the trick. The program followed a standardized order, teaching magic tricks of increasing difficulty as the program advanced interspersed with handicraft sessions to create a magician’s costume. The program had dual objectives: to instruct the children to perform magic tricks and to train cognitive abilities through a play activity using various techniques, which scientific studies have associated with improved executive functions (MTA Cooperative Group, 1999; Espinosa, 2006; Ives, 2006; Orjales, 2007; Fabiano et al., 2009). The training was conceived to be conducted by a therapist who was also a professional magician.

Integrity of the Treatment/Therapist Fidelity to the Program

This refers to the degree to which the therapist delivered treatment as intended. A treatment integrity checklist was used to examine the following of the schedule and tasks by the therapist while delivering the program. During each session, the facilitator recorded how many of the session’s objectives were completed. Following Pavuluri et al. (2004), integrity was assessed as a percentage (percentage of delivered treatment components/number of planned components for that session*100), considering 80% set as a minimally acceptable level.

Adherence to the Program/Subject Involvement

A record was kept of the participants’ attendance and the number of hours they practiced at home with the help of a register. Before the beginning of the program, parents were provided with a register on which they were asked to note daily, the amount of time (in minutes) their children practiced magic tricks at home. The parents were not specifically asked to encourage their children to practice. The register was submitted at the end of the program. The study included a weekly calculation of the practice time.

Subject Satisfaction

In the final evaluation at the end of the program, the children and parents were asked about their satisfaction with the program, using a Likert scale from 1 (not satisfied) to 5 (completely satisfied), evaluating the effect of the program in various aspects: knowledge of magic, entertainment, and use of magic, following of instructions, organization, memory, shifting attention, following the norms and personal comments. The scores varied from 10 to 50. Two items concerning social validation of the program were included. The questionnaire was also submitted to the tutor or teacher of reference of the student.

Follow-Up Questionnaire Three Months Later

A follow-up session was held 3 months after the program with parents and children to evaluate their performance in psychological tests used to determine the effectiveness of the program.

Instruments

Five Digits Test

This test (FDT; Sedó, 2007) measures processing speed (reaction time) principally through the components of Reading (Reading-FDT) and Counting (Counting-FDT); inhibition through the component of Choosing (Choosing-FDT) and cognitive flexibility/alternating attention through the component Shifting (Shifting-FDT). The reliability coefficients for Spanish samples α = 0.94 (Reading-FDT), α = 0.92 (Counting-FDT), α = 0.86 (Choosing-FDT), and α = 0.90 (Shifting-FDT). The test is very effective in neuropsychological analysis and suggests high intercorrelations between the four components in the test, between 0.66 and 0.93. The results are presented as centile scores, with higher scores indicating better performance.

D2 Test of Attention

This test (D2; Brickenkamp and Seisdedos Cubero, 2012) measures sustained attention using a concentration performance index (Concentration-D2), selective attention using an omission errors index (Omission-D2), degree of impulsivity using a commission errors index (Commission-D2), and processing speed. This provides a score for Total test effectiveness [taken from the total n° responses (number of characters correctly processed) less the omission and commission errors); the total number of correct answers, as a measure of the work performed as well as the Attention variation index (Variation-D2), which evaluates the stability of work overtime. In most studies, the results of the D2 test have proven highly reliable (r > 0.90). The results are presented as centile scores, with higher scores indicating better performance (Jiménez et al., 2012).

Coding and Digit Span Subtests of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children

The Coding subtest (Coding-WISC; Wechsler, 2005) evaluated processing speed and sustained attention. Digit span (Digit Span-WISC; Wechsler, 2005) evaluates attention, short-term memory, and working memory. The results of these subtests are on a scaled score and have an average reliability of 0.84.

Battery of Neuropsychological Assessment for Executive Function in Children

This is a comprehensive evaluation (ENFEN; Portellano et al., 2011) of the global development of children between the ages of 6 and 12. It consists of six tasks which measure distinct or simultaneous cognitive processes. The Phonological Fluency task evaluates phonological fluency; the Semantic Fluency tasks evaluate semantic fluency. The Gray Path task evaluates processing speed. The Color Path task evaluates mental flexibility. The Rings task evaluates thought organization, anticipation, sequencing, and working memory; the final task, Interference evaluates working memory, prospective memory, and resistance to interference – inhibition. For the study, raw scores were presented, given that the manual only offers sten scores. Higher scores indicate better performance in all tests except the Rings task in which scores are reversed. Different studies show evidence of the suitability of the instrument (Navarro-Soria et al., 2020). Table 2 summarizes the characteristics of the measures used.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of labeling of the subtests, cognitive skills measured, type and range of score.

Statistical Analysis

The study consisted of an open trial of a manualized program for performing magic tricks in groups. Given that the objective was to assess the viability of the program in a communal (non-artificial) environment, the sample was selected by convenience (Lucas, 2013).

To evaluate behavioral changes after the treatment, an initial non-parametric analysis was conducted to determine if there were any significant differences between the three moments of evaluation as a whole period: Pre-Post-Post2, by means of the Friedman X2 test (n = 10). Non-parametric tests were used due to the size of the sample. Secondly, non-parametric post hoc tests were conducted to identify exactly when these differences occurred (Pre-Post n = 11, Post-Post 2 n = 10), using Wilcoxon Z-test. Effect size, using Kendall’s W, and statistical power were reported for each significant difference.

The clinical significance or magnitude of the individual change was also calculated using the Reliable Change Index (RCI; Jacobson and Truax, 1991), according to which a change in (direct) scores greater than 1.96 SD is considered statistically and clinically significant in each participant.

The feasibility of the program was determined by evidence that the treatment is acceptable for the target population and may be applied reliably based on the dropout rate, attendance rates, the hours of practice at home, the participant satisfaction questionnaires, and the fidelity of the therapist to the program.

Results

Changes After Treatment and Follow-Up Assessment

A comparison of the average results of the Pre, Post, and Post 2 (3 months after the end of the program) tests showed statistically significant changes at the three moments of evaluation (Pre-Post-Post2) with moderate effect sizes (all over 0.40) in: Coding-WISC, Color Path-Battery of Neuropsychological Assessment for Executive Function in Children (ENFEN), Rings-ENFEN, Total Responses-D2 and Correct Responses-D2, Omission-D2, Effectiveness-D2, Choosing-FDT, and Shifting-FDT. Post hoc tests were also conducted using the Wilcoxon Z-test for a two-by-two comparison of the three moments of evaluation: Pre-Post and Post-Post2; to see if changes appear after the program, and last 3 months after. Table 3 shows the significant differences, effect size, and power.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Descriptive statistics and non-parametric differences in repeated tasks Pre-Post-Post 2 (X2 Friedman), post hoc tests (Wilcoxon Z), and effect size.

In terms of processing speed, the analysis showed significant changes in the three moments as a whole (Pre-Post-Post 2) in Effectiveness-D2 and Total Responses-D2, and changes exclusively in Pre-Post in Counting-FDT and Gray Path-ENFEN. It did not show changes in Reading-FDT nor Coding-WISC.

Regarding attention, there were several sources providing information. As regards sustained attention, Concentration-D2 and Coding-WISC both show statistically significant improvements at the three moments, and in Pre-Post, changes remain in Post 2. In selective attention, Omissions-D2 showed significant differences at the three moments. As for shifting attention, the Shifting-FDT test showed statistically significant changes at the three moments, and specifically in Pre-Post; those changes remain in Post-2. Likewise, the change already mentioned at the three moments in Color Path-ENFEN was an evidence of this improvement. The attention variation indexshowed statistically significant changes in all three moments.

With respect to inhibition/impulsivity, statistically significant changes were observed in Choosing-FDT both at the three moments (Pre-Post-Post 2) and at the post hoctest (Pre-Post). This was not the case in the calculation of Commission-D2 nor in Interference-ENFEN.

Regarding mental flexibility(working memory tasks and short-term memory), there were statistically significant changes at the three moments in Color Path-ENFEN and Ring-ENFEN (p< 0.01), thus confirming, in post hoctests, a Pre-Post change that remains in Post-2 (where there were no significant changes in the absence of the program). On the other hand, statistically significant changes were not observed in Interference-ENFEN nor in the tasks associated with Phonological fluency-ENFEN and Semantic Fluency-ENFEN.

Clinically Significant Changes

Table 4shows the RCI scores for the various tasks. Participants 2, 3, 7, and 11 showed statistically significant clinical changes in the greatest number of variables. The tests or tasks which showed the greatest statistically significant change were Correct Responses-D2, Comission-D2; three of these children also showed improvements in Color Path-ENFEN (2 and 3), Omission-D2 (2, 7, and 11), and Rings-FDT (2, 3, and 7). Another three showed improvement in Semantic Fluency-ENFEN (2, 3, and 6) and Concentration-D2 (2, 7, and 10). Additionally, participants 2 and 7 improved in Phonological Fluency-ENFEN while participants 2 and 4 improved in Digit Span-WISC.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Reliable change index (RCI) post-pre program for each participant.

It is also interesting to highlight those tasks where individuals showed clinically significant changes with lower scores, specifically in Choice-FDT (2, 3, 5, and 6), and Interference-ENFEN (1, 8, and 9). However, two participants showed worse results in the Post for Shifting-FDT (1 and 6) and Comission-D2 (4 and 10). Surprisingly, participant 2, one of those showing most improvement in all tests, had the lower score in Total Responses-D2, Total Effectiveness-D2, and Choice-FDT.

Regarding the two participants with dyslexia, participant 6 showed worse results in Shifting-FDT and Choice-FDT, but improved Semantic Fluency-ENFEN, while participant 5 showed improvements in Rings-FDT, with statistically worse Post scores only in Choice-FDT.

In summary, changes in group-mean scores related to processing speed, were shown by clinically significant improvements in three of the 11 participants in Rings-FDT and one in Total Effectiveness-D2. As for attention, clinically significant improvements appeared in: (1) selective attention, as three participants showed significant improvements in Omissions-D2; (2) sustained attention, with improvement of three participants in Concentration-D2; and (3) shifting attention, not so clearly in this case, as three participants improved significantly at a clinical level for the Color Path-ENFEN and two obtained worse scores after the intervention in Shifting-FDT. However, attention stability of attentionseemed to improve in the performance of four of the 11 participants (Variation-D2). In terms of inhibition and impulsivity, individual patterns were miscellaneous: only one participant improved at a clinical level in Choosing-FDT, whereas four others seemed to do it significantly worse, among them, the two participants diagnosed with dyslexia. Four participants performed significantly lower Commission-D2, but three performed clinically worse in Interference-ENFEN. Regarding the rest of executive functions, three children showed changes in Semantic Fluency-ENFEN and two in Phonological Fluency-ENFEN, but the tests did not allow these differences to be captured in mental flexibility or working memory.

Feasibility: Integrity of the Treatment, Adherence to the Program, and Subject Satisfaction

The feasibility of the program was calculated based on the integrity of the application by the therapist-magician, the drop-out rate, and the adherence of the participants to the treatment (attendance rate), hours of practice registered by parents and the satisfaction questionnaire completed by the children, parents, and teachers. It was hoped that the changes would persist 3 months later.

Regarding the integrity of the treatment over the course of 10 sessions in two groups, overall integrity was 85.7%. For the hospital group, integrity was 91.1%, while at the school integrity was 81.3%. An analysis of the sessions showed than none fell below the 80% cut-off for acceptable treatment integrity. The sessions were conducted within the allotted time (60–75 min), although time was always tighter in the school group. The qualitative evaluation of the therapist indicated greater difficulty in maintaining the pace of sessions imparted at the school as an extracurricular activity.

Adherence of the participants to the program included participation in the weekly sessions and hours of practice at home, recorded by the parents. All the participants remained in the program to the end, attending all 10 sessions. The overall rate of attendance (a total of 110 sessions, two groups by 10 sessions) for the 11 participants was 96.36%, seven children attended all the sessions and four children missed only one session. Considering the overall baseline attendance criteria of 70%, this was considered an excellent result for this indicator (Pavuluri et al., 2004).

With reference to practice at home, four of the 11 children practiced between the sessions at home, observed and recorded by the parents. Of the children who attended sessions at the hospital, only one practiced at home between sessions from 10 to 25 min per week during the first six sessions, stopping practice thereafter. In the school group, three children practiced at home, from 3 to 90 min per week. The variation between the three was enormous. One participant only practiced at home until session 5, while another two managed to dedicate significant amounts of time throughout the course of the program.

Regarding the satisfaction questionnaire, of the 10 items dealing with changes in daily life, 69.1% of all responses by the children were 4 or 5 (somewhat or very much), while in the case of parents, similar responses were given 26.4% of the time. In the qualitative analysis, nine children considered the ability to do magic tricks after the program, as a significant change, which was confirmed by parents of all the children and having more friendsafter participating in the program, confirmed by the parents of three children. Eight of the children marked 4 (somewhat) for it is easier to follow instruction than before, and they feel able to stay seated more time(conformed by the parents of three children), and seven indicated that they learned to be better organized, have a better memory, and can better follow norms of behavior(coinciding with the response from parents in only two cases). Notably, nine of the children affirmed that they had greatly enjoyed the program, a highly positive result for the therapist-magician. One participant commented on the benefit of now having something to do when bored. Two of the children mentioned the importance of making new friends. Four of the parents commented that their children were highly motivated to attend the sessions and in two cases improvements were reported in academic performance. As for teachers, the three questionnaires which were returned provided little useful information. All the children and parents affirmed that they would recommend the program to others and that the program should receive financial support from social services in order to be available to more children.

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to evaluate in a second clinical trial in community settings, after conducting a previous pilot study, the effectiveness and feasibility of MAGNITIVE program for groups of children with ADHD; a program based in the implementation of self-instructions and other cognitive-behavioral techniques through the teaching of magic tricks. Under two different types of analysis of treatment effectiveness, findings reveal a significant improvement in these participants regarding cognitive abilities. On the one hand, based on changes in participants’ performance (group mean scores) of different cognitive tasks measuring processing speed, attention processes (shifting attention, sustained attention, selective attention, stability of variation in attention, and inhibition), mental flexibility and other executive functions such as phonological and semantic fluency. And secondly, as a complementary analysis, RCI changes for each participant were calculated between two moments. Taking the two methodologies into account, this training program particularly increased sustained attention, shifting attention, and mental flexibility of the participants. Processing speed performance significantly changed when the group mean scores were taken. Meanwhile, the impact of this training should be further studied in randomized clinical trials and larger samples, especially in order to clarify its effects in terms of selective attention and inhibition, given the great individual differences within this sample.

In general terms, statistical analysis showed that in all cases where there were significant changes at the three assessed moments (Pre-Post-Post2), the post hocconfirmed this improvement in Pre-Post, and sometimes also in Post-Post2. It was never the case that for a statistically significant change at the three moments (Pre-Post-Post2), the post hoctests revealed that the change was significant only Post-Post2 without previous significant changes at Pre-Post. Therefore, changes appeared always in the presence of the program, and in any case, could be maintained when the program is finished but, improvements do not occur alone, in the absence of the program. Furthermore, under no circumstances the acquired skills in the presence of the program worsened 3 months later (after the program was finished).

In connection with the improvements in processing speed and sustained attention, the results obtained were consistent with the results in the first pilot study of MAGNITIVE program (Bonete et al., 2016), and with previous studies based on cognitive-behavioral interventions that emphasize these cognitive skills (Minder et al., 2019), self-instructions (Arco Tirado et al., 2004; Ramalho et al., 2011), and studies examining the effects of different play activities (Blasco-Fontecilla et al., 2015; Schmitt et al., 2018).

No previous interventions were found, reporting improvement in processes such as organization, sequencing, and anticipation with a cognitive-behavioral focus (Shuai et al., 2017). Some studies based on computerized cognitive training programs are promising (Johnstone et al., 2012; Dovis et al., 2015; Minder et al., 2019; Passarotti et al., 2020) although there is also evidence of differences among interventions to find generalized improvement of ADHD symptomatology (Smith et al., 2020). The present study does show changes in tasks that involve mental flexibility (Color Path-ENFEN and Rings-ENFEN), needed for planning. It is interesting to highlight how they are processes that the empirical evidence shows as specially affected in people with ADHD (Pineda et al., 1998; Woods et al., 2002; Willcutt et al., 2005a) and they are also key processes in the mastery of magic (Rensink and Kuhn, 2014). The work by Spencer (2012), that used magic inside the academic curriculum, pointed at improvements in planning and sequencing. Our study constitutes a relevant empirical evidence as there are few previous studies incorporating magic on their cognitive training program for children with ADHD (Harte and Spencer, 2014; Wiseman and Watt, 2018; Bagienski and Kuhn, 2019).

In relation to inhibition/impulsivity, there was a statistically significant change in one of the tasks, and clinically significant improvements in some of the participants in different tests associated with inhibition, although other participants showed a significantly worse performance. This cognitive ability seems to be the least benefitted from the program, compared to the rest of abilities. It is worth considering whether the learning of magic tricks does not help in training inhibitory control (Diamond and Ling, 2016) or whether it is the tasks selected to assess the cognitive processes involved that fail to show the impact of this learning on impulsivity management (Arco Tirado et al., 2004; Pérez, 2007; Mattfeld et al., 2016).

The results in terms of feasibility are promising. Until now, few evidence-based interventions focused on improving cognitive abilities through play activities (Spencer, 2012; Blasco-Fontecilla et al., 2015), which makes these findings unique and highly relevant. Firstly, this study proves that it can be applied consistently (treatment integrity) and participants in general showed adherence to the curriculum (attendance and practice at home) and high levels of engagement satisfaction. The study also made it possible to analyze the program’s effectiveness, as positive changes could be found in different contexts (hospital or health center and school setting) which is highly valuable in testing treatment effectiveness (Smith et al., 2007; Rosa et al., 2017). Interestingly, the engagement rate in the program and the high attendance level show how the use of play activities for cognitive training becomes a source of greater motivation to create change. Besides, the content in itself makes easier its introduction in school curriculum or extracurricular education offered by schools (Dias and Seabra, 2016). This would facilitate the incorporation of many children with these needs into de program (Pérez, 2007).

In conclusion, following the step-wise model for validating and disseminating interventions (Smith et al., 2007; Leon et al., 2011), MAGNITIVE as manualized program, showed in a second clinical trial improvements in Pre-Post that were maintained 3 months after the end of magic initiation training. This means that the program was effective (Kraemer and Kupfer, 2005) for training without having to resort to medication. Even more, some gains in attention were not only maintained but keep improving in the absence of the program (see Table 3).

Previous studies with play activities such as chess games (Blasco-Fontecilla et al., 2015) and computer games (Pérez, 2007; Prins et al., 2013; Dovis et al., 2015; Martinovic et al., 2016; Johnstone et al., 2017) highlight the potential of these contents in keeping up motivation while training abilities and promoting generalization, which is so difficult to obtain (Espinosa, 2006; Cortese et al., 2015; Meyer et al., 2020; Passarotti et al., 2020). Mastering magic requires constant practice from the interested person, and as this field provides endless possibilities, it promotes creativity. Participants are expected to internalize these lessons and keep applying them in various contexts of their life, thus promoting generalization. This natural tendency in the children to show the tricks in every stage they have could become an advantage, comparing to other attempts to promote generalization (like proposing different virtual reality settings). Children enjoy performing their new tricks in front of friends, family, teachers, etc.; which is a powerful reinforcement. The next step would be to examine whether the program could create a greater impact if it had a broader reach (Bagienski and Kuhn, 2019; Spencer et al., 2019), or whether it could spark the child’s motivation to strengthen his/her skills for different tricks so that it could extend in time the benefits of the program with no need for another intervention.

Among the limitations of the study, the sample recruited was small. Larger samples with a control group of comparison may increase statistical power. The lack of a wait list group or comparison with another training package prevents the direct attribution of the changes to MAGNITIVE program against other unspecified aspects of the program. There is the fact that the same psychotherapist carried out the whole program with two groups (11 children in total), which also reduces the generalization of results. Another limitation refers to the chosen tests to examine changes in inhibition and impulsivity among other cognitive abilities. Practice at home was not requested consistently nor registered in a reliable manner. It would be interesting to analyze the program’s effects if the package includes regulated home tasks within the program or psychoeducational intervention to parents (Montoya et al., 2011). In addition, an impact analysis of the program on participants’ interpersonal relations (Ison and Morelato, 2002; Bonete and Molinero, 2016) and on self-esteem (Spencer, 2012; Green et al., 2013) is lacking. Mastery in magic offers the children a resource for improving social interactions and promotes acceptance and integration in activities with peers. ADHD diagnosis implies certain behaviors that hinder interaction with other children and is associated with receiving constant criticism due to their behavior (Storebo et al., 2019; Willis et al., 2019). Magic tricks are an opportunity for them to receive positive attention from peers and adults, providing them with a social reinforcement for their skill, thus transforming their behavior in a source of pleasant moments in the group (instead of perceiving themselves as a cause of constant uneasiness).

In conclusion, this study shows the effectiveness in a second clinical trial of MAGNITIVE, a cognitive training through magic, whose objective is improving deficient cognitive abilities in a group of children with ADHD as part of a play activity. The study is draw from a manualized program as a starting point, which could spread over time, encouraging expertise in performing magic tricks. It has shown an adequate implementation feasibility both in school and hospital environments. MAGNITIVE program is ready to be validated with larger samples in multisite randomized clinical trials.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

This study was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee at Universidad Francisco de Vitoria. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

SB and AO contributed to the conception and design of the work, assessed the children, and prepared the first draft of the manuscript with the assistance of IG-F. CM analyzed the data set, wrote the results section, and reviewed the paper draft. SB led the paper draft and revised the last version of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The authors received financial support for the research and publication of this article from Universidad Francisco de Vitoria.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the participants and their families, the staff at Antoniorrobles Public School (San Lorenzo de El Escorial, Madrid), David Moreno Sanz, the therapist-magician, Javier Blumenfeld from the pediatrics ward, and El Escorial Hospital for their participation in the research project and allowing us to use their facilities for the program. This project was approved by the Ethics Committee of Universidad Francisco de Vitoria.

References

American Psychiatric Association (APA)(2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5thEdn. Washington, DC: American psychiatric association.

Google Scholar

Arco Tirado, J. L., Fernández Martín, F., and Hinojo Lucena, F. J. (2004). Trastorno por déficit de atención con hiperactividad: intervención psicopedagógica. Psicothema 16, 408–414.

Google Scholar

Bagienski, S., and Kuhn, G. (2019). The crossroads of magic and wellbeing: a review of wellbeing-focused magic programs, empirical studies, and conceivable theories. Int. J. Wellbeing 9, 41–65. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v9i2.740

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (1997). ADHD and the nature of self-control. New York: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Blasco-Fontecilla, H., González-Pérez, M., García-López, R., Poza-Canoa, B., Pérez-Moreno, M., León-Martínez, V., et al. (2015). Eficacia del ajedrez en el tratamiento del trastorno por déficit de atención e hiperactividad: un estudio prospectivo abierto. Revista de Psiquiatria y Salud Mental 9, 13–21. doi: 10.1016/j.rpsm.2015.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonete, S., and Molinero, C. (2016). “The interpersonal problem-solving process: assessment and intervention” in Problem-solving: Stratgies, challenges and outcomes. ed. K. Newton(New York: Nova Science Publishers), 103–132.

Google Scholar

Bonete, S., Osuna, A., Molinero, C., Álvarez, M., and Blumenfeld, J. (2016). “Cognitive training through magic tricks for children with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder” in Comunicación Presentada en el VIII Congreso Internacional de Psicología y Educación ; Junio: Alicante, España, 15–17.

Google Scholar

Bransford, J. D., and Stein, B. S. (1993). The IDEAL problem solver. 2ndEdn. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.

Google Scholar

Brickenkamp, R., and Seisdedos Cubero, N. (2012). D2: Test de atención: Manual. 4AEdn. Madrid: TEA.

Google Scholar

Capri, T., Santoddi, E., and Fabio, R. A. (2020). Multi-source interference task paradigm to enhance automatic and controlled processes in ADHD. Res. Dev. Disabil. 97:103542. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2019.103542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colomer, C., Berenguer, C., Roselló, B., Baixuli, I., and Miranda, A. (2017). The impact of inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms, and executive functions on learning behaviours of children with ADHD. Front. Psychol. 8:540. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cordier, R., Vilaysack, B., Doma, K., Wilkes-Gillan, S., and Speyer, R. (2018). Peer inclusion in interventions for children with ADHD: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Biomed. Res. Int. 2018, 1–51. doi: 10.1155/2018/7693479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortese, S., Ferrin, M., Brandeis, D., Buitelaar, J., Daley, D., Dittmann, R. W., et al. (2015). Cognitive training for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis of clinical and neuropsychological outcomes from randomized controlled trials. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 54, 164–174. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cueli, M., Rodríguez, C., Cabaleiro, P., García, T., and González-Castro, P. (2019). Differential efficacy of neurofeedback in children with ADHD presentations. J. Clin. Med. 8:204. doi: 10.3390/jcm8020204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deault, L. C. (2010). A systematic review of parenting in relation to the development of comorbidities and functional impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 41, 168–192. doi: 10.1007/s10578-009-0159-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A., and Ling, D. S. (2016). Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 34–48. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dias, N. M., and Seabra, A. G. (2016). Intervention for executive functions development in early elementary school children: effects on learning and behaviour, and follow-up maintenance. Educ. Psychol. 37, 468–486. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2016.1214686

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dovis, S., Van der Oord, S., Wiers, R. W., and Prins, P. J. M. (2015). Improving executive functioning in children with ADHD: training multiple executive functions within the context of a computer game. A randomized double-blind placebo controlled trial. PLoS One 10:e0121651. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121651

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DuPaul, G. J., McGoey, K. E., Eckert, T. L., and VanBrakle, J. (2001). Preschool children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: impairments in behavioural, social, and school functioning. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 40, 508–515. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200105000-00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espinosa, A. L. (2006). El tratamiento del niño y adolescentes con TDAH en Atención Primaria desde el punto de vista de la evidencia. Revista Pediatria de Atención Primaria 8(Suppl. 4), 64–114.

Google Scholar

Fabiano, G. A., Pelham, W. E., Coles, E. K., Gnagy, E. M., Chornis-Tuscano, A., and O’Connor, B. C. (2009). A meta-analysis of behavioural treatments for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29, 129–140. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabio, R. A., Caprì, T., Iannizzotto, G., Nucita, A., and Mohammadhasani, N. (2019). Interactive avatar boosts the performances of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in dynamic measures of intelligence. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 22, 588–596. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2018.0711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabio, R. A., Tindara, C., Nasrin, M., Antonio, G., Gagliano, A., and Gabriella, M. (2018). Frequency bands in seeing and remembering: comparing ADHD and typically developing children. Neuropsychol. Trends 24, 97–116. doi: 10.7358/neur-2018-024-fabi

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farré i Riba, A., and Narbona García, J. (2013). EDAH: Escalas para la evaluación del trastorno por déficit de atención con hiperactividad. Madrid: TEA.

Google Scholar

Germanò, E., Gagliano, A., and Curatolo, P. (2010). Comorbidity of ADHD and dyslexia. Dev. Neuropsychol. 27, 475–493. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.494748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, D., Schertz, M., Gordon, A. M., Moore, A., Schejter Margalit, T., Farquharson, Y., et al. (2013). A multi-site study of functional outcomes following a themed approach to hand-arm bimanual intensive therapy for children with hemiplegia. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 55, 527–533. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harte, D., and Spencer, K. (2014). Sleight of hand: magic, therapy, and motor performance. J. Hand Ther. 27, 67–69. doi: 10.1016/j.jht.2013.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ison, M. S., and Morelato, G. S. (2002). Family contexts and developmental of cognitive skills for the resolution of interpersonal problems in children. Psykhe 11, 149–157.

Google Scholar

Ives, L. E. (2006). Intervenciones no farmacológicas en el entorno familiar de niños con trastorno por déficit de atención con/sin hiperactividad. Revista pediatría de Aten. Primaria 8(Suppl. 4), 57–67.

Google Scholar

Jacobson, N. S., and Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: a sta-tistical approach to defining meaningful change in psy-chotherapy research. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 59, 12–19. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.59.1.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiménez, J. E., Hernández, S., García, E., Díaz, A., Rodríguez, C., and Martín, R. (2012). Test de atención D2 datos normativos y desarrollo evolutivo de la atención en educación primaria. Eur. J. Educ. Psychol. 5, 93–106. doi: 10.30552/ejep.v5i1.79

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnstone, S., Roodenrys, S., Blackman, R., Johnston, E., Loveday, K., Mantz, S., et al. (2012). Neurocognitive training for children with and without AD/HD. Atten. Defic. Hyperact. Disord. 4, 11–23. doi: 10.1007/s12402-011-0069-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnstone, S. J., Roodenrys, S. J., Johnson, K., Bonfield, R., and Bennett, S. J. (2017). Game-based combined cognitive and neurofeedback training using focus pocus reduces symptom severity in children with diagnosed AD/HD and subclinical AD/HD. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 116, 32–44. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufman, A., and Kaufman, N. (1997). K-BIT test Breve de Inteligencia [K-BIT. Kaufman brief intelligence test]. Madrid: TEA.

Google Scholar

Kraemer, H., and Kupfer, D. (2005). Size of treatment effects and their importance to clinical research and practice. Biol. Psychiatry 59, 990–996. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhn, G., Amlani, A. A., and Rensink, R. A. (2008). Towards a science of magic. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 349–354. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhn, G., Gaffaratti, H. A., Teszka, R., and Rensink, R. A. (2014). A psychologically-based taxonomy of misdirection. Front. Psychol. 5:1392. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01392

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lambez, B., Harwood-Gross, A., Golumbic, E. Z., and Rassovsky, Y. (2020). Non-pharmacological interventions for cognitive difficulties in ADHD: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Psychiatr. Res. 120, 40–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2019.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langberg, J. M., Epstein, J. N., and Graham, A. J. (2008). Organizational-skills interventions in the treatment of ADHD. Expert. Rev. Neurother. 8, 1549–1561. doi: 10.1586/14737175.8.10.1549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leon, A. C., Davis, L. L., and Kraemer, H. C. (2011). The role and interpretation of pilot studies in clinical research. J. Psychiatr. Res. 45, 626–629. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lucas, S. R. (2013). Beyond de existence proof: ontological con-ditions, epistemological implications, and in-depth inter-view research. Qual. Quant. 48, 387–408. doi: 10.1007/s11135-012-9775-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinovic, D., Burgess, G. H., Pomerleau, C., and Marin, C. (2016). Computer games that exercise cognitive skills: what makes them engaging for children?Comput. Hum. Behav. 60, 451–462. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.02.063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattfeld, A. T., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Biederman, J., Spencer, T., Brown, A., Fried, R., et al. (2016). Dissociation of working memory impairments and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the brain. Neuroimage Clin. 10, 274–282. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meichenbaum, D. H., and Goodman, J. (1971). Training impulsive children to talk themselves. A means of developing self-control. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 77, 115–126. doi: 10.1037/h0030773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, K. N., Santillana, R., Miller, B., Clapp, W., Way, M., Bridgman-Goines, K., et al. (2020). Computer-based inhibitory control training in children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): evidence for behavioral and neural impact. PLoS One 15:e0241353. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0241352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minder, F., Zuberer, A., Brandeis, D., and Drechsler, R. (2019). Specific effects of individualized cognitive training in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): the role of pre-training cognitive impairment and individual training performance. Dev. Neurorehabil. 22, 400–414. doi: 10.1080/17518423.2019.1600064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miranda, A., Presentación, M. J., and Soriano, M. (2002). Effectiveness of a school-based multicomponent program for the treatment of children with ADHD. J. Learn. Disabil. 35, 547–563. doi: 10.1177/00222194020350060601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montoya, A., Colom, F., and Ferrin, M. (2011). Is psychoeducation for parents and teachers of children and adolescents with ADHD efficacious? A systematic literature review. Eur. Psychiatry 26, 166–175. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2010.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MTA Cooperative Group (1999). A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 56, 1073–1086. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarro-Soria, I., Juárez-Ruiz de Mier, R., García-Fernández, J. M., González-Gómez, C., Real-Fernández, M., Sánchez-Muñoz, M., et al. (2020). Detection of executive performance profiles using the ENFEN battery in children diagnosed with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Front. Psychol. 11:552322. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.552322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orjales, I. (2007). El tratamietno cognitive en niños con trastorno por deficit de atención con hiperactividad (TDAH): revisión y nuevas aportaciones. Anuario de psicología clínica y de la salud 3, 19–30.

Google Scholar

Parris, B. A., Kuhn, G., Mizon, G. A., Benattayallah, A., and Hodgson, T. T. (2009). Imaging the impossible: an fMRI study of impossible causal relationship in magic tricks. NeuroImage 45, 1033–1039. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Passarotti, A. M., Balaban, L., Colman, L. D., Katz, L. A., Trivedi, N., Liu, L., et al. (2020). A preliminary study on the functional benefits of computerized working memory training in children with pediatric bipolar disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Front. Psychol. 10:3060. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.03060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavuluri, M. N., Graczyk, P. A., Henry, D. B., Carbray, J. A., Heidenreich, J., and Miklowitz, D. J. (2004). Child‐ and family-focused cognitive-behaviour therapy for paediatric bipolar disorder: development and preliminary results. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 43, 528–537. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200405000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelham, W., and Fabiano, G. (2008). Evidence-based psychosocial treatments for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 37, 184–214. doi: 10.1080/15374410701818681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pérez, E. (2007). Programa de intervención neuropsicológica en el trastorno por déficit de atención e hiperactividad. EduPsykhé 6, 269–291.

Google Scholar

Pineda, D., Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Cadavid, C., Mancheno, S., and Mejía, S. (1998). Executive dysfunction in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Int. J. Neurosci. 96, 177–196.

Google Scholar

Pinho, T. D., Manz, P. H., DuPaul, G. J., Anastopoulos, A. D., and Weyandt, L. L. (2017). Predictors and moderators of quality of life among college students with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 23, 1736–1745. doi: 10.1177/1087054717734645

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pistoia, M., Abad-Mas, L., and Etchepareborda, M. C. (2004). Abordaje psicopedagógico del trastorno por déficit de atención con hiperactividad con el modelo de entrenamiento de las funciones ejecutivas. Rev. Neurol. 38(Suppl. 1), S149–S155. doi: 10.33588/rn.38S1.2004059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portellano, J. A., Martínez Arias, R., and Zumárraga, L. (2011). Manual ENFEN evaluación neuropsicológica de las funciones ejecutivas en niños. Madrid: TEA.

Google Scholar

Prins, P. J. M., Ten Brink, E., Dovis, S., Ponsioen, A., Geurts, H. M., de Vries, M., et al. (2013). “Braingame Brian”: toward an executive function training program with game elements for children with ADHD and cognitive control problems. Games Health J. 2, 44–49. doi: 10.1089/g4h.2013.0004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramalho, J., Garcia-Senoran, M. M., and Gonzalez, S. G. (2011). Self-instructions: strategy of attentional regulation in ADHD. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica 24, 180–185. doi: 10.1590/S0102-79722011000100021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rensink, R. A. (2010). Seeing seeing. Psyche 16, 68–78.

Google Scholar

Rensink, R. A. (2015). “A function-centred taxonomy of visual attention” in Phenomenal qualities: Sense, perception and consciousness. eds. P. Coates and S. Coleman(Oxford: University Press).

Google Scholar

Rensink, R. A., and Kuhn, G. (2014). A framework for using magic to study the mind. Front. Psychol. 5:1508. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, M., Moore, D., Gwernan-Jones, R., Thompson-Coon, J., Ukoumunne, O., Rogers, M., et al. (2015). Non-pharmacological interventions for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) delivered in school settings: systematic reviews of quantitative and qualitative research. Health Technol. Assess. 19, 1–470. doi: 10.3310/hta19450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rimestad, M. L., Lambek, R., Zacher Christiansen, H., and Hougaard, E. (2019). Short-and long-term effects of parent training for preschool children with or at risk of ADHD: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Atten. Disord. 23, 423–434. doi: 10.1177/1087054716648775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Salinas, E., Navas, M., González, P., Fominaya, S., and Duelo, M. (2006). La escuela y el trastorno por déficit de atención con/sin hiperactividad (TDAH). Revista pediatría de atención primaria 8, 175–198.

Google Scholar

Rosa, V. O., Schmitz, M., Moreira-Maia, C. R., Wagner, F., Londero, I., Bassotto, C. F., et al. (2017). Computerized cognitive training in children and adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder as add-on treatment to stimulants: feasibility study and protocol description. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 39, 65–76. doi: 10.1590/2237-6089-2016-0039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossignoli-Palomeque, T., Perez, E., and González-Marqués, J. (2018). Brain training in children and adolescents: is it scientifically valid?Front. Psychol. 9:565. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitt, S. A., Korucu, I., Napoli, A. R., Bryant, L. M., and Purpura, D. J. (2018). Using block play to enhance preschool children’s mathematics and executive functioning: a randomized controlled trial. Early Child. Res. Q. 44, 181–191. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.04.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scionti, N., Cavallero, M., Zogmaister, C., and Marzocchi, G. M. (2020). Is cognitive training effective for improving executive functions in preschoolers? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 10:2812. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sedó, M. A. (2007). FDT-Test de los cinco digitos. Madrid, Spain: TEA Ediciones.

Google Scholar

Sheridan, S. M., Dee, C. C., Morgan, J. C., McCormick, M. E., and Walker, D. (1996). A multimethod intervention for social skills deficits in children with ADHD and their parents. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 25, 57–76. doi: 10.1080/02796015.1996.12085802

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrestha, M., Lautenschleger, J., and Soares, N. (2020). Non-pharmacologic management of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: a review. Transl. Pediatr. 9(Suppl. 1), S114–S124. doi: 10.21037/tp.2019.10.01

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shuai, L., Daley, D., Wang, Y. F., Zhang, J. S., Kong, Y. T., Tan, X., et al. (2017). Executive function training for children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Chin. Med. J. 130:549. doi: 10.4103/0366-6999.200541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, T., Scahill, L., Dawson, G., Guthrie, D., Lord, C., Odom, S., et al. (2007). Designing research studies on psychosocial interventions in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 354–366. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0173-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. D., Vitulano, L. A., Katsovich, L., Li, S., Moore, C., Li, F., et al. (2020). A randomized controlled trial of an integrated brain, body and social intervention for children with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 24, 780–794. doi: 10.1177/1087054716647490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonuga-Barke, E., Brandeis, D., Cortese, S., Daley, D., Ferrin, M., Holtmann, M., et al. (2013). Nonpharmacological interventions for ADHD: systematic review and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials of dietary and psychological treatments. Am. J. Psychiatry 170, 275–289. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12070991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer, K. (2012). Hocus focus: evaluating the academic and functional benefits of integrating magic tricks in the classroom. J. Int. Assoc. Spec. Educ. 13, 87–99.

Google Scholar

Spencer, K., Yuen, H. K., Darwin, M., Jenkins, G., and Kirklin, K. (2019). Development and validation of the Hocus focus magic performance evaluation scale for health professions personnel in the United States. J. Educ. Evalu. Health Prof. 16:8. doi: 10.3352/jeehp.2019.16.8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Storebo, O. J., Elmose Andersen, M., Skoog, M., Joost Hansen, S., Simonsen, E., Pedersen, N., et al. (2019). Social skills training for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children aged 5 to 18 years. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 6:CD008223. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD008223.pub3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Storebo, O. J., Gluud, C., Winkel, P., and Simonsen, E. (2012). Social-skills and parental training plus standard treatment versus standard treatment for children with ADHD–the randomised SOSTRA trial. PLoS One 7:e37280. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, Z. (2017). The effectiveness of verbal self-instruction program on the symptoms of ADHD: controlled before and after study. NeuroQuantology 15, 121–126. doi: 10.14704/nq.2017.15.4.1146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamm, L., Hughes, C., Ames, L., Pickering, J., Silver, C. H., Stavinoha, P., et al. (2010). Attention training for school-aged children with ADHD: results of an open trial. J. Atten. Disord. 14, 86–94. doi: 10.1177/1087054709347446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Velõ, S., Keresztény, Á., Ferenczi-Dallos, G., and Balázs, J. (2019). Long-term effects of multimodal treatment on psychopathology and health-related quality of life of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Front. Psychol. 10:2037. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veloso, A., Vicente, S. G., and Filipe, M. G. (2020). Effectiveness of cognitive training for school-aged children and adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 10:2983. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, S., Richels, C., Michalek, A., and Raymer, A. (2015). Psychosocial treatments for ADHD: a systematic appraisal of the evidence. J. Atten. Disord. 19, 3–10. doi: 10.1177/1087054712447857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (2005). Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para niños-IV [Wechsler’s intelligence scale for children-IV (WISC-IV)]. Madrid, Spain: TEA.

Google Scholar

White, S., Ollendick, T., Albano, A., Oswald, D., Johnson, C., Michael, A., et al. (2012). Randomized controlled trial: multimodal anxiety and social skill intervention for adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 382–394. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1577-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., and Pennington, B. F. (2005a). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1336–1346. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., Chhabildas, N., and Hulslander, J. (2005b). Neuropsychological analyses of comorbidity between reading disability and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: in search of the common deficit. Dev. Neuropsychol. 27, 35–78. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2701_3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willis, D., Siceloff, E. R., Morse, M., Neger, E., and Flory, K. (2019). Stand-alone social skills training for youth with ADHD: a systematic review. Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 22, 348–366. doi: 10.1007/s10567-019-00291-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiseman, R., and Watt, C. (2018). Achieving the impossible: a review of magic-based interventions and their effects on wellbeing. PeerJ 6:e6081. doi: 10.7717/peerj.6081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiseman, R., and Watt, C. (2020). Conjuring cognition: a review of educational magic-based interventions. PeerJ 8:e8747. doi: 10.7717/peerj.8747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, S. P., Lovejoy, D. W., and Ball, J. D. (2002). Neuropsychological characteristics of adults with ADHD: a comprehensive review of initial studies. Clin. Neuropsychol. 16, 12–34. doi: 10.1076/clin.16.1.12.8336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuberer, A., Minder, F., Brandeis, D., and Drechsler, R. (2018). Mixed-effects modeling of neurofeedback self-regulation performance: moderators for learning in children with ADHD. Neural Plast. 2018, 1–15. doi: 10.1155/2018/2464310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, MAGNITIVE program, cognitive training program, magic tricks, effectiveness, feasibility

Citation: Bonete S, Osuna Á, Molinero C and García-Font I (2021) MAGNITIVE: Effectiveness and Feasibility of a Cognitive Training Program Through Magic Tricks for Children With Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder. A Second Clinical Trial in Community Settings. Front. Psychol. 12:649527. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.649527

Received: 04 January 2021; Accepted: 05 March 2021;
Published: 01 April 2021.

Edited by:

Pamela Bryden, Wilfrid Laurier University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Rosa Angela Fabio, University of Messina, Italy
Seyed Mohammad Mahdi Moshirian Farahi, Carleton University, Canada

Copyright © 2021 Bonete, Osuna, Molinero and García-Font. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Saray Bonete, s.bonete.prof@ufv.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.