Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 09 March 2021
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Autobiographical Memory, Narrative Identity, and Mental Health View all 22 articles

Mental Time Travel in Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder: Current Gaps and Future Directions

  • 1Department of Psychology, The New School for Social Research, New York, NY, United States
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, United States

Introduction

Exposure to potentially traumatic events is a common experience throughout the world. Although many people typically do not experience mental health issues following such events, a significant minority of individuals will develop a psychiatric disorder. Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is the most frequent negative mental health outcome of traumatic events (e.g., Hoge et al., 2004) and is associated with considerable functional impairment (e.g., Silverstein et al., 2018). Although recent decades have been marked by important advances in PTSD treatments, a subset of patients still does not respond to first-line pharmacotherapy or extant evidence-based psychotherapies (Lancaster et al., 2016), underscoring the need for new treatment development. Elucidation of the neurocognitive processes underlying the pathogenesis and maintenance of PTSD may represent a critical step toward developing more optimal treatments.

Among the neurocognitive processes that have been shown to be altered in individuals with PTSD are autobiographical memories (AMs), a class of self-referential, long-term memories comprised of both episodic and semantic details. AMs may be retrieved deliberately (voluntarily) or unexpectedly (involuntarily). AMs are critical for wellbeing and functioning, as they are implicated in numerous clinically-relevant processes, including problem-solving; the creation of self-concept; and the formation and maintenance of social-bonds (Bluck et al., 2005)— all processes that are dysregulated in PTSD (e.g., Lord et al., 2020). Given the integral role of AM in PTSD, it is unsurprising that numerous studies have examined AM alterations related to this disorder. One such alteration that has received considerable attention is known as the overgeneral memory (OGM), a phenomenon in which individuals demonstrate difficulty recalling discreet, discontinuous autobiographical events. That is, an AM marked by reduced specificity, which may take place repeatedly or over the course of several days. OGM has been shown to be a predictor of posttraumatic symptoms (Moore and Zoellner, 2007) and an effective target for therapy. Indeed, AM specificity training has been shown to achieve PTSD symptom reduction in half the number of sessions required using standard cognitive processing therapy treatments (e.g., Maxwell, 2016). In addition to OGM, other AM alterations have been implicated in PTSD, including changes in the emotional intensity associated with the recollection of a particular type of memory (e.g., Rubin, 2010; Niziurski et al., 2017); variations in the availability and coherence of different memory types (e.g., Rubin et al., 2008); increases in intrusive, emotionally-laden memories and prospections (e.g., Berntsen and Rubin, 2015); and greater centrality of traumatic experiences to self-identity (e.g., Berntsen and Rubin, 2006).

An important function of the episodic memory system is the construction of prospective memories, also known as episodic future-thinking (EFT). Through a phenomenon known as mental time travel (MTT; Tulving, 1985), we are not only able to experience ourselves in the past, but we are also able to experience ourselves in the future. Building upon MTT theory, Schacter and Addis (2007) put forth the Constructive Episodic Simulation Hypothesis, which posits that the cognitive processes of remembering the past and thinking about the future rely on similar underlying mechanisms, and both draw upon information stored in episodic memory. Episodic memory and EFT operate by binding the context of an experience to the experience itself for the construction and reconstruction of scenes (Schacter and Addis, 2007). Importantly, this theory suggests that EFT relies on the flexible recombination of information drawn from past experiences to construct novel future scenarios. While this scenario construction can be adaptive for processes such as planning and goal-attainment, clinically-relevant biases in AM may contribute to the onset and maintenance of various disorders, including PTSD (Hallford et al., 2018).

MTT is thought to serve several critical functions, including decision-making (Aupperle et al., 2011), problem-solving (Suddendorf and Corballis, 1997), and goal-setting and implementation (Breeden et al., 2016). However, further research is needed to elucidate the relationship between PTSD and alterations in MTT. While the examination of the AM component of MTT is ubiquitous in PTSD research, there is a relative dearth of information on the future thinking component of this process (however, see, e.g., Blix and Brennen, 2011; Zlomuzica et al., 2017). In line with the Constructive Episodic Simulation Hypothesis, these studies show that putative AM alterations associated with PTSD are also found in the content and characteristics of future imaginings among such individuals. The relevance of EFT and the construction of a future self in relation to psychopathology converges with a growing body of research demonstrating that prospection related to the self is associated with a wide range of cognitive, affective, and behavioral processes. For instance, we tend to evaluate ourselves in the present by comparing ourselves to the future self we hope to achieve (Bak, 2015). Relatedly, neurocognitive findings consistently link PTSD with negative self-appraisals (Engelbrecht and Jobson, 2020).

The goal of the current article is to highlight the importance of MTT in PTSD. Although changes in MTT may reflect a mechanism underlying several mental health disorders, both the study and the treatment of PTSD have long focused on potential changes in the characteristics and content in AM. Given this history and the long-standing debates related to the role of memory in this disorder, as well as the targeted use of AM in treatments (e.g., Brewin, 2011), this commentary will be limited to PTSD. However, alterations in AM are by no means constrained to PTSD, and certain MTT changes may reflect transdiagnostic processes (e.g., Kellogg et al., 2020). With this opinion piece, we will (1) briefly review and consider the extant literature on memory and future-thinking in PTSD; (2) elucidate gaps in PTSD MTT research; (3) discuss future directions for PTSD MTT research. It is our hope that this piece will shed light on an understudied cognitive process and underscore areas where additional research may broaden our understanding of PTSD pathogenesis and eventually inform the development of future PTSD treatments.

Alterations in Memory and Future Thinking in PTSD

Voluntariness, Intrusiveness, and Content

Considerable AM research in PTSD has focused on memories that arise spontaneously, namely, involuntary AMs, intrusive AMs, and flashbacks. Although these terms are often used interchangeably in research studies, recent attempts to create a taxonomy have been employed to study potentially distinct phenomena associated with these processes (Kvavilashvili, 2014). For instance, Kvavilashvili suggests that involuntary AMs arise spontaneously without attempt at recall; may occur in response to internal and external triggers; and can be positive, negative, or neutral in valence (e.g., Berntsen and Hall, 2004). Intrusive AMs, by contrast, may be spontaneous memories of predominantly negative events that repeatedly interrupt consciousness, often emerging against one's will, and may interrupt one's activity at the time. Compared to intrusive AMs, involuntary AMs may be more random in nature and will often go unnoticed. Lastly, flashbacks might represent a type of spontaneous and intrusive memory specifically related to a traumatic event. Flashbacks have been characterized as vivid, emotionally-laden, and lacking temporal perspective, or possessing a “here and now” like quality (e.g., Zlomuzica et al., 2017). Future research into these potential distinctions may not only help to better characterize the phenomenology of PTSD but also to refine treatments aimed at reducing distress related to such AM processes.

Although there remains debate in the field about the extent to which involuntary memories and flashbacks may or may not reflect poorly integrated memories, there is now substantial evidence in line with cognitive models of PTSD indicating that stressful memories, such as those experienced by individuals with PTSD, are more, not less, accessible. Moreover, it is likely due to the increased availability of these highly stressful memories that individuals with PTSD are more likely to recall trauma-related memories. Interestingly, research has revealed that this is true for involuntary and voluntary AMs (Berntsen, 2010). For example, several studies have shown that while individuals with PTSD may think more about their trauma-related involuntary memories and experience greater subjective reactions to these memories, they do so for voluntary trauma-related AM's as well (e.g., Berntsen, 2001; Rubin, 2010). Despite the growing body of work elucidating the basic cognitive processes underlying involuntary AMs in PTSD, considerable debate and calls for additional research into the distinct qualities of flashbacks in PTSD continue (Brewin, 2015).

PTSD studies have also sought to investigate the differential effects of trauma on the encoding and recollection of voluntary and involuntary AM's. Disagreement in the field regarding this topic is centered around two assumptions about PTSD symptomatology: (1) traumatic events are frequently reexperienced via involuntary flashbacks, and (2) voluntary access to traumatic memory is impaired such that it is partially to completely inaccessible. These PTSD characteristics have been included in the DSM-V (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) are proposed in theoretical models of PTSD (e.g., Ehlers and Clark, 2000). Howeverindings pertaining to accessibility of traumatic AMs have indicated that both involuntary and voluntary memory have been shown to be enhanced by stressful events (e.g., Hall and Berntsen, 2008). Moreover, this enhancement has been associated with increased levels of PTSD symptoms (e.g., Rubin et al., 2008). Given the persistence and scope of this debate, future research would benefit from comparing voluntary and involuntary memories in individuals with reliable PTSD diagnoses (Berntsen and Rubin, 2014), using mixed-methodologies to examine differences between memory types. Of particular interest would be the further employment of task-based methodologies that allow for the measurement of intrusive memories in real-time, such as the trauma-film paradigm (e.g., Holmes and Bourne, 2008) or the AM diary recording method (e.g., Schönfeld and Ehlers, 2017).

The extent to which involuntary AM's are specific or not may depend on whether that AM is related to trauma. For example, Schönfeld and Ehlers (2017) found that compared to individuals without PTSD, traumatic AM's appeared more specific, as they were experienced more vividly and were associated with a subjective sense of “nowness.” However, the group with PTSD perceived their traumatic memories as less intentional, indicating that while traumatic and nontraumatic memories may be similarly involuntary post-trauma, individuals with PTSD may overestimate the frequency of trauma-related involuntary memories because they are noticed more often than non-trauma related involuntary memories (Kvavilashvili, 2014). By contrast, nontraumatic memories in the group with PTSD were associated with reduced continuity between past self and present self; and were less specific (Schönfeld and Ehlers, 2017). The potential differences in specificity for involuntary AMs related and unrelated to trauma are intriguing and may help to explain the Janus-like phenomenon often attributed to PTSD, where on the one hand, individuals with PTSD recall highly vivid, trauma-related AMs and on the other appear to have difficulty recalling other memories, perhaps unrelated to the trauma.

Future episodic intrusions have received considerably less attention than flashbacks. In their investigation of mental imagery as a predictor of future suicidal behavior, Holmes et al. (2007) found that depressed, formerly-suicidal patients unanimously reported ruminating on the imagery of future suicide attempts. The authors termed these future-related intrusions, flashforwards, because of their similarity to flashbacks. Both trauma-related flashforwards and flashbacks are emotionally-laden, contain rich detail, and evoke a phenomenological sense of “nowness.” Survivors of Chernobyl's nuclear disaster similarly reported flashforwards, projecting themselves into futures in which they develop health problems caused by their prior radiation exposure (Loganovsky and Zdanevich, 2013). Closely related to this phenomenon, Berntsen and Rubin (2015) examined the construct of pretraumatic stress in combat veterans before, during, and after deployment. Pretraumatic stress involves future-directed thoughts about what might be experienced over the course of combat. Interestingly, pretraumatic stress reactions were reliable predictors of PTSD both during and after combat-related trauma exposure. Additionally, flashforwards, heightened arousal, and associated avoidance behavior were experienced at the same level of post-traumatic stress reactions to traumatic events experienced before and during deployment, indicating that pretraumatic stress and posttraumatic stress may represent two subjectively distinct manifestations of the same underlying phenomenon. Together, these studies point to potential therapeutic targets in imagery features of flashbacks and flashforwards. One such intervention to consider is imagery rescripting, a method that involves focusing on imagery to modify the meaning of a traumatic event (for review, see Hackmann, 2011). Additionally, although studies have indicated that future autobiographical imaginings tend to be overgeneralized in response to non-trauma related cues, to our knowledge, no studies have examined whether flashforwards for non-trauma-related or trauma-related in PTSD are also less specific.

Coherence

As previously noted, it has been consistently demonstrated that individuals with PTSD exhibit overgeneralized AM for voluntarily-recalled, nontraumatic memories (Moore and Zoellner, 2007; Ono et al., 2015). However, findings regarding alterations in voluntarily-recalled, traumatic memories in individuals with PTSD are mixed. Certain studies show increased fragmentation and disorganization in voluntarily recalled traumatic memories in individuals with PTSD (e.g., Halligan et al., 2003), while other studies show no significant group differences in the qualities of voluntarily-recalled traumatic memories (e.g., Jelinek et al., 2009).

The extant literature on memory and PTSD reveals conflicting findings regarding whether or not PTSD involves disorganization and fragmentation of memory. The basic mechanisms view proposes that there is heightened availability of trauma or stressful AMs compared to non-stressful or traumatic AMs, regardless of whether this memory is retrieved voluntarily or involuntarily (Rubin et al., 2008). Although other researchers have argued in favor of models that support fragmented memories (e.g., Monroe and Mineka, 2008) and lack of control narratives, variance in education level and narrative skill make it challenging to determine lack of coherence found in several studies (Gray and Lombardo, 2001; McNally, 2003). Ongoing research is needed to fully appreciate how AMs are shaped by trauma, as following a trauma, certain parts of an event may be enhanced while others become harder to recall. For example, one study found that negative emotion enhances memory for items central to the event but diminishes coherence for non-central, contextual items around the traumatic event (e.g., Bisby et al., 2018).

The special mechanism view (e.g., van der Kolk and Fisler, 1995) hypothesizes that there is disrupted encoding, integration, and voluntary retrieval of memories around a traumatic event. For example, a recent study found that the presence of fragmentation and disorganization across a trauma narrative is positively associated with emotional intensity surrounding that trauma. That is to say, highly emotional moments in a traumatic event are more likely to be associated with a disruption in cognitive processing that in turn creates disorganization and fragmentation (Brewin, 2015). Further research is needed to clarify which types of memories— trauma vs. general AMs, involuntary vs. voluntary memories—if any are characterized by disorganization and fragmentation.

There is very little PTSD research on disorganization and fragmentation of prospections, and the few studies that do exist show mixed results. Two studies specifically have examined temporal distribution in both AM and future thinking. Zlomuzica et al., 2017 tested MTT and temporal distribution using a novel virtual reality paradigm that probed not only episodic content but spatiotemporal context in MTT. When compared to their healthy counterparts, PTSD participants performed worse overall in the MTT test, and specifically on items that probed questions of when? and what? Significantly, PTSD participants were less able to employ information from episodic memory to solve current or future problems, and when emotional arousal was probed, they had higher levels of negative arousal and lower levels of positive arousal. By contrast, Blix and Brennen (2011) examined the relationship between trauma exposure and temporal distribution in AMs and prospection and found no group differences between trauma-exposed participants and healthy controls without a PTSD diagnosis. Additionally, it will be helpful for future research to examine other dimensions of future self-coherence. For instance, a growing body of literature has found that greater future self-continuity, the extent to which you believe your current self overlaps with your future self, is associated with reduced impulsivity and positive health outcomes (Hershfield, 2011). To our knowledge, no studies have looked at whether this aspect of self may be altered in PTSD and how it might be related to the specificity of characteristics of MTT.

Specificity and Vividness

When recalling the past, individuals with PTSD reliably demonstrate OGM bias. Several theories attempt to explain the overgeneralization of AM seen in individuals with PTSD (for reviews, see Moore and Zoellner, 2007; Ono et al., 2015). This effect has been shown in both traumatic and nontraumatic memories (Schönfeld and Ehlers, 2017), using a range of methodologies both inside and outside the laboratory (e.g., Brown et al., 2013; Schönfeld and Ehlers, 2017). Notably, OGM prompted by positive cues was shown to be a risk factor for post-traumatic stress after a traumatic experience (Bryant et al., 2008), indicating a possible negativity bias in individuals with PTSD.

Recent PTSD research shows a similar pattern of overgeneralization in EFT in a range of post-traumatic populations with heterogeneous forms of trauma exposure using different methodologies (e.g., Brown et al., 2013; Zlomuzica et al., 2017). One study found that this effect was only present in response to positive prompts about the future (Kleim et al., 2014). Further research needs to be done to clarify whether overgeneralization of EFT in individuals with PTSD is specific to the valence of the cue eliciting the response and whether the overgeneralization of EFT remains during spontaneous EFT.

It was recently shown that individuals with PTSD tend to generate greater external detail (e.g., non-episodic details including semantic information, repetitions, and metacognitive statements) than they do internal detail (e.g., episodic information about the central event across a range of modalities including perceptual, emotional, spatial, and temporal) during memory retrieval and EFT (Brown et al., 2014). Although preliminary, these results show that in addition to overgeneralization of the spatial and temporal features of an event, internal detail also lacks specificity during MTT. New research is showing that cognitive processes related to depression can be altered using specificity training for the future (Hallford et al., 2020), and it will be important to determine whether such findings will generalize to PTSD.

Changes in Self-Concept and Event Centrality

Cognitive frameworks of memory posit that one of its functions may be to inform and update our self-conception (Conway and Pleydell-Pearce, 2000). Conway and colleagues' seminal Self-Memory-System Model proposes that our goals and self-conceptions are constrained by AMs that contradict our current self-views. Given the central role of memory in PTSD, it is unsurprising then that traumatic memories are associated with negative beliefs about one's self in that past, present, and future (Beck, 1976). Cognitive frameworks relating memory to self are supported by experimental evidence. For example, PTSD has been associated with subjective feelings of increased societal alienation and permanent changes in the self across diverse trauma-exposed populations (Brewin, 2011); PTSD patients endorsed significantly more trauma-related memories when prompted to recall a self-defining memory than their trauma-exposed individuals without PTSD (Sutherland and Bryant, 2005); and trauma centrality has consistently been associated with greater PTSD symptomatology (e.g., Brown et al., 2010). Jobson et al. (2014) found that PTSD participants recalling AMs across a range of individualist and collectivist cultures showed similar reductions in expressions of autonomy and self-determination. In addition, longitudinal studies have demonstrated negative changes in self-perception significantly predict PTSD status follow-up (Kleim et al., 2007) as well as diminished response to exposure therapy (Ehlers et al., 1998).

In contrast to past and present self-conceptions, less is known about how our future self-concept and future goals are affected by PTSD. The Self-Memory-System Model predicts that future goals should be congruent with current symptomology. There is considerably less experimental evidence supporting the relationship between PTSD and changes in future self-conception. One study found that PTSD patients experienced disorder-congruent changes in cognitive tasks related to MTT and that trauma related-memories were significantly predicted by future goals related to trauma (Krans et al., 2017). In an experimental manipulation, Brown and colleagues demonstrated that self-efficacy, a facet of our self-identity, can be promoted by boosting AMs involving self-efficacious behavior in a sample of veterans with PTSD. Importantly, veterans in the enhanced self-efficacy condition exhibited enhanced social problem-solving and imagined the future with more goal-oriented statements than those in the control condition (Brown et al., 2016). Taken together, these findings suggest that negative self-conceptions may be modifiable and may represent a potential therapeutic target for future interventions.

Another facet of self-identity that has become an important correlate in PTSD research relates to how one views a traumatic event within the narrative of their life story. There is now considerable research showing that individuals who view their trauma as more central to their past and future and who view the event(s) as a turning point in their life will exhibit greater PTSD symptom severity. It may be that considering an event as more central increases the accessibility of those trauma memories, leading to PTSD-related symptoms. In contrast, it might be that events that are more profound and disruptive may also be seen as more central. Regardless of the directionality, studies that employ the Centrality of Event Scale (CES; Berntsen and Rubin, 2006) have found positive associations between the CES and PTSD symptoms (e.g., Brown et al., 2010; Rubin et al., 2014). One study attempted to reduce PTSD symptoms by experimentally manipulating event centrality (Vermeulen et al., 2019). Although symptoms of PTSD did not decrease, centrality did. Notably, other research using a modified version of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) found that reductions in trauma centrality contributed to a decrease of PTSD, suggesting that trauma centrality may be modifiable, which in turn, may contribute to recovery from a traumatic stressor (Boals and Murrell, 2016). Such strategies are similar to those used in treatments such as Narrative Exposure Therapy (Robjant and Fazel, 2010), but additional research would help to understand the specific putative therapeutic mechanisms.

Although less is known about the relationship between PTSD and the centrality for future anticipated events, there are reasons to believe that similar trauma-focused biases about the past would apply to the future. First, Robinaugh and McNally (2011) did a principal component analysis on the CES and found that the future-oriented items on the CES were significantly associated with PTSD severity. Notably, this future-related factor was more strongly associated with PTSD symptom severity than the other two factors identified by the principal component analysis: factor 1, the extent to which the trauma is viewed as a turning point in one's life, and factor 2, the extent to which the trauma is integrated with one's other memories and its centrality to identity. Taken together, these findings indicate that the perception of trauma as central to one's future may be more damaging than the perception of trauma as central to one's current self. Second, studies examining temporal self-appraisals in PTSD show that while trauma-exposed individuals without PTSD hold a relatively optimistic view of the future, this is less often the case among those with PTSD (Brown et al., 2011; Parnes et al., 2020). As mentioned, re-scripting future anticipated events, perhaps related to pretraumatic stress, may help to interrupt some of the trauma-related expectations held by individuals with PTSD.

Discussion and Future Directions

Over the last few decades, research on memory specificity has illuminated important mechanisms underlying PTSD, which in turn have guided novel treatments for the disorder (Maxwell, 2016). Further research using a wider range of neurocognitive methodologies is necessary to elucidate the key mechanisms underlying the relationship between MTT and PTSD. Such research is critical, as MTT may account for the social and functional impairments related to the disorder. In particular, future studies should investigate the role of the future-self in PTSD. Given the close relations between MTT and self-identity, therapeutic approaches that target MTT and self-narrative, such as imagery rescripting (Brockman and Calvert, 2017), metacognition (Davis et al., 2016), self-efficacy enhancement (Cusack et al., 2019), and specificity training for memory and prospection (Erten and Brown, 2018) may represent interventions and should be further tested in clinical PTSD populations.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aupperle, R., Melrose, J., Stein, M., and Paulus, M. (2011). Executive function and PTSD: disengaging from Trauma. Neuropharmacology 62, 686–694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bak, W. (2015). Possible selves: implications for psychotherapy. Int. J. Mental Health Addict. 13, 650–658. doi: 10.1007/s11469-015-9553-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders. Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders. Oxford: International Universities Press.

Google Scholar

Berntsen, D. (2001). Involuntary memories of emotional events: do memories of traumas and extremely happy events differ? Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 15, S135–S158. doi: 10.1002/acp.838

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berntsen, D. (2010). The unbidden past: involuntary autobiographical memories as a basic mode of remembering. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 19, 138–142. doi: 10.1177/0963721410370301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berntsen, D., and Hall, N. M. (2004). The episodic nature of involuntary autobiographical memories. Mem. Cogn. 32, 789–803. doi: 10.3758/BF03195869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berntsen, D., and Rubin, D. C. (2006). The centrality of event scale: a measure of integrating a trauma into one's identity and its relation to post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms. Behav. Res. Ther. 44, 219–231. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berntsen, D., and Rubin, D. C. (2014). Involuntary memories and dissociative Amnesia: assessing key assumptions in PTSD research. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2, 174–186. doi: 10.1177/2167702613496241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berntsen, D., and Rubin, D. C. (2015). Pretraumatic stress reactions in soldiers deployed to Afghanistan. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 3, 663–674. doi: 10.1177/2167702614551766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bisby, J. A., Horner, A. J., Bush, D., and Burgess, N. (2018). Negative emotional content disrupts the coherence of episodic memories. J. Exp. Psychol. General. 147, 243–256. doi: 10.1037/xge0000356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blix, I., and Brennen, T. (2011). Mental time travel after trauma: the specificity and temporal distribution of autobiographical memories and future-directed thoughts. Memory 19, 956–967. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2011.618500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bluck, S., Alea, N., Habermas, T., and Rubin, D. C. (2005). A tale of three functions: the self-reported uses of autobiographical memory. Social Cogn. 23, 91–117. doi: 10.1521/soco.23.1.91.59198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boals, A., and Murrell, A. R. (2016). I Am > trauma: experimentally reducing event centrality and PTSD symptoms in a clinical trial. J. Loss Trauma 21, 471–483. doi: 10.1080/15325024.2015.1117930

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breeden, P., Dere, D., Zlomuzica, A., and Dere, E. (2016). The mental time travel continuum: on the architecture, capacity, versatility and extension of the mental bridge into the past and future. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 421–434. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2015-0053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewin, C. R. (2011). The nature and significance of memory disturbance in posttraumatic stress disorder. Ann. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 7, 203–227. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewin, C. R. (2015). Re-experiencing traumatic events in PTSD: new avenues in research on intrusive memories and flashbacks. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 6:27180. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v6.27180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brockman, R., and Calvert, F. (2017). Imagery rescripting for PTSD and personality disorders: theory and application. J. Contem. Psychother. 47, 23–30. doi: 10.1007/s10879-016-9329-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, A. D., Antonius, D., Kramer, M., Root, J. C., and Hirst, W. (2010). Trauma centrality and PTSD in veterans returning from Iraq and Afghanistan. J. Traum. Stress 23, 496–499. doi: 10.1002/jts.20547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, A. D., Kouri, N. A., Rahman, N., Joscelyne, A., Bryant, R. A., and Marmar, C. R. (2016). Enhancing self-efficacy improves episodic future thinking and social-decision making in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res. 242, 19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.05.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, A. D., Addis, D. R., Romano, T. A., Marmar, C. R., Bryant, R. A., Hirst, W., and Schacter, D. L. (2014). Episodic and semantic components of autobiographical memories and imagined future events in post-traumatic stress disorder. Memory 22, 595–604. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2013.807842

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, A. D., Buckner, J. P., and Hirst, W. (2011). Time, before, and after time: temporal self and social appraisals in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 42, 344–348. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, A. D., Root, J. C., Romano, T. A., Chang, L. J., Bryant, R. A., and Hirst, W. (2013). Overgeneralized autobiographical memory and future thinking in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 44, 129–134. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryant, R. A., Sutherland, K., and Guthrie, R. M. (2008). Impaired specific autobiographical memory as a risk factor for posttraumatic stress after trauma. Int. J. Emerg. Mental Health 10, 74–77. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.4.837

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, M. A., and Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychol. Rev. 107, 261–288. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.107.2.261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cusack, S. E., Coleman, J. A., Rappaport, L. M., and Sheerin, C. (2019). Moderation of improvement in self-efficacy following group psychotherapy for PTSD. Psychol. Serv. 16, 657–663. doi: 10.1037/ser0000260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, L., Leonhardt, B., Siegel, A., Brustuen, B., Luedtke, B., Vohs, J., et al. (2016). Metacognitive capacity predicts severity of trauma-related dysfunctional cognitions in adults with posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry.Res. 237, 182–187. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehlers, A., and Clark, D. M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 38, 319–345. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(99)00123-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., Dunmore, E., Jaycox, L., Meadows, E., and Foa, E. B. (1998). Predicting response to exposure treatment in PTSD: the role of mental defeat and alienation. J. Traum. Stress 11, 457–471. doi: 10.1023/A:1024448511504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engelbrecht, A., and Jobson, L. (2020). Self-concept, post-traumatic self-appraisals and post-traumatic psychological adjustment: what are the relationships? Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 48, 463–480. doi: 10.1017/S1352465820000156

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erten, M. N., and Brown, A. D. (2018). Memory specificity training for depression and posttraumatic stress disorder: a promising therapeutic intervention. Front. Psychol. 9:419. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, M., and Lombardo, T. (2001). Complexity of trauma narratives as an index of fragmented memory in PTSD: a critical analysis. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 15, S171–S186. doi: 10.1002/acp.840

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hackmann, A. (2011). Imagery rescripting in posttraumatic stress disorder. Cogn. Behav. Pract. 18, 424–32. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpra.2010.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, N. M., and Berntsen, D. (2008). The effect of emotional stress on involuntary and voluntary conscious memories. Memory 16, 48–57. doi: 10.1080/09658210701333271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallford, D. J., Austin, D. W., Takano, K., and Raes, F. (2018). Psychopathology and episodic future thinking: a systematic review and meta-analysis of specificity and episodic detail. Behav. Res. Ther. 102, 42–51. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2018.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallford, D. J., Sharma, M. K., and Austin, D. W. (2020). Increasing anticipatory pleasure in major depression through enhancing episodic future thinking: a randomized single-case series trial. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 42, 751–764. doi: 10.1007/s10862-020-09820-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halligan, S. L., Michael, T., Clark, D. M., and Ehlers, A. (2003). Posttraumatic stress disorder following assault: the role of cognitive processing, trauma memory, and appraisals. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 71, 419–431. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.71.3.419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hershfield, H. E. (2011). Future self-continuity: how conceptions of the future self transform intertemporal choice. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1235, 30–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06201.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoge, C. W., Castro, C. A., Messer, S. C., McGurk, D., Cotting, D. I., and Koffman, R. L. (2004). Combat duty in Iraq and Afghanistan, mental health problems, and barriers to care. N. Engl. J. Med. 351, 13–22. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa040603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmes, E. A., and Bourne, C. (2008). Inducing and modulating intrusive emotional memories: a review of the trauma film paradigm. Acta Psychol. 127, 553–566. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmes, E. A., Crane, C., Fennell, M. J. V., and Williams, J. M. G. (2007). Imagery about suicide in depression—‘flash-forwards'? J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 38, 423–434. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.10.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jelinek, L., Randjbar, S., Seifert, D., Kellner, M., and Moritz, S. (2009). The organization of autobiographical and nonautobiographical memory in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). J. Abnorm. Psychol. 118, 288–298. doi: 10.1037/a0015633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jobson L, Moradi, A.R., Ramimi-Movaghar, V., Conway, M.A., Dalgleish, T. Culture and the remembering of trauma (2014). Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2, 696–713.

Kellogg, R. T., Chirino, C. A., and Gfeller, J. D. (2020). The complex role of mental time travel in depressive and anxiety disorders: an ensemble perspective. Front. Psychol. 11:1465. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleim, B., Ehlers, A., and Glucksman, E. (2007). Early predictors of chronic post-traumatic stress disorder in assault survivors. Psychol. Med. 37, 1457–1467. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleim, B., Graham, B., Fihosy, S., Stott, R., and Ehlers, A. (2014). Reduced specificity in episodic future thinking in posttraumatic stress disorder. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2, 165–173. doi: 10.1177/2167702613495199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krans, J., Peeters, M., Näring, G., Brown, A. D., de Bree, J., and van Minnen, A. (2017). Examining temporal alterations in social anxiety disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder: the relation between autobiographical memory, future goals, and current self-views. J. Anxiety Disord. 52, 34–42. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2017.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kvavilashvili, L. (2014). Solving the mystery of intrusive flashbacks in posttraumatic stress disorder: comment on Brewin 2014. Psychol. Bull. 140, 98–104. doi: 10.1037/a0034677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lancaster, C. L., Teeters, J. B., Gros, D. F., and Back, S. E. (2016). Posttraumatic stress disorder: overview of evidence-based assessment and treatment. J. Clin. Med. 5:105. doi: 10.3390/jcm5110105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loganovsky, K. N., and Zdanevich, N. A. (2013). Cerebral basis of posttraumatic stress disorder following the chernobyl disaster. CNS Spectr. 18, 95–102. doi: 10.1017/S109285291200096X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, K. A., Suvak, M. K., Holmes, S., Shields, N., Lane, J. E. M., Sijercic, I., et al. (2020). Bidirectional relationships between posttraumatic stress disorder and social functioning during cognitive processing therapy. Behav. Ther. 51, 447–460. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2019.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maxwell, K. (2016). A comparison of treatments for posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms: Memory Specificity Training (MeST) and Cognitive Processing Therapy (CPT) (Dissertation), UNT Digital Library https://digital.library.unt.edu/

McNally, R. J. (2003). Progress and controversy in the study of posttraumatic stress disorder. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 54, 229–252. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monroe, S. M., and Mineka, S. (2008). Postscript: making important distinctions–diagnostic models, theoretical models, and the mnemonic model of PTSD. Psychol. Rev. 115, 1096–1098. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.115.4.1096

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, S. A., and Zoellner, L. A. (2007). Overgeneral autobiographical memory and traumatic events: an evaluative review. Psychol. Bull. 133, 419–437. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.3.419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niziurski, J., Johannessen, K., and Berntsen, D. (2017). Emotional distress and positive and negative memories from military deployment: the influence of PTSD symptoms and time. Memory 26, 1–12. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2017.1418380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ono, M., Devilly, G., and Shum, D. (2015). A meta-analytic review of overgeneral memory: the role of trauma history, mood, and the presence of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychol. Trauma.8, 157–164. doi: 10.1037/tra0000027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parnes, M. F., Boals, A., Brown, A. D., and Eubank, J. (2020). Heterogeneity in temporal self-appraisals following exposure to potentially traumatic life events: a latent profile analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 277, 515–523. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.08.050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinaugh, D. J., and McNally, R. J. (2011). Trauma centrality and PTSD symptom severity in adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse. J. Trauma. Stress 24, 483–486. doi: 10.1002/jts.20656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robjant, K., and Fazel, M. (2010). The emerging evidence for narrative exposure therapy: a review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 1030–1039. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubin, D. (2010). The coherence of memories for trauma: evidence from posttraumatic stress disorder. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 857–865. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubin, D. C., Berntsen, D., and Bohni, M. K. (2008). A memory-based model of posttraumatic stress disorder: evaluating basic assumptions underlying the PTSD diagnosis. Psychol. Rev. 115, 985–1011. doi: 10.1037/a0013397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubin, D. C., Boals, A., and Hoyle, R. H. (2014). Narrative centrality and negative affectivity: Independent and interactive contributors to stress reactions. J. Experi. Psychol.: Gen. 143, 115–1170. doi: 10.1037/a0035140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schacter, D. L., and Addis, D. R. (2007). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 362, 773–786. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schönfeld, S., and Ehlers, A. (2017). Posttraumatic stress disorder and autobiographical memories in everyday life. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 5, 325–340. doi: 10.1177/2167702616688878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silverstein, M., Lee, D., Seligowski, A., and Worley, C. (2018). Functional impairment: the role of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms, negative mood regulation, and interpersonal problems. Traumatology 25, 51–57. doi: 10.1037/trm0000164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suddendorf, T., and Corballis, M. (1997). Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genet. Soc. Gen. Psychol. Monogr. 123, 133–167.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sutherland, K., and Bryant, R. A. (2005). Self-defining memories in post-traumatic stress disorder. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 44, 591–598. doi: 10.1348/014466505X64081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canad. Psychol. 26, 1–12.

Google Scholar

van der Kolk, B. A., and Fisler, R. (1995). Dissociation and the fragmentary nature of traumatic memories: overview and exploratory study. J. Trauma. Stress 8, 505–525. doi: 10.1002/jts.2490080402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermeulen, M., Brown, A. D., Raes, F., and Krans, J. (2019). Decreasing event centrality in undergraduates using cognitive bias modification of appraisals. Cognit. Ther. Res. 43, 214–225. doi: 10.1007/s10608-018-9936-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zlomuzica, A., Woud, M. L., Machulska, A., Kleimt, K., Dietrich, L., Wolf, O. T., et al. (2017). Deficits in episodic memory and mental time travel in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder. Progr. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 83, 42–54. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), autobiographical memory, prospection, future thinking, cognition, mental time travel, flashbacks, flashforwards

Citation: Rahman N and Brown AD (2021) Mental Time Travel in Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder: Current Gaps and Future Directions. Front. Psychol. 12:624707. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.624707

Received: 01 November 2020; Accepted: 08 February 2021;
Published: 09 March 2021.

Edited by:

Laura Jobson, Monash University, Australia

Reviewed by:

Rikke Jensen, Aarhus University, Denmark
Tine Gehrt, Aarhus University, Denmark

Copyright © 2021 Rahman and Brown. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nadia Rahman, cmFobW45NzQmI3gwMDA0MDtuZXdzY2hvb2wuZWR1

Present address: Nadia Rahman, The Trauma and Global Mental Health Lab at the New School for Social Research, New York, NY, United States

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.