Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 27 July 2021
Sec. Environmental Psychology

The Power and Peril of Precise vs. Round Health Message Interventions to Increase Stair Use

  • 1Department for Taxation, Accounting and Finance, Center for Risk Management, University of Paderborn, Paderborn, Germany
  • 2Institute of Management and Organization, Leuphana University of Lüneburg, Lüneburg, Germany

Taking the stairs vs. an elevator generate benefits for the individual by increasing overall physical activity, health, and wellbeing. In the present paper, we report two pre-registered field intervention studies that examine how health message interventions can motivate individuals to change their behavior. We empirically contrasted opposing predictions from the literature as to whether numerically round (60.00%) or precise (61.87%) health messages are more effective in causing people to use the stairs over taking the elevator. Both interventions were compared to a control condition (no-health message). Contrary to our hypotheses and extant findings, both intervention studies did not produce a significant positive effect of the interventions relative to the control condition. In recent years such null findings have received increasingly more appreciation, particularly in the light of evident downsides of file-drawered studies. We discuss a number of moderating factors that may determine when and why nudging interventions are (in-) effective (e.g., a priori behavioral prevalence, pre-established habits, ceiling effects, and building infrastructure), as well as limitations and avenues for future research.

Introduction

A more sedentary lifestyle, such as choosing elevators and cars over stairs and bicycles, increases individual’s health risk and impairs wellbeing. The WHO lists physical inactivity as well as a high body mass index (BMI) as death risk factors with an occurrence of 6% and 10%, respectively. High blood pressure is ranked as the number one cause for death with an occurrence of 26% in Germany (World Health Organization, 2005). Sufficient physical activity can decrease the prevalence of hypertension (Diaz and Shimbo, 2013; Hegde and Solomon, 2015) and overweight (Mertens and Van Gaal, 2000). Nudging is a widely respected, promising approach to change behavior by alternating the choice architecture that, in turn, directs people toward more healthier behavior (Sunstein and Thaler, 2008). As there is a plethora of successful nudging interventions across various contexts, nudging is considered a highly suitable soft policy tool to promote healthy lifestyle choices (e.g., Lehner et al., 2016 for an overview of implications).

In the present paper, we build on prior research on choice-architecture interventions and examine a novel research question that contrasts the impact of numerically round (60.00%) vs. precise (61.87%) health messages that foster healthy behavior. Specifically, we report two pre-registered field intervention studies1 (Experiment 1 and Experiment 2) that investigate the (in)efficacy of point-of-choice prompts that state health risk reductions by choosing stairs over elevators to alter behavior.

Physical Activity and Active Transport

For adults, the WHO recommends a minimum of 150 min of moderate-intensity physical activity per week or 75 min of vigorous-intensity activity per week to decrease the risk of non-communicable diseases, such as cardiovascular diseases (World Health Organization, 2010). Based on the BMI, approximately 54% of the German adult population is overweight and about 20% obese (World Health Organization, 2015; Schienkiewitz et al., 2017). In the United States, 2018 even 42% of the adult population was obese (Hales et al., 2020). Although the (dis)advantages of (in)sufficient physical activity are well investigated, surprisingly few actions have been taken. Rare exceptions suggest pragmatic strategies on how to increase the population’s physical activity, such as general improvement of bicycling infrastructure (Jordan et al., 2012) or awareness campaigns (Tuso, 2015).

Stair climbing is a prominent example for physical activity that can be easily integrated into everyday life, with limited time expenses. As most public places and workplaces are equipped with stairs, stair climbing appears to be suitable to increase physical activity without monetary or time expenditure. Increasing physical activity in everyday life seems a suitable approach to countervail current obesity rates. Prior studies have reported success in behavior change toward more physical activity by increasing stair use in a workplace environment (e.g., Kerr et al., 2004; Eves et al., 2006, 2012). In contrast, other studies failed to report an overall positive impact of their intervention (e.g., Coleman and Gonzalez, 2001; Kerr et al., 2001; Avitsland et al., 2017). Thus, the empirical evidence appears inconclusive, and there is little evidence for interventions on stair use for campus environments with a young(er) target population (e.g., Landais et al., 2020). In the present research, we aim at refining evidence on how to shape a successful intervention—to that end, we applied a health message intervention and empirically contrasted the use of numerically precise (vs. round) numbers.

Risk Communication Using Nudges

From prospect theory (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979; Tversky and Kahneman, 1992), we know that people perceive risks incorrectly. Underestimation of severe health risks potentially undermines sufficient physical activity. Well-designed and credible risk communication is crucial to successfully address risks (Wachinger et al., 2013). But how can credibility of communicated risks be improved? Jenkins et al. (2019) find that communication of numeric estimates for risk is perceived as more credible than verbal probability expressions. Witteman et al. (2011) find that round numeric values are perceived as more reliable than precise numeric values in conveying health risks. However, perceived risk is smaller for round numeric than for precise numeric values, which contradicts the goal of successful risk communication. Another highly relevant factor is the increased sensitivity to relative risk increase compared to absolute risk increase (Furedi, 1999; Gigerenzer et al., 2008; Visschers et al., 2009).

While various choice-architecture interventions have been implemented to increase physical activity by pointing out its benefits on health, few nudging interventions have focused on risk communication to nudge people to increase physical activity. We make use of insights on research on health risk communication to implement these insights into point-of-choice prompts.

The Efficacy of Numerically Round Vs. Precise Health Messages

There is surprisingly little evidence on the relevance of the numbers used in health risk messages used to nudge people to physical activity. We seek to shed light on the relevance of choosing the most effective numbers to nudge people via a health message. Not all numbers are created equal but, instead, are psychologically perceived in different ways—either as precise numbers (e.g., 81.27%) or as round numbers (e.g., 80.00%) with more trailing zeros. Numeric precision coincides with different perceptions of the numbers’ informational content (Loschelder et al., 2013, 2016). Precise information is perceived as an indicator of higher confidence within communication (Welsh et al., 2011), and people are also more likely to follow a precise adviser (Jerez-Fernandez et al., 2014; Schultze and Loschelder, 2020). Recipients expect the communicator to be as accurate and detailed as possible but not more than needed (Grice, 1975; Zhang and Schwarz, 2012).

In contrast, King and Janiszewski (2011) state that people prefer round over precise numbers as responding time for such numbers is lower, indicating a higher processing fluency, which increases liking (Winkielman et al., 2003). Higher precision of numbers (and fewer trailing zeros) inhibits cognitive processing fluency, it increases uncertainty that leads to the favoring of round numbers (Thomas et al., 2010). In a similar vein, Kettle and Häubl (2010) demonstrate that round numbers are likely to increase processing fluency and velocity due to greater frequency in language.

In sum, competing predictions emerge from the literature: Rounded numbers might “feel right” when a decision is based on emotions (Wadhwa and Zhang, 2015). If one considers the choice for or against stair use as an emotional act or habitual behavior, rounded numbers should be more effective for health message interventions than precise numbers. In contrast, if numeric precision effectively evokes the perception of increased competence and accuracy (e.g., Schultze and Loschelder, 2020), health risk message interventions with precise numbers (e.g., “stairclimbing decreases heart problems by 61.87%”) could be more effective than round-numbered interventions (e.g., “heart problems decrease by 60.00%”).

Study Goals and Hypotheses

We aim at replicating and shedding light on (partially) inconclusive results from studies that applied nudges to increase stair use by using the setting of a German University Campus (e.g., Kerr et al., 2004; Eves et al., 2006, 2012; Müller-Riemenschneider et al., 2010; Rogers et al., 2010; Burger and Shelton, 2011; Lewis and Eves, 2012; Graham et al., 2013). Replications in the field of nudging are highly important to reveal the true potency of nudges given that replication attempts often fail to replicate previous seminal findings (see Scheibehenne et al., 2016; for a meta-analysis). DellaVigna and Linos (2020) show that nudging interventions published in academic journals in comparison with interventions by so-called nudge units (i.e., private or publicly funded organizations that implement behavior change interventions based on the nudging approach) differ markedly in effect size. Scientific studies report an average impact of nudges of 8.7%, while nudge units report real-world effects of only 1.4%. This difference in effect size may be largely explained by publication bias that favors large and significant effects over studies with only a small or even null effect (DellaVigna and Linos, 2020).

A further aim of the present work is to investigate for the first time whether round or precise numbers differ in their effectiveness when integrated into choice-architecture interventions. We term this combination round vs. precise health risk message, respectively. Prior theorizing allows for competing predictions for round or precise health message. The results offer insights for the design of more effective nudges to foster healthy behavior.

To sum up, we hypothesize (1) an increase in stair use during the nudge intervention phase for both health message conditions compared to baseline, whereas there should be no change in the control condition. With respect to competing predictions for numeric precision, (2a) a higher increase in stair use should emerge for the round than for the precise health risk message condition. Alternatively, (2b) a higher increase in stair use could also emerge for the precise (vs. round) health risk message condition, if precision indeed conveys informational accuracy and credibility.

Experiment 1

We first investigated whether a nudge in form of a poster placed at the point-of-choice displaying a health risk message would decrease the use of elevators compared to (1) a baseline phase and (2) a no-intervention control group. The health message communicated a reduction in health risk, which has been shown to have a beneficial impact on physical activity (Janssen et al., 2018). In addition, we empirically contrasted round vs. precise messages. We included a follow-up phase (without posters) to investigate the durability of effects. The effect of health messages on elevator use should result in an interaction effect of phase and intervention type.

Methods

Design

Experiment 1 realized a 3 (Phase: baseline vs. intervention vs. follow-up) × 3 (Intervention: precise vs. round health risk message vs. control group) design with daily elevator rides as the dependent variable.

Participants and Study Setting

We chose three campus buildings of the Leuphana University of Lüneburg that are largely identical in their architecture. In order to omit a spill-over effect, the buildings were chosen from different faculties. As we conducted an observational study, we did not approach participants to assess demographic variables. The experimental conditions were randomly assigned to the university buildings. The stairwells are well visible upon entering all three buildings, with the elevators slightly around a corner (see further details in the Supplementary Material).

We conducted a post-hoc power analysis in G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2007) for a repeated measures ANOVA using these parameters: three measurements (baseline vs. intervention vs. follow-up), three conditions (precise health risk message vs. round health risk message vs. control condition), α = 0.05, a moderate assumed population effect size of f = 0.25 (Cohen, 1992), and an assumed conservative correlation between baseline and intervention measurement of r = 0.2. Accordingly, the present study was powered at 1−β = 98.87%, with a total of 23,766 elevator rides.

Material and Procedure

We placed two signs with round (precise) health risk messages on each floor in the intervention buildings at the elevator doors and walls between elevators and stairs, i.e., the point-of-choice. The signs contained the health risk message: “Only 7 min of stairclimbing helps to reduce your risk for a heart attack by 60% (round) vs. 61.87% (precise)” (see Supplementary Figures 3, 4). We based our message on the finding that men whose daily level of vigorous intensity leisure activity equals an average of 7 min stair climbing have a 62% reduction in coronary death (Yu et al., 2003).

We collected data for 36 days—16 days baseline phase, 10 days intervention, and 10 days follow-up (no intervention) for all experimental conditions. The study took place during lecture time of the academic year to ensure consistent visitor traffic. We collected data for objective elevator use by daily reading out the meters integrated into elevators. Reading out took place daily around 9.00 am to ensure a constant measurement interval. In total 23,766 elevator rides were measured, which were all included into subsequent analyses.

Results

We conducted a 3 (Phase: baseline vs. intervention vs. follow-up) × 3 (Intervention: precise risk message vs. round risk message vs. control group) ANOVA with repeated measures for the first factor. The 3 × 3 ANOVA revealed a highly significant main effect for buildings, F (2, 99) = 19.175, p < 0.001, showing that there were overall differences in absolute elevator use between the different intervention buildings. Contrary to our hypotheses, however, there was no significant effect for phase, F (2, 99) = 0.027, p = 0.974, and no significant interaction of Phase × Intervention, F (4, 99) = 0.393, p = 0.813. Thus, there were no differences in elevator rides over the different phases of the experiment and neither of our interventions differed relative to the baseline phase and the control group (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Illustration of elevator use over baseline period, intervention period, and follow-up period.

Discussion

The results for Experiment 1 did not reveal a positive effect for either of our interventions. Instead, we observed highly consistent and robust elevator traffic. As the daily total of elevator rides served as the dependent variable for our analysis, we cannot account for who (and how many individuals) used the elevator, nor for how many participants elected to use the stairs instead. Data on stair use are missing for this experiment; hence, it may be premature to conclude that there was no increase in total building traffic. To address this shortcoming, we decided for a second intervention experiment at the same campus that quantified both absolute elevator rides and stair use to allow for detecting potential changes in the stairs-vs.-elevator-use ratio.

Experiment 2

Experiment 2 tested whether health message interventions would increase stair use rate relative to (1) a baseline period and (2) a control group—again, we contrasted numerically round vs. precise health risk message interventions.

Methods

Design

Experiment 2 realized a 2 (Phase: baseline vs. intervention) × 3 (Intervention: precise health risk message vs. round health risk message vs. control group) design with relative stair-vs.-elevator use as the key dependent measure.

Participants and Study Setting

We kept the study setting as close as possible to Experiment 1. However, the dependent variable addressed limitations of Experiment 1. We chose three different campus buildings to target different participants than in Experiment 1. Again, we randomly assigned experimental conditions to buildings. A post-hoc power analysis in G*Power (Faul et al., 2007) for a repeated measures ANOVA with two measurements (baseline vs. intervention), three intervention conditions (precise vs. round health messages vs. control condition), α = 0.05, a moderate effect size of f = 0.25 (Cohen, 1992), and an assumed conservative correlation between measurements of r = 0.2 showed that the study was powered at 1−β = 76.53%.

Material and Procedure

To allow comparability, we used the same posters and health messages as in Experiment 1 (see Supplementary Figures 3, 4). We collected data for 20 days – 10 days baseline and 10 days intervention for all experimental conditions. All measurements took place during the semester break, which allowed us to count and to closely observe individuals’ decisions more accurately (due to overall lower traffic). Measurement took place by in-situ observations on the ground floor for 1 h per building daily at randomized times between 8.00 am and 12.00 am. The number of individuals taking the elevator vs. stairs was counted. All observations took place by a carefully instructed observer to avoid potential differences in counting behavior of different observers. We assume no experimenter demand effects, as (1) having persons sitting in the hallway is not unusual at the chosen campus and (2) the trained observer stated that he was barely noticed by most people. In total, 1,497 stair walks and 177 elevator rides were observed. Due to our pre-defined exclusion criteria, individuals with visible disabilities or obviously heavy luggage, 30 elevator rides were excluded. Therefore, 1,497 stair uses and 147 elevator rides were subjected to final analyses. Retaining these data in our analyses did not change the pattern of results.

Results

We conducted a 2 (Phase: baseline vs. intervention) × 3 (Intervention: precise vs. round health message vs. control group) ANOVA with repeated measures for the first factor. The 2 × 3 ANOVA revealed no significant main effect for Phase, F (1, 54) = 0.752, p = 0.390, showing that intervention phase did not differ from the baseline period. Contrary to our hypotheses, there was no effect of intervention, F (2, 54) = 0.075, p = 0.928, and no interaction of Phase × Intervention, F (2, 54) = 0.523, p = 0.595. Thus, the (precise and round) health risk message interventions did not alter the ratio of stairs-vs.-elevator use relative to baseline period and the no health message control condition.

Discussion

The results of Experiment 2 did not reveal the predicted, positive effect(s) of our nudging interventions. In contrast, stair use was constant and surprisingly high across all study sites for the entire duration of the experiment, i.e., baseline and intervention phase (see Table 1 and Figure 2). By observing elevator vs. stair use, we controlled for the extent of traffic in the study sites during our experiment. Although we thereby addressed the evident limitations of the dependent measure in Experiment 1, we did not find the predicted significant effects for our health risk message interventions in Experiment 2 either.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean stair use rates for intervention conditions.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Illustration of stair use rates over baseline period and intervention period.

General Discussion

The present work aimed at replicating the effect of nudging people to increase stair use to facilitate a higher level of physical activity in everyday life. Expanding prior research, we examined for the first time a potential difference between numerically round and precise health risk messages in a nudging framework. We expected a significant decrease in elevator use (and increase in stair use) for the two health message conditions relative to (1) the baseline phase and (2) the no-intervention control condition. We (3) also contrasted competing predictions regarding whether round or precise health message would exert a stronger effect on physical activity.

Contrary to our hypotheses, our experimental manipulations did not produce a significant effect on stair (vs. elevator) use relative to the baseline period and control condition. There are several possible explanations for our null results: (1) individuals may have had pre-established habits that are difficult to alter with nudging interventions (see De Wijk et al., 2016), (2) unknowingly, the behavior that we aimed to change already constituted the (highly prevalent) default option in this study environment (i.e., a ceiling effect), (3) there is likely a high consciousness for healthy behavior in the studied population, (4) the infrastructure of the buildings in our study may have already favored (and nudged) the use of stairs, and (5) the sign displaying the nudge may have been ineffective because it was either not appealing or not sufficiently attended to.

First of all, we like to highlight that we do not consider it likely that the poster intervention itself was ineffective because the displayed signs were not appealing or did not catch the attention of our participants. Eves et al. (2006) showed that a simple poster intervention can successfully increase stair use. We considered findings on sensitivity to perception of relative health risks (Visschers et al., 2009) and to risk reduction (Janssen et al., 2018). Finally, we used specific health risk messages including numbers (round and precise), which have been shown to be more effective than unspecific messages (Puig-Ribera and Eves, 2010). We believe that other factors that concern the investigated population and the infrastructure are more plausible explanations for our null effects.

Nudging interventions via health risk messages aim at changing current behavior and habits toward a societally desired behavior, i.e., improved individual health. However, if the targeted behavior already constitutes the norm, it becomes very difficult to observe additional, incremental benefits due to the presence of a so-called “ceiling effect.” The data of Experiment 2 in particular indicate that the to-be-changed behavior was already the norm as more than 90% percent of all participants already used the stairs (and decided to not use the elevator). Supporting this ceiling effect argument, the measured level of stair use was much higher than in other studies (e.g., Eves et al., 2006). This may be due to the fact that the university where we conducted the present studies puts a distinct emphasis on sustainability-related subjects that overlap in individual and public health. It appears plausible that students and employees who chose the university are aware of health- and sustainability-related topics and may therefore use stairs instead of elevators.

Apart from these person-related factors, the architectural infrastructure of the sites that we used may also account for the high percentage of stair users. The buildings may, in fact, be constructed in a way that (inadvertently) nudges stair use instead of elevator use. An infrastructural nudging influence may have been at play in our studies as individuals enter the buildings right into the stairwell, while the elevators are not highly visible at first glance (see Supplementary Figures 1, 2, 5). Centrality and accessibility of stairs constitute key factors for individuals to use them (e.g., Nicoll, 2007; Bassett et al., 2013).

As outlined in the introduction, the literature offers different and competing predictions regarding whether precise or round numbers more effectively foster stair (vs. elevator) use. For now, the present pattern of results leads to the conclusion that the numerical precision of a health risk message is not substantial. This empirical null finding should be treated with caution, however, as (1) the evident ceiling effect in Experiment 2 may have masked any potential difference in round vs. precise risk messages and/or (2) the true difference between precise and round message interventions may be smaller than the moderately sized effect for which we powered, rendering future research necessary to illuminate these competing predictions (and underlying mechanisms).

Assessing the effect of our health risk messages on the individual level would allow for a nested multilevel model, which we cannot use due to our coarse data measurement. We refrained from identifying individuals for ethical reasons and did not reach out to volunteering individuals to reveal the true effect of our intervention on the sample level and to omit a self-selection bias. While we cannot fully rule out that the observed (null-) effect is driven by specific individuals, we consider this highly unlikely: A spill-over effect between our experimental conditions is unlikely as buildings from different faculties were chosen. In any case, this is of relatively minor interest as we observe a ceiling effect, which is the strongest explanation for our (null-) findings. Nonetheless, future research may further assess the (in-) efficacy of nudging interventions on an individual level to control for further unpredicted, interpersonal factors (see, e.g., Vetter and Kutzner, 2016; Venema et al., 2019; Raghoebar et al., 2020).

As a first step, future studies could present participants with both types of health risk messages and assess participants’ willingness to use stairs as a function of these health risks. Follow-up studies could illuminate the underlying processes accounting for a differential effectiveness of round or precise health risk message. Apart from this, future studies should attempt to study the effect of health risk messages in settings, in which both options (e.g., stairs vs. elevator) are similarly visible and attractive—without an architecturally built-in stair nudge. To assure that the targeted behavior does not yet constitute the prevalent norm, researchers should certainly approximate the percentage of the desired behavior in a brief pilot study. In conclusion, future studies are needed to conclusively disentangle whether the present findings (1) constitute a true null effect that—for problematic file-drawer distortions (Friese and Frankenbach, 2019)—should not disappear in our file drawer or (2) solely emerged because of unforeseen ceiling effects, for which our risk message interventions did not succeed to facilitate incremental benefits in terms of elevated health behavior.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found in the following link: https://osf.io/t3b96/?view_only=0064dbb0a69346c898328bf12c99f160.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval were not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed equally and qualify for authorship of the paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://osf.io/t3b96/?view_only=0064dbb0a69346c898328bf12c99f160

Footnotes

1. ^Diverging from our pre-registrations we did not examine the effect of (descriptive) social norms as we originally stated. We thank our reviewers for reminding us that we have diverged from our original goal during our research process.

References

Avitsland, A., Solbraa, A. K., and Riiser, A. (2017). Promoting workplace stair climbing: sometimes, not interfering is the best. Arch. Public Health 75:2. doi: 10.1186/s13690-016-0170-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bassett, D. R., Browning, R., Conger, S. A., Wolff, D. L., and Flynn, J. I. (2013). Architectural design and physical activity: an observational study of staircase and elevator use in different buildings. J. Phys. Act. Health 10, 556–562. doi: 10.1123/jpah.10.4.556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burger, J. M., and Shelton, M. (2011). Changing everyday health behaviors through descriptive norm manipulations. Soc. Influ. 6, 69–77. doi: 10.1080/15534510.2010.542305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 112, 155–158. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coleman, K. J., and Gonzalez, E. C. (2001). Promoting stair use in a US-Mexico border community. Am. J. Public Health 91, 2007–2009. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.91.12.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Wijk, R. A., Maaskant, A. J., Polet, I. A., Holthuysen, N. T., van Kleef, E., and Vingerhoeds, M. H. (2016). An in-store experiment on the effect of accessibility on sales of wholegrain and white bread in supermarkets. PLoS One 11:e0151915. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0151915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DellaVigna, S., and Linos, E. (2020). RCTs to scale: Comprehensive evidence from two nudge units. NBER Working Paper Series 27594, 73. Available at: http://www.nber.org/papers/w27594 (Accessed October 28, 2020).

Google Scholar

Diaz, K. M., and Shimbo, D. (2013). Physical activity and the prevention of hypertension. Curr. Hypertens. Rep. 15, 659–668. doi: 10.1007/s11906-013-0386-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eves, F. F., Webb, O. J., Griffin, C., and Chambers, J. (2012). A multi-component stair climbing promotional campaign targeting calorific expenditure for worksites; a quasi-experimental study testing effects on behaviour, attitude and intention. BMC Public Health 12:423. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-423

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eves, F. F., Webb, O. J., and Mutrie, N. (2006). A workplace intervention to promote stair climbing: Greater effects in the overweight. Obesity 14, 2210–2216. doi: 10.1038/oby.2006.259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friese, M., and Frankenbach, J. (2019). p-Hacking and publication bias interact to distort meta-analytic effect size estimates. Psychol. Methods 25, 456–471. doi: 10.1037/met0000246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furedi, A. (1999). The public health implications of the 1995 ‘pill scare’. Hum. Reprod. Update 5, 621–626. doi: 10.1093/humupd/5.6.621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gigerenzer, G., Gaissmaier, W., Kurz-Milcke, E., Schwartz, L. M., and Woloshin, S. (2008). Helping doctors and patients make sense of health statistics. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 8, 53–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6053.2008.00033.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, D. J., Linde, J. A., Cousins, J. M., and Jeffery, R. W. (2013). Environmental modifications and 2-year measured and self-reported stair-use: A worksite randomized trial. J. Prim. Prev. 34, 413–422. doi: 10.1007/s10935-013-0323-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grice, H. P. (1975). “Logic and conversation,” in Syntax and Semantics 3: Speech Acts. eds. P. Cole and J. L. Morgan (New York, NY: Academic Press), 41–58.

Google Scholar

Hales, C. M., Carroll, M. D., Fryar, C. D., and Ogden, C. L. (2020). Prevalence of obesity and severe obesity among adults: United States, 2017–2018. NCHS Data Brief. Available at: https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/databriefs/db360-h.pdf (Accessed July 6, 2021).

Google Scholar

Hegde, S. M., and Solomon, S. D. (2015). Influence of physical activity on hypertension and cardiac structure and function. Curr. Hypertens. Rep. 17:77. doi: 10.1007/s11906-015-0588-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janssen, E., Ruiter, R. A. C., and Waters, E. A. (2018). Combining risk communication strategies to simultaneously convey the risks of four diseases associated with physical inactivity to socio-demographically diverse populations. J. Behav. Med. 41, 318–332. doi: 10.1007/s10865-017-9894-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkins, S. C., Harris, A. J. L., and Lark, R. M. (2019). When unlikely outcomes occur: the role of communication format in maintaining communicator credibility. J. Risk Res. 22, 537–554. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2018.1440415

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jerez-Fernandez, A., Angulo, A. N., and Oppenheimer, D. M. (2014). Show me the numbers: precision as a cue to others’ confidence. Psychol. Sci. 25, 633–635. doi: 10.1177/0956797613504301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jordan, S., Weiß, M., Krug, S., and Mensink, G. B. M. (2012). Überblick über Primärpräventive Maßnahmen zur Förderung von Körperlicher Aktivität in Deutschland. Bundesgesundheitsbl. Gesundheitsforsch. Gesundheitsschutz 55, 73–81. doi: 10.1007/s00103-011-1396-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47, 263–292.

Google Scholar

Kerr, J., Eves, F., and Carroll, D. (2001). Can posters prompt stair use in a worksite environment? J. Occup. Health 43, 205–207. doi: 10.1539/joh.43.205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerr, N. A., Yore, M. M., Ham, S. A., Kerr, N. A., Yore, M. M., and Ham, S. A. (2004). Increasing stair use in a worksite through environmental changes. Am. J. Health Promot. 18, 312–316. doi: 10.4278/0890-1171-18.4.312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kettle, K., and Häubl, G. (2010). Numeric fluency and preference. Adv. Consum. Res. 37, 150–152.

Google Scholar

King, D., and Janiszewski, C. (2011). The sources and consequences of the fluent processing of numbers. J. Mark. Res. 48, 327–341. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.48.2.327

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landais, L. L., Damman, O. C., Schoonmade, L. J., Timmermans, D. R. M., Verhagen, E. A. L. M., and Jelsma, J. G. M. (2020). Choice architecture interventions to change physical activity and sedentary behavior: a systematic review of effects on intention, behavior and health outcomes during and after intervention. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 17:47. doi: 10.1186/s12966-020-00942-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehner, M., Mont, O., and Heiskanen, E. (2016). Nudging – A promising tool for sustainable consumption behaviour? J. Clean. Prod. 134, 166–177. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2015.11.086

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, A. L., and Eves, F. F. (2012). Testing the theory underlying the success of point-of-choice prompts: A multi-component stair climbing intervention. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 13, 126–132. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loschelder, D. D., Friese, M., Schaerer, M., and Galinsky, A. D. (2016). The too-much-precision effect: When and why precise anchors backfire with experts. Psychol. Sci. 27, 1573–1587. doi: 10.1177/0956797616666074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mertens, I. L., and Van Gaal, L. F. (2000). Overweight, obesity, and blood pressure: The effects of modest weight reduction. Obes. Res. 8, 270–278. doi: 10.1038/oby.2000.32

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller-Riemenschneider, F., Nocon, M., Reinhold, T., and Willich, S. N. (2010). Promotion of physical activity using point-of-decision prompts in Berlin underground stations. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 7, 3063–3070. doi: 10.3390/ijerph7083063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicoll, G. (2007). Spatial measures associated with stair use. Am. J. Health Promot. 21, 346–352. doi: 10.4278/0890-1171-21.4s.346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norton, K., Norton, L., and Sadgrove, D. (2010). Position statement on physical activity and exercise intensity terminology. J. Sci. Med. Sport 13, 496–502. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2009.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfeifer, K., Winfried, B., Ferrari, N., Füzéki, E., Geidl, W., Graf, C., et al. (2016). Nationale Empfehlungen für Bewegung und Bewegungsförderung. eds. A. Rütten and K. Pfeifer, 128. FAU Erlangen-Nürnberg.

Google Scholar

Pucher, J., and Buehler, R. (2008). Making cycling irresistible: Lessons from the Netherlands, Denmark and Germany. Transp. Rev. 28, 495–528. doi: 10.1080/01441640701806612

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puig-Ribera, A., and Eves, F. F. (2010). Promoting stair climbing in Barcelona: similarities and differences with interventions in English-speaking populations. Eur. J. Pub. Health 20, 100–102. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckp059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raghoebar, S., Van Kleef, E., and Vet, E. D. (2020). Increasing the proportion of plant-based foods available to shift social consumption norms and food choice among non-vegetarians. Sustainability 12:5371. doi: 10.3390/su12135371

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, Y., Hazlewood, W. R., Marshall, P., Dalton, N., and Hertrich, S. (2010). “Ambient influence: Can twinkly lights lure and abstract representations trigger behavioral change?” UbiComp’10 - Proceedings of the 2010 ACM Conference on Ubiquitous Computing. September 26–29, 2010; (New York, NY, USA: Association for Computing Machinery), 261–270.

Google Scholar

Scheibehenne, B., Jamil, T., and Kadiyali, E.-J. (2016). Bayesian Evidence Synthesis Can Reconcile Seemingly Inconsistent Results: The Case of Hotel Towel Reuse. Psychol. Sci. 27, 1043–1046. doi: 10.1177/0956797616644081

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schienkiewitz, A., Mensink, G. B. M., Kuhnert, R., and Lange, C. (2017). Übergewicht und Adipositas bei Erwachsenen in Deutschland. J. Health Monit. 2, 21–28. doi: 10.17886/RKI-GBE-2017-025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultze, T., and Loschelder, D. D. (2020). How numeric advice precision affects advice taking. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 34, 303–310. doi: 10.1002/bdm.2211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sunstein, C. R., and Thaler, R. H. (2008). Nudge - Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness. New Haven: Yale University Press.

Google Scholar

Thomas, M., Simon, D. H., and Kadiyali, V. (2010). The price precision effect: Evidence from laboratory and market data. Mark. Sci. 29, 175–190. doi: 10.1287/mksc.1090.0512

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuso, P. (2015). Strategies to increase physical activity. Perm. J. 19, 84–88. doi: 10.7812/TPP/14-242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. J. Risk Uncertain 5, 297–323. doi: 10.1007/BF00122574

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Venema, T. A. G., Kroese, F. M., De Vet, E., and De Ridder, D. T. D. (2019). The one that I want: Strong personal preferences render the center-stage nudge redundant. Food Qual. Prefer. 78:103744. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.103744

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vetter, M., and Kutzner, F. (2016). Nudge me if you can - how defaults and attitude strength interact to change behavior. Compr. Results Social Psychol. 1, 8–34. doi: 10.1080/23743603.2016.1139390

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visschers, V. H., Meertens, R. M., Passchier, W. W., and De Vries, N. N. (2009). Probability information in risk communication: a review of the research literature. Risk Anal.: Int. J. 29, 267–287. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2008.01137.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wachinger, G., Renn, O., Begg, C., and Kuhlicke, C. (2013). The risk perception paradox - implications for governance and communication of natural hazards. Risk Anal. 33, 1049–1065. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01942.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wadhwa, M., and Zhang, K. (2015). This number just feels right: the impact of roundedness of price numbers on product evaluations. J. Consum. Res. 41, 1172–1185. doi: 10.1086/678484

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Welsh, M. B., Navarro, D. J., and Begg, S. H. (2011). “Number preference, precision and implicit confidence.” in Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. eds. L. Carlson, C. Hoelscher, and T. ShipleyJuly, 20–31, 2011; 1521–1526.

Google Scholar

Winkielman, P., Schwarz, N., Fazendeiro, T. A., and Reber, R. (2003). “The hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative judgment,” in The Psychology of Evaluation: Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion. eds. J. Musch and K. C. Klauer (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 189–217.

Google Scholar

Witteman, H. O., Zikmund-Fisher, B. J., Waters, E. A., Gavaruzzi, T., and Fagerlin, A. (2011). Risk estimates from an online risk calculator are more believable and recalled better when expressed as integers. J. Med. Internet Res. 13:e54. doi: 10.2196/jmir.1656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2005). The European Health Report 2005. Public health action for healthier children and populations. WHO Library Cataloguing in Publication Data. Available at: http://www.euro.who.int/__data/assets/pdf_file/0004/82435/E87325.pdf (Accessed July 6, 2021).

Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2010). Global recommendations on physical activity for health. WHO Press. Available at: https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/44399/9789241599979_eng.pdf?sequence=1 (Accessed July 6, 2021).

Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2015). The European Health Report 2015. Targets and beyond – reaching new frontiers in evidence. Available at: http://www.euro.who.int/__data/assets/pdf_file/0006/288645/European-health-report-2015-full-book-en.pdf (Accessed July 6, 2021).

Google Scholar

Yu, S., Yarnell, J. W. G., Sweetnam, P. M., and Murray, L. (2003). What level of physical activity protects against premature cardiovascular death? The Caerphilly study. Heart 89, 502–506. doi: 10.1136/heart.89.5.502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y. C., and Schwarz, N. (2012). How and why 1 year differs from 365 days: a conversational logic analysis of inferences from the granularity of quantitative expressions. J. Consum. Res. 39, 248–259. doi: 10.1086/662612

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: nudging, stair use, health, health risk perception, numeric precision

Citation: Krull S, Boecker L and Loschelder DD (2021) The Power and Peril of Precise vs. Round Health Message Interventions to Increase Stair Use. Front. Psychol. 12:624198. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.624198

Received: 30 October 2020; Accepted: 28 June 2021;
Published: 27 July 2021.

Edited by:

Eric Brymer, Australian College of Applied Psychology (ACAP), Australia

Reviewed by:

Tina A. G. Venema, Aarhus University, Denmark
Marco Tagliabue, Oslo Metropolitan University, Norway

Copyright © 2021 Krull, Boecker and Loschelder. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sebastian Krull, c2ViYXN0aWFuLmtydWxsQHVuaS1wYWRlcmJvcm4uZGU=; David D. Loschelder, bG9zY2hlbGRlckBsZXVwaGFuYS5kZQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.