Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 18 March 2021
Sec. Psychopathology

The Negative Association Between Positive Psychological Wellbeing and Loss Aversion

\nIbuki KoanIbuki KoanTakumi NakagawaTakumi NakagawaChong Chen
Chong Chen*Toshio MatsubaraToshio MatsubaraHuijie LeiHuijie LeiKosuke HagiwaraKosuke HagiwaraMasako HirotsuMasako HirotsuHirotaka YamagataHirotaka YamagataShin NakagawaShin Nakagawa
  • Division of Neuropsychiatry, Department of Neuroscience, Yamaguchi University Graduate School of Medicine, Ube, Japan

When making decisions, people tend to overweigh the impact of losses compared to gains, a phenomenon known as loss aversion (LA). A moderate amount of LA may be adaptive as it is necessary for protecting oneself from danger. However, excessive LA may leave people few opportunities and ultimately lead to suboptimal outcomes. Despite frequent reports of elevated LA in specific populations such as patients with depression, little is known about what psychological characteristics are associated with the tendency of LA. Based on the neurobiological studies of LA, we hypothesized that positive psychological wellbeing may be negatively associated with people's tendency of LA. In the present study, we set out to test this hypothesis in a sample of young adults. We evaluated LA using a decision-making task in which subjects were asked to decide whether to accept or reject a series of coin-toss gambles. Our results revealed that individuals with more advanced personal growth as assessed by the Ryff's Psychological Well-being Inventory showed reduced LA. To our knowledge, this is the first report demonstrating an association between positive psychological wellbeing and LA. These findings suggest that personal growth might be employed as interventional targets for correcting excessive LA in vulnerable populations.

Introduction

When making decisions, people tend to overweigh the impact of losses compared to gains (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979). For instance, when presented with gambles with equal chances of winning and losing, in order to accept a gamble, people demand almost twice the amount of potential gains compared to losses (Tom et al., 2007). This phenomenon, known as loss aversion (LA), has been an extensively studied decision bias (Barberis, 2013, but see Yechiam and Hochman, 2013; Gal and Rucker, 2018; Yechiam, 2019 for critiques of the concept).

Generally speaking, a moderate amount of LA may be adaptive and preferred since it is necessary for protecting oneself from danger (Li et al., 2012). In contrast, in certain situations, excessive LA may lead to negative, undesirable consequences. Excessive LA may prevent people from exploring the environment and leave people few potentially profitable opportunities. Previous research has indicated elevated LA in individuals with childhood trauma (Huh et al., 2016), depression (Chandrasekhar Pammi et al., 2015; Huh et al., 2016; Engelmann et al., 2017) (in particular depressed patients with a history of suicide attempt, see Baek et al., 2017), and obsessive-compulsive disorder (Sip et al., 2018). Healthy subjects also show increased LA in response to negative stimuli such as unpleasant odors (Stancak et al., 2015) or fearful faces (Schulreich et al., 2016).

Despite these findings, little is known about what psychological characteristics predict people's tendency of LA. This knowledge would be rather valuable, given that the identified psychological characteristics may be employed as interventional targets for correcting excessive LA in vulnerable populations. It may also help us achieve a better understanding of the psychological mechanism of LA.

Based on the neurobiological studies of LA, we hypothesized that positive psychological wellbeing may be negatively associated with people's tendency of LA. Specifically, the amygdala and the striatum have been suggested as the neural substrate of LA. For instance, the possibility of financial losses activates the amygdala and striatum (Krawczyk and D'Esposito, 2013) and increases the functional connectivity between these two brain areas (Charpentier et al., 2015). Furthermore, neural LA (i.e., the slope of the decrease in activation for increasing losses being greater than the slope of the increase in activation for increasing gains) has been detected in the ventral striatum (Tom et al., 2007). In contrast, bilateral amygdala lesions eliminate behavioral LA (De Martino et al., 2010).

Importantly, a previous study reported that when confronted with potentially aversive information, individuals with more positive psychological wellbeing, as evaluated by the Ryff's Psychological Well-being Inventory (Ryff, 1989), show reduced activation in the amygdala (Van Reekum et al., 2007). Meanwhile, they also show increased activation in the ventral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). The ventral ACC and its closely related ventromedial prefrontal cortex are believed to exert cognitive control and emotion regulation through modulating the activation of the amygdala and ventral striatum (Ochsner et al., 2012; Jhang et al., 2018). Therefore, it may be speculated that people with more positive psychological wellbeing are better able to inhibit their activation of the amygdala and striatum in response to potential losses, therefore demonstrating reduced LA. This speculation is consistent with a recent report that individuals with greater psychological wellbeing use more rational, adaptive decision-making strategies (Páez-Gallego et al., 2020). In the present study, we set out to investigate the association between LA and psychological wellbeing in a sample of healthy young adults.

Materials and Methods

Participants

This research was part of an ongoing larger study, the aim of which was to predict future mental health status in healthy young adults using high-level cognitive functions. The study was carried out in accordance with the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Yamaguchi University Hospital. Subjects were recruited via posters placed on campus and department homepage. Data of 33 subjects collected at the baseline of the study for the first author's thesis research were used for the analysis here. Seven subjects had to be excluded due to too many gamble acceptances (95.30 ± 2.40%, n = 6) or zero gamble acceptance (n = 1) in the LA task, which hinders reliable estimation of the LA coefficient (see below). The data of the remaining 26 subjects were used for the present analysis. Notably, including six of the excluded subjects for whom we were able to obtain an estimated LA coefficient (reliability aside), the negative association between personal growth and LA coefficient identified in this study remained unchanged (n = 32; for Pearson correlation: r = −0.319, p = 0.038, one-tailed test; for linear regression with all six domains of psychological wellbeing as the independent variables and LA coefficient as the dependent variable, only personal growth was significant and the unstandardized coefficient B = −0.049, p = 0.034, one-sided test); for details of this data, please see the Supplementary Figure 1.

All subjects agreed to participate in this study and provided written informed consent after receiving a detailed explanation of the study. Subjects were well-informed that they were free to decline to participate and withdraw from the study at any point after agreeing to participate if they wished. They were also well-informed that their names would be withheld when data is reported. The inclusion criteria were being 20–39 years old at the time of the visit and the exclusion criteria were (1) having any self-reported mental health disorders, (2) receiving medical examinations due to suspicion of any mental health disorders, (3) being suspected of mental health disorders by the research staff and subsequently diagnosed as having a mental health disorder by the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview conducted by a psychiatrist, or (4) being unable to perform the laboratory tests and answer the questionnaires for this study due to severe physical conditions or other reasons. No participant was excluded owing to meeting any of the exclusion criteria.

Measures and Loss Aversion Task

Subjects first answered questions about their demographic characteristics, including gender, age, occupation, education level, and socioeconomic status such as their personal income (for students, this is their monthly allowance plus their part-time earnings if any), whether they had made a student loan, their parents' education levels and family income.

We used an established experimental paradigm (Tom et al., 2007; De Martino et al., 2010) to evaluate LA. The task was programmed using MATLAB R2018b (MathWorks) and Psychtoolbox 3 (http://psychtoolbox.org/). In this task, subjects were asked to decide whether to accept or reject a series of 64 coin-toss gambles that offered equal (50%) chances of gaining or losing different amounts of money (Figure 1). If the head of the coin faced up, the subject would win a variable amount of money (shown in blue); if the tail faced up, the subject would lose a variable amount of money (shown in red). Gambles included 64 different pairs of combinations, which were created by randomly sampling without replacement from a matrix with possible gambles consisting of eight levels of gains (from 2000 to 4800 incremented in steps of 400, all JPY) and eight levels of losses (from 1000 to 2400 incremented in steps of 200). The purpose of employing this asymmetric gain/loss matrix was to be more sensitive around the typical indifference points (i.e., losses are weighted about twice as strongly as gains, Tom et al., 2007; He et al., 2010; Gelskov et al., 2016) to efficiently capture subjects' LA tendency. Before the task, subjects practiced four trials to get familiar with the task and response keys.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Illustration of a typical trial. On each trial, subjects had to decide whether to accept or reject a coin-toss gamble that offered equal (50%) chances of gaining or losing different amounts of money.

Subjects were told that they would receive about 3,000 JPY (roughly $30 USD) for participating in this research and they were asked to imagine that the payment would be adjusted based on their choices (they were fully aware that this would not be actually implemented): at the end of the experiment one trial would be randomly selected and subjects' actual decision in that trial would be used to adjust their payment. Specifically, if they accepted the gamble in that trial, the gamble would be executed. If the head faced up, they would receive an additional gain equal to the amount shown together with the head in that trial. However, if the tail faced up, the amount shown along with the tail in that trial would be subtracted from their original payment of 3,000 JPY. If they rejected the gamble in that trial, their payment 3,000 JPY would remain unchanged.

In this study, each subject received a fixed payment of ~3,000 JPY for participating in this study. We did not actually implement the performance-adjusted payment based on a randomly selected trial but instead asked subjects to imagine vividly that kind of payment being executed. It was because our final objective was to develop useful tools for predicting mental health problems in a public health setting: it is impossible to pay people a certain amount of money based on their choices in any public health predictive tools. Meanwhile, previous research has shown that people's decision-making with hypothetic rewards highly resembles that with real rewards (Kühberger et al., 2002; Madden et al., 2003) (See Hertwig and Ortmann, 2001 for an extensive discussion of this issue).

We calculated the LA coefficient (λ) for each subject using the following equation:

λ=-βlossβwin    (1)

here, βloss and βwin are the unstandardized regression coefficients for the loss and gain variables, respectively, estimated from logistic regression for each subject to predict their choices of acceptance or rejection (De Martino et al., 2010; Charpentier et al., 2015). λ = 1 indicates loss neutral, λ > 1 loss averse, and λ < 1 gain seeking. The logistic regression was estimated using the fitglm function in MATLAB R2018b. Bigger λ reflects higher LA. Furthermore, to investigate whether higher LA led to poor performance (i.e., reward outcomes) in the present study, following the instruction explained to subjects, we randomly selected one trial for each subject and calculated their adjusted payment (see above). We repeated this process 100 times and used the mean adjusted payment for each subject as their performance reward.

We used the Japanese version (Kitamura et al., 2004) of the extensively employed 84-item Ryff's Psychological Well-being Inventory (Ryff, 1989). This scale assesses six domains of psychological wellbeing: autonomy, environmental mastery, personal growth, positive relations with others, purpose in life, and self-acceptance. Each domain was measured with 14 items of statements rated on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (“strongly disagree”) to 6 (“strongly agree”). Autonomy evaluates the degree of self-determination and independence. Environmental mastery measures the sense of mastery and competence in managing one's environment. Personal growth measures the feeling of continued psychological development as well as the tendency to pursue such development. The domain of positive relations with others evaluates to what extent an individual has warm, satisfying, and trusting relationships with others. Purpose in life evaluates to what extent one has goals in life and a sense of direction. Self-acceptance evaluates to what extent one possesses a positive attitude toward the self.

Personality, Mood, and Emotion Regulation Measures

We also considered the potential involvement of personality, mood, and emotion regulation in the association between psychological wellbeing and LA. We used the Behavioral Inhibition System and Behavioral Activation System scales (BIS/BAS, Carver and White, 1994; Takahashi et al., 2007) to assess four biological personality traits of BIS, drive, fun seeking, and reward responsiveness. The BIS measures individuals' response to aversive stimuli such as anxiety, fear, and worry. We used the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II, Beck et al., 1996; Kojima and Furukawa, 2003) to assess depressive symptoms that occurred within the past 2 weeks and the Y-1 subscale of the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI, Spielberger et al., 1983; Hidano et al., 2000) to assess state anxiety. Lastly, we used the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ, Gross and John, 2003; Yoshizu et al., 2013) to assess individual differences in their use of two emotion regulation strategies, reappraisal and suppression.

Data Analysis

The statistical analysis was conducted with IBM SPSS Statistics 26.0 and MATLAB R2018b. As already described, we fitted a logistic regression to estimate each subject's LA coefficient λ. We examined the relation of λ with psychological wellbeing and other measures using Pearson correlation, Student's t-test, and linear regression. For data analysis, income was coded as 1 for ~50,000JPY (upper bound not included), 2 for 50,000~100,000JPY, 3 for 100,000~150,000JPY, and 4 for 150,000~200,000JPY. Parents' education level was coded as 1 for elementary school level, 2 for junior high school level, 3 for senior high school level, 4 for vocational school level, 5 for undergraduate level, 6 for master's level, and 7 for doctorate level. Family income (annual) was coded as 1 for ~2,000,000 JPY (upper bound not included), 2 for 2,000,000~4,000,000 JPY, 3 for 4,000,000~6,000,000 JPY, 4 for 6,000,000~8,000,000 JPY, 5 for 8,000,000~10,000,000 JPY, 6 for 10,000,000~15,000,000 JPY, 7 for 15,000,000~20,000,000 JPY, and 8 for 20,000,000~ JPY. P < 0.05 was considered statistically significant. Since we predicted a negative association between psychological wellbeing and LA, one-tailed tests were conducted. Statistical power analysis was conducted for linear regression models and single regression coefficients using G*Power version 3.1.9.7 (Faul et al., 2009).

Results

The sample consisted of 26 subjects, with a mean age of 23.56 years (SD 3.06 years). The mean score of each measure is shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The score of each measure and their association with λ.

On average, subjects accepted 57.86 ± 23.05% of the gambles. We collapsed the 8 by 8 gain/loss combinations into a 4 by 4 matrix and calculated the probability of gamble acceptance at each cell (which thus consists of four gain/loss combinations) for each subject. The color-coded heatmaps depicting the probability of gamble acceptance averaged across all subjects are shown in Figure 2. As can be seen, on average, subjects were indifferent to gambles (indicated in green colored cells) in which the potential gain was somewhat less than twice the amount of the potential loss. We fitted a logistic regression model for each subject to estimate their loss aversion coefficient λ. On average, the subjects had a mean λ of 1.93 (SD = 0.68, range 1.17–3.87), comparable to previous reports (Tom et al., 2007; He et al., 2010; Gelskov et al., 2016).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Probability of gamble acceptance. Color-coded heatmaps depicting the probability of gamble acceptance at each level of gain/loss within that cell averaged across all subjects (n = 26). One cell consists of four potential gain/loss combinations. Red indicates high willingness to accept the gamble, and blue indicates low willingness to accept the gamble.

As we were interested in what domain(s) of psychological wellbeing might be associated with LA, we next examined the correlation between the six domains of psychological wellbeing and λ (Table 1). Specifically, we found three domains that were negatively correlated with λ, environmental mastery (r = −0.464, p = 0.008, one-tailed test), personal growth (r = −0.592, p = 0.001, one-tailed test), and purpose in life (r = −0.357, p = 0.037, one-tailed test), with the first two domains remaining significant after Bonferroni correction (0.05/6).

To confirm the association, we included all six domains in a linear regression to predict λ (model 1). The model was significant [F(6, 19) = 3.118, p = 0.027, R2 = 0.496] and only personal growth (B = −0.073, p = 0.0045, one-tailed test) and autonomy (B = 0.031, p = 0.045, one-tailed test) were significant in predicting λ. The statistical power for this linear regression (model 1) was 0.920. The statistical power for the regression coefficients of personal growth and autonomy were 0.950 and 0.646, respectively.

To rule out the confounding of demographic factors that may affect decision-making and LA, we conducted a second linear regression analysis using personal growth and autonomy to predict λ, adding gender, age, personal income, student loan, father's education, mother's education, and family income as covariates (model 2). The model remained significant [F(9, 15) = 2.584, p = 0.050, R2 = 0.608] and only personal growth was significant in predicting λ (B = −0.062, p = 0.0015, one-tailed test). The statistical power for this linear regression (model 2) was 0.961. Thus, across model 1 and 2, a negative association between personal growth and λ was consistently identified with a high statistical power. A scatter plot of this association is shown in Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Scatter plot (with a regression line) of the association between personal growth and LA coefficient λ. Each circle represents a subject. n = 26.

To investigate the potential involvement of personality, mood, and emotion regulation in the association between personal growth and LA, we conducted three additional linear regression models using personal growth to predict λ, with model 3 adding personality measures (i.e., drive, fun seeking, reward responsiveness, and BIS) as covariates, model 4 adding mood measures (i.e., depression and anxiety) as covariates, and model 5 adding emotion regulation measures (i.e., reappraisal and suppression) as covariates. All three models remained significant [F(5, 20) = 3.137, p = 0.030, R2 = 0.440 for model 3; F(3, 22) = 4.068, p = 0.019, R2 = 0.357 for model 4; F(3, 22) = 4.089, p = 0.019, R2 = 0.358 for model 5] and in all three models, only personal growth significantly predicted λ (B = −0.041, p = 0.005 for model 3; B = −0.045, p = 0.0015 for model 4; B = −0.042, p = 0.003 for model 5; all one-tailed tests). The statistical power for model 3–5 were 0.881, 0.842, and 0.844, respectively.

Finally, to investigate whether excessive LA is associated with poor reward outcomes, we conducted a correlation analysis between LA coefficient λ and performance reward. The correlation was significant (r = −0.767, p = 0.000) such that greater LA was associated with fewer performance reward (Figure 4). We further confirmed that among the six dimensions of psychological wellbeing, only personal growth was significantly correlated with performance reward (r = 0.392, p = 0.024, one-tailed test) and this correlation disappeared after partialling out λ (r = −0.120, P = 0.284, one-tailed test). This indicates that the positive association between personal growth and performance reward was driven by LA.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Scatter plot (with a regression line) of the association between LA coefficient λ and performance reward (JPY). Each circle represents a subject. n = 26.

Discussion

In line with our hypothesis, in a sample of healthy young adults, we found one specific domain of psychological wellbeing, personal growth, is negatively associated with LA. More advanced personal growth is associated with reduced LA. This association stayed stable even after controlling demographic factors and considering the influence of personality, mood, and emotion regulation measures. Furthermore, greater LA is associated with fewer performance reward in our task. Individuals with higher personal growth have reduced LA, and this gains them more performance reward. To our knowledge, this is the first report demonstrating an association between positive psychological wellbeing measures and LA.

Here, personal growth measures an individual's sense of continued psychological development (or improvement) as well as the tendency to pursue such development, for instance, through having new experiences that challenge oneself or broaden one's horizons (Ryff, 1989). It is possible that this kind of seeing growth in oneself and being open to new experiences restrains one's tendency to overweigh the impact of losses compared to gains. This speculation is consistent with the observation that individuals with more positive psychological wellbeing including personal growth show increased activation in the ventral ACC and reduced activation in the amygdala when confronted with potentially aversive information (Van Reekum et al., 2007). In other words, these individuals demonstrate typical brain activations that indicate reduced LA.

While we observed such an association, the precise underlying mechanism remains uninvestigated. In contrast to the original LA theory that emphasizes a decision mechanism of overweighing losses over gains, several authors have recently argued that LA is driven by attention rather than decision mechanisms (e.g., Yechiam and Hochman, 2013; Lejarraga et al., 2019). It remains for future studies to probe whether individuals with more advanced personal growth are better able to balance their attention toward potential losses and gains, or they have a more balanced decision strategy while considering losses and gains simultaneously. Still, a third possibility might be that individuals with more advanced personal growth are better able to use a secondary, positive reappraisal strategy during decision-making (Balzarotti et al., 2016). This is in line with another recent report that when explicitly instructed to use a “regulate” strategy by considering each choice in a bigger context, people show reduced LA accompanied by increased activation in the PFC and reduced activation in the amygdala (Sokol-Hessner et al., 2013).

The recent positive psychology movement (e.g., Seligman, 2012) has suggested the possibility of continued psychological development and growth. It is stimulating to see whether the improvement of such personal growth can result in reduced LA, which may be further employed as interventional targets for correcting excessive LA in vulnerable populations.

A limitation of the present study is that we used a small sample of young healthy participants and excluded seven subjects in our main analysis whose LA could not be reliably estimated. Although the statistical power of the association we identified was high, further investigation is required to confirm the association in samples with a bigger sample size and broader age range as well as clinical samples with known excessive LA. Another limitation is that we used just mixed gambles from an asymmetric gain/loss matrix and did not include gain-only gambles that allow us to disentangle the specific effect of LA from that of diminishing sensitivity to payoff magnitude (Yechiam and Hochman, 2013; Walasek and Stewart, 2015) or more general risk-aversion (e.g., De Martino et al., 2010; Sokol-Hessner et al., 2013). Future research is required to overcome this limitation and provide further mechanical accounts of the association we found and the association between self-reported happiness and risk-taking behavior in situations involving losses identified by a previous study (Yechiam et al., 2016).

Although it has been reported that gender (Schmidt and Traub, 2002) and culture (Wang et al., 2017) may affect loss aversion, we did not find such a gender difference in loss aversion in our small Japanese sample. Future well-designed studies may investigate whether such gender and cultural differences exist in loss aversion and in the association between loss aversion and personal growth.

In summary, in a sample of healthy young adults, we found that individuals with more advanced personal growth as evaluated by the Ryff's Psychological Well-being Inventory show reduced LA. To our knowledge, this is the first report demonstrating an association between positive psychological wellbeing and LA. These findings suggest that personal growth might be employed as interventional targets for correcting excessive LA in vulnerable populations.

Data Availability Statement

The dataset that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board of Yamaguchi University Hospital. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

CC, HL, and SN: conceptualization and methodology. IK, TN, CC, TM, KH, and MH: investigation. IK and CC: formal analysis and writing—original draft preparation. IK, TN, CC, TM, HL, KH, MH, HY, and SN: writing—review and editing. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by grants from JSPS KAKENHI (Grant Numbers 19K17063) and Yamaguchi University Fund to CC.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Alexander Genauck for his assistance on model fitting.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.641340/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Figure 1. Scatter plot (with a regression line) of the association between personal growth and LA coefficient λ without removing subjects with high gamble acceptances. Each circle represents a subject. n = 32. The mean of λ is 1.75 (SD 0.72) and the median 1.57 (IQR 1.22–2.10).

References

Baek, K., Kwon, J., Chae, J. H., Chung, Y. A., Kralik, J. D., Min, J. A., et al. (2017). Heightened aversion to risk and loss in depressed patients with a suicide attempt history. Sci. Rep. 7:11228. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-10541-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balzarotti, S., Biassoni, F., Villani, D., Prunas, A., and Velotti, P. (2016). Individual differences in cognitive emotion regulation: Implications for subjective and psychological well-being. J. Happiness Stud. 17, 125–143. doi: 10.1007/s10902-014-9587-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barberis, N. C. (2013). Thirty years of prospect theory in economics: a review and assessment. J. Econ. Perspect. 27, 173–196. doi: 10.1257/jep.27.1.173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., Ball, R., and Ranieri, W. F. (1996). Comparison of beck depression inventories-IA and-II in psychiatric outpatients. J. Pers. Assess. 67, 588–597. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carver, C. S., and White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67:319. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chandrasekhar Pammi, V. S., Pillai Geethabhavan Rajesh, P., Kesavadas, C., Rappai Mary, P., Seema, S., Radhakrishnan, A., et al. (2015). Neural loss aversion differences between depression patients and healthy individuals: a functional MRI investigation. Neuroradiol. J. 28, 97–105. doi: 10.1177/1971400915576670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charpentier, C. J., Martino, B. D., Sim, A. L., Sharot, T., and Roiser, J. P. (2015). Emotion-induced loss aversion and striatal-amygdala coupling in low-anxious individuals. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 569–579. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Martino, B., Camerer, C. F., and Adolphs, R. (2010). Amygdala damage eliminates monetary loss aversion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 3788–3792. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910230107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engelmann, J. B., Berns, G. S., and Dunlop, B. W. (2017). Hyper-responsivity to losses in the anterior insula during economic choice scales with depression severity. Psychol. Med. 47, 2879–2891. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717001428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., and Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gal, D., and Rucker, D. D. (2018). The loss of loss aversion: will it loom larger than its gain? J. Consum. Psychol. 28, 497–516. doi: 10.1002/jcpy.1047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelskov, S. V., Madsen, K. H., Ramsøy, T. Z., and Siebner, H. R. (2016). Aberrant neural signatures of decision-making: pathological gamblers display cortico-striatal hypersensitivity to extreme gambles. NeuroImage 128, 342–352. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J. J., and John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85:348. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, Q., Xue, G., Chen, C., Lu, Z., Dong, Q., Lei, X., et al. (2010). Serotonin transporter gene-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) influences decision making under ambiguity and risk in a large Chinese sample. Neuropharmacology 59, 518–526. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2010.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertwig, R., and Ortmann, A. (2001). Experimental practices in economics: a methodological challenge for psychologists? Behav. Brain Sci. 24, 383–403. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01004149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hidano, T., Fukuda, M., Iwawaki, S., Soga, Y., and Spielberger, C. D. (2000). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory-JYZ. Tokyo: Jitsumukyoiku-Shuppan Co., Ltd.

Google Scholar

Huh, H. J., Baek, K., Kwon, J. H., Jeong, J., and Chae, J. H. (2016). Impact of childhood trauma and cognitive emotion regulation strategies on risk-aversive and loss-aversive patterns of decision-making in patients with depression. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 21, 447–461. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2016.1230053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jhang, J., Lee, H., Kang, M. S., Lee, H. S., Park, H., and Han, J. H. (2018). Anterior cingulate cortex and its input to the basolateral amygdala control innate fear response. Nat. Commun. 9, 1–16. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-05090-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47, 263–291. doi: 10.2307/1914185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Kitamura, T., Kishida, Y., Gatayama, R., Matsuoka, T., Miura, S., and Yamabe, K. (2004). Ryff's psychological well-being inventory: factorial structure and life history correlates among Japanese University students. Psychol. Rep. 94, 83–103. doi: 10.2466/pr0.94.1.83-103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kojima, M., and Furukawa, T. (2003). Manual for the Japanese Version of Beck Depression Inventory, 2nd Edn. Tokyo: Nihon Bunka Kagakusha Co., Ltd.

Krawczyk, D. C., and D'Esposito, M. (2013). Modulation of working memory function by motivation through loss-aversion. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 762–774. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kühberger, A., Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M., and Perner, J. (2002). Framing decisions: hypothetical and real. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 89, 1162–1175. doi: 10.1016/S0749-5978(02)00021-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lejarraga, T., Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M., Pachur, T., and Hertwig, R. (2019). The attention–aversion gap: how allocation of attention relates to loss aversion. Evol. Hum. Behav. 40, 457–469. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2019.05.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y. J., Kenrick, D. T., Griskevicius, V., and Neuberg, S. L. (2012). Economic decision biases in evolutionary perspective: how mating and self-protection motives alter loss aversion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 102, 550–561. doi: 10.1037/a0025844

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Madden, G. J., Begotka, A. M., Raiff, B. R., and Kastern, L. L. (2003). Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 11:139. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.11.2.139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., Silvers, J. A., and Buhle, J. T. (2012). Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation: a synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1251:E1. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06751.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Páez-Gallego, J., Gallardo-López, J. A., López-Noguero, F., and Rodrigo-Moriche, M. P. (2020). Analysis of the relationship between psychological well-being and decision making in adolescent students. Front. Psychol. 11:1195. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57:1069. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, U., and Traub, S. (2002). An experimental test of loss aversion. J. Risk Uncertain. 25, 233–249. doi: 10.1023/A:1020923921649

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulreich, S., Gerhardt, H., and Heekeren, H. R. (2016). Incidental fear cues increase monetary loss aversion. Emotion 16:402. doi: 10.1037/emo0000124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E. (2012). Flourish: A Visionary New Understanding of Happiness and Well-Being. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.

Google Scholar

Sip, K. E., Gonzalez, R., Taylor, S. F., and Stern, E. R. (2018). Increased loss aversion in unmedicated patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. Front. Psychiatry 8:309. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sokol-Hessner, P., Camerer, C. F., and Phelps, E. A. (2013). Emotion regulation reduces loss aversion and decreases amygdala responses to losses. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 341–350. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Gorssuch, R. L., Lushene, P. R., Vagg, P. R., and Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar

Stancak, A., Xie, Y., Fallon, N., Bulsing, P., Giesbrecht, T., Thomas, A., et al. (2015). Unpleasant odors increase aversion to monetary losses. Biol. Psychol. 107, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, Y., Yamagata, S., Kijima, N., Shigemasu, K., Ono, Y., and Ando, J. (2007). Gray's temperament model: development of Japanese version of BIS/BAS scales and a behavior genetic investigation using the twin method. Jpn. J. Pers. 15, 276–289. doi: 10.2132/personality.15.276

CrossRef Full Text

Tom, S. M., Fox, C. R., Trepel, C., and Poldrack, R. A. (2007). The neural basis of loss aversion in decision-making under risk. Science 315, 515–518. doi: 10.1126/science.1134239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Reekum, C. M., Urry, H. L., Johnstone, T., Thurow, M. E., Frye, C. J., Jackson, C. A., et al. (2007). Individual differences in amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex activity are associated with evaluation speed and psychological well-being. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 237–248. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.2.237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walasek, L., and Stewart, N. (2015). How to make loss aversion disappear and reverse: tests of the decision by sampling origin of loss aversion. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 144:7. doi: 10.1037/xge0000039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M., Rieger, M. O., and Hens, T. (2017). The impact of culture on loss aversion. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 30, 270–281. doi: 10.1002/bdm.1941

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yechiam, E. (2019). Acceptable losses: the debatable origins of loss aversion. Psychol. Res. 83, 1327–1339. doi: 10.1007/s00426-018-1013-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yechiam, E., and Hochman, G. (2013). Losses as modulators of attention: review and analysis of the unique effects of losses over gains. Psychol. Bull. 139:497. doi: 10.1037/a0029383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yechiam, E., Telpaz, A., Krupenia, S., and Rafaeli, A. (2016). Unhappiness intensifies the avoidance of frequent losses while happiness overcomes it. Front. Psychol. 7:1703. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshizu, J., Sekiguchi, R., and Amemiya, T. (2013). Development of a Japanese version of emotion regulation questionnaire. Jpn J. Res. Emot. 20, 56–62. doi: 10.4092/jsre.20.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: decision making, gamble, loss aversion, psychological wellbeing, personal growth, positive psychology

Citation: Koan I, Nakagawa T, Chen C, Matsubara T, Lei H, Hagiwara K, Hirotsu M, Yamagata H and Nakagawa S (2021) The Negative Association Between Positive Psychological Wellbeing and Loss Aversion. Front. Psychol. 12:641340. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.641340

Received: 15 December 2020; Accepted: 22 February 2021;
Published: 18 March 2021.

Edited by:

Susana Jiménez-Murcia, Bellvitge University Hospital, Spain

Reviewed by:

Paula Jauregui, University of Deusto, Spain
Anne Sauvaget, University Hospital of Nantes, France

Copyright © 2021 Koan, Nakagawa, Chen, Matsubara, Lei, Hagiwara, Hirotsu, Yamagata and Nakagawa. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chong Chen, cchen@yamaguchi-u.ac.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.