Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 12 May 2021
Sec. Movement Science

What Cognitive Mechanism, When, Where, and Why? Exploring the Decision Making of University and Professional Rugby Union Players During Competitive Matches

  • 1Faculty of Health and Life Sciences, Coventry University, Coventry, United Kingdom
  • 2Research Centre for Sport Coaching, Carnegie School of Sport, Leeds Beckett University, Leeds, United Kingdom

Over the past 50 years decision making research in team invasion sport has been dominated by three research perspectives, information processing, ecological dynamics, and naturalistic decision making. Recently, attempts have been made to integrate perspectives, as conceptual similarities demonstrate the decision making process as an interaction between a players perception of game information and the individual and collective capability to act on it. Despite this, no common ground has been found regarding what connects perception and action during performance. The differences between perspectives rest on the role of stored mental representations, that may, or may not facilitate the retrieval of appropriate responses in time pressured competitive environments. Additionally, in team invasion sports like rugby union, the time available to players to perceive, access memory and act, alters rapidly between specific game situations. Therefore, the aim of this study was to examine theoretical differences and the mechanisms that underpin them, through the vehicle of rugby union. Sixteen semi-elite rugby union players took part in two post-game procedures to explore the following research objectives; (i) to consider how game situations influence players perception of information; (ii) to consider how game situations influence the application of cognitive mechanisms whilst making decisions; and (iii) to identify the influence of tactics and/or strategy on player decision making. Deductive content analysis and elementary units of meaning derived from self-confrontation elicitation interviews indicate that specific game situations such as; the lineout, scrum or open phases of play or the tackle situation in attack or defence all provide players with varying complexity of perceptual information, formed through game information and time available to make decisions. As time increased, players were more likely to engage with task-specific declarative knowledge-of the game, stored as mental representations. As time diminished, players tended to diagnose and update their knowledge-in the game in a rapid fashion. Occasionally, when players described having no time, they verbalised reacting on instinct through a direct connection between perception and action. From these findings, clear practical implications and directions for future research and dissemination are discussed.

Introduction

At all levels of participation, the game of rugby union is an intense physical contest, where frequent high-speed collisions and forceful contacts define each phase of the game (Garraway et al., 2000; Reardon et al., 2017; Tee et al., 2018). The increasing physical demands of the sport that have come with its professionalisation, has been matched by a focus on developing the physical robustness of all players (Cunniffe et al., 2009; Austin et al., 2011; Tee et al., 2017). Consequently, where physical markers may have previously differentiated players, the gains in this area are now becoming more marginal (O’Connor and Larkin, 2015). A more consistent differentiator of performance in invasion sports can be aligned with expertise in game intelligence and decision making (Farrow and Raab, 2008; Memmert, 2011; O’Connor and Larkin, 2015; Gleeson and Kelly, 2020). This work shows that players, both individually and collectively, must be able to consistently select the appropriate courses of action to meet their intended goal, to score more points than their opponent (Richards et al., 2012, 2017). Given these consistent findings, supporting a coach’s understanding of and role in developing this skill set seems crucial (Bar-Eli et al., 2011).

The significance of player decision making in invasion sports is emphasised by the ever-increasing volume of research dedicated to making sense of it (Toner et al., 2015; Araújo et al., 2019; Raab and Araújo, 2019; Williams and Jackson, 2019; Gleeson and Kelly, 2020). Currently, there is no agreed perspective on how players make decisions or, in turn, how coaches can best develop their players decision making (Araújo et al., 2019; Raab and Araújo, 2019; Williams and Jackson, 2019). Instead, three distinct views have emerged that offer different interpretations of the role of cognitive mechanisms within a players decision making process (Gigerenzer and Selten, 2002; Raab, 2003). Predominately, these differences centre on the presence or absence of memory representations that facilitate the selection of a course of action (Cappuccio, 2015; Raab and Araújo, 2019). The information processing view is that decisions are made through a deliberate and conscious interaction with memory representations, which have been formed over time (Toner and Moran, 2015; Birch et al., 2019). Conversely, the ecological view is that decision making occurs through a direct connection between a player and their environment, without reference to memory representation (Cappuccio, 2015; Raab and Araújo, 2019). Finally, the naturalistic decision making view is that conscious recourse to memory representations is dependent on the typicality of the information presented to the decision maker (Klein et al., 1986, 2010); typical situations reduce reference to memory representations (Macquet, 2009; Macquet and Kragba, 2015; Johnston and Morrison, 2016). The divergency in viewpoint has catalysed authors to explain findings in different ways and adopt distinct terminology to capture similar concepts. The question of language is also confounded by conflicting practical implications offered across perspectives (Richards and Collins, 2020). Unfortunately, coaches are left to navigate through convoluted solutions to an intricate practical problem. To temper these differences, Ashford et al. (2021) developed a communal language, housed in a unified conceptual framework, which attempts to facilitate the assimilation of knowledge borne from each perspective and to progress collective understanding of player decision making (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A unified conceptual framework for player decision making in invasion team sports (Ashford et al., 2021).

The unified conceptual framework compartmentalises decision making into three components: the game; the player; and the coach. None are mutually exclusive, as they interact with the other (Passos et al., 2008; Richards et al., 2017). The Game sits at the centre, as the goal and rules of the game interact to create problems that both the Player and the Coach need to solve. The player interacts directly with both the game and the coach, by performing shared solutions to the problems the game presents (Ashford et al., 2021). The Coach acts as a central agent in (co)creating, sharing and developing a view of the game with players through on and off field planning and delivery of coaching.

For players, the decision making process is actuated by the perception of The Game (Correia et al., 2012; Johnston and Morrison, 2016). Rules create the landscape of contextualised game information that players, in their quest to achieve the Pre-Lusory goal (i.e., to score more points than your opponent; Suits, 1978), are individually and collectively required to act on (Correia et al., 2012; Macquet and Kragba, 2015; Johnston and Morrison, 2016; Birch et al., 2019). Furthermore, the rules constrain players to select from a finite list of legal actions (Ashford et al., 2020).

Through interaction with the game, decision making of The Player is, in part, governed through task dependent Perception of Information (Ashford et al., 2021). Research has demonstrated that skilled perception is task dependent, that is, player’s visual fixations and verbal descriptions change in line with changes in the demands of the task, presumably as they search for means to a successful outcome (Roca et al., 2011, 2012, 2013). Moreover, expert players demonstrate an improved connection to the information offered by the performance environment over time (Abernethy et al., 2008; Esteves et al., 2012; Araújo et al., 2019; Williams and Jackson, 2019). Players’ perception of game information is inextricably linked with an Individual and Collective Capability to Act (Silva et al., 2014; Richards et al., 2017); more specifically, players Actions-for the Game, Knowledge-of the Game and Knowledge-in the Game. Actions-for the Game are shaped by the physical (e.g., speed) (Passos et al., 2012) and technical (Paterson et al., 2013) attributes of players. With experience, game information and relevant capabilities become coupled and refined, as inherent understanding of action capabilities more effectively guides the player to highly salient game information (Jackson et al., 2006; Davids et al., 2008). Knowledge-of the Game connects the search for game information and the selection of a relevant capability to act together through a deeper understanding of why specific decisions are more appropriate in one situation than another (McPherson and Vickers, 2004; Poplu et al., 2008; Lex et al., 2015; Johnston and Morrison, 2016). This characteristic of expertise has been attributed to the development over time of task-specific declarative knowledge stored as verbal and/or visual memory representations (Birch et al., 2019). Additionally, it has been proposed that players are required to update their knowledge-in the game in order to adapt to changes in expectations (Macquet and Kragba, 2015). So, when new information arises, such as an opposing player showing unexpected behaviour, player’s situation-specific knowledge requires updating (McPherson and Vickers, 2004).

Finally, within the framework, the Coach is responsible for creating a Common Frame of Reference to facilitate the collective response of their players to the game (Tee et al., 2018; Ashford et al., 2021). There is a general consensus across perspectives that the coordination of team play is a product of reference to a global top-down shared mental model (Richards et al., 2012, 2017) and the local bottom-up self-organisation of players through a shared perception and response to game information (Steiner et al., 2017; Ribeiro et al., 2019a, b), which the coach can influence by developing their players’ capabilities. However, at this point, this common ground remains conceptual (Steiner et al., 2017; Ribeiro et al., 2019a; Richards and Collins, 2020) as empirical research has been reduced to the study of top-down (Richards et al., 2012) and bottom-up (Bourbousson et al., 2010, 2011) approaches alone (Richards and Collins, 2020). In response, the current study investigates when, how and why players adopt top-down (shared mental model) or bottom-up (shared affordances) decision making processes (Richards and Collins, 2020).

In developing the unified conceptual framework Ashford et al. (2021) created a view on the, often disputed, role of cognition in decision making in team sports (Raab, 2003). The framework proposes that it is important to understand what cognitive mechanisms facilitate decision making in team sport, when, where and why. The work of Changeux and Dehaene (1989), Changeux (2009) have offered a view on the presence/absence of memory representations from the school of neurobiology;

“What is the mechanism that links them together to form a uniquely meaningful entity? This question – known among neurobiologists as the binding problem – is far from being settled” (Changeux, 2009; p. 54).

Similarly, the purpose of this paper is an empirical investigation into what mechanism is used by rugby union players, when, where and why in their decision making. Mahlo (1974) once suggested that a players decision making in team invasion sports, will sit on a continuum between direct perception, a process of analysis and execution, with memory acting as a mediator between perception and analysis. Conversely, Changeux’s (2009) work in neurobiology and neuropsychology has suggested that humans synaptic processes that account for analysis decision making processes, may be too slow to deal with the speed of game situations. Interestingly, this justification forms the conceptual basis for a direct relationship between perception and action offered from the ecological approach to decision making (Raab and Araújo, 2019). Changeux (2009) has championed the presence of memory representations within their work, suggesting that even when situations remove the possibility for conscious thought, representations create neural embodiments of meaning that allow humans to satisfy a situation. That is, even when players rely on intuition and non-conscious decision making when the time to analyse is absent, networks of neurons fire in a synchronized fashion through mental representations that are deemed meaningful over time. This evidence offers the view that meaningful representations, stored in memory are the central mechanism for effective decision making.

In light of the evidence reviewed, one way a deeper consideration of the presence or absence of mental representations can be investigated is to explore different sport-specific contexts within which players are making decisions (Bar-Eli et al., 2011; Birch et al., 2019). The laws of rugby union create game situations that vary drastically in the complexity of information, time available to decide and the desired technical/physical requirements (Tee et al., 2018). Similarly, Raab (2003) suggested that all game moments within team sports possess a level of cognitive complexity, which is defined by the amount of choices, attributes, and time available to a player within a decision making moment. Moreover, he hypothesised that placing emphasis during practice on the development of different forms of capability to act would have specific advantages, which depend on the cognitive complexity of the task environment. Raab found that novice handball players who had developed explicit knowledge-of the task were advantaged when cognitive complexity was high, whereas those who had developed actions-for rather than knowledge-of the task (i.e., implicit learning) were advantaged when cognitive complexity was low (Raab, 2003). Similarly, Basevitch et al. (2019) compared expert and near expert soccer players intuition and option generation in a decision making task. Participants were required to pick a first option and follow up with other appropriate actions to video footage, but were also required respond to occluded images at 400 ms, 200 ms and 0ms in both cued and non-cued environments. Their findings demonstrated that experts intuition and option generation were complimentary of one another across both conditions. At 400ms option generation was deemed more successful, whilst at 0ms and in a non-cued environment, their first option was significantly more successful. Time available seemed to coincide with their decision making process. These findings from Raab (2003) and Basevitch et al. (2019) offer an implication that the application of decision making processes at an individual level may depend on the demands of different game moments (Araújo and Bourbousson, 2016).

Raab (2003) suggested that investigation into implicit or explicit cognitive processes within team sports must consider the role of the environment where one must question whether a cognitive mechanism, whether absent, good or bad, rational or irrational, may be best understood relative to its contextual surroundings (Raab, 2003). Consequently, specific game situations may demand a player’s use of meaningful cognitive mechanisms to make decisions (Raab, 2003; Lex et al., 2015). For instance, the time (Basevitch et al., 2019) and options (Raab, 2003) offered to players by game situations may demand an implicit coupling of perception and action resulting in player’s self-organisation (Passos et al., 2008; Raab and Araújo, 2019), intuition and heuristic driven decision making (Raab and Laborde, 2011; Raab and Gigerenzer, 2015), diagnosis of limited options rapidly (Macquet and Fleurance, 2007; Macquet, 2009) or deliberate evaluation of situational probability and mental rehearsal of possible courses of action (Macquet, 2009; Macquet and Kragba, 2015). Furthermore, at a collective team level, situations of high cognitive complexity may benefit from reference to a global top-down shared mental model (Richards et al., 2012, 2017; Richards and Collins, 2020), whilst situations of low cognitive complexity may be best to rest on the local bottom-up shared perception of, and response to game information by team members (Correia et al., 2012). Therefore, the objectives of this study were clear; (i) to consider how game situations influence players perception of information; (ii) to consider how game situations influence the application of cognitive mechanisms whilst making decisions, more specifically players actions-for, knowledge-of and knowledge-in the game; and (iii) to identify the influence of tactics and/or strategy, such as a common frame of reference, on player decision making.

Recent research considering player decision making in team invasion sports have adopted self-confrontation interviews to collect meaningful data, which present players with footage of their past actions in order to stimulate verbalisations of their experiences during the event (Macquet and Kragba, 2015; Johnston and Morrison, 2016; Gleeson and Kelly, 2020), such as their lived decision making experience (Varela and Shear, 1999; Macquet, 2009; Bourbousson et al., 2010, 2011). The current study adopted this approach to consider the perspective and thoughts of the player in the context of a single team invasion sport, rugby union (Gleeson and Kelly, 2020; Richards and Collins, 2020). In an attempt to mitigate the fallibility associated with data generated through verbalisation methods (Cappuccio, 2015; Raab and Araújo, 2019), the self-confrontation process was initially framed along a continuum, where players were able to classify their decision making as No-thought, Fast-thought or Slow-thought (Macquet, 2009; Macquet and Kragba, 2015; Johnston and Morrison, 2016). Considering the research aims, we hypothesise that individual players would describe their decision making dependent on particular game situations where decisions were an implicit (no thought) direct connection between their actions for the game and their environment (Cappuccio, 2015; Raab and Araújo, 2019); fast and frugal through updating their knowledge-in the game (Bennis and Pachur, 2006; Schulz, 2018); or slow and deliberate in an offline fashion through their knowledge-of the game (Richards et al., 2017; Raab and Araújo, 2019). Whilst from a team perspective, we hypothesise that players would share instances of implicit (no thought) bottom-up organisation of players through a shared perception and response to game information (Ribeiro et al., 2019a, b). fast and frugal interactions with teammates (Araújo et al., 2015) and slow top-down coordination (Steiner et al., 2017) through global tactical principles informed by a shared mental model.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Sixteen adult rugby union players from a team in the highest tier of British university (UNI) rugby (n = 8) and a professional (PRO) rugby team (n = 8) volunteered to take part in the study (see Table 1 for playing positions). The UNI players ranged in age from 20 to 27 years (M = 22.5 years, SD = 2.1 years) and had been playing rugby for 7-22 years (M = 14.5 years, SD = 4.5 years). The PRO players ranged in age from 19 to 29 years (M = 24, SD = 3.42 years) and had been playing rugby for 11 to 17 years (M = 14.6 years, SD = 2.3 years). Within the UNI group, two players had previously played professionally whilst another two had represented England students. In alignment with Swann et al. (2015) both groups of players have been identified as semi-elite populations, as the PRO Team participate in the second tier of English Rugby Union, whilst the highest tier of British University rugby union now require’s players to train as full time athletes and regularly results in players progression to the highest tier of Professional rugby. For the purpose of this study findings have been separated by group to identify similarities and differences in accordance with the study aims (Johnston and Morrison, 2016).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Player positions by population.

Game Footage

UNI and PRO matches were filmed at the half-way line adjacent to the rugby pitch. The camera was operated throughout to ensure that all players included in the study were identified. Following each match, the individual player’s actions, and the game context (situations) were analysed by each team’s dedicated performance analyst. The player analysis was conducted within 24-h of the game using SportsCode Elite software (V11, Hudl, Lincoln, Nebraska, United States of America). This approach was taken to minimise the disruption of player’s typical interaction with performance analysis between games and because in-house analysts are better able to perceive individual and collective decisions and actions in reference to their teams’ tactical approach, whilst utilising repeatable methods of analysis. For every decision identified, the analyst clipped the footage 5 s prior and 7 s after the decision in order to capture the situation leading into and the outcome of the action, respectively. On occasions when actions lasted longer than 7 s the clip was extended. Video clips for each player were compiled into a film in the order they occurred during the match. For the purpose of data collection, each player’s video clips were then edited into a changed order using Adobe Premier Pro video editing software (Adobe, San Jose, California, United States of America) by the lead author. The rationale for this was to monitor the consistency of player’s decision classifications (Kinrade et al., 2015) and to reduce a possible learning effect between stages of the procedure.

Procedure

The data collection procedure took place in a mutually agreed, quiet location within 48-h of the final whistle of each game. UNI and PRO players completed this process twice (n = 32 games). Within the procedure, players were required to complete three stages of data collection; (i) unstimulated recall of involvements, actions and decisions made within the game; (ii) decision classification; and (iii) a repeat of decision classification alongside a self-confrontation interview. Each stage of the procedure was recorded using a dictaphone.

Stage 1: Unstimulated Recall

Prior to watching any game footage (Gleeson and Kelly, 2020), players were first asked to verbalise their involvements, actions and decisions within the game. The rationale for collecting unstimulated data was to identify what actions and decisions players could recount from the game without prior stimulation from game footage. This method served a clear purpose, as it enabled a consideration of the trustworthiness of the data captured during stage 3 of the procedure (Bourbousson et al., 2011). Furthermore, unstimulated verbalisations could then be cross referenced to stimulated verbalisations of the decision process during the same game moment.

Stage 2: Decision Classification

On both data collection occasions’ classifications of No-thought, Fast-thought and Slow-thought were defined for the players by the first author. No-thought classifications were defined as a decision where no conscious thought took place in leading to an action, Fast-thought classifications were defined as a rapid conscious decision leading to an action, whilst Slow-thought classifications were defined as a slow deliberate conscious decision where a number of options are considered leading to an action. Following this, players had the opportunity to ask clarification questions before beginning the video classification task. The task commenced when players were shown a video of their actions and decisions in the order they occurred during the game. Each clip was played once, and the video paused at its conclusion where the final frame remained on the screen. This initiated players to classify using a second laptop (with screen occluded) whether the clip represented a No-thought, Fast-thought or Slow-thought decision. Players had a 5 s limit to classify each decision before the researcher played the next clip (Mulligan et al., 2012).

Stage 3: Self-Confrontation Interviews

After responding to all clips once, the players repeated the decision classification process by watching the same clips presented in a different order. In the second round of decision classification the 5 s time limit was removed. Instead players were asked to verbalise how they came to their decision through self-confrontation elicitation interviews (Von Cranach and Harré, 1982; Feigean et al., 2018; Gleeson and Kelly, 2020). Within self-confrontation interviews players are likely to digress to reflect-on action or self-rationalise and justify their decision making (Gleeson and Kelly, 2020). Therefore, the interviewer encouraged the re-enactment of their lived experiences during the game via prompts such as “why slow/fast/no thought?;” “what are you thinking?;” “what information are you responding to?;” or “talk me through what happened?”.

Data Analysis

The reliability of the UNI and PRO players No-, Fast- and Slow-thought decision classifications between stages 2 and 3, were confirmed by calculating the intra-class correlation coefficient (Chronbach’s alpha: UNI = 0.92; PRO = 0.89). For the qualitative data, all interviews were audio recorded and transcribed verbatim. Each transcript was read numerous times to ensure familiarity and understanding (Taylor and Collins, 2019). Due to the large amount of qualitative data drawn from the unstimulated and self-confrontation interviews, the data was analysed in two stages. The first stage employed qualitative coding and theme development procedures commonly used in sport coaching research (Braun et al., 2016; Miles et al., 2020; North et al., 2020). Initially, verbalisations were attributed to Unstimulated responses and No-thought, Fast-thought and Slow-thought decision classifications. Within each component, the unified conceptual framework for decision making in team invasion sports was employed to conduct a deductive content analysis (Johnston and Morrison, 2016; Kyngäs et al., 2020). This allowed a hierarchy of deductive themes to emerge - perception of information, actions-for, knowledge-of, knowledge-in and a common frame of reference – which helped comparisons within and across populations to be made. Analysis was enhanced by using qualitative software (QSR NVivo12). To examine the relation between the frequency of No-, Fast-, and Slow-thought decision classifications made within each deductive theme by each player group chi-square tests of independence were performed.

The second stage explored each prescribed theme deductively to abduct “elementary units of meaning” in relation to each study aim (Thereau, 2003; Macquet and Kragba, 2015; Gleeson and Kelly, 2020). Furthermore, units explored player’s experiences of cognitive, perceptual, psychological, sensorimotor, physical and environmental events, inclusive of the game situation, teammates, opposition and the coach (Varela and Shear, 1999; Gleeson and Kelly, 2020). Where possible, codes were created in line with cognitive mechanisms underpinning the unified conceptual framework for decision making in team invasion sports (Ashford et al., 2021). Thus, due to the range of verbalisation data collected, inductive reasoning was used to abduct data that could not be satisfied by underpinning deductive themes. Again, results were compared across populations (Johnston and Morrison, 2016).

In accordance with recommendations by Tracy (2010), the credibility of the data was appraised through triangulation of quantitative decision-classification and qualitative self-confrontation interview data (Macquet, 2009). To safeguard sincerity in our approach a critical friend, independent of the authorship, was invited to review, comment and edit our deductive analysis and construction of elementary units of meaning for both UNI and PRO groups of players (Smith and Caddick, 2012; Cronin and Lowes, 2016). Finally, rigour was elevated by only including players who completed the full procedure twice1 and by conducting interviews within 48 h of the final whistle of the game in an agreed and consistent location (Tracy, 2010).

Results

To signpost the reader through the results, numerous examples of raw data have been included in Tables 2-4. Firstly, Table 2 displays elementary units of meaning constructed from Unstimulated verbalisation data, where player’s recollections were aligned to their decision classification and stimulated verbalisation during stage 3 of the procedure. Figure 2 presents quantitative findings, comprised of a deductive content analysis, specifically the collation of player’s decision classifications and references to each deductive theme; the perception of information; actions-for the game; knowledge-of the game; knowledge-in the game and a common frame of reference (Ashford et al., 2021). Table 3 demonstrates elementary units of meaning constructed to explore the decision making process and cognitive mechanisms described during stimulated verbalisations of decision making processes. Finally, data emerged, displaying how four distinct game moments in rugby union; the lineout; the scrum; defensive situations and offensive situations2, are commonly associated with distinct forms of decision making processes (see Table 4) as hypothesised by Raab (2003). However, the association is not just complexity, but also time available.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Thematic analysis of Unstimulated verbalisations, stimulated verbalisations and decision classifications of the same game moment [number in brackets denotes the number of player references to this theme].

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Thematic analysis of UNI and PRO Rugby Union players verbalisations of their decision making process [number in brackets denotes the number of references made to theme].

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. UNI and PRO Players verbalisations of decision making processes in response to specific game situations.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Percentage of No-, Fast- and Slow-thought decision classifications for UNI and PRO groups for each deductive theme.

Deductive Content Analysis

From the sixteen players who took part, 1896 decisions were verbalised during self-confrontation interviews (UNI: 826; PRO; 1070). Players in the UNI group verbalised 111 Unstimulated decisions whereas players in the PRO group verbalised 110 decisions. Both groups of players referenced their actions-for the game most often (UNI = 50%: PRO = 50%), followed by perception of information (UNI = 31.43%; PRO = 22.22%) and a common frame of reference (UNI = 20%; PRO = 17.78%). Knowledge-of (UNI = 7.14%; PRO = 6.67%), and knowledge-in (UNI = 2.86%; PRO = 3.33%) the game were referenced least often.

During the stimulated self-confrontation interview process, 83 and 119 No-thought, 318 and 471 Fast-thought and 314 and 370 Slow-thought decision classifications were made by the UNI and PRO players, respectively. Figure 2 presents the relative frequency of No-, Fast- and Slow-thought decision classifications made within each deductive theme. Chi-squared tests of independence confirmed differences in the frequencies of decision classification for each deductive theme (all X2 (1, N > 137) > 17.52, p < 0.01). Observation of Figure 2 shows that for both groups of players Fast-thought decision classification were most frequent within the perception of information theme. Fast-thought decision classifications were also prevalent within the actions-for the game theme, as were No-thought classifications; Slow-thought classifications were relatively less frequent. Within the knowledge-of and knowledge-in the game themes, no decisions were classified as No-thought and very few decisions were classified as No-thought within the common frame of reference theme. Furthermore, within the knowledge-of the game theme there is an observable difference in the relative frequency of Fast- and Slow-thought decisions between the two player groups. A large majority of decisions of UNI players were Slow-thought whereas PRO players show more of a balance of Fast- and Slow-thought decisions. Within the knowledge-in the game theme Fast-thought decisions were most prevalent for both player groups. Finally, within the common frame of reference theme while Slow-thought decision classifications were most prevalent for both groups of players, this seemed to be particularly the case for UNI players.

Elementary Units of Meaning

Unstimulated Verbalisations

Table 2 demonstrates that players, were able to recall and verbalise their decisions and thought process leading to a decision. These verbalisations were cross-referenced to player’s stimulated references to the same decisions during stage 3 of the data collection procedure. This analysis indicates that player’s often recalled decisions that were later classified as Fast-Thought and Slow-Thought, but not No-thought. Additionally, the units of meaning derived from unstimulated verbalisations were representative of the units abducted from Fast-thought and Slow-thought decision classifications. Player’s often only verbalised their decision making process in relation to an action that was significant in memory following the game (See Table 2).

Self-Confrontation Interviews

Table 3 exhibits the deductive and inductive elementary units of meaning that emerged from each deductive theme; perception of information, actions-for, knowledge-of, knowledge-in and a common frame of reference.

Perception of Information

No thought

Players described intuitively responding to audio information and having no time to think about their decision;

“Just caught the ball without any time to make a decision, felt afterward that I didn’t know what decision to make. Didn’t know whether to pass it, give it to **** out the back. I just carried.” (PRO Player 6, Game 2).

Players described perceiving discrete visual information, with specific reference to; their opponentsmovements, opponents body position, teammates movements and the rugby ball. For both populations’ players verbalise reacting without conscious thought to their opponents’ or teammates movements. For instance,

“Just tackled him, I’ve just stood up and he’s ran into me.” (PRO Player 6, Game 1).

Players also verbalised instances where seeing the ball initiated unconscious responses;

“The ball just kind of pops up, it’s not expected yeah I didn’t know or had a thought about who was around me or what was around me so, it was just catch and carry.” (UNI Player 4, Game 2)

Fast thought

For both groups the perception of information is referred to most frequently within Fast-thought classifications (> 34% of responses in both player groups). Eight elementary units of meaning included; audio information, haptic information, opponents body positions, opponent’s movements, teammates movements, defensive and offensive pictures, and time. Players from both groups refer to their opponents and teammates movements frequently;

“He’s stepping in, I’ve sort of anticipated him stepping in as well, then I thought about it, but it was definitely a last second thing.” (UNI Player 5, Game 1).

Players also refer to the perception of global information in the form of offensive and defensive pictures, for example;

“As I’m walking in I’m scanning to see what picture they’re showing me and then I’ll call wherever the space is.” (PRO Player 8, Game 1).

However, PRO players verbalised the perception of discrete information more often than the UNI group, such as their opponent’s body positions. The majority of these instances were verbalised during their decision making in the tackle area;

“As the tackle unfolded he started to fall toward the touchline at which point I pulled him and pressed his shoulders and head down toward the touchline. It was fast thought based on feel of, oh we can actually turn the ball over here, so I manipulated his body rather than going for the ball so he’d go out of touch.” (PRO Player 2, Game 1).

Slow thought

Units included; audio information, haptic information, the defensive and offensive picture, opponents’ movements, teammates movements and having time to perceive. Analysis demonstrates frequent references to more global sources of game information regarding the defensive or offensive picture provided by their opponents;

“The picture changes as we go up, I knew that I could get pressure on him as he didn’t have an option on the outside.” (PRO Player 2, Game 2).

Finally, both populations recall having sufficient time to perceive and interpret wider sources of game information, their opponents’ movements and/or their teammates movements whilst making decisions;

“A lot of time on the ball and a lot of time to compose myself. I was hoping the ball was going to run dead so when it held up I still had a lot of time on the ball so, when I picked it up and turned around I had about 15m to work with and compose myself.” (UNI Player 3, Game 2).

“I’ve got a lot of time. I’m the third guy away from the ruck so instantly I know it’s my job to go at their ball carrier.” (PRO Player 7, Game 1).

In summary, during No-thought verbalisations players recalled perceiving discrete information due to a lack of time to see, hear or feel anything else. Whilst during Fast-thought verbalisations, players referred to the perception of discrete sources of information; such as opponents’ movements or body positions, and global information; such as the defensive picture painted by their opponents to make decisions, whilst the PRO group recounted using discrete information to make decisions more often than the UNI group. Conversely, interpretation of Slow-thought verbalisations indicates that an increase in time offered by particular game situations results in a tendency to focus on global information, such as the offensive or defensive picture provided by their opponents (See Table 3).

Actions-for the Game

No thought

Player’s described reacting unconsciously to game information through their actions. For example, both PRO and UNI players describe similar processes in different game situations;

“It was just an instinctive pass on the inside. I’m under a bit of pressure and there’s my get out.” (PRO Player 1, Game 1).

“I don’t remember what was going on there, I definitely turned it over but it was bit of an instinct thing to just get the ball back.” (UNI Player 4, Game 2).

Less frequently, specific individuals on both teams indicated that rehearsal of actions allowed them to act appropriately. For example, the Hooker from the PRO group of players (Player 5) indicated that all of his lineout throws were No-thought;

“That’s just me following my process – we’ve rehearsed where he should be so I just throw it.” (Game 1).

Fast thought

Players describe moments of self-awareness whilst making decisions, however PRO players refer to this more frequently than the UNI group. For example;

“I’m sprinting thinking I’m going to compete in the air, but then in looking at the flight of the ball and I know that it’s too far in front of me and I know I’m not going to get to it.” (PRO Player 1, Game 2).

Players verbalisations regarding their actions-for the game often referred to having knowledge of their intentions to achieve a specific outcome. Players from both populations consistently refer to “knowing,” “having to,” or “needing to” act in a specific way to solve the problem in front of them. For example;

“I just dropped my shoulder to try and get through. I’m first to the ruck, my decision is always to clear any threat.” (PRO Player 3, Game 1).

Slow thought

Slow-thought verbalisations regarding players actions-for the game were more infrequent than No-Thought and Fast-thought classifications, but often recounted clear thought processes during decision making, irrespective of whether the outcome was successful or not. For example, UNI and PRO players recall being self-aware during their decision making;

“Because we ran a kick chase line, he was my opposite number and I nominated him, as my man, tracked him.” (UNI Player 2, Game 2)

“But instead I made him step back on my inside shoulder and he’s actually quite a good stepper for a big bloke, then I just thought I need to lunge at his ankles here to get him.” (PRO Player 6, Game 2)

Players also described being more aware of the capabilities of their teammates within their verbalisations of slow thought decisions;

“The reason being was ****, the prop, told me to run near him and he does it in training it’s like a proper old man’s trick just like nudging someone. So like, as I caught it I heard that and I thought s**t where’s that coming from. I looked up and saw him.” (PRO Player 5, Game 2).

Within Slow-thought decisions, most verbalisations of player’s actions-for the game tended to be dependent on intentions derived from Knowledge-of what action was appropriate for the game situation specifically.

To summarise, No-thought verbalisations referred exclusively to players unconscious reaction to perceptual information, whilst individual player’s made reference to the importance of rehearsal. Interpretations of the Fast-thought data suggests that their intentions, shaped by their perception of information, guided resultant action in time-pressured situations. Whilst, Slow-thought classifications indicated that action-for the game depend on a player’s, self-awareness, intentions and an awareness of their teammates action capabilities (See Table 3).

Knowledge-of the Game

Fast thought

Within fast thought classifications, players occasionally referred to their knowledge-of their opponents, roles and responsibilities, self-awareness and tactics. However, the results indicate that PRO players verbalised these units more often. Furthermore, they referred to tactical self-awareness and knowledge-of teammates when coming to fast thought decisions;

“He’s a very good off loader, so I knew that his first instinct would be to give the ball, so I jumped at the chance haha.” (PRO Player 5, Game 2)

Slow thought

Players referred to consciously using knowledge-of their; opponents, roles and responsibilities, self-awareness, tactics and their teammates whilst making decisions (>23% of responses). The main difference between the groups verbalisations rested on the PRO players making reference to knowledge-of their opponents to inform their decision making more frequently;

“I looked up here and saw their 7 **** was in front of me, who’s their most effective jackler, so I had time to think if he makes the tackle there’s less chance of him then making the turnover. Just thought, get him to make the tackle, stay tight.” (PRO Player 5, Game 1).

The vast majority of slow thought verbalisations regarding players knowledge-of the game make reference to the importance of executing specific roles and responsibilities that are essential within particular game situations;

“I have a specific job here. So as 9 picks I know that he’s my primary focus and then I’m just pushing off, but as he didn’t pass he steps back inside. The whole time I’m in this position I’ve got a role to fulfil.” (PRO Player 4, Game 2).

On numerous occasions both groups refer to perceiving and acting based on their tactical approach to the game;

“That’s our game plan, to play the bottom corners and that’s why I looked. Yeah that is the reason why I’ve looked. Erm, I’ve just kicked it because he was a bit high in the line.” (UNI Player 2, Game 1).

Thus, knowledge-of the game was rarely made reference to during UNI and PRO players Fast-thought verbalisations but on occasion, players referred to the team’s tactics and the roles and responsibilities that guided their decision making. The Professional players made reference to tactical self-awareness and using knowledge-of their teammates during Fast-thought classifications. Analysis of Slow-thought responses indicate that when players had time, they often referred to their knowledge-of tactics, roles and responsibilities as navigators of perceptual search and choice of action. In addition, the Professional players knowledge-of the game extended to knowledge of their opponents’ tendencies, tactics and behaviour (See Table 3). No reference to knowledge-of the game was made during No-thought verbalisations.

Knowledge-in the Game

Fast thought

Player’s referred to their knowledge-in the game most often during Fast-thought decision classifications. In coding these verbalisations into elementary units of meaning, players across both groups described that their expectations of the situation were regularly unsatisfied. Verbalisations indicate that players had to adapt quickly to changes in expectations regarding game situations. UNI players referred to adapting to changes in expectations frequently (>67%) during verbalisations of Fast-thought decisions, indicating a tendency to form unreliable expectations within game situations.

“I’m pretty sure I was supposed to run a 3 on that one, but I ended up getting the ball unexpectedly.” (UNI Player 4, Game 1).

In addition to changes in expectations, players from both groups describe the impact of game context on their decision making in Fast-thought instances;

“That was probably happening a bit quicker and like a said probably a bit of panic. The context of the game as well as they were coming back into it as well.” (UNI Player 1, Game 2).

Finally, UNI and PRO players refer to dealing with legitimate risks or threats to their offensive or defensive integrity. PRO Player 5 describes an instance where he’d noticed an opposing player, who he knew to be strong at competing for the ball, in close proximity as he was tackled;

“Then as I land, I’m more conscious of my groundwork because of the threat of ****, so I’m kicking my hips and rolling, so he just didn’t have a target to get on the ball.” (Game 1).

Slow thought

Players verbalised having to update their knowledge-in the game less often during Slow-thought decision classifications. However, units of elementary meaning demonstrate that when they did it coincided with similar themes; changes in expectations, the game context and realistic threats or risks to offensive or defensive integrity. However, PRO players made reference to the game context more often than the UNI players;

“Again this was a case of awareness on the field. I’m not wanting to take the ball to get personal glory out of what is a no win situation so told him to carry again” (PRO Player 1, Game 2).

Player’s form expectations of situations from their perception of the game situation and their knowledge-of the game, however these expectations are frequently unmet (See Table 3). Furthermore, following recognition of this, players tended to take the first option available to them. This is most evident within verbalisations of Fast-thought decision classifications where knowledge-in the game is referred to consistently. During Slow-thought verbalisations, the PRO players made reference to the use of game context, such as the score, the time left in the game and their awareness of offensive or defensive momentum more often than the UNI group. No reference to knowledge-in the game was made during No-thought verbalisations.

Common Frame of Reference

No thought

On five occasions PRO players referred to common language and tactical rules initiating action without conscious thought. For example, PRO Player 3 verbalised having “hit the ruck on autopilot” (Game 2) without thinking about it. One UNI player referred to the use of common language whilst verbalising a no thought decision.

Fast thought

Players often referred to the use of tactical rules to come to decisions within their Fast-thought classifications. For example, multiple players from the Professional group refer to a “tackler plus one” rule instigated by their coaches. Players often describe having to make a rapid judgment as to whether it is their responsibility to join the original tackler in order to slow the ball down;

“We have a tackler + 1 rule, so if you are going to go in there, be effective and slow the ball down.” (PRO Player 7, Game 1).

Similarly, UNI Player 8 indicated that they too follow tactical rules, for instance when their scrum half is caught up in the ruck situation, they resort to a typical course of action;

“That’s a fast thought, if 9 doesn’t go in we bullet. If 9 goes into the ruck it’s a pick and go, simple.” (Game 1).

These rules tend to coincide with the use of shared terminologies that are included in a wider common language between teammates and coaches. For the PRO players, 13 terms were verbalised that captured tactical rule’s and plays, whilst UNI players verbalised 16. Player’s recounted hearing, or using these terms to guide the decisions and actions of themselves and others in time-pressured situations;

“I was screaming 3 and because I was coming around the corner, I had a bit more depth. When I got outside the second last defender, it was essentially a 3 vs 1 on the edge” (UNI Player 4, Game 2).

In addition to common language, players also described general communication between them and their teammates in coming to decisions;

“**** told me just to carry so I just carried” (UNI Player 6, Game 2).

Slow thought

A common frame of reference was referred to frequently during verbalisations of Slow-thought classifications (both groups > 30%). Elementary units of meaning indicate a clear role of verbal and non-verbal communication between players when making decisions. These were coded into; a shared common language; general communication between teammates; and tactical rules. Both groups describe shared understanding of a common language that influences players collective and individual decision making by guiding perception to more relevant game information, informing teammates of information players are unable to perceive visually, and shaping clear intentions of how they should act given the circumstances. For example;

“He then brings us in for a “truck” which is a slow decision to carry the ball into contact. He then say’s “apple,” which is a box kick so I’ve got to get wide for the kick chase.” (PRO Player 5, Game 2).

“That was just us running an addidas, **** was running a hard line and then one of their lads jumped out and that meant **** was the only option.” (UNI Player 4, Game 2).

These terms also actuated players decisions to execute rehearsed tactical rules. For instance;

“The call reminds me of my course of action after I’ve thrown the ball in. At 99% of mauls my role is to get the ball at the back, it’s just my job.” (Professional Player 3, Game 1).

Finally, players recall the use of general communication between them and their teammates whilst making collective decisions;

“Our aim is generally to get the ball to the edge within our team pattern so there was no point me carrying it because we’d have had a weaker clear if I’d carry it, so I told him to carry it and talked him through carrying it and then knew I was hitting the ruck straight away.” (PRO Player 1, Game 1).

To summarise these findings, players refer to a common frame of reference across all decision classifications (See Table 3). Both UNI and PRO players refer to the use of a common language that is made up of specific terminologies that guide players collective perception of information and actions-for the game within their decision making processes. These terms house tactical rules for players that are included in a wider common language, resulting in planned and prescribed actions and movements. Such terms are referred to most often during Slow-thought verbalisations, whilst occasionally in Fast-thought and on a handful of occasions during No-thought classifications.

Game Moments

Table 4 highlights the perceptual information, cognitive mechanisms and tactical strategies verbalised by players in specific game situations, most specifically, the lineout, the scrum, defensive situations and offensive situations.

Lineout

Individual players from each team were given sole responsibility for running and calling their lineout. Players used a common language shared between their forward unit to initiate the movement coordination of a throw by the hooker, lift by two supporting players and jump and catch by the jumper. Players described having a menu of tactical rules, housed under particular shared terminologies, that were selected before the game after watching and identifying how their opponents tended to behave. For Slow-thought verbalisations, UNI Player 8 and PRO Player 7, both verbalise how they would enter the lineout with a pre-call, which they would immediately execute if their opponents presented the space to them. If not, they referenced that a Fast-thought was required to perceive the defensive picture rapidly before calling a second term (or body movement) that teammates would respond too. Both players consistently refer to the importance of having time to perceive and assess the initial defensive picture before adapting.

Scrum

Scrum situations were exclusively classified as Slow-thought where players referred to the execution of a “checklist” in response to haptic game information (See Table 4). The data implies that this checklist is formed from knowledge-of what action is appropriate given the feel of their scrummaging position. Alongside this, both teams had a common language that would initiate how they intended to approach the scrum, that is, being aggressive or being more conservative.

Defensive situations

Defensive situations ebbed and flowed between open phase play, and tackle situations. For both instances, players verbalised that tactical rules often dictated what the players role, responsibility and intentions should look like. But, on multiple occasion’s the players described being consciously aware of too many pieces of game information with too little time, resulting in paralysis by analysis and missed tackles. The data implies that the tactical rules drove players to focus less on the perceptual information provided by opponents and more on rules associated their own movement behaviour. For example, in open phase play, players who played the position of centre, were required to abide by the rule of waiting on the threat of a hard line of running, before moving toward the other option which more often than other positions, which often resulted in missed tackles. Similarly, forward verbalised their role and responsibility of their positioning next to the ruck situation, their position demanded a role (terminology) and responsibility (which opponent). Player’s verbalised feeling internalised pressure in these moments which resulted in numerous missed tackles.

Offensive situations

Offensive situations also shifted between open phase play (carrying, passing and kicking) and tackle situations (tackle, ruck, maul). Nonetheless, both teams often refer to a framework of play that drove their positioning and intentions. Slow-thought decisions often coincided with moments of open phase play, for instance players verbalised calling a tactical play housed under a common term to pre-plan their action response to the defensive picture provided by their opponent (See Table 4). No-thought and Fast-thought decisions coincided with moments where players initial assessment of situational probability was unsatisfied resulting in a need to reassess or react to game information. These moments were often verbalised during local interactions between attackers and defenders and within the tackle situation and ruck.

Discussion

Sixteen adult rugby players conducted three stages of data collection within 48 h of two competitive fixtures to: (i) consider how game situations influence players perception of information; (ii) to consider how game situations influence the application of cognitive mechanisms whilst making decisions; and (iii) to identify the influence of tactics and/or strategy on player decision making. (Raab, 2003; Bar-Eli et al., 2011; Araújo and Bourbousson, 2016; Birch et al., 2019; Bourbousson et al., 2019; Ashford et al., 2021). Players were required to verbalise their lived decision making experience during self-confrontation interviews, which were framed along a continuum of No-thought, Fast-thought and Slow-thought (Varela and Shear, 1999; Macquet, 2009; Bourbousson et al., 2010; Macquet and Kragba, 2015; Johnston and Morrison, 2016). Verbalisations were subject to two stages of analysis including; a deductive content analysis in relation to Ashford et al.’s (2021) unified framework for decision making in team invasion sports; and the deductive and inductive collation of responses into elementary units of meaning (Thereau, 2003; Macquet and Kragba, 2015; Gleeson and Kelly, 2020). As anticipated, our interpretation of the data indicates that game situations have a clear impact on players perception of information and the cognitive mechanisms used, or not used, within the decision making process. Furthermore, tactics and strategy seem to have a significant impact on player decision making.

Perception of Information

Verbalisations of fast and frugal decisions coincide with references to the perception of global (Lex et al., 2015; Johnston and Morrison, 2016), and more discrete (Jackson et al., 2006; Johnston and Morrison, 2016) information. Our findings suggest that players perceive game information on a global scale; such as the width of the defensive line, identifying space in backfield or identifying advantages and disadvantages between attacking and defensive numbers, as pictures that invite particular courses of action (Lex et al., 2015). This is comparable to the findings of Johnston and Morrison (2016) who compared perceptual strategies of skilled and less-skilled rugby league players. They suggested that skilled players perceive cues of a global nature whilst lesser able players focus more on discrete local information, such as the body positioning of opponent’s (Johnston and Morrison, 2016). In contrast, our findings demonstrate that both groups of players perceived discrete information as frequently as global information when making decisions classified as Fast-thought. Players make reference to their opponent’s body positions, opponents’ movements and teammates movements as actuators of the decision making process (Passos et al., 2008; Correia et al., 2012, 2013). Interestingly, the perception of global, or discrete information seems dependent on the situational context placed on the decision maker. The perception of global information, such as the width of the defensive line, often coincided with moments where players were waiting for the game to come to them in open phase play, such as a backline waiting to receive the ball at the lineout (Macquet and Kragba, 2015). Instead, recall of discrete information during player verbalisations tended to correspond with tackle situations, for example, a one vs one situation between a ball carrier and a tackler (Jackson et al., 2006; Correia et al., 2012, 2013).

The analysis of No-thought and Slow-thought verbalisations demonstrates that players are presented with variances in time available to make decisions during a game of rugby union (Raab, 2003). The No-thought data implies that players often refer to having no time to perceive game information, whilst during Slow-thought verbalisations both groups often allude to having increased time, providing an opportunity to view a global array of visual perceptual information. These findings provide further evidence to the idea that perception is task dependent as semi-elite player’s verbal descriptions change in line with different game situations (Lenzen et al., 2009; Roca et al., 2011, 2012, 2013). In rugby union, the rules dictate that a contest for possession of the ball occurs every time attacking and defensive players meet (Ashford et al., 2021). Subsequently, our findings demonstrate that players who are involved in the contest for possession, such as the ball carrier, tackler and supporting players, have minimal time to perceive and act on game information. In such time pressured situations, a psychological refractory period may occur, where players are unable to perceive a secondary source of game information (Araújo et al., 2004; Maslovat et al., 2013). Therefore, players must rely on the initial perception of discrete information in the local array, such as their opponent’s movements and body positions (Correia et al., 2013). Conversely, players who are not involved in the contest, are given time to perceive a global array of offensive or defensive pictures offered by their opponents, to communicate and collectively execute a decision for the next phase of play (Johnston and Morrison, 2016).

Players communicated the use of different perceptual sources, presenting visual (Johnston and Morrison, 2016), acoustic (Davids et al., 2008) and haptic information (Lenzen et al., 2009), whose use is predicated on the position they play and the game situation (Raab, 2003). Whilst the majority of decisions within open play are actuated by global or discrete visual information, players also perceive acoustic information from teammates, especially in backline positions. Furthermore, in particular game situations such as the scrum (See Table 2) players mostly rely on the perception of haptic information to make decisions. For instance, Props from the UNI and PRO playing groups indicate that the conscious awareness of haptic information; such as the strength of their physical scrummaging position and their bind between themselves, their teammates and their opponents actuates their decision making process. Consequently, inferences suggest that rugby union coaches should guide players to global, discrete, audio or haptic perceptual information based on the time available to a player within particular game situations.

Actions-for the Game

Analysis of verbalisations making reference to player’s actions-for the game suggest that UNI and PRO players perception of information is guided by their physical (Passos et al., 2012) and technical (Paterson et al., 2013) capability to act (Ashford et al., 2021). Within Unstimulated, Fast-thought and Slow-thought data, players often recount being consciously aware of their physical and technical capabilities when perceiving and acting on game information. In contrast, analysis of No-thought verbalisations often unearthed tendencies to refer to an self-organised response, which in the majority, still resulted in positive outcomes. Players from both teams also rarely verbalised decisions where they had misjudged perceptual information. This may present evidence that both UNI and PRO players physical and technical capabilities to act are coupled with highly salient game information (Davids et al., 2008; Passos et al., 2008; Correia et al., 2013). Thus, players perception of information may, at times, depend on a refined connection to their technical and physical capability (Jackson et al., 2006; Esteves et al., 2011; Paterson et al., 2013).

For the majority of No-thought classifications, players could not remember nor verbalise how or why they came to act in such a way. Instead, they referred to reacting without thinking or based on a feel for the situation. This may suggest that within these moments, the driving mechanism of the players decision making process is a successful (or unsuccessful) forming of a direct online relationship between game information and their technical and/or physical capabilities without need for memory representation (Cappuccio, 2015; Wilson et al., 2018; Raab and Araújo, 2019). It is important to note however, that such verbalisations of implicit self-organisation were rare, yet this may be due to the methods adopted within this study. Furthermore, despite players indicating that they’d decided based on feel or through reaction, they often impressed wider knowledge of what was required to satisfy the situation within their verbalisations. Subsequently, these findings from No-thought classifications may be interpreted in two ways; first, as evidence for a direct and enactive relationship between perception and action within the decision making process (Cappuccio, 2015) or second, evidence for Changeux’s (2009) view, that even in situations where players are denied access to conscious thought, neural embodiments of meaning stored as representations allow for players to satisfy the demands of the game situation. Analysis of the data suggests that players had an instinct of what was right given the game situation, suggesting that Changeux’s (2009) view may be better placed to explain these findings. Additionally, whilst verbalising a particular No-thought classification, a PRO player made reference to the importance of rehearsing his lineout throw in hope of removing conscious thought from his decision making process. Interestingly, our data suggests that despite these preferences, the decision of where to throw was still cognitively driven through memory representations, as the player refers to having to listen, interpret and act on a common term which initiated where the ball needed to be thrown (McPherson and Vickers, 2004; Richards et al., 2012, 2017). Instead, the player’s desire to remove conscious thought from the decision, was in reference to the execution of the throw itself, perhaps aware of the need to free resources for the decision making process.

In contrast, elementary units of meaning abducted from Fast-thought and Slow-thought classifications suggest that player’s actions are often driven by situation specific intentions. However, these intentions are determined by a player’s deep declarative and procedural understanding of what, how and why to act on given game information (Anderson, 1982; Macquet and Fleurance, 2007; Lenzen et al., 2009). This challenges a widely accepted contemporary view that the coupling of a player’s perception and action are bound directly (Passos et al., 2008; Raab and Araújo, 2019). Instead, the data of the current study suggests that perception, whilst coupled and refined with salient game information, is often predicated by the retrieval of conscious memory representations that initiate what action is selected and why (Lenzen et al., 2009; Macquet and Kragba, 2015; Johnston and Morrison, 2016). Furthermore, players described being self-aware of their technical and physical capability during the decision making process. Interestingly, these moments demonstrate a tendency to engage in meta-cognitive processes in fast paced game situations, where players consciously assess whether they possess the technical, and/or physical capability to influence the outcome positively (Price et al., 2019, 2020). Finally, the interpretation of data suggests that statements that describe the relationship between perception and action as one directly connected, for instance that players must “perceive to act and act to perceive” (Fajen et al., 2008; Silva et al., 2013), should be extended to account for the interceding mechanism of memory representation (McPherson and Vickers, 2004). That is, in order to perceive to act and act to perceive (bottom-up), one must possess the knowledge of when, where and why to perceive and act accordingly (top-down; Anderson, 1982; Ashford et al., 2021).

Knowledge-of the Game

The influence of game situations are clearly demonstrated in our findings of the deductive content analysis. No references were made to knowledge-of or knowledge-in the game during verbalisations of No-thought classifications (Raab, 2003; Bar-Eli et al., 2011), whilst reference to the use of knowledge-of the game was verbalised only on occasion during Fast-thought classifications. Instead knowledge-of the game was predominately recounted during Slow-thought decision classifications where players were presented with more time and an increased number of options to select from. Within these instances, player’s share how their knowledge-of opponents, tactics, roles and responsibilities, self-awareness and teammates all influenced their decision making process (Gréhaigne et al., 1999, 2005; Richards et al., 2012; Macquet and Kragba, 2015). Our findings suggest that in time rich situations, memory representations, stored as task-specific declarative knowledge, are the key connecting variable between the perception of game information and the decision to act in a certain way (Anderson, 1982; Lex et al., 2015; Johnston and Morrison, 2016; Birch et al., 2019). Thus, when players have time, their search for game information and their selection of a relevant capability to act are both guided by knowledge-of their team’s tactical approach (Mulligan et al., 2012; Richards et al., 2017). This presents clear evidence that rugby players store and use memory representations to perceive, interpret and act on tactics, roles and responsibilities successfully (Poplu et al., 2008; Johnston and Morrison, 2016; Schulz, 2018; Birch et al., 2019; Raab and Araújo, 2019).

The elementary units of meaning comprised from player’s knowledge-of the game all relate to the successful execution of pre-planned intentions for performance (McPherson and Vickers, 2004; MacMahon and McPherson, 2009). Our data demonstrates that improved memory representations allow players to form suitable plans of action, through tactics, roles and responsibilities, which enhance their capability to effectively perceive and operate in dynamic game environments (McPherson and Vickers, 2004; Ashford et al., 2021). Such plans of action guide perceptual strategies, the retrieval of memory representations and subsequent action, for example, knowing when, where and why to kick the ball up field in order to relieve defensive pressure (McPherson and Vickers, 2004; Macquet and Fleurance, 2007). These extended to the recall of knowledge-of opponent’s tactics or individual tendencies to make effective decisions during Fast-thought and Slow-thought verbalisations. Interestingly, research has suggested that being consciously aware of movement tendencies can be detrimental for anticipatory performance (Memmert and Furley, 2007; Jackson et al., 2020). However, such research has focussed on player’s ability to detect deceptive intent in one vs one situations (Jackson et al., 2006, 2020). Whereas, in the context of this study, references made to knowledge-of opponents by players were often made in association to game situations where players had sufficient time to make decisions, such as the lineout or scrum. Consequently, we encourage coaches and players to develop knowledge-of opponent’s tactical tendencies to inform player decision making.

Knowledge-in the Game

Interpretation of these findings suggest that reference to knowledge-in the game was made predominately during Fast-thought verbalisations (Raab and Gigerenzer, 2015; Hafenbrädl et al., 2016). Interestingly, players describe that they tend to form expectations of what will happen based on their assessment of game information (Macquet, 2009; Eccles and Tran, 2012). In the first instance, this demonstrates that players attempt to make sense of game information and engage in an assessment of situational probability to successfully anticipate an outcome (Eccles and Tran, 2012; Macquet and Kragba, 2015; Loffing and Cañal-Bruland, 2017; Helm et al., 2020). However, the findings suggest that players were often inaccurate when calculating situational probability from their assessments of game information. Players described having to update their perception of the situation as game information altered before them (McPherson and Vickers, 2004). Macquet and Fleurance (2007), Macquet (2009) would suggest that at this moment, players would deem this information atypical and be required to process available information in relation to formed mental representations to generate an appropriate response. Interpretation of these findings suggest that, in these instances, players took the first viable option presented to them to quickly satisfy the situation and remove risk (Johnson and Raab, 2003; Raab and Gigerenzer, 2015). Interestingly, UNI players were required to update and adapt to game incidents more often than the PRO playing group. This is likely due to an over reliance of the UNI playing group on sticking to tactical intentions (Passos et al., 2008; Bourbousson et al., 2011); and PRO players increased perceptual capability to form an accurate assessment of situational probability (Eccles and Tran, 2012; Loffing and Cañal-Bruland, 2017; Helm et al., 2020). Furthermore, Den Hartigh et al. (2018) Skill Theory may offer some insight to these findings. They compared the recall ability of selected and non-selected soccer players to consider the complexity and declarative knowledge base within verbalisations. Their findings demonstrated that declarative knowledge was not dependent of complexity or competitive level, suggesting a dissociation exists between players declarative knowledge and knowledge-in-action. This disassociation may explain the inaccuracies in both populations to successfully assess the situational probability of game situations (Den Hartigh et al., 2018).

Players recognised legitimate risks or threats to their attacking or defensive integrity within Fast and Slow-thought classifications, for instance, losing possession of the ball to their opponents or conceding a try. Macquet and Kragba (2015) found that elite female basketball player’s make sense of game information to make decisions in two steps: (i) an assessment of the situation and (ii) anticipation of situations as they develop to manage risk. Similarly, our findings demonstrate that a clear relationship exists between player’s sense-making and decision making, as player’s knowledge-of the game seems to guide their perceptual assessment of the situation (Del Missier et al., 2010; Macquet and Kragba, 2015). Identifying risks and threats coincided with sudden perceptual acknowledgement of an opponent’s capability to disrupt momentum. Furthermore, these verbalisations referenced incidents that occurred in fast paced situations that disrupt player’s situational probability, such as an opposing player attempting to steal the ball in a ruck situation, (Ashford et al., 2021). Raab and Gigerenzer (2015) identify that when humans are required to make decisions in time pressured situations they tend to rely on heuristics, that is, bias of what has worked before (Jackson et al., 2006). Consequently, when player’s perceived legitimate risks or threats, they often used deeper declarative knowledge-of the game to update their knowledge-in the game as they took the first option that presented itself (Raab and Laborde, 2011; Raab and Gigerenzer, 2015).

The influence of game context during player decision making was verbalised by the PRO group more frequently than the UNI group (González Víllora et al., 2013; Light et al., 2014). Game context has been explored frequently in decision making research, referred to as contextual priors (Mann et al., 2014; Cañal-Bruland and Mann, 2015; Levi and Jackson, 2018; Broadbent et al., 2019). For instance, Lenzen et al. (2009) indicated that verbalisations derived from self-confrontation interviews with handball players demonstrated that the score, players involved, and match difficulty all influence the decision making process. Johnson and Raab (2003) found that soccer players think about their scoring potential (i.e., penetrate the opponent’s defensive line and score) and their safety potential (i.e., losing the ball or conceding) as they progress throughout the game. Relatedly, Levi and Jackson (2018) indicated that player’s extract dynamic contextual priors from game information that effects their decision making process. Dynamic contextual priors include the score status, feelings of momentum, assessment of personal performance and external instruction from coaches or teammates (Levi and Jackson, 2018; Gredin et al., 2019). Our findings demonstrate regular moments where the PRO group of players reflected on their offensive and/or defensive momentum when making decisions. On occasion, descriptions of momentum led to the suggestion that players felt levels of situational favourableness, where the game felt completely in their control (Levi and Jackson, 2018). In contrast, the UNI group only make reference to game context in a state of panic when the opposing team came back into the game, demonstrating negative feelings of momentum. Offensive and defensive momentum was often used by PRO players to make informed assessments of risk associated within their decision making (González Víllora et al., 2013; Light et al., 2014; Macquet and Kragba, 2015).

Common Frame of Reference

Player’s common frame of reference was verbalised frequently within Slow-thought decision classifications. Our findings present evidence that both teams operate, coordinate and communicate their decision making based on a shared mental model of performance (See Table 2, Offensive situations, scrum & lineout; Richards et al., 2012, 2017; Tee et al., 2018). Players referred to game moments where the profound use of a common language had many purposes during individual and collective decision making (Richards et al., 2017). These included perceptual strategies such as; the guidance of teammates perception; to inform teammates of perceptual information they could not visually perceive; to allow players to make sense of game information; to assess situational probability; to identify threats and/or risks; and finally, to notice sudden changes in expectations (Richards et al., 2012; Ashford et al., 2021). Furthermore, our data suggests that a common language was the key mechanism upholding each team’s tactical “framework” they were working toward (McKay and O’Connor, 2018). Consequently, our findings suggest that a shared common language not only guides the perception of players, but also initiates their decisions and actions in an intentional pre-planned fashion. Specific common terms often demanded the execution of particular tactical rules, roles and responsibilities by both groups of players’ (McKay and O’Connor, 2018). Thus, the deep declarative understanding of a shared common language between players, connects their perception of information to their individual and collective actions-for the game, knowledge-of the game and knowledge-in the game (Ashford et al., 2021). In turn, the consistencies in the data raise a clear question regarding the current theoretical differentiation between shared mental models (Richards et al., 2012, 2017) and shared affordances (Silva et al., 2013) for team coordination. In rugby union, they are, in fact dependent on a deep declarative understanding of teammates capabilities and a clear shared common language (Richards et al., 2012, 2017). Balagué et al. (2019) suggests that coaches should imprint strategy with their player’s, which guides player’s attention to perceive affordances. Subsequently, in relation to the data demonstrated in this study, shared affordances are a likely byproduct of tactical shared mental models (Richards and Collins, 2020).

Interpretation of our data suggests that within key moments (Tee et al., 2018), players used tactical rules, housed within common terms, as either beneficial or detrimental for their decision making performance (See Table 2 for raw data examples). Raab (2003) suggested that the application of decision making processes at an individual level may depend on the cognitive complexity of different game situations. More specifically, those who had developed explicit knowledge-of the task were advantaged when cognitive complexity was high, whereas those who had developed actions-for rather than knowledge-of the task (i.e., implicit learning) were advantaged when cognitive complexity was low (Raab, 2003). However, our data suggests that player’s regarded time as the key dependent variable for the application of decision making processes over that of cognitive complexity (Raab, 2003). Game moments such as the lineout, scrum and open phase play offer player’s more time to make decisions and within these moments player’s verbalised the perception of global information, such as the opponents defensive picture (Johnston and Morrison, 2016), the communication of a common term (Richards et al., 2017) which actuated the coordinated execution of tactical rules, roles and responsibilities for all players involved (Del Missier et al., 2010; McKay and O’Connor, 2018). In contrast, when player’s verbalised having limited time, such as dealing with a threat at the breakdown, players recalled perceiving discrete information, such as their opponents body position (Macquet and Kragba, 2015) and tended to take the first option presented to them (Raab and Laborde, 2011; Raab and Gigerenzer, 2015).

Raw data examples emerging through self-confrontation data indicates that players errors in judgement tended to materialise when a misalignment occurred between the time available to a player and their application of a cognitive mechanism (See Table 2, Defensive situations). Verbalisations regarding decisions in specific defensive situations highlight that both groups of players followed a shared tactical rule, which internalised the accepted responsibility to wait for one opponents’ movement to finish before perceiving the next. Frequently, these situations resulted in missed tackles by players as their decision to perceive the second attacker came too late. Here, the data presents that coaches have employed a top-down shared mental model to guide player’s perception and action through set roles and responsibilities in specific game situations (Richards et al., 2012, 2017). However, interpretation of our data suggests that players have on occasion become blinded or paralysed by an over-reliance on a specific memory representation that dictated their behaviour rather, than behaving in response to game perception (Passos et al., 2008; Correia et al., 2013). Players recalled being unable to anticipate the deceptive movements of their opponents (Jackson et al., 2006), unable to make sense of information to identify legitimate attacking threats (Macquet and Kragba, 2015) and unable to adapt quickly enough in response to their opponent’s movements. Consequently, whilst player verbalisations suggest that such decisions are still cognitively driven, in such incidents, a top-down shared mental model can be overbearing if too prescriptive and controlling of player behaviour (Steiner et al., 2017; Ribeiro et al., 2019a, b).

Conclusion of the Findings

The aims of this study were three-fold; (i) to consider how game situations influence players perception of information; (ii) to consider how game situations influence the application of cognitive mechanisms whilst making decisions; and (iii) to identify the influence of tactics and/or strategy on player decision making. Players perception of game information, was in large, dependent on the task or game situation (Roca et al., 2011, 2013). When players had time, they often verbalised global information, whilst as time decreased their perception of information became more discrete (Johnston and Morrison, 2016; Basevitch et al., 2019). The application and use of cognitive mechanisms varied from situation to situation, however the majority of player verbalisations were found to be cognitively driven by memory representations stored as knowledge-of and knowledge-in the game (McPherson and Vickers, 2004; Changeux, 2009; Johnston and Morrison, 2016; Richards et al., 2017). Memory representations were often verbalised as the connection between the perceptual search for information and an ideal action response, thus, UNI and PRO rugby players often formed plans of action in regard to what to look for, hear or feel (Macquet, 2009; Lex et al., 2015). Subsequently, if player’s initial assessment of situational probability was accurate, players described having time to consider options to come to an appropriate decision (Macquet and Kragba, 2015). Finally, players consistently verbalised the role of a shared common language which implies that players coordinate perception and action by communicating. This tactical focus suggests that players are required to interpret verbal terminologies between teammates, which identifies the existence and use of shared internal mental representations (McPherson and Vickers, 2004; Richards and Collins, 2020).

Conversely, player decision making was frequently verbalised during Fast-thought decision classifications, where player’s initial assessments of game situations were inaccurate. Players response to these incidents often coincided with a rapid ability to make sense of game information (Macquet and Kragba, 2015) and a heightened capability to satisfy the situation by taking the first option available to them (Raab and Gigerenzer, 2015). In contrast, verbalisations of No-thought decisions occasionally presented evidence of sub conscious decision making made through feel or reaction. These findings were interpreted in two ways, one as evidence of a direct relationship between perception and action, where players self-organised their behaviour implicitly in response to game information without need for memory representations (Raab and Araújo, 2019). Two, as evidence for neural embodiments of meaning, where representations are formed from a network of neurons that fire in synchronicity following the perception of information (Changeux, 2009). Given the nature of player descriptions the evidence seems to suggest that representations still drive what the player needs to achieve in the decision making process. Despite these interpretations, such descriptions were infrequent and coincided with moments where players had a split second to react to game information (less than 200 decisions across both populations). Nonetheless, if players are required to update, diagnose or self-organise to game information, these incidents must be accounted for in representative training environments (Passos et al., 2008; Raab and Laborde, 2011).

Raab et al. (2019), suggested that the player’s perspective must be considered when seeking to understand cognitive mechanisms and decision making processes (Raab et al., 2019; Gleeson and Kelly, 2020). Throughout the study there has been an attempt to mitigate the fallibility of verbalisation methods which included framing self-confrontation interviews on a continuum of No/Fast/Slow-Thought. Furthermore, the collection of unstimulated data facilitated cross-reference to player’s responses to self-confrontation elicitation questions. Despite these, the limitations of this approach are unavoidable regarding the reliability of stimulated recall to explore player decision making. It is also important that the limited references made by players to implicit, self-organised decision making processes are a likely consequence of this approach. Thus, it is suggested that future study in this area looks to integrate experimental and ecological task design alongside self-confrontation interviews adopted in this study. That way, the limitations of both methodological approaches can be considered to enhance the trustworthiness of the findings.

Practical Implications

The data presents that player decision making in rugby union may best be understood by considering the context in which the decision is made (Simon, 1956). Whether implicit, explicit, or through the presence or absence of cognitive mechanisms, player decision making occurs relative to its environment within a game situation (Todd and Gigerenzer, 2001; Gigerenzer and Selten, 2002; Raab, 2003). Recently, Morgan et al. (2020) have explored coaches’ perceptions of decision making in rugby union and through semi-structured interviews indicated that the coaches who participated stressed the importance of a balance between structure and chaos when supporting the development of their players’ decision making. They suggest that deliberate coaching strategies, such as off-field video analysis, game-plans and strategy were as equally important as questioning or the use of game based representative scenarios. Practically therefore, in relation to the findings from this study, we suggest that coaches adopt two types of decision making training environments. Firstly, the use of intentional decision making training (explicit learning) should be adopted to develop cognitive decisions where players know what information to search for and what action is appropriate to satisfy the game information (Bar-Eli et al., 2011). This may include explicit slow deliberate sense making activities such as; video analysis sessions, previews and team debriefs (Richards et al., 2012, 2017; Richards and Collins, 2020); coach led if–then rules of thumb (heuristics; McPherson, 1999; McPherson and Vickers, 2004) and the development of individual and collective roles and responsibilities (McKay and O’Connor, 2018) through tactical rules (McKay and O’Connor, 2018). Subsequently, we agree with Richards et al. (2012, 2017) position that players should be encouraged to explicitly connect their perception of information to their declarative knowledge-of the game through slow and deliberate environments to make better informed decisions in rapid competitive environments (Ashford et al., 2021).

In contrast, the game presents player’s with incidents where tactical intentions can no longer be referenced i.e., when players have limited time or when game information demands a change in expectations (Passos et al., 2008; McKay and O’Connor, 2018). To support the betterment of player decision making in these moments, coaches can utilise incidental decision making training (Bar-Eli et al., 2011). This approach conforms to variable design principles such as non-linear pedagogy (Chow, 2013; Correia et al., 2019) in that practice provides frequent exposure to the perception of information in order to identify invariances between the information perceived and the action planned (Bar-Eli et al., 2011). Furthermore, in such uncertain environments, coaches should consider guiding players to adopt ‘take-the-first’ heuristics (Raab and Laborde, 2011; Raab and Gigerenzer, 2015) through exploratory coach behaviours (e.g., divergent questioning; Cushion et al., 2011; O’Connor et al., 2018) and practice manipulation (e.g., rule modifications; Passos et al., 2008; Chow, 2013; Balagué et al., 2019; Ashford et al., 2020).

Following the interpretation of our data, we advise coaches to consistently reinforce the following coaching strategies within their practice. First, is the thoughtful development and reinforcement of a common language, where all player’s (and coaches) possess clarity of accepted verbal, visual and haptic information (Richards et al., 2012, 2017; McKay and O’Connor, 2018). If players are able to conceptualise the meaning of terminologies and how they are operationalised during games and training, coaches may consider co-constructing further development of language with the playing group (Richards et al., 2012, 2017). A common language rests on ensuring clarity and coherence between all involved, as a single term must mean the same thing to one player and it does to everyone else. Secondly, we suggest that an adopted common language should be intertwined with a shared “way of playing” housed under a clear common frame of reference (Tee et al., 2018; Ashford et al., 2021). This does not suggest that coaches must prescribe how players behave, but instead suggests that by imprinting strategy (Balagué et al., 2019) coaches can offer a framework as a scaffold for collective and coordinated decision making so players are still able to search for, communicate, decide and act as the game unfolds (Tee et al., 2018).

Thirdly, “What is tactically desirable must be technically possible” (Launder and Piltz, 2013). Thus, coaches should consistently seek to develop and refine technical and physical capabilities of their players as they afford a wider scope of game information to select from Paterson et al. (2013); Wilson et al. (2018). With this, is a caveat that when supporting player’s development of technique, coaches should not shy away from decoupled, massed, blocked and instructional practices (Williams and Hodges, 2005) but ensure that technique is then refined and recoupled to game information. Finally, the findings of this study demonstrate that player’s individual and collective perceptual strategies, retrieval of memory representations, actions and use of language are consistently underpinned by players deep declarative knowledge-of the game (Anderson, 1982; Johnston and Morrison, 2016). By supporting player’s game intelligence and a deeper understanding of why, coaches can support players development of self-awareness, meta-cognitive strategies (Price et al., 2019, 2020); realistic evaluations of their performance (MacNamara et al., 2010a, b); valid assessments of physical and technical capabilities (Ashford et al., 2021); and finally, the ability to consider game context during performance to make informed decisions (Levi and Jackson, 2018).

To conclude, this study has presented a breadth of evidence to suggest that game situations in rugby union impact semi-elite players decision making processes in similar ways. In reference to the thorough exploration of player decision making conducted in this study, research should now extend further than semi-elite playing populations and consider elite participants (i.e., Premiership & International players). Furthermore, study into how to best support players development of decision making must progress further than coach perceptions. Instead, we agree with Morgan et al. (2020) conclusive remarks, that future research with rugby union coaches should employ self-confrontation elicitation interviews to explore how the demands of the game are perceived and captured by the coach and impressed onto players within effective learning environments.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Carnegie Faculty Research Ethics Leeds Beckett University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

MA, AA, and JP contributed to the proposed purpose, design, materials and methods of the project, ethical considerations, and data analysis. MA contributed to the recruitment and data collection and wrote the manuscript. AA and JP edited and commented the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ Five UNI players and two PRO players completed the procedure once, therefore their data was removed from the study.
  2. ^ The lineout is a set-piece phase of play, which involves the ball being thrown back into the field after it has left (Sayers, 2005). It must have at least two, but commonly includes four to seven, players from each team standing perpendicular to the sideline, 2 m apart with the first player at least 5 m away. The team in possession requires a player to throw the ball straight down the middle of the two lines, whilst two players’ lift a jumper into the air to catch and receive the ball (Croft et al., 2011; Sayers, 2011, 2005). Opponents may also lift someone to compete for the ball. Initiation of movement is often sparked by a ‘caller’ from the team in possession the ball, who then decides where the ball will be thrown and caught. The scrum is also a set-piece situation, which involves a maximum of eight players from each team (forward) forming three rows that are bound together to compete for the ball; (the front row; hooker and props; second-row’s and back row; flankers and number eight; Trewartha et al., 2015). The two sides must then bind together to compete for possession of the ball in a coordinated pushing action (Hendricks et al., 2014; Trewartha et al., 2015). For the team in possession of the ball, the scrum is seen as an opportunity to launch attacks, whilst opponent’s often aim to disrupt or steal the ball back (Trewartha et al., 2015). Physical domination can often result in the penalisation of an opposing team (Hendricks et al., 2014). Finally, offensive and defensive moments of the game are defined by the team in possession of the ball (Tee et al., 2018). Rugby union is a unique sport as when a tackle occurs, a physical contest for possession of the ball follows (Tee et al., 2018). This area of the game is called a ruck where an attacking team will support a ball carrying player by securing the ball, before it can be passed away to a player in open space. Conversely, the defence are firstly required to tackle a player, which then provides an opportunity to win the ball back (Tee et al., 2018). Therefore, defensive and offensive situations repeatedly ebb and flow between moments of open-phase play and tackle situations.

References

Abernethy, B., Zawi, K., and Jackson, R. C. (2008). Expertise and attunement to kinematic constraints. Perception 37, 931–948. doi: 10.1068/p5340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychol. Rev. 89, 369–406. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.89.4.369

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Araújo, D., and Bourbousson, J. (2016). “Theoretical perspectives on interpersonal coordination for team behaviour,” in Intepersonal Coordination and Performance in Social Systems, 1st Edn, eds P. Passos, J. Y. Chow, and K. Davids (London: Routledge).

Google Scholar

Araújo, D., Davids, K., Bennett, S. J., Button, C., and Chapman, G. (2004). “Emergence of sport skills under constraints,” in Skill Acquisition in Sport: Research, Theory and Practice, eds A. M. Williams and N. J. Hodges (London: Routledge).

Google Scholar

Araújo, D., Davids, K., Diniz, A., Rocha, L., Santos, J. C., Dias, G., et al. (2015). Ecological dynamics of continuous and categorical decision-making: the regatta start in sailing. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 15, 195–202. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2014.928749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Araújo, D., Hristovski, R., Seifert, L., Carvalho, J., and Davids, K. (2019). Ecological cognition: expert decision-making behaviour in sport. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 12, 1–25. doi: 10.1080/1750984X.2017.1349826

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashford, M., Abraham, A. K., and Poolton, J. (2021). A communal language for decision-making in team invasion sports. Int. Sport Coach. J. 8, 122–129. doi: 10.1123/iscj.2019-0062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashford, M., Burke, K., Barrell, D., Abraham, A. K., and Poolton, J. (2020). The impact of rule modifications on player behaviour in a talent identification and development environment: a case study of the Rugby Football Union’s Wellington Academy Rugby Festival. J. Sport Sci. 38, 2670–2676. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2020.1795559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Austin, D., Gabbett, T., and Jenkins, D. (2011). Repeated high-intensity exercise in professional rugby union. J. Sport Sci. 29, 1105–1112. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2011.582508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balagué, N., Pol, R., Torrents, C., Ric, A., and Hristovski, R. (2019). On the relatedness and nestedness of constraints. Sports Med. Open 5:6. doi: 10.1186/s40798-019-0178-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Eli, M., Plessner, H., and Raab, M. (2011). Judgment, Decision-Making and Success in Sport. Malden, MA: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Basevitch, I., Tenenbaum, G., Filho, E., Razon, S., Boiangin, N., and Ward, P. (2019). Anticipation and situation-assessment skills in soccer under varying degrees of informational constraint. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 42, 1–11. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2019-0118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bennis, W. M., and Pachur, T. (2006). Fast and frugal heuristics in sports. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 7, 611–629. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2006.06.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birch, J. E., Moe, V. F., and Breivik, G. (2019). “Knowledge, consciousness, and sporting skills,” in Handbook of Embodied Cognition and Sport Psychology, 1st Edn, ed. M. L. Cappuccio (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press).

Google Scholar

Bourbousson, J., Feigean, M., and Seiler, R. (2019). Team cognition in sport: how current insights into how teamwork is achieved in naturalistic settings can lead to simulation studies. Front. Psychol. 10:2082. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bourbousson, J., Poizat, G., Saury, J., and Seve, C. (2011). Description of dynamic shared knowledge: an exploratory study during a competitive team sports interaction. Ergonomics 54, 120–138. doi: 10.1080/00140139.2010.544763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bourbousson, J., Sève, C., and McGarry, T. (2010). Space–time coordination dynamics in basketball: Part 2. The interaction between the two teams. J. Sports Sci. 28, 349–358. doi: 10.1080/02640410903503640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braun, V., Clarke, V., and Weate, P. (2016). “Using thematic analysis in sport and exercise research,” in Routledge Handbook of Qualitative Research in Sport and Exercise, eds B. Smith and S. Sparkes (London: Routledge), 191–205.

Google Scholar

Broadbent, D. P., Gredin, N. V., Rye, J. L., Williams, A. M., and Bishop, D. T. (2019). The impact of contextual priors and anxiety on performance effectiveness and processing efficiency in anticipation. Cogn. Emot. 33, 589–596. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2018.1464434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cañal-Bruland, R., and Mann, D. L. (2015). Time to broaden the scope of research on anticipatory behavior: a case for the role of probabilistic information. Front. Psychol. 6:1518. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cappuccio, M. L. (2015). Introduction: when embodied cognition and sport psychology team-up. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 14, 213–225. doi: 10.1007/s11097-015-9415-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Changeux, J. P. (2009). The Physiology of Truth: Neuroscience and Human Knowledge. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Changeux, J. P., and Dehaene, S. (1989). Neuronal models of cognitive functions. Cognition 33, 63–109. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(89)90006-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chow, J. (2013). Nonlinear learning underpinning pedagogy: evidence, challenges, and implications. Quest 65, 469–484. doi: 10.1080/00336297.2013.807746

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Correia, V., Araujo, D., Duarte, R., Travassos, B., Passos, P., and Davids, K. (2012). Changes in practice task constraints shape decision-making behaviours of team games players. J. Sci. Med. Sport 15, 244–249. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2011.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Correia, V., Araújo, D., Vilar, L., and Davids, K. (2013). From recording discrete actions to studying continuous goal-directed behaviours in team sports. J. Sports Sci. 31, 546–553. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2012.738926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Correia, V., Carvalho, J., Araújo, D., Pereira, E., and Davids, K. (2019). Principles of nonlinear pedagogy in sport practice. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedag. 24, 117–132. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2018.1552673

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Croft, H., Chong, A., and Wilson, B. (2011). Virtual reality assessment of rugby lineout throw kinematics. Sports Technol. 4, 2–12. doi: 10.1080/19346182.2012.663535

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cronin, C. J., and Lowes, J. (2016). Embedding experiential learning in HE sport coaching courses: an action research study. J. Hosp. Leis. Sport Tour. Educ. 18, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jhlste.2016.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunniffe, B., Proctor, W., Baker, J., and Davies, B. (2009). An evaluation of the physiological demands of elite rugby union using global positioning system tracking software. J. Strength Cond. Res. 23, 1195–1203. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0b013e3181a3928b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cushion, C., Harvey, S., Muir, B., and Nelson, L. (2011). Developing the Coach Analysis and Intervention System (CAIS): establishing validity and reliability of a computerised systematic observation instrument. J. Sports Sci. 30, 201–216. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2011.635310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davids, K., Button, C., and Bennett, S. (2008). Dynamics of Skill Acquisition: A Constraints-Led Approach. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Del Missier, F., Mäntylä, T., and Bruine de Bruin, W. (2010). Executive functions in decision making. Think. Reason. 16, 69–97. doi: 10.1080/13546781003630117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Den Hartigh, R. J., Van Der Steen, S., Hakvoort, B., Frencken, W. G., and Lemmink, K. A. (2018). Differences in game reading between selected and non-selected youth soccer players. J. Sports Sci. 36, 422–428. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2017.1313442

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccles, D. W., and Tran, K. B. (2012). Getting them on the same page: strategies for enhancing coordination and communication in sports teams. J. Sport Psychol. Act. 30–40. doi: 10.1080/21520704.2011.649229

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esteves, P. T., Araújo, D., Davids, K., Vilar, L., Travassos, B., and Esteves, C. (2012). Interpersonal dynamics and relative positioning to scoring target of performers in 1 vs. 1 sub-phases of team sports. J. Sports Sci. 30, 1285–1293. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2012.707327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esteves, P. T., de Oliveira, F., and Araújo, D. (2011). Posture-related affordances guide attacks in basketball. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 12, 639–644. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.06.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fajen, B. R., Riley, M. A., and Turvey, M. T. (2008). Information, affordances, and the control of action in sport. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 40, 79–107.

Google Scholar

Farrow, D., and Raab, M. (2008). “A recipe for expert decision making,” in Developing Sport Expertise: Researchers and Coaches Put Theory into Practice, 1st Edn, eds D. Farrow, J. Baker, and C. MacMahon (London: Routledge), 137–150.

Google Scholar

Feigean, M., R’kiouak, M., Seiler, R., and Bourbousson, J. (2018). Achieving teamwork in naturalistic sport settings: an exploratory qualitative study of informational resources supporting football players’ activity when coordinating with others. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 38, 154–166. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.06.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garraway, W. M., Lee, A. J., and Hutton, S. J. (2000). Impact of professionalism on injuries in rugby union. Br. J. Sports Med. 34, 348–351.

Google Scholar

Gigerenzer, G., and Selten, R. (eds) (2002). Bounded Rationality: The Adaptive Toolbox. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.

Google Scholar

Gleeson, E., and Kelly, S. (2020). Phenomenal decision-making in elite soccer: making the unseen seen. Sci. Med. Football 4, 1–9. doi: 10.1080/24733938.2019.1595113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

González Víllora, S., López, G., Miguel, L., Gutiérrez-Díaz, D., and Pastor-Vicedo, J. (2013). Tactical awareness, decision making and skill in youth soccer players (under-14 years). J. Hum. Sport Exerc. 8, 412–426. doi: 10.4100/jhse.2012.82.09

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gredin, N. V., Broadbent, D. P., Williams, A. M., and Bishop, D. T. (2019). Judgement utility modulates the use of explicit contextual priors and visual information during anticipation. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 45, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2019.101578

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gréhaigne, J.-F., Godbout, P., and Bouthier, D. (1999). The foundations of tactics and strategy in team sports. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 18, 159–174. doi: 10.1123/jtpe.18.2.159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gréhaigne, J.-F., Richard, J.-F., and Griffin, L. (2005). Teaching and Learning Team Sports and Games. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Hafenbrädl, S., Waeger, D., Marewski, J. N., and Gigerenzer, G. (2016). Applied decision making with fast-and-frugal heuristics. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 5, 215–231. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.04.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helm, F., Cañal-Bruland, R., Mann, D. L., Troje, N. F., and Munzert, J. (2020). Integrating situational probability and kinematic information when anticipating disguised movements. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 46:01607. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2019.101607

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hendricks, S., Lambert, M. I., Brown, J. C., Readhead, C., and Viljoen, W. (2014). An evidence-driven approach to scrum law modifications in amateur rugby played in South Africa. Br. J. Sports Med. 48, 1115–1119. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2013-092877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, R., Warren, S., and Abernethy, B. (2006). Anticipation skill and susceptibility to deceptive movement. Acta Psychol. 123, 355–371. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, R. C., Barton, H., and Bishop, D. T. (2020). Knowledge is power? Outcome probability information impairs detection of deceptive intent. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 50, 1017–1044. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101744

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, J. G., and Raab, M. (2003). Take the first: option-generation and resulting choices. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 91, 215–229. doi: 10.1016/S0749-5978(03)00027-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, D., and Morrison, B. W. (2016). The application of naturalistic decision-making techniques to explore cue use in Rugby League playmakers. J. Cogn. Eng. Decis. Mak. 10, 391–410. doi: 10.1177/1555343416662181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinrade, N. P., Jackson, R. C., and Ashford, K. J. (2015). Reinvestment, task complexity and decision making under pressure in basketball. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 20, 11–19. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2015.03.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, G., Calderwood, R., and Clinton-Cirocco, A. (2010). Rapid decision making on the fire ground: the original study plus a postscript. J. Cogn. Eng. Decis. Mak. 4, 186–209. doi: 10.1518/155534310X12844000801203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, G. A., Calderwood, R., and Clinton-Cirocco, A. (1986). Rapid decision making on the fire ground. Proc. Hum. Fact. Soc. Annu. Meet. 30, 576–580. doi: 10.1177/154193128603000616

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kyngäs, H., Mikkonen, K., and Kääriäinen, M. (2020). The Application of Content Analysis in Nursing Science Research. New York, NY: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-30199-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Launder, A. G., and Piltz, W. (2013). Play Practice: Engaging and Developing Skilled Players from Beginner to Elite. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Lenzen, B., Theunissen, C., and Cloes, M. (2009). Situated analysis of team handball players’ decisions: an exploratory study. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 28, 54–74. doi: 10.1123/jtpe.28.1.54

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levi, H. R., and Jackson, R. C. (2018). Contextual factors influencing decision making: perceptions of professional soccer players. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 37, 19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lex, H., Essig, K., Knoblauch, A., and Schack, T. (2015). Cognitive representations and cognitive processing of team-specific tactics in soccer. PLoS One 10:e0118219. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Light, R. L., Harvey, S., and Mouchet, A. (2014). Improving ‘at-action’ decision-making in team sports through a holistic coaching approach. Sport Educ. Soc. 19, 258–275. doi: 10.1080/13573322.2012.665803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loffing, F., and Cañal-Bruland, R. (2017). Anticipation in sport. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 16, 6–11. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacMahon, C., and McPherson, S. L. (2009). Knowledge base as a mechanism for perceptual-cognitive tasks: skill is in the details! Int. J. Sport Psychol. 40, 565–579.

Google Scholar

MacNamara, A., Button, A., and Collins, D. (2010a). The role of psychological characteristics in facilitating the pathway to elite performance part 1: identifying mental skills and behaviors. Sport Psychol. 24, 52–73. doi: 10.1123/tsp.24.1.52

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacNamara, A., Button, A., and Collins, D. (2010b). The role of psychological characteristics in facilitating the pathway to elite performance part 2: examining environmental and stage-related differences in skills and behaviors. Sport Psychol. 24, 74–96. doi: 10.1123/tsp.24.1.74

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macquet, A., and Kragba, K. (2015). What makes basketball players continue with the planned play or change it? A case study of the relationships between sense-making and decision-making. Cogn. Tech. Work 17, 345–353. doi: 10.1007/s10111-015-0332-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macquet, A. C. (2009). Recognition within the decision-making process: a case study of expert volleyball players. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 21, 64–79. doi: 10.1080/10413200802575759

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macquet, A. C., and Fleurance, P. (2007). Naturalistic decision-making in expert badminton players. Ergonomics 50, 1433–1450. doi: 10.1080/00140130701393452

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahlo, F. (1974). Acte Tactique en Jeu [Tactical Action in Play]. Paris: Vigot.

Google Scholar

Mann, D. L., Schaefers, T., and Cañal-Bruland, R. (2014). Action preferences and the anticipation of action outcomes. Acta Psychol. 152, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maslovat, D., Chua, R., Spencer, H. C., Forgaard, C. J., Carlsen, A. N., and Franks, I. M. (2013). Evidence for a response preparation bottleneck during dual-task performance: effect of a startling acoustic stimulus on the psychological refractory period. Acta Psychol. 144, 481–487. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKay, J., and O’Connor, D. (2018). Practicing unstructured play in team ball sports: a rugby union example. Int. Sport Coach. J. 5, 273–280. doi: 10.1123/iscj.2017-0095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McPherson, S. L. (1999). Expert-novice differences in performance skills and problem representations of youth and adults during tennis competition. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 70, 233–251. doi: 10.1080/02701367.1999.10608043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McPherson, S. L., and Vickers, J. N. (2004). Cognitive control in motor expertise. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2, 274–300. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2004.9671746

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Memmert, D. (2011). Sports and creativity. Encycl. Creat. 2, 373–378. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-375038-9.00207-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Memmert, D., and Furley, A. (2007). “I spy with my little eye!”: breadth of attention, inattentional blindness, and tactical decision making in team sports. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 29, S162–S162.

Google Scholar

Miles, M. B., Huberman, A. M., and Saldaña, J. (2020). Qualitative Data Analysis: A Methods Sourcebook. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.

Google Scholar

Morgan, K., Mouchet, K., and Thomas, G. (2020). Coaches’ perceptions of decision making in rugby union. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedag. 25, 394–409. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2020.1725458

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mulligan, D., McCracken, J., and Hodges, N. J. (2012). Situational familiarity and its relation to decision quality in ice-hockey. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 10, 198–210. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2012.672009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

North, J., Piggott, D., Rankin-Wright, A. J., and Ashford, M. (2020). An empirical examination of UK coaches’ issues and problems, and their support and advice networks. Int. Sport Coach. J. 7, 283–294. doi: 10.1123/iscj.2019-0049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connor, D., and Larkin, P. (2015). “Decision-making for rugby,” in The Science of Sport, eds K. Till and B. Jones (Rugby: The Crowood Press Ltd).

Google Scholar

O’Connor, D., Wardak, D., Goodyear, P., Larkin, P., and Williams, M. (2018). Conceptualising decision-making and its development: a phenomenographic analysis. Sci. Med. Football 2, 261–271. doi: 10.1080/24733938.2018.1472388

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Passos, P., Araújo, D., Davids, K., and Shuttleworth, R. (2008). Manipulating constraints to train decision making in Rugby Union. Int. J. Sports Sci. Coach. 3, 125–140. doi: 10.1260/174795408784089432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Passos, P., Cordovil, R., Fernandes, O., and Barreiros, J. (2012). Perceiving affordances in rugby union. J. Sports Sci. 30, 1175–1182. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2012.695082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paterson, G., van der Kamp, J., Bressan, E., and Savelsbergh, G. (2013). The effects of perception-action coupling on perceptual decision-making in a self-paced far aiming task. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 44, 179–196. doi: 10.7352/IJSP.2013.44.179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poplu, G., Ripoll, H., Mavromatis, S., and Baratgin, J. (2008). How do expert soccer players encode visual information to make decisions in simulated game situations? Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 79, 392–398. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2008.10599503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, A., Collins, D., Stoszkowski, J., and Pill, S. (2019). Coaching games: comparisons and contrasts. Int. Sport Coach. J. 6, 126–131. doi: 10.1123/iscj.2018-0015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, A., Collins, D., Stoszkowski, J., and Pill, S. (2020). Strategic understandings: an investigation of professional academy youth soccer coaches’ interpretation, knowledge, and application of game strategies. Int. Sport Coach. J. 7, 151–162. doi: 10.1123/iscj.2019-0022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raab, M. (2003). Decision making in sports: influence of complexity on implicit and explicit learning. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1, 406–433. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2003.9671728

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raab, M., and Araújo, D. (2019). Embodied cognition with and without mental representations: the case of embodied choices in sports. Front. Psychol. 10:1825. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raab, M., Bar-Eli, M., Plessner, H., and Araújo, D. (2019). The past, present and future of research on judgment and decision making in sport. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 42, 25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.10.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raab, M., and Gigerenzer, G. (2015). The power of simplicity: a fast-and-frugal heuristics approach to performance science. Front. Psychol. 6:1672. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raab, M., and Laborde, S. (2011). When to blink and when to think: preference for intuitive decisions results in faster and better tactical choices. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 82, 89–98. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2011.10599725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reardon, C., Tobin, D. P., Tierney, P., and Delahunt, E. (2017). The worst case scenario: locomotor and collision demands of the longest periods of gameplay in professional rugby union. PLoS One 12:e0177072. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ribeiro, J., Davids, K., Araújo, D., Guilherme, J., Silva, P., and Garganta, J. (2019a). Exploiting bi-directional self-organizing tendencies in team sports: the role of the game model and tactical principles of play. Front. Psychol. 10:2213. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ribeiro, J., Davids, K., Araújo, D., Silva, P., Ramos, J., Lopes, R., et al. (2019b). The role of hypernetworks as a multilevel methodology for modelling and understanding dynamics of team sports performance. Sports Med. 49, 1337–1344. doi: 10.1007/s40279-019-01104-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, P., and Collins, D. (2020). Team cognition in sport: how current insights into how teamwork is achieved in naturalistic settings can lead to simulation studies. Front. Psychol. 11:81. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, P., Collins, D., and Mascarenhas, D. (2017). Developing team decision-making: a holistic framework integrating both on-field and off-field pedagogical coaching process. Sport Coach. Rev. 6, 57–75. doi: 10.1080/21640629.2016.1200819

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, P., Collins, D., and Mascarenhas, D. R. D. (2012). Developing rapid high-pressure team decision-making skills. The integration of slow deliberate reflective learning within the competitive performance environment: a case study of elite netball. Reflect. Pract. 13, 407–424. doi: 10.1080/14623943.2012.670111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roca, A., Ford, P. R., McRobert, A. P., and Williams, A. M. (2011). Identifying the processes underpinning anticipation and decision-making in a dynamic time-constrained task. Cogn. Process. 12, 301–310. doi: 10.1007/s10339-011-0392-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roca, A., Ford, P. R., McRobert, A. P., and Williams, A. M. (2013). Perceptual-cognitive skills and their interaction as a function of task constraints in soccer. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 35, 144–155. doi: 10.1123/jsep.35.2.144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roca, A., Williams, A. M., and Ford, P. R. (2012). Developmental activities and the acquisition of superior anticipation and decision making in soccer players. J. Sports Sci. 30, 1643–1652. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2012.701761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sayers, M. (2005). “A three dimensional analysis of lineout throwing in rugby union,” in Science in Football V, eds T. Reilly, J. Cabri, and D. Araújo (New York, NY: Routledge), 95–102.

Google Scholar

Sayers, M. (2011). Kinematic analysis of line-out throwing in elite international rugby union. J. Sports Sci. Med. 10, 553–558.

Google Scholar

Schulz, A. W. (2018). Efficient Cognition: The Evolution of Representational Decision Making. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Silva, P., Duarte, R., Sampaio, J., Aguiar, P., Davids, K., Araújo, D., et al. (2014). Field dimension and skill level constrain team tactical behaviours in small-sided and conditioned games in football. J. Sports Sci. 32, 1888–1896. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2014.961950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silva, P., Garganta, J., Araujo, D., Davids, K., and Aguiar, P. (2013). Shared knowledge or shared affordances? Insights from an ecological dynamics approach to team coordination in sports. Sports Med. 43, 765–772. doi: 10.1007/s40279-013-0070-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, H. A. (1956). Rational choice and the structure of the environment. Psychol. Rev. 63, 129–138. doi: 10.1037/h0042769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, B., and Caddick, N. (2012). Qualitative methods in sport: a concise overview for guiding social scientific sport research. Asia Pac. J. Sport Soc. Sci. 1, 60–73. doi: 10.1080/21640599.2012.701373

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steiner, S., Macquet, A.-C., and Seiler, R. (2017). An integrative perspective on interpersonal coordination in interactive team sports. Front. Psychol. 8:1440. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suits, B. (1978). The Grasshopper: Games, Life, and Utopia. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

Google Scholar

Swann, C., Moran, A., and Piggott, D. (2015). Defining elite athletes: issues in the study of expert performance in sport psychology. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 16, 3–14. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.07.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, J., and Collins, D. (2019). Shoulda, Coulda, Didnae - Why Don’t high-potential players make it? Sport Psychol. 33, 85–96. doi: 10.1123/tsp.2017-0153

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tee, J. C., Ashford, M., and Piggott, D. (2018). A tactical periodization approach for rugby union. Strength Cond. J. 40, 1–13. doi: 10.1519/SSC.0000000000000390

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tee, J. C., Lambert, M. I., and Coopoo, Y. (2017). Impact of fatigue on positional movements during professional rugby union match play. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 12, 554–561. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.2015-0695

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thereau, J. (2003). “Course of action analysis & Course of action centred design,” in Handbook of Cognitive Task Design, ed. E. Hollanger (Mahwah, NJ: Lawerence Erlbaum), 55–81. doi: 10.1201/9781410607775.ch4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todd, P. M., and Gigerenzer, G. (2001). Putting naturalistic decision making into the adaptive toolbox. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 14, 381–383. doi: 10.1002/bdm.396

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toner, J., Montero, B. G., and Moran, A. (2015). The perils of automaticity. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 19, 431–442. doi: 10.1037/gpr0000054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toner, J., and Moran, A. (2015). Enhancing performance proficiency at the expert level: considering the role of ‘somaesthetic awareness’. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 16, 110–117. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.07.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tracy, S. J. (2010). Qualitative quality: eight “Big-Tent” criteria for excellent qualitative research. Qual. Inq. 16, 837–851. doi: 10.1177/1077800410383121

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trewartha, G., Preatoni, E., England, M. E., and Stokes, K. A. (2015). Injury and biomechanical perspectives on the rugby scrum: a review of the literature. Br. J. Sports Med. 49, 425–433. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2013-092972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varela, F., and Shear, J. (1999). The view from within: first-person methodologies in the study of consciousness. J. Conscious. Stud. 6, 1–14.

Google Scholar

Von Cranach, M., and Harré, R. (1982). The Analysis of Action: Recent Theoretical and Empirical Advances. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Williams, A. M., and Hodges, N. J. (2005). Practice, instruction and skill acquisition in soccer: challenging tradition. J. Sports Sci. 23, 637–650. doi: 10.1080/02640410400021328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, A. M., and Jackson, R. C. (2019). Anticipation in sport: Fifty years on, what have we learned and what research still needs to be undertaken? Psychol. Sport Exerc. 42, 16–24. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.11.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, R., Dicks, M., Milligan, G., Poolton, J., and Alder, D. (2018). An examination of action capabilities and movement time during a soccer anticipation task. Mov. Sport Sci. 102, 61–70. doi: 10.1051/sm/2019001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: rugby union, decision making, time, mental representation, situation, self-confrontation

Citation: Ashford M, Abraham A and Poolton J (2021) What Cognitive Mechanism, When, Where, and Why? Exploring the Decision Making of University and Professional Rugby Union Players During Competitive Matches. Front. Psychol. 12:609127. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.609127

Received: 22 September 2020; Accepted: 13 April 2021;
Published: 12 May 2021.

Edited by:

Quincy J. Almeida, Wilfrid Laurier University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Bessem Mkaouer, University of Manouba, Tunisia
Carlos Eduardo Gonçalves, University of Coimbra, Portugal

Copyright © 2021 Ashford, Abraham and Poolton. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michael Ashford, ad5631@Coventry.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.