Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 21 July 2021
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Securing Your Relationship: Quality of Intimate Relationships During the COVID-19 Pandemic Can Be Predicted by Attachment Style

\r\nStephanie J. Eder*Stephanie J. Eder1*Andrew A. NicholsonAndrew A. Nicholson1Michal M. StefanczykMichal M. Stefanczyk2Micha&#x; PieniakMichał Pieniak2Judit Martínez-MolinaJudit Martínez-Molina3Ondra Pe&#x;outOndra Pešout4Jakub BinterJakub Binter5Patrick SmelaPatrick Smela1Frank Scharnowski,,,Frank Scharnowski1,6,7,8David Steyrl,David Steyrl1,6
  • 1Department of Cognition, Emotion, and Methods in Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
  • 2Institute of Psychology, University of Wrocław, Wrocław, Poland
  • 3Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
  • 4Department of Psychology, Jan Evangelista Purkyně University in Ústí nad Labem, Ústí nad Labem, Czechia
  • 5Faculty of Science, Charles University, Prague, Czechia
  • 6Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Psychiatric Hospital, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 7Neuroscience Center Zürich, University of Zürich and Swiss Federal Institute of Technology, Zurich,, Switzerland
  • 8Zürich Center for Integrative Human Physiology (ZIHP), University of Zürich, Zurich, Switzerland

The COVID-19 pandemic along with the restrictions that were introduced within Europe starting in spring 2020 allows for the identification of predictors for relationship quality during unstable and stressful times. The present study began as strict measures were enforced in response to the rising spread of the COVID-19 virus within Austria, Poland, Spain and Czech Republic. Here, we investigated quality of romantic relationships among 313 participants as movement restrictions were implemented and subsequently phased out cross-nationally. Participants completed self-report questionnaires over a period of 7 weeks, where we predicted relationship quality and change in relationship quality using machine learning models that included a variety of potential predictors related to psychological, demographic and environmental variables. On average, our machine learning models predicted 29% (linear models) and 22% (non-linear models) of the variance with regard to relationship quality. Here, the most important predictors consisted of attachment style (anxious attachment being more influential than avoidant), age, and number of conflicts within the relationship. Interestingly, environmental factors such as the local severity of the pandemic did not exert a measurable influence with respect to predicting relationship quality. As opposed to overall relationship quality, the change in relationship quality during lockdown restrictions could not be predicted accurately by our machine learning models when utilizing our selected features. In conclusion, we demonstrate cross-culturally that attachment security is a major predictor of relationship quality during COVID-19 lockdown restrictions, whereas fear, pathogenic threat, sexual behavior, and the severity of governmental regulations did not significantly influence the accuracy of prediction.

Introduction

Viral Threat and Political Measures

In spring 2020, the world-wide spread of the severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) led to stressful and insecure conditions across many nations –which corresponded with severe health, economic, and social disruptions (United Nations [UN], 2020; World Health Organization [WHO], 2020). In Europe, the actual spread and effects of the virus, as well as national counter measures, differed between countries. For example, Spain had one of the highest infection and mortality rates in Europe, leading to limited access to medical care and the modification of public facilities to be used as field hospitals and morgues. Consequently, a nationwide state of alarm was issued, and the free movement of citizens was drastically restricted (Ministerio de la Presidencia, 2020). Despite lower case counts, Austria responded with curfew restrictions to limit the spread of the virus (Bundesministerium für Soziales, Gesundheit, Pflege und Konsumentenschutz [BSGPK], 2020), and even countries with very low case numbers such as Czech Republic and Poland adopted strict defensive measures, such as prohibiting gatherings and introducing minimal distance measures (Dziennik Ustaw, 2020; Dziennik Ustaw, 2020). Varying viral impacts and medical capacities notwithstanding, all of these countries eventually introduced strict regulations, and all of them encouraged or reinforced “social distancing” by prohibition of gatherings or movement restrictions.

The Crisis and Intimate Relationships

Previous research has shown that stress impacts both relationship quality (i.e., how good people subjectively perceive their relationship to be) (cf. Randall and Bodenmann, 2009, 2017), and the way intimate partners conjointly deal with stress (Falconier et al., 2015). Relationship quality in turn is closely linked to many parameters of well-being, including psychological and bodily health (South and Krueger, 2013; Pieh et al., 2020). Independent of the elicited stress, social isolation that removes other contacts facilitates an exclusively dyadic relationship for couples living together, and the rise in domestic violence since the implementation of movement restrictions suggests that this is not healthy for all romantic relationships (Bradbury-Jones and Isham, 2020).

Importantly, attachment style may be an influential factor that might determine how such restrictions and stressors influence relationship quality. Attachment theory has originally been proposed as an ethological framework that explains children’s reactions to stressful situations (Ainsworth and Bowlby, 1991). Individual developmental trajectories of attachment remain highly influential in adulthood, becoming activated upon stress exposure (Gillath et al., 2006) and influencing behaviors within intimate relationships as adults (Mikulincer et al., 2002). Similar to relationship satisfaction, secure attachment is significantly correlated with greater health and well-being (McWilliams and Bailey, 2010), where securely attached adults, whether in a relationship or not, may have overall more psychological resources to cope with distress. A variety of studies have found that securely attached individuals, as opposed to those with non-secure attachment styles, are affected more by distress (Zakin et al., 2003), and in turn are more prone to distort their representations of themselves and their social environment (Mikulincer et al., 1998). Furthermore, attachment security is crucial for maintaining satisfactory intimate relationships, and several studies have shown connections between attachment security and relationship outcomes, although determining causality poses a problem (see Mikulincer et al., 2002 for a review). A variety of dynamic factors may at least partially account for such effects—for example, securely attached persons are known to engage in more mutual forms of conflict resolution than anxious or avoidant types (Corcoran and Mallinckrodt, 2000), and conflict style in turn has a major influence on relationship satisfaction (Cann et al., 2008).

Feeney (2002) notes that as opposed to securely attached persons, insecurely attached individuals’ evaluation of their own relationship as more strongly influenced by their partners’ recent behavior. Therefore, when predicting reported relationship quality, taking recent events into account (e.g., amount of fights) in addition to attachment style to the partner may aid in the prediction of models aiming to capture such interactions. Moreover, the same events could be appraised differently by securely and insecurely attached individuals (Collins, 1996), making it essential to consider both actual events relevant to relationships and inner working models (i.e., the attachment to the partner). Thus, depending on the inner resources to deal with external stress and the inner representations of social relations, the COVID-19 crisis could turn out to be stabilizing for some relationships, and destabilizing for others.

Stressful periods such as the 2003 SARS outbreak have been shown to have significant impacts on intimate relationships that last far longer than the actual threat, evidenced by higher stress levels and increased divorce rates (Census and Statistics Department, 2007; Lee et al., 2007). One of the social behavioral systems affected severely by stress is sexual satisfaction and engaging in sexual activity with a partner (Bodenmann et al., 2006; Yehuda et al., 2015), an important and often defining feature of intimate relationships in humans (Hazan and Shaver, 1987). Critically, however, the role of sexuality in stable relationships and its impact on psychological aspects such as well-being and relationship quality is still omitted in many studies (cf. Heiman et al., 2011). Notably, frequency of sexual intercourse has indirectly been shown to be affected by external crises, where around 9 months after severe threats, birth rates reliably go down throughout cultures, whereas milder threats such as low-severity storm advisories lead to increased birth rates (Evans et al., 2010; Chandra et al., 2018; Richmond and Roehner, 2018a,b). A more subjective measure of sexuality in couples, sexual satisfaction, has been shown to relate to relationship quality as well as relationship stability, where interestingly, this effect seems to be stronger for men than women (Sprecher, 2002). Indeed, the “relationship state model” proposed by Birnbaum and Finkel (2015) concludes based on empirical evidence that sex plays an important role in dyadic bonding processes, but varies in influence over different stages of relationship development.

Importantly, recent studies have linked attachment systems to a variety of sexual behaviors (e.g., Davis et al., 2006; Butzer and Campbell, 2008), where interestingly, sexual satisfaction might even mediate the effects of attachment anxiety on relationship satisfaction in women (Birnbaum, 2007). Moreover, securely attached persons tend to have less promiscuous and casual sex (Bogaert and Sadava, 2002). It has furthermore been suggested that low levels of environmental pathogens coincide with more liberal sexual behavior including promiscuity and casual sex and may even account for the evolution of cross-cultural differences in sexual norms (Thornhill et al., 2009; Tybur et al., 2015). Both aspects make sexual behavior a particularly interesting facet of human functioning and behavior during a world-wide medical crisis, and an indispensable factor to consider when observing intimate relationships. Here, we consider the frequency of engaging in sexual behaviors with a partner and sexual satisfaction as potential predictors of relationship quality during an external crisis, the COVID-19 pandemic.

Predicting Relationship Quality Using Machine-Learning

The present study investigates a period in the spring of 2020, during which strict movement restrictions in response the COVID-19 pandemic were implemented and subsequently relaxed in Austria, Poland, Spain and Czech Republic. Over a period of 7 weeks, individuals from these countries completed self-report questionnaires in relation to their romantic relationships, partner attachment, and sexual behavior, as well as questions related to the pandemic and the perceived threat associated with the virus. Our goal was to determine which variables predict (i) relationship quality during this crisis, and (ii) changes in relationship quality during the crisis. More specifically, we hypothesized that attachment security (both anxious and avoidant dimensions) and sexual behavior (sexual activity with partner and sexual satisfaction) would be major predictors of both overall relationship quality and maintaining relationship quality during these unstable times, where more secure attachment, higher sexual satisfaction, and more sexual activity would lead to higher relationship quality.

Based on literature, we explored a range of other variables that may improve the prediction accuracy, or potentially influence, the effect of attachment and sexuality on relationship quality (see Table 1). Variables related to the relationship itself include: (i) duration of the relationship, which is expected to be inversely related to overall relationship satisfaction (Birditt et al., 2009), however, relationships that have existed for longer prior to the pandemic might be more stable and less subject to changes in quality over the course of the lockdown, (ii) whether dyadic partners live together, (iii) physical contact in general and specific to the partner, since touching interactions are known to relate to bonding as well as stress management, yet are poorly investigated (Brennan et al., 1998; Ditzen et al., 2007; Van Anders et al., 2013), and (iv) demonstration of affection in romantic couples (i.e., kissing) which has been found to relate to conflict resolution and relationship satisfaction (Gulledge et al., 2003; Floyd et al., 2009). Moreover, we also investigated aspects known to load on the dimensions independence, intimacy, and agreement/conflicts of relationship quality, respectively (see Hassebrauck and Fehr, 2002). Except for “conflicts,” which are known to negatively influence relationship satisfaction, we expected that these dimensions would positively contribute to maintaining high relationship quality during the crisis. Additionally, environmental measures included (i) actual pathogenic threat (local spread of infections and mortality), and (ii) stringency of measures introduced by a given nation’s government. Here, we aimed to explore if macro-level factors related to the pandemic would have a predictive value for relationship quality or change in relationship quality, and if so, how they interact with psychological variables. Further, we directly included a subjective assessment of threat associated with economic loss, as well as the subjective fear of the virus (independent of local prevalence or mortality). Moreover, we included demographic information potentially influencing aspects of romantic relationships.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Input features (“predictors”) used to predict reported relationship quality and change in relationship quality.

We employed two types of robust, cross-validated machine-learning models: Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator (LASSO) (Tibshirani, 1996), and Extremely Randomized Trees (ExtraTrees) (Geurts et al., 2006). These models allow one to incorporate many variables while avoiding over-fitting, and potentially to make accurate predictions in a novel pool of participants. Extremely randomized trees are in addition non-parametric and can detect complex non-linear interactions between the variables beyond linear correlations. Taken together, this approach outperforms conventional statistical models in sufficiently large datasets when dealing with multiple input variables and allows one to make generalizable conclusions about the relations between predictor variables and target variables.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Our sample consisted of 313 adult participants (mean age = 32.02 years, SD age = 12.99; 236 female; 267 heterosexual) who reported to be in a romantic relationship (mean duration = 7.18 years, SD = 8.58 years) and who repeatedly participated over a 7-weeks period. Participants resided in four main countries: Austria (n = 104), Poland (n = 87), Spain (n = 65), Czech Republic (n = 33), and four additional regions (Germany, Netherlands, Italy, Pakistan; n = 24).

No exclusion criteria were applied, but all cases with missing variables of interest were excluded for the machine learning models (see section “Results” for the number of samples included in each model).

Characteristics of the Studied Countries and Time Period

We actively sampled within four European countries, which markedly differed in the prevalence of the virus and rates of mortality, as well as the stringency of implemented measures by governments and economic change (Petherick et al., 2020, see also Figure 1 in Eder et al., 2021). In a sufficiently large sample, these local differences allow for characterizing the predictive value of aforementioned macro-level variables. The chosen study period coincided with the implementation and loosening of governmental measures, including curfews in the participating countries. Accordingly, the first survey was completed by participants between March 16th and 22nd (coinciding with the first curfews in Austria, a nationwide state of alarm in Spain and Czech Republic and a state of epidemic danger in Poland). The last survey was completed between April 27th and May 3rd, 2020, when the most drastic restrictions had been lifted in all countries.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Permutation feature importance (A) and model-based feature importance (B) over linear models with relationship quality as a target. (C) Use of attachment security (overall ECR-R score) as a predictor by the non-linear models.

The following section characterizes the socio-political climate during the study period within the investigated countries.

Highly Affected: Spain

In Spain, a nationwide state of alarm was issued on March 14th 2020 (Ministerio de la Presidencia, 2020). Within the first week of the survey, free movement of citizens was limited to essential activities, and international borders were closed except for the return of residents (March 15th, 16th). The dramatic increase of infections during the second week of the study lead to limited access to Intensive Care Units and to triage regulations (Redacción la Vanguardia, 2020). Facilities such as hotels were used as provisional hospitals (Enguix, 2020), others as morgues (Redaccioì i ageÌncie la Vanguardia, 2020). At the beginning of the third week (March 30th), all non-essential activities were shut down. These restrictions were only slightly lifted in weeks five, coinciding with the governmental prescription of face masks in public transportation (Redacción la Vanguardia, 2020). Throughout the surveys, Spain had one of the highest infections and mortality rates in Europe (cf. Hale et al., 2020) and was objectively under a higher pathogenic threat than the other observed countries.

Middle Field: Austria

In Austria, the first week of the study coincided precisely with the first nationwide curfew, encouragement of social isolation and shut-down of non-essential infrastructure. Public meetings of people not living together were prohibited, where the government insinuated that meetings of people not living in the same household were generally prohibited (the legal foundations for such a wide-ranging prohibition were not necessarily given). Minimum distances of 1 m between persons in public spaces was prescribed (Bundesministerium für Soziales, Gesundheit, Pflege und Konsumentenschutz [BSGPK], 2020). The economic shut-down in Austria was slowly phased out toward the end of the study, while wearing face masks became mandatory in many situations (Bundesministerium für Soziales, Gesundheit, Pflege und Konsumentenschutz [BSGPK], 2020). Infection rates in Austria were successfully decreased by the measurements, and at no time were there shortages in medical aid.

Low Infection Rates and Strict Regulations: Poland and Czech Republic

On March 13th 2020, the Polish government declared a “state of epidemic danger,” which led to limited use of public space, mandatory quarantine at the borders, and closing of public schools and universities (Dziennik Ustaw, 2020). As of March 24th 2020, citizens were only allowed to leave their residences for work or to access essential infrastructure such as grocery stores, and social gatherings were restricted to two persons. Mandatory interpersonal distances were introduced in both public spaces and working environments (Dziennik Ustaw, 2020). Restrictions were phased out starting April 20th, at the beginning of weeks 6 of this study (Dziennik Ustaw, 2020). However, around that time, a third of the population believed that the government deliberately manipulated the case count (Molenda et al., 2020). Thus, the corona crisis in Poland is not only marked by relatively strict regulations, but also by divided opinions on the extent of the medical threat and political divisions (Michalak, 2020).

Similarly, the Czech government declared a “national state of emergency” on March 12th 2020, shortly before the start of this study (Ministry of Health of the Czech Republic [MHCR], 2020). A series of governmental regulations to prevent the spread of COVID-19 had been implemented since March 2nd, including the shutdown of schools, ban on sales in shops and restaurants, closing the national boarders, as well as wearing face-masks in public. Gathering of more than two individuals were prohibited, and interpersonal distances of 2 m between individuals were mandatory in all public spaces. The first victim of COVID-19 was confirmed at the end of the first week of this study (March 22nd). The government gradually lifted restriction measures starting April 20th, week six of this study (Ministry of Health of the Czech Republic [MHCR], 2020).

Procedure

Weekly surveys (administered via SoSci Survey)1 were sent as a link via e-mail to participants who had been recruited over social media at the beginning of the study period and who agreed to be contacted for the purposes of the study. Participants were informed about the aim of the study and that they could stop participating at any point; they were fully debriefed and received the option to leave a contact address to be informed of the results of the study. Communication with the participants took place in their native language.

Surveys

To measure attachment security, we administered the Experiences in Close Relationships Revised Scale (ECR-R, Fraley et al., 2000) at the beginning and at the end of the study, which allowed us to estimate the temporal stability of this input feature. Validated translations of the questionnaire were used, and if no translation was available the questionnaire was translated by a native speaker and reviewed by another native speaker (Polish version: Lubiewska et al., 2016; Czech version: Cígler et al., 2019; Spanish version modified from Fernández-Fuertes et al., 2011).

Additionally, we assessed information regarding participants’ romantic relationship. Here, we surveyed one general question directly targeting perceived relationship quality (4-point scale) and one binary question each investigating (i) if the partners engaged in joint activities, (ii) perceived tolerance for each other, (iii) if important topics are discussed with the partner, and (iv) taking time for each other. For each given week, (i) number of conflicts with the partner, (ii) frequency of joint sexual behavior, (iii) sexual satisfaction, (iv) kissing, (v) general physical interaction, and (vi) close physical contact with the partner were assessed (6-point scale from (“not at all” to “5x and more”).

To include local and temporal changes in governmental restrictions, we utilized a “stringency index” for each country and week as described by Petherick et al. (2020), denoting the gravity of governmental restrictions in response to the pandemic. As a subjective measure of how the restrictions affected the participants’ economic status, we asked if they were threatened by economic loss due to responses to the pandemic.

Further, we included confirmed cases and deaths per million citizens (Sources: Eurostat, 2020; Hale et al., 2020) for each week and country as a measure of viral spread and mortality. Again, we probed participants for more a subjective measure of perceived viral threat, defined here as the perceived fear of the virus. Here, questions associated with fear of infection, perceived threat to own health, and to the health of other people emotionally close to the participant were collapsed to create the variable “fear of the virus”.

Demographic and personal items were collected, including sex, sexual orientation, relationship status, and country of residence. Categorical features were one-hot (dummy) encoded.

In summary, Table 1 lists all input features included in our machine learning models. Phrasing and scoring of the questions are publicly available in the OSF-project.

Analysis

We fit two types of machine learning models, each trying to predict the target variables of relationship quality and change in relationship quality. One model was linear (LASSO, Tibshirani, 1996), and the other non-linear (ExtraTrees, Geurts et al., 2006). The models were evaluated within a nested cross-validation procedure (90/10, 100 repeats each), where hyper-parameter tuning took place in the inner loop and only used training data from the current loop (Cawley and Talbot, 2010). Cross-validation was stratified, controlling for participant ID to counteract subject cluster learning.

The chosen models are particularly suited to handle many input features (predictors) while at the same avoiding over-fitting. Extremely randomized trees are non-parametric (the LASSO, in contrast, imposes a linear structure) and handle complex interactions between the variables, beyond what traditional regression analysis could encompass. We compared the models’ performance in the respective hold-out sets to a trivial predictor, which uses the mean of all target variables for each prediction. The reported p-values indicate if the models perform significantly better than such a trivial predictor. We report an R2 value as a measure of how much of the variance in the target variables can be predicted by our models in new pools of participants (the hold-out sets). Notably, this value will be smaller than when a conventional model is being fit to all data, however, the results are models capable of actual predictions for unknown data. Therefore, it is likely that our models capture real existing structures in the data and not artifacts.

To estimate the actual importance of each predictor for the forecast, we report the median permutation feature importance (PFI) for the better-performing model as the proportional loss of explained variance if a variable is replaced by a random (non-informative) array of that variable (Breiman, 2001). Thus, if a given predictor has a PFI of 10%, this indicates that 10% of the previously explained variance in the target is lost when the models cannot meaningfully access the given predictor variable. Further, we also show model-based feature importance. In case of the linear model, this are the coefficients assigned to a variable by the models, normalized each by the sum of all coefficients. In case of the non-linear model, this is how much reduction in the error is caused by a specific variable, averaged over the model.

For computing the machine-learning models, we utilized the free machine-learning library scikit-learn (Version 0.22.2., Pedregosa et al., 2011, scikit-learn.org, most importantly the functions “ExtraTreesRegressor”/”Lasso” to initialize the models, “GroupShuffleSplit” to stratify the cross-validation procedure and “permutation importance” as the main measure of feature importance). All analyses were conducted in Python 3.7.7. and R (R Core Team, 2017).

Results

Relationship Quality Throughout the Crisis

Overall, relationship quality remained fairly constant throughout the aforementioned restrictions, as the majority of all participants did not report any changes. However, exactly one third of all participants did report changes in their relationship quality. We aimed to predict these inter-individual differences regarding the stability of relationship quality below.

Predicting Overall Relationship Quality

On average, around 30% of the variance in relationship quality during the lockdown was successfully predicted by the linear models (LASSO), whereas the non-linear models (ExtraTrees) on average predicted 22% of this variance (LASSO: R2avg = 0.29, R2median = 0.33, p < 0.001; ExtraTrees: R2avg = 0.22, R2median = 0.23, p < 0.001; Ntrials = 731).

Based on PFI, the most important predictors with an importance over 5% for the linear model were age (13.92%, younger age predicting higher quality), conflicts (12.88%, less fights predicting higher quality) and attachment anxiety (8.62%, less anxiety scores predicting higher quality) (Figure 1A); whereas for the non-linear model, conflicts (6.32%, same directionality), joint activities (5.68%, more joint activity predicting higher quality), time investment (7.24%, more investment predicting higher quality) and attachment anxiety (5.60%, same directionality) were contributing most to the explained variance.

The most important features remained the same when assessing model-based feature importance, although in a different order: Conflicts (0.13), attachment anxiety (0.12), age (0.11), joint activity (0.09), time investment (0.07), and avoidant attachment (0.07) for the linear models (Figure 1B); joint activity (0.25), conflicts (0.14), attachment anxiety (0.13), avoidant attachment (0.09), time investment (0.09), and tolerance (0.06) for the non-linear models. For the better performing linear models, Figure 1 shows the PFI and model-based importance of all features.

The above models consider attachment anxiety and avoidance as two separate predictors. To assess the overall importance of attachment style relative to other variables, we then combined both subscales of the ECR-R and used it as a single predictor. Here, the ECR-R score indicating overall attachment (in)security is by far the most important feature (responsible for 19.11 and 11.03% of the explained variance in the linear and non-linear model, respectively).

Figure 1C shows how this most informative predictor (the sum of anxiety and avoidance) is linked to relationship quality in our non-linear models. It indicates that a non-linear use of this measure is most beneficial when predicting relationship quality.

Changes in Relationship Quality Were Not Predicted by the Surveyed Variables

As opposed to the overall quality, changes in relationship quality could not be predicted by our models better than by a trivial predictor (LASSO: R2avg = –0.14, R2median = –0.06, p = 1; ExtraTrees: R2avg = –0.31, R2median = –0.14, p = 1; Ntrials = 708). The only feature with a model-based feature importance above zero in both model types was the participants’ answer to the item “my partner makes time for me” (model-based feature importance: LASSO = 0.58; ExtraTrees = 0.24; PFIboth = 0). A post hoc analysis revealed a Pearson correlation of R = 0.21 (BF01 = 0.695) between this time-investment and the change in relationship satisfaction over time.

Temporal Stability of Attachment Security

An important predictor for relationship quality was attachment security as measured by the ECR-R, and we performed a post hoc group comparison with all participants that had filled in the questionnaire both times (around 4 weeks apart). There was no difference in attachment security between the beginning and end phase of the lockdown (mean decrease by 0.05%, BF01 = 14.924; N = 140). The shared variance was 76.91%, which is in line with the good 3-weeks test-retest reliability reported for the ECR-R (84–85% shared variance, Sibley et al., 2005).

Discussion

The aim of the current study was to predict relationship quality as movement restrictions in response to the COVID-19 pandemic were implemented and relaxed in four European states. Our linear models predicted around 30% of the variance in self-reported relationship quality, where attachment style was one of the most important predictors. This is in line with studies reporting that ECR-R scores explain a large proportion of individual differences in emotional experiences/responses within relationships (Sibley et al., 2005). However, the same variables could not predict changes in relationship quality over that time period.

Predictors of Relationship Quality

Nationwide movement restrictions constitute special circumstances for people in romantic relationships, where due to curfews, couples become each other’s only form of social contact. Indeed, during unstable times such as the COVID-19 pandemic, people may have to endure increased stress levels in response to the medical threat associated with the virus and subsequent societal changes, which both in turn might further influence relationship dynamics. In line with our hypotheses, attachment security was a major predictor of relationship satisfaction during the time of curfews and social restrictions. These findings corroborate previous studies that have linked attachment styles to relationship quality (e.g., Mikulincer et al., 2002) where these effects may even be exacerbated by the stressful effects of the COVID-19 crisis (cf. Gillath et al., 2006). Mikulincer et al. (2002) propose three main avenues by which relationship quality may be influenced by attachment style: (i) secure attachment may orient individuals toward a positive bias of dyadic interactions; (ii) positive representations of the self and others may affect conflict management; and (iii) the satisfaction of other psychological needs may be enhanced in securely attached individuals (Mikulincer et al., 2002). Interestingly, in the current study, the dimension “anxiety” was a more important predictor than the subscale “avoidance.” Potentially, this difference may be enhanced by a general decrease of external security in times of crises.

Secure attachment may further relate to an internal locus of control (Hexel, 2003), where securely attached individuals may have more mechanisms to cope with the stress a world-wide crisis elicits (Feeney, 1995). Indeed, an analysis of subjective health in our sample shows that attachment security is a predictor of a heightened sense of health (Eder et al., 2021). Speculatively, this may mean that the effects of secure attachment are not limited to the relationship per se, but may exert an influence over other psychological mechanisms.

Importantly, the accuracy of predictions for relationship quality could have been enhanced by sampling information from both partners. Senchak and Leonard (1992) report that the pairing of attachment styles, rather than individual attachment itself, is associated with marital satisfaction. Specifically, as compared to couples where both partners are securely attached (“secure couples”), couples where a securely attached person is in a relationship with an insecurely attached person do not differ from “insecure couples” with regards to marital satisfaction. However, securely attached persons tend to pair with securely attached partners (Senchak and Leonard, 1992), potentially buffering this confounding factor. Nevertheless, incorporating information about both partners might have aided in the prediction of relationship quality and possibly even the prediction of changes within this variable. This one-sided perspective and lack of dyadic data must be noted as a clear limitation of the current study and offers a starting point for more extensive research.

Attachment theory has traditionally classified attachment into discrete styles (Feeney and Noller, 1990; Ainsworth and Bowlby, 1991), although many have argued that attachment security should be treated as a continuum (Fraley et al., 2015; Haslam et al., 2020; Lubiewska and Van de Vijver, 2020). Indeed, the method by which our non-linear models utilize the (continuous) ECR-R score in order to optimize the predictions of relationship quality (Figure 1C) suggests that a non-continuous use of the variable (divided to the categories secure/less secure) may be justified as a simplified measure when investigating implications for adult relationship satisfaction.

Interestingly, the age of participants had a relatively high feature importance, where younger participants self-reported higher quality scores with respect to their relationships. Some longitudinal evidence may support this conclusion, such as an increase in unpleasant aspects in the relationship over time (Birditt et al., 2009), where correlations between younger age and higher relationship satisfaction have been reported previously, albeit at small effect sizes (Lenger et al., 2019). Both longitudinal and cross-sectional studies would be of value to follow up on this finding.

In both linear and non-linear models, another important predictor of how participants rated their relationship quality was the number of arguments with their partner they reported per week, where frequency of conflicts has previously been associated with lower relationship quality lowered relationship quality (Kluwer and Johnson, 2007). Our results confirm the predictive value of the amount of conflicts, and suggest that simplified behavioral observations or surveys on relationship quality should include this dimension. Indeed, our findings emphasize the dimension “agreement” over other dimensions of intimate relationships, such as intimacy and independence, but also sexuality (cf. Hassebrauck and Fehr, 2002). This is surprising, since engaging in sexual behavior is an important aspect of pair-bonding that has repeatedly been demonstrated (Birnbaum and Finkel, 2015; McNulty et al., 2016). Moreover, even though sexual satisfaction cannot predict changes in relationship quality or vice versa in some studies (Byers, 2005; Joel et al., 2020), as is in line with our results, it does seem to relate to relationship quality at a given point in time (Byers, 2005; Joel et al., 2020), and in some studies even to future relationship quality (Fallis et al., 2016). However, the actual frequency of sexual intercourse alone is not a telling measure of relationship satisfaction (Loewenstein et al., 2015), and a curvilinear relationship between sexual frequency and overall happiness has been proposed (Muise et al., 2016), which, if applicable to relationship satisfaction, should nevertheless have been detected by our non-linear models. “Sexual afterglow”—evidenced in elevated levels of sexual satisfaction—might facilitate pair bonding beyond sex (Meltzer et al., 2017); however, the effect could have been captured by our predictor “sexual satisfaction.” Overall, neither sexual activity nor satisfaction was predictive of reported relationship quality in our models, and our data do not provide further evidence to the large body of literature connecting these variables. A deeper understanding of circumstances and motives (“sexual goals”) of sexual behavior in romantic relationships may help to clarify this relationship (cf. Muise et al., 2017).

Previous studies have found that changes in relationship quality are more difficult to predict from self-report variables than relationship quality at a given point in time (Joel et al., 2020). Our results show that this may be the case even in a period where many quantifiable environmental factors act on intimate relationships, since our machine learning models failed to predict changes in relationship quality. The fact that we did not observe changes for most participants may in part account for this difficulty in prediction, since variance in the target variable is important to identify predictors of this target. Possibly, surveying information with respect to interpersonal variations in stress-management and personality characteristics such as optimism, self-efficacy, and resilience (so-called “psychological capital”) could have improved our prediction, since personality-dependent stress management is known to strongly alleviate the effects of “objective” environmental stressors (McCrae, 1990; Mills and Huebner, 1998; Somerfield and McCrae, 2000; Avey et al., 2009). Of course, a dyadic view and information on both partners’ perspective might have further improved our chances.

While none of the environmental features or the features that do predict general relationship quality in the sample seem to add information about changes in quality, there is one exception: perceived time investment by the partner had a non-zero model-based feature importance and was post hoc shown to correlate with changes in relationship quality. While this study cannot conclusively support the role of time investment for temporal changes in relationship quality, this may provide an interesting starting point for further studies.

Generalizability

The cross-national composition of our sample and the fact that our models are evaluated by their performance in hold-out sets of “novel” subjects suggests a relatively high generalizability. However, we did not exclude or control for clinical psychological conditions, which might have influenced self-report measures during these globally stressful times. Cross-cultural studies on similar situations are needed in order to further investigate the role of attachment style in crisis-like situations, where future studies should also take into account both partners in a romantic relationship (see Kenny et al., 2006).

Conclusion

The current study provides evidence on the complex interactions between relationship quality, attachment style, and demographic variables in couples during exceptional and stressful circumstances. We examined and predicted relationship quality as lockdown measures due to the COVID-19 pandemic were implemented in four European states. We found that attachment security was a highly important predictor for relationship quality, and that country of residence, case counts, and the stringency of governmental measures did not contribute to accurate predictions. Changes in relationship quality could not be accurately predicted with the given input variables using the currently implemented machine learning approach.

In conclusion, we demonstrate that attachment security is an important predictor of relationship quality during the unstable times of the COVID-19 pandemic, as opposed to fear/perceived threat, sexuality, and macro-level environmental factors such as the stringency of movement restrictions.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: https://osf.io/ybjre/ Open Science Framework.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements; however, potentially intimate questions were additionally approved by the Institutional Review Board of Charles University, Faculty of Science. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

SE designed the study. SE, MS, MP, JM-M, OP, and JB translated items and collected the data. DS, PS, and SE analyzed the data. SE wrote the manuscript. AN and FS revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.soscisurvey.com

References

Ainsworth, M., and Bowlby, J. (1991). An ethological approach to personality development. Am. Psychol. 46, 331–341. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.46.4.333

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avey, J. B., Luthans, F., and Jensen, S. M. (2009). Psychological capital: a positive resource for combating employee stress and turnover. Hum. Resour. Manage. 48, 677–693. doi: 10.1002/hrm.20294

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birditt, K. S., Jackey, L. M., and Antonucci, T. C. (2009). Longitudinal patterns of negative relationship quality across adulthood. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 64, 55–64. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbn031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birnbaum, G. E. (2007). Attachment orientations, sexual functioning, and relationship satisfaction in a community sample of women. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 24, 21–35. doi: 10.1177/0265407507072576

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birnbaum, G. E., and Finkel, E. J. (2015). The magnetism that holds us together: sexuality and relationship maintenance across relationship development. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 1, 29–33. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2014.11.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bodenmann, G., Ledermann, T., Blattner, D., and Galluzzo, C. (2006). Associations among everyday stress, critical life events, and sexual problems. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 194, 494–501. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000228504.15569.b6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogaert, A. F., and Sadava, S. (2002). Adult attachment and sexual behavior. Pers. Relat. 9, 191–204. doi: 10.1111/1475-6811.00012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradbury-Jones, C., and Isham, L. (2020). The pandemic paradox: the consequences of COVID-19 on domestic violence. J. Clin. Nurs. 29, 2039–2049. doi: 10.1111/jocn.15296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breiman, L. (2001). Random forests. Mach. Learn. 45, 5–32. doi: 10.1023/A:1010933404324

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brennan, K. A., Wu, S., and Loev, J. (1998). “Adult romantic attachment and individual differences in attitudes toward physical contact in the context of adult romantic relationships,” in Attachment Theory and Close Relationships, eds J. A. Simpson and W. S. Rholes (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 394–428.

Google Scholar

Bundesministerium für Soziales, Gesundheit, Pflege und Konsumentenschutz [BSGPK] (2020). Coronavirus – Rechtliches [Coronavirus – Legal Aspects]. Available online at: https://www.sozialministerium.at/Informationen-zum-Coronavirus/Coronavirus—Rechtliches.html (accessed May 3, 2020).

Google Scholar

Butzer, B., and Campbell, L. (2008). Adult attachment, sexual satisfaction, and relationship satisfaction: a study of married couples. Pers. Relat. 15, 141–154. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2007.00189.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byers, E. S. (2005). Relationship satisfaction and sexual satisfaction: a longitudinal study of individuals in long-term relationships. J. Sex Res. 42, 113–118. doi: 10.1080/00224490509552264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cann, A., Norman, M. A., Welbourne, J. L., and Calhoun, L. G. (2008). Attachment styles, conflict styles and humour styles: interrelationships and associations with relationship satisfaction. Eur. J. Pers. 22, 131–146. doi: 10.1002/per.666

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cawley, G. C., and Talbot, N. L. (2010). On over-fitting in model selection and subsequent selection bias in performance evaluation. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 11, 2079–2107.

Google Scholar

Census and Statistics Department (2007). Marriage and divorce trends in Hong Kong, 1981 to 2006. Hong Kong Monthly Digest of Statistics. Available online at: https://www.statistics.gov.hk/pub/B70711FB2007XXXXB0100.pdf (accessed November, 2007).

Google Scholar

Chandra, S., Christensen, J., Mamelund, S. E., and Paneth, N. (2018). Short-term birth sequelae of the 1918–1920 influenza pandemic in the United States: state-level analysis. Am. J. Epidemiol. 187, 2585–2595. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwy153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cígler, H., Cvrčková, A., Daňsovác, P., Hašto, J., Charvát, M., Ježek, S., et al. (2019). Experiences in close relationships: české verze metod pro měření vazby vycházející z dotazníku ECR. E Psychol. 13, 57–74. doi: 10.29364/epsy.359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, N. L. (1996). Working models of attachment: implications for explanation, emotion, and behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71, 810–832. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.71.4.810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corcoran, K. O. C., and Mallinckrodt, B. (2000). Adult attachment, self-efficacy, perspective taking, and conflict resolution. J. Couns. Dev. 78, 473–483. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.2000.tb01931.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, D., Shaver, P. R., Widaman, K. F., Vernon, M. L., Follette, W. C., and Beitz, K. (2006). “I can’t get no satisfaction”: insecure attachment, inhibited sexual communication, and sexual dissatisfaction. Pers. Relat. 13, 465–483. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2006.00130.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ditzen, B., Neumann, I. D., Bodenmann, G., von Dawans, B., Turner, R. A., Ehlert, U., et al. (2007). Effects of different kinds of couple interaction on cortisol and heart rate responses to stress in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 565–574. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dziennik Ustaw (2020). poz. 433-697 [Journal of Laws of the Republic of Poland]. Available online at: http://isap.sejm.gov.pl/isap.nsf (accessed May 15, 2020).

Google Scholar

Eder, S. J., Steyrl, D., Stefanczyk, M. M., Pieniak, M., Martínez Molina, J., Pešout, O., et al. (2021). Predicting fear and perceived health during the COVID-19 pandemic using machine learning: a cross-national longitudinal study. PLoS One 16:e0247997. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0247997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enguix, S. (2020). Carrera por Instalar Hospitales de Campaña en las Ciudades más Afectadas. Available online at: http://hemeroteca.lavanguardia.com/preview/2020/03/22/pagina33/296617044/pdf.html?search=IFEMA (accessed March 22, 2020).

Google Scholar

Eurostat (2020). Population on 1 January (online data code:TPS00001). Available online at: https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/databrowser/view/tps00001/default/table?lang=en (accessed April 23, 2020).

Google Scholar

Evans, R. W., Hu, Y., and Zhao, Z. (2010). The fertility effect of catastrophe: US hurricane births. J. Popul. Econ. 23, 1–36. doi: 10.1007/s00148-008-0219-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falconier, M. K., Jackson, J. B., Hilpert, P., and Bodenmann, G. (2015). Dyadic coping and relationship satisfaction: a meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 42, 28–46. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fallis, E. E., Rehman, U. S., Woody, E. Z., and Purdon, C. (2016). The longitudinal association of relationship satisfaction and sexual satisfaction in long-term relationships. J. Fam. Psychol. 30, 822–831. doi: 10.1037/fam0000205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feeney, J. A. (1995). Adult attachment, coping style and health locus of control as predictors of health behaviour. Aust. J. Psychol. 47, 171–177. doi: 10.1080/00049539508257520

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feeney, J. A. (2002). Attachment, marital interaction, and relationship satisfaction: a diary study. Pers. Relat. 9, 39–55. doi: 10.1111/1475-6811.00003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feeney, J. A., and Noller, P. (1990). Attachment style as a predictor of adult romantic relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58, 281–291. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández-Fuertes, A. A., Orgaz, B., Fuertes, A., and Carcedo, R. (2011). La evaluación del apego romántico en adolescentes españoles: validación de la versión reducida del Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R). An. Psicol. 27, 827–833.

Google Scholar

Floyd, K., Boren, J. P., Hannawa, A. F., Hesse, C., McEwan, B., and Veksler, A. E. (2009). Kissing in marital and cohabiting relationships: effects on blood lipids, stress, and relationship satisfaction. West. J. Commun. 73, 113–133. doi: 10.1080/10570310902856071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraley, R. C., Hudson, N. W., Heffernan, M. E., and Segal, N. (2015). Are adult attachment styles categorical or dimensional? A taxometric analysis of general and relationship-specific attachment orientations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 109, 354–368. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., and Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item response theory analysis of self-report measures of adult attachment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 350–365. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geurts, P., Ernst, D., and Wehenkel, L. (2006). Extremely randomized trees. Mach. Learn. 63, 3–42. doi: 10.1007/s10994-006-6226-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillath, O., Mikulincer, M., Fitzsimons, G. M., Shaver, P. R., Schachner, D. A., and Bargh, J. A. (2006). Automatic activation of attachment-related goals. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 32, 1375–1388. doi: 10.1177/0146167206290339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gulledge, A. K., Gulledge, M. H., and Stahmannn, R. F. (2003). Romantic physical affection types and relationship satisfaction. Am. J. Fam. Ther. 31, 233–242. doi: 10.1080/01926180390201936

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hale, T., Webster, S., Petherick, A., Phillips, T., and Kira, B. (2020). Oxford COVID-19 Government Response Tracker, Blavatnik School of Government. Available online at: https://www.bsg.ox.ac.uk/research/research-projects/coronavirus-government-response-tracker (accessed May 18, 2020).

Google Scholar

Haslam, N., McGrath, M. J., Viechtbauer, W., and Kuppens, P. (2020). Dimensions over categories: a meta-analysis of taxometric research. Psychol. Med. 50, 1418–1432. doi: 10.1017/S003329172000183X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hassebrauck, M., and Fehr, B. (2002). Dimensions of relationship quality. Pers. Relat. 9, 253–270. doi: 10.1111/1475-6811.00017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hazan, C., and Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52, 511–524. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.3.511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heiman, J. R., Long, J. S., Smith, S. N., Fisher, W. A., Sand, M. S., and Rosen, R. C. (2011). Sexual satisfaction and relationship happiness in midlife and older couples in five countries. Arch. Sex. Behav. 40, 741–753. doi: 10.1007/s10508-010-9703-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hexel, M. (2003). Alexithymia and attachment style in relation to locus of control. Pers. Individ. Dif. 35, 1261–1270. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00333-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joel, S., Eastwick, P. W., Allison, C. J., Arriaga, X. B., Baker, Z. G., Bar-Kalifa, E., et al. (2020). Machine learning uncovers the most robust self-report predictors of relationship quality across 43 longitudinal couples studies. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 117, 19061–19071. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1917036117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., and Cook, W. L. (2006). Dyadic Data Analysis. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Kluwer, E. S., and Johnson, M. D. (2007). Conflict frequency and relationship quality across the transition to parenthood. J. Marriage Fam. 69, 1089–1106. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00434.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, A. M., Wong, J. G., McAlonan, G. M., Cheung, V., Cheung, C., Sham, P. C., et al. (2007). Stress and psychological distress among SARS survivors 1 year after the outbreak. Can. J. Psychiatry 52, 233–240. doi: 10.1177/070674370705200405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenger, K. A., Gordon, C. L., and Nguyen, S. P. (2019). A word to the wise: age matters when considering mindfulness in romantic relationships. Contemp. Fam. Ther. 41, 115–124. doi: 10.1007/s10591-018-9479-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loewenstein, G., Krishnamurti, T., Kopsic, J., and Mcdonald, D. (2015). Does increased sexual frequency enhance happiness? J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 116, 206–218. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2015.04.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lubiewska, K., Gogowska, K., Mickiewicz, K., Wyrzykowska, E., Winiewski, C., Izdebski, P., et al. (2016). Skala experience in close relationships-revised: struktura, Rzetelno oraz Skrócona Wersja Skali w Polskiej Próbie. Psychol. Rozwojowa 21, 49–63. doi: 10.4467/20843879PR.16.004.4793

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lubiewska, K., and Van de Vijver, F. J. (2020). Attachment categories or dimensions: the adult attachment scale across three generations in Poland. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 37, 233–259. doi: 10.1177/0265407519860594

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrae, R. R. (1990). Controlling neuroticism in the measurement of stress. Stress Med. 6, 237–241. doi: 10.1002/smi.2460060309

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNulty, J. K., Wenner, C. A., and Fisher, T. D. (2016). Longitudinal associations among relationship satisfaction, sexual satisfaction, and frequency of sex in early marriage. Arch. Sex. Behav. 45, 85–97. doi: 10.1007/s10508-014-0444-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McWilliams, L. A., and Bailey, S. J. (2010). Associations between adult attachment ratings and health conditions: evidence from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Health Psychol. 29, 446–453. doi: 10.1037/a0020061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meltzer, A. L., Makhanova, A., Hicks, L. L., French, J. E., McNulty, J. K., and Bradbury, T. N. (2017). Quantifying the sexual afterglow: the lingering benefits of sex and their implications for pair-bonded relationships. Psychol. Sci. 28, 587–598. doi: 10.1177/0956797617691361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michalak, B. (2020). Koronawirus a Wybory Prezydenckie. Czym Grozi Głosowanie Podczas Epidemii? [Coronavirus and the Presidential Elections. What’s the Risk of Voting During a Pandemic]. Available online at: http://archiwum.batory.org.pl/upload/files/Programy%20operacyjne/Forum%20Idei/B_Michalak_Koronawirus%20a%20wybory.pdf (accessed March 22, 2020).

Google Scholar

Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., Cowan, P. A., and Cowan, C. P. (2002). Attachment security in couple relationships: a systemic model and its implications for family dynamics. Fam. Process 41, 405–434. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2002.41309.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikulincer, M., Orbach, I., and Iavnieli, D. (1998). Adult attachment style and affect regulation: strategic variations in subjective self–other similarity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 75, 436. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.2.436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mills, L. B., and Huebner, E. S. (1998). A prospective study of personality characteristics, occupational stressors, and burnout among school psychology practitioners. J. Sch. Psychol. 36, 103–120. doi: 10.1016/S0022-4405(97)00053-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ministerio de la Presidencia (2020). Real Decreto 463/2020, de 14 de Marzo, Por el Que se Declara el Estado de Alarma para la Gestión de la Situación de Crisis Sanitaria Ocasionada Por el COVID-19. Available online at: https://www.boe.es/eli/es/rd/2020/03/14/463/con (accessed May 15, 2020).

Google Scholar

Ministry of Health of the Czech Republic [MHCR] (2020). COVID-19 epidemic in the Czech Republic [Onemocnìní Aktuálnì MZÈR]. Ministerstvo Zdravotnictví Èeské Republiky. Available online at: https://koronavirus.mzcr.cz/en/ (accessed May 18, 2020).

Google Scholar

Molenda, Z., Marchlewska, M., Górska, P., Michalski, P., Szczepańska, D., Furman, A., et al. (2020). Koronawirus w Polsce: Perspektywa Psychologii Społecznej [Coronavirus in Poland: Social Psychology Perspective]. Polish Academy of Sciences. Available online at: https://psych.pan.pl/pl/koronawirus-w-polsce-perspektywa-psychologii-spolecznej-raport/ (accessed May 15, 2020).

Google Scholar

Muise, A., Boudreau, G. K., and Rosen, N. O. (2017). Seeking connection versus avoiding disappointment: an experimental manipulation of approach and avoidance sexual goals and the implications for desire and satisfaction. J. Sex Res. 54, 296–307. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2016.1152455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muise, A., Schimmack, U., and Impett, E. A. (2016). Sexual frequency predicts greater well-being, but more is not always better. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 7, 295–302. doi: 10.1177/1948550615616462

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pedregosa, F., Varoquaux, G., Gramfort, A., Michel, V., Thirion, B., Grisel, O., et al. (2011). Scikit-learn: machine learning in Python. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 12, 2825–2830.

Google Scholar

Petherick, A., Hale, T., Phillips, T., and Webster, S. (2020). Variation in Government Responses to COVID-19. Blavatnik School Working Paper. Available online at: https://www.bsg.ox.ac.uk/research/publications/variation-government-responses-covid-19 (accessed May 6, 2020).

Google Scholar

Pieh, C., Oñ Rourke, T., Budimir, S., and Probst, T. (2020). Relationship quality and mental health during COVID-19 lockdown. PLoS One 15:e0238906. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2017). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Randall, A. K., and Bodenmann, G. (2009). The role of stress on close relationships and marital satisfaction. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29, 105–115. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Randall, A. K., and Bodenmann, G. (2017). Stress and its associations with relationship satisfaction. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 13, 96–106. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Redacción la Vanguardia (2020). Cronología del Estado de Alarma. Available online at: https://www.lavanguardia.com/vida/20200418/48595848780/cronologia-del-estado-de-alarma.html (accessed April 18, 2020).

Google Scholar

Redaccioì i ageÌncie la Vanguardia (2020). Madrid Guardarà Cadàvers en Una Pista de gel pel Collapse Funerari. Available online at: http://hemeroteca.lavanguardia.com/preview/2020/02/13/pagina24/297014519/pdf.html?search=Palacio%20de%20hielo (accessed March 24, 2020).

Google Scholar

Richmond, P., and Roehner, B. M. (2018a). Coupling between death spikes and birth troughs. Part 1: evidence. Phys. A Stat. Mech. Appl. 506, 97–111. doi: 10.1016/j.physa.2018.04.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richmond, P., and Roehner, B. M. (2018b). Coupling between death spikes and birth troughs. Part 2: comparative analysis of salient features. Phys. A Stat. Mech. Appl. 506, 88–96. doi: 10.1016/j.physa.2018.04.050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senchak, M., and Leonard, K. E. (1992). Attachment styles and marital adjustment among newlywed couples. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 9, 51–64. doi: 10.1177/0265407592091003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sibley, C. G., Fischer, R., and Liu, J. H. (2005). Reliability and validity of the revised experiences in close relationships (ECR-R) self-report measure of adult romantic attachment. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 31, 1524–1536. doi: 10.1177/0146167205276865

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Somerfield, M. R., and McCrae, R. R. (2000). Stress and coping research: methodological challenges, theoretical advances, and clinical applications. Am. Psychol. 55, 620–625. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.6.620

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

South, S. C., and Krueger, R. F. (2013). Marital satisfaction and physical health: evidence for an orchid effect. Psychol. Sci. 24, 373–378. doi: 10.1177/0956797612453116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sprecher, S. (2002). Sexual satisfaction in premarital relationships: associations with satisfaction, love, commitment, and stability. J. Sex Res. 39, 190–196. doi: 10.1080/00224490209552141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thornhill, R., Fincher, C. L., and Aran, D. (2009). Parasites, democratization, and the liberalization of values across contemporary countries. Biol. Rev. 84, 113–131. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2008.00062.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tibshirani, R. (1996). Regression shrinkage and selection via the lasso. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B 58, 267–288.

Google Scholar

Tybur, J. M., Inbar, Y., Güler, E., and Molho, C. (2015). Is the relationship between pathogen avoidance and ideological conservatism explained by sexual strategies? Evol. Hum. Behav. 36, 489–497. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.01.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

United Nations [UN] (2020). Policy Brief: The Impact of COVID-19 on Food Security and Nutrition. Available online at: https://reliefweb.int/sites/reliefweb.int/files/resources/sg_policy_brief_on_covid_impact_on_food_security.pdf (accessed June, 2020).

Google Scholar

Van Anders, S. M., Edelstein, R. S., Wade, R. M., and Samples-Steele, C. R. (2013). Descriptive experiences and sexual vs. nurturant aspects of cuddling between adult romantic partners. Arch. Sex. Behav. 42, 553–560. doi: 10.1007/s10508-012-0014-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization [WHO] (2020). Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19) Pandemic. Available online at: https://www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019 (accessed July, 2020).

Google Scholar

Yehuda, R., Lehrner, A., and Rosenbaum, T. Y. (2015). PTSD and sexual dysfunction in men and women. J. Sex. Med. 12, 1107–1119. doi: 10.1111/jsm.12856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zakin, G., Solomon, Z., and Neria, Y. (2003). Hardiness, attachment style, and long term psychological distress among Israeli POWs and combat veterans. Pers. Individ. Dif. 34, 819–829. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00073-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: attachment style, relationship quality, COVID-19, intimate relationships, machine learning, pair bond

Citation: Eder SJ, Nicholson AA, Stefanczyk MM, Pieniak M, Martínez-Molina J, Pešout O, Binter J, Smela P, Scharnowski F and Steyrl D (2021) Securing Your Relationship: Quality of Intimate Relationships During the COVID-19 Pandemic Can Be Predicted by Attachment Style. Front. Psychol. 12:647956. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.647956

Received: 30 December 2020; Accepted: 28 June 2021;
Published: 21 July 2021.

Edited by:

Willem J. M. I. Verbeke, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Jacques J. D. M. Van Lankveld, Open University of the Netherlands, Netherlands
Valeria Saladino, University of Cassino, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Eder, Nicholson, Stefanczyk, Pieniak, Martínez-Molina, Pešout, Binter, Smela, Scharnowski and Steyrl. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stephanie J. Eder, c3RlcGhhbmllLmVkZXJAdW5pdmllLmFjLmF0

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.